network oscillations in the prefrontal cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex...

84
VU Research Portal Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex van Aerde, K.I. 2008 document version Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Link to publication in VU Research Portal citation for published version (APA) van Aerde, K. I. (2008). Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex: Cortical Interactions and Columnar Microcircuits. General rights Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. • Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research. • You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain • You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal ? Take down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. E-mail address: [email protected] Download date: 26. Jul. 2021

Upload: others

Post on 28-Feb-2021

6 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

VU Research Portal

Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex

van Aerde, K.I.

2008

document versionPublisher's PDF, also known as Version of record

Link to publication in VU Research Portal

citation for published version (APA)van Aerde, K. I. (2008). Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex: Cortical Interactions and ColumnarMicrocircuits.

General rightsCopyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright ownersand it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights.

• Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research. • You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain • You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal ?

Take down policyIf you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediatelyand investigate your claim.

E-mail address:[email protected]

Download date: 26. Jul. 2021

Page 2: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Network Oscillations in thePrefrontal Cortex

Cortical Interactions and Columnar Microcircuits

Page 3: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

The studies described in this thesis have been performed at the Department of IntegrativeNeurophysiology of the Center for Neurogenomics and Cognitive Research at the VUUniversity Amsterdam.

The cover shows two neurons (nerve cells) from a cell culture, one star shaped (top),one pear shaped (bottom), after histological staining. The dark purple dots around thestar shaped cell body and along its extensions, the so called dendrites, are synapses: theplaces where the neuron is contacted by other cells (here invisible) from the culture.

Druk: DPP-UtrechtISBN 978 90 8659 252 4

Page 4: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

VRIJE UNIVERSITEIT

Network Oscillations in thePrefrontal Cortex

Cortical Interactions and Columnar Microcircuits

ACADEMISCH PROEFSCHRIFT

ter verkrijging van de graad Doctor aande Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam,op gezag van de rector magnificus

prof.dr. L.M. Bouter,in het openbaar te verdedigen

ten overstaan van de promotiecommissievan de faculteit der Aard- en Levenswetenschappen

op woensdag 5 november 2008 om 13.45 uurin de aula van de universiteit,

De Boelelaan 1105

door

Karlijn Ingeborg van Aerde

geboren te Tilburg

Page 5: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

promotor: prof.dr. A.B. Brussaard

copromotor: prof.dr. H.D. Mansvelder

Page 6: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Content

1 Introduction 7

2 Existence of two Subnetworks 19

3 Participation of Interneurons 33

4 Cortical Interaction 43

5 Discussion 53

A Materials and Methods 59

B Supplemental Tables and Figures 67

References 73

Summary 77

Samenvatting 79

Acknowledgements 81

List of Publications 82

Curriculum Vitae 83

Page 7: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention
Page 8: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Chapter 1

Introduction

Rationale of this thesis

Working memory and (selective) attention are necessary for normal performance. Whenthese functions are impaired, symptoms will occur that are seen for instance in patientssuffering from attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). They have difficulty ig-noring some sensory input (like that it started raining outside), and concentrating onsomething particular (like what the teacher says). Medications used to treat ADHD,such as methylphenidate (Ritalin), improve attention function most likely by interferingwith dopamine signaling, but possibly also by interfering with acetylcholine signaling(Castellanos and Tannock, 2002). However, neuronal network mechanisms underlyingworking memory and attention behavior remain elusive.

Brain activity during attention and working memory tasks can be visualized by theelectric encephalogram (EEG, Figure 1). Electrical oscillations in the beta (15–30 Hz) andgamma (30–100 Hz)-band of the human EEG are associated with cognitive processing andincrease when human subjects are performing in a working memory task (Tallon-Baudryet al., 1998; Fries et al., 2001; Howard et al., 2003; Palva et al., 2005). People sufferingfrom attention disorders show abnormalities in power and phase relations in theta andgamma frequencies (Yordanova et al., 2001; Ford et al., 2007). These abnormalities couldreflect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved inprocessing information necessary for attention and working memory function. However,how oscillations in the human EEG relate to activity in cortical neuronal networks ispoorly understood.

Oscillations exist in all shapes and sizes and can be measured by EEG

Human brain oscillations were described for the first time by Hans Berger in 1924. Heperformed the measurements on his 10-year old son, who was sitting quietly in a chairwith his eyes closed. He called the ∼ 10 Hz oscillations that were dominating the signal,the alpha rhythm. Later other rhythms were described as well: beta (15–30 Hz) andgamma (30–100 Hz) oscillations, when subjects had their eyes open, and slower delta(0.5–4 Hz) and theta (4–8 Hz) oscillations that are dominant during sleep. What becameapparent from these early studies is that the electrical oscillatory activity in the human

7

Page 9: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

8

a b

1 sec

50 µV

aroused

relaxed

sleepy

asleep

deep sleep

Figure 1.1: EEG reflects different behavioral states. (a) EEG set-up. (b) example EEG tracesduring different states ranging from deep sleep to arousal display different oscillation frequencies.

EEG is correlated with the behavioral state of the person: either awake or asleep; relaxedor alert (Figure 1.1); and ultimately on a more gross level: alive or (brain-) dead (Buzsakiand Draguhn, 2004).

On a more subtle level, the occurrence of specific frequency bands in EEG record-ings correlates with aspects of cognitive performance and brain activity. High-frequencyoscillations in the beta (14–30 Hz) and gamma range (30–80 Hz) have been linked tocortical output, communication between remote cortical sites, attentional processing andworking memory (Figure 1.2) both in humans (Tallon-Baudry et al., 1998; Howard et al.,2003; Kahana, 2006; Fan et al., 2007) and animals (Bouyer et al., 1987; Fries et al., 2001;Pesaran et al., 2002; Buzsaki and Draguhn, 2004). Gamma oscillations also appear dur-ing REM (rapid-eye-movement) sleep: the phase of the sleep cycle when people dream,and hence “see” and “hear” things and process information, although the informationis not from the outside world. Thus, understanding how the occurrence of specific fre-quencies in the EEG is brought about by neuronal network activity may be a first stepto understanding how neuronal network activity in the brain and cognitive behavior arerelated.

What kind of activity is reflected in the EEG?

EEG electrodes measure voltage changes that are resulting from electrical currents. Theelectrical currents are generated by neuronal activity: when neurotransmitters activatepost-synaptic ionotropic receptors to become permeable to positively charged ions, alocal current sink is generated (Figure 1.3). A current source is formed when positivecharge is flowing out of the cell into the extracellular space. Thus, when glutamate ac-tivates AMPA-receptors and Na+-ions flow into the postsynaptic neuron current sink isgenerated that will locally be registered as a negative potential change. When GABA-receptors are activated and Cl−-ions are flowing into the cell, a current source is generatedthat leads to a positive potential change at the extracellular electrode. Local sinks and

Page 10: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Introduction 9

a

b

Memory condition

Control (dimming) condition

c

d

-400 0 400 800 1200 1600 ms

-400 0 400 800 1200 1600 ms

S1 ON DELAY S2 ON

Fre

quen

cy (

Hz)

100

70

45

30

20

15

Time (ms)0 400 800 1200

Time (ms)0 400 800 1200

Fre

quen

cy (

Hz)

100

70

45

30

20

15

Figure 1.2: High-frequency oscillations are associated with working memory. (a–b) Experi-mental design. In the memory condition, two stimuli, S1 and S2, were presented for 400 ms separatedby a 800 ms delay. Subjects were to detect matching S2 (20% of the trials). In the dimming condition(b), no second stimulus appeared. Instead, the fixation cross could either dim (80% of the trials) orremain the same (20%), in which case subjects had to respond. In this control condition, S1 does nothave to be memorized and acts only as a warning stimulus. (c–d) Timefrequency (TF) representationof the energy averaged across single trials at electrode C3, grand average across subjects, in both con-ditions. Time is presented on the x-axis. Frequency is presented on the y-axis on a logarithmic scale.The energy level is coded on a color scale: yellow areas show an enhancement of energy compared withprestimulus level, and red areas show decrease. Three areas of enhanced high-frequency activity canbe observed (white boxes): (1) an ON response, peaking at 280 ms and 30 Hz, higher in the memorythan in the dimming condition; (2) an OFF response, peaking at ;680 ms (e.g., 280 ms after S1 offset),similar in both conditions; and (3) a g-band activity during the delay, in the memory condition only.(Adapted from Tallon-Baudry et al., 1998)

sources generated in single neurons will summate and the EEG electrodes will only reg-ister the sum of the local sinks and sources generated in the cell bodies and dendritesof the cortical neurons (Figure 1.3). When electrical activity in this neuronal networkbecomes synchronized, local sinks and sources become synchronized and electrical oscil-lations show up in the EEG (Steriade et al., 1990; Lopes da Silva, 1991; Steriade et al.,1993). EEG and local field potentials are recorded outside neurons, extracellularly, andreflect the summed (synaptic) activity of the surrounding cells.

The extracellular field potential predominantly reflects the local synaptic currents,and not so much action potentials that are of very short duration (Olejniczak, 2006).

Page 11: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

10

Figure 1.3: Scalp recordings depend on the depth of synaptic activity in the cortex. Left:A potential recorded from a scalp electrode following activation of thalamic inputs. The terminals ofthalamocortical neurons make excitatory connections in layer 4. Thus, the site of the inward currentflow [sink] is in layer 4 and the site of the outward current flow [source] is in the superficial layers.Since the recording electrode on the scalp is closer to the site of outward current flow, it records apositive potential. Right: A potential recorded from excitatory inputs from callosal neurons in thecontralateral cortex that terminate in the superficial layers. A negative potential [upward deflection]is recorded because the electrode is closer to the site of inward current flow than that of the outwardflow. (Adapted from Kandel, 1991)

This is nicely illustrated in an recent in vivo study in cats by (Viswanathan and Freeman,2007). Here, synaptic activity and spiking activity were separated using the visual-systemcircuitry to create stimuli that elicited synaptic activity with and without associated spikedischarges. In this way, the study suggestes that both tissue oxygen levels and the fieldpotential reflected more synaptic than spiking activity.

Do oscillations have a function?

In human and rat brain neurons can fire phase-locked to the field oscillations, i.e. neuronsfire at a specific phase of the oscillation cycle, in the theta- and gamma-band (Csicsvari etal., 2003; Huxter et al., 2003; Klausberger et al., 2003; Jacobs et al., 2007), but does thisshow beyond doubt that brain oscillations serve a function? A long standing debate in theoscillation field is whether field oscillations serve specific functions, or whether they onlyreflect the ongoing neuronal activity, and can be considered as mere epiphenomena. Oneway to solve this issue is to artificially induce field oscillations and study their effects onsingle cells and network activity. When artificial sinusoidal field potentials are applied torat hippocampal slices this results in sinusoidal fluctuations of the membrane potentialof CA3 pyramidal cells, with a linear input-output relationship (Deans et al., 2007).Pyramidal cells are more sensitive to low frequency alternating current fields (10 Hz)than high frequency (50 Hz) current fields, which is inline with resonance data, in whichpyramidal neurons are stimulated with sinusoidal membrane potential changes in whole-cell voltage-clamp experiments (Pike et al., 2000; Deans et al., 2007). Membrane potential

Page 12: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Introduction 11

a

b c

Figure 1.4: Slow oscillatory stimulation enhances declarative memory performance. (a)Time-course of experiment. Indicated are time points of learning and recall of memory tasks, psy-chometric control tests, stimulation intervals, period of lights off (horizontal gray bar), and sleeprepresented by a hypnogram. W, wake; 1-4, sleep stages 1-4. (b) Performance on the declarativepaired-associate memory task across the retention period of nocturnal sleep for stimulation and shamstimulation. Performance is expressed as difference between the number of correct words reported atrecall testing and learning. The list contained 46 experimental word-pairs (**P, 0.01). (c) Performancespeed on the non-declarative procedural motor skill task across the retention interval expressed as thedifference in the number of correctly tapped sequences per 30 s between recall testing and learning.Data are the means ± s.e.m. (adapted from Marshall et al., 2006)

fluctuations of 0.1 mV, which is a fraction of the ∼ 15 mV of depolarization necessary tobring a neuron to spiking, are sufficient to influence spike timing (Radman et al., 2007).At the network level, application of alternating current fields modulates the frequencyand power of on going pharmacologically-induced fast network oscillations (Deans etal., 2007). Exogenously applied field oscillations also influence cognition. In a study byMarshall et al., application of low-frequency oscillations at frontolateral electrodes duringslow-wave sleep, but not during REM sleep, increased the amount of memorized words(Figure 1.4) (Marshall et al., 2006). Thus, field oscillations are not only the reflection ofneuronal activity, but also influence spike timing, network activity and cognition.

In vitro models of fast network oscillations

Fast network oscillations can be induced pharmacologically with several substances suchas the ionotropic glutamate receptor agonist kainate, metabotropic glutamate receptoragonist DHPG and muscarinic acetylcholine receptor agonist carbachol in hippocampus,somatosensory and entorhinal cortex (Whittington et al., 1995; Buhl et al., 1998; Fisahnet al., 1998; Palhalmi et al., 2004). These agonists have in common that they increaseneuronal network activity, albeit via different routes. The fast network oscillations thatare induced via these different pathways differ in frequency and power characteristics andunderlying mechanisms (Palhalmi et al., 2004). Kainate-induced oscillations are mainly

Page 13: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

12

Figure 1.5: Different types of fast network oscillations rely on different mechanisms. Gammaactivity in the hippocampus evoked by ionotropic or metabotropic receptor agonists in vitro. Leftpanel, carbachol-induced gamma oscillations in the CA3 subfield are blocked by both the AMPA (?-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazole propionic acid) receptor antagonist NBQX (2,3-dioxo-6-nitro-1,2,3,4-tetrahydrobenzo[f]quinoxaline-7- sulphonamide) and the GABAA receptor antagonist bicu-culline. Right panel, kainateinduced gamma oscillations in the CA3 region are insensitive to theAMPA receptor antagonist GYKI 53655, but abolished by bicuculline. Gamma oscillatory activitywas investigated with extracellular field potential recording. The schemes above the panels indicateputative mechanisms of gamma activity (IN, interneuron; PN, principal neuron). The diagram inthe center illustrates the relative dependence of oscillations on fast excitatory and inhibitory synaptictransmission in different paradigms. (adapted from Bartos et al., 2007)

dependent on inhibitory transmission, whereas DHPG- and carbachol-induced oscillationsrely on both glutamatergic and GABAergic transmission (Figure 1.5) (Cunningham etal., 2003; Palhalmi et al., 2004; Mann and Paulsen, 2005; Mann et al., 2005; Bartos et al.,2007). These last two types of oscillations again differ in modulation by benzodiazepineZolpidem that changes GABA-receptor kinetics (Palhalmi et al., 2004). It is likely thatthese different types of fast network oscillations occur during different states in vivo. Asacetylcholine levels are increased in the medial prefrontal cortex during visual attentiontasks (Figure 1.6) (Passetti et al., 2000; Kozak et al., 2006; Parikh et al., 2007), theapplication of muscarinic acetylcholine agonist carbachol may mimic this state of highvigilance.

Both power and frequency of oscillations in acute brain slices of hippocampus aretemperature-dependent. Carbachol-induced fast network oscillations recorded at 37 ◦Ccan fall in the gamma frequency range, whereas they fall in the beta frequency range whenrecorded at lower temperatures (Dickinson et al., 2003). The classification of oscillationfrequencies into alpha, beta and gamma bands is commonly used for EEG recordings.However, since electrical oscillations recorded in acute brain slices can not be correlatedto behavioral states, and because mechanisms can depend on the agonist used to inducethe oscillations (Figure 1.5), oscillations recorded in acute brain slices will be referred toas carbachol-induced fast network oscillations throughout this thesis.

Page 14: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Introduction 13

Detected cue

Missed cue

a

b c

Figure 1.6: Acetylcholine concentration is increased during cue detection. (a) Trials involvingcue detection were classified as such based on cue-elicited disengagement from ongoing behavior andmonitoring of the food ports. A missed cue was defined as such based on the absence of a cue-evokedshift in behavior. (b–c) Histograms depicting choline signal levels (using mean and s.e.m.) in trialsinvolving detected and missed cues. (adapted from Parikh et al., 2007)

Fast network oscillations in hippocampus

Gamma-band oscillations have been most extensively studied in the rodent hippocampus(Buzsaki et al., 1983; Bartos et al., 2007). There are several reasons for this. The factthat gamma-band oscillations in the hippocampus occur during specific behaviors, suchas during exploration, makes the hippocampus an attractive model (Bragin et al., 1995;

Page 15: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

14

a b

Figure 1.7: Time relationship between field potentials and spiking activity of identifiedhippocampal neurons during oscillations. (a) Time sequence of firing of different neuron typesduring an oscillatory cycle. Top trace shows representative average field oscillatory wave. Pyramidalcells (PC) fired at the negative peak of the oscillation followed by the interneurons (basket cells (BC);axo-axonic cells (AAC); oriens lacunosum-moleculare cells (OLM); radiatum cells (RC) and interneuronspecific cells (IS). (b) Schematic diagram of the connectivity among phase-coupled neuron types in theCA3 hippocampal circuitry taking part in the gamma oscillation. (adapted from Hajos et al., 2004)

Csicsvari et al., 2003; Montgomery and Buzsaki, 2007). Secondly, hippocampal oscilla-tions display fast network oscillations with much higher power, than oscillations generatedby neocortical networks (Buhl et al., 1998; Chrobak and Buzsaki, 1998; Csicsvari et al.,2003; Mann et al., 2005; Bartos et al., 2007). It is generally assumed that this resultsfrom the one layered pyramidal cell structure of the hippocampal circuit (Bartos et al.,2007). And finally, much is known about the different cell types and their connectivityin the hippocampus (Somogyi and Klausberger, 2005).

Both in vivo and in vitro studies have addressed the role of the different cell typesin generating and sustaining fast network oscillations. In the hippocampus more thanfifteen different types of interneurons have been identified based on axonal and dendriticmorphology and chemical markers (Somogyi and Klausberger, 2005). Both in vivo andin vitro studies show that different interneuron types have distinct roles in sustaininghippocampal network oscillations (Figure 1.7) (Hajos et al., 2004; Somogyi and Klaus-berger, 2005; Oren et al., 2006; Freund, 2007; Tukker et al., 2007). Some interneuronsare participating in theta, gamma and ripple oscillations, whereas other types do not(Klausberger et al., 2003).

For fast network oscillations, fast spiking, palvalbumin-positive (PV) interneurons arethought to control spike timing of pyramidal cells and network oscillations since they tar-get the pyramidal cell soma or axon initial segment forming synapses with predominantlyGABA-receptors containing alpha1 subunits (Figure 1.7) (Hajos et al., 2004; Mann et al.,2005; Freund, 2007). Intrinsic active membrane properties make fast spiking cells suitablefor sustaining high frequency firing (Pike et al., 2000). Non-fast spiking CCK-positive

Page 16: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Introduction 15

Figure 1.8: Synaptic feedback model for gamma oscillations in the hippocampus. A modelof network oscillations generated by the recurrent feedback through an inhibitory loop. (adapted fromMann and Paulsen, 2005)

perisomatic interneurons with synapses dominated by alpha2 subunits (Kawaguchi andKondo, 2002; Bacci et al., 2003) are thought to be more involved in modulation of fastnetwork oscillations and in shaping the activity of subgroups and assemblies of pyrami-dal cells (Freund, 2003; Klausberger et al., 2005). Cannabinoids that mainly affect thenon-fast spiking CCK positive interneurons by activating CB1 receptors, reduce oscilla-tion power in kainate-induced fast network oscillations (Hajos et al., 2000). In contrast,dendritic targeting interneurons including oriens-lacunosum moleculare (O-LM) cells andradiatum cells show less or sometimes no significant phase coupling (Hajos et al., 2004;Oren et al., 2006; Tukker et al., 2007), suggesting a prominent role for perisomatic in-terneurons in fast network oscillations. An exception to this rule seem to be bistratifiedinterneurons, which together with glutamaterigic input from the CA3 area, target den-dritic shafts of pyramidal cells (Tukker et al., 2007). Action potential firing by theseinterneurons occurs strongly phase-locked to gamma oscillations.

Interneurons can also differ in the relative timing to the local oscillation cycle: CCKpositive cells in the CA1 region are found to fire even before pyramidal cell firing, incontrast to all other interneuron types, including fast spiking PV positive cells, whichfire after the pyramidal cell fires (Tukker et al., 2007). This could result from feed-forwardinhibition as in the hippocampal circuitry fast network oscillations are generated in CA3and then relayed to the CA1 area (Fisahn et al., 1998).

From the order of pyramidal cell firing and excitatory and inhibitory inputs a synapticfeedback model has been proposed for hippocampal oscillations, in which pyramidal cellsare connected to other pyramidal cells as well as peri-somatic targeting interneurons(Figure 1.8) (Fisahn et al., 1998; Mann and Paulsen, 2005; Mann et al., 2005; Oren etal., 2006). In this model pyramidal cell spiking is followed shortly by excitatory inputsfrom other synchronized firing pyramidal cells. The EPSPs remain subthreshold howeversince inhibitory inputs through a negative feedback loop arrive after a disynaptic delay.Only when the IPSPs are sufficiently decayed can pyramidal cells fire again, after whichthe next cycle commences.

Page 17: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

16

Fast network oscillations in neocortex

Thus far, carbachol-induced oscillations have been described in hippocampus and entorhi-nal cortex (Fisahn et al., 1998; van Der Linden et al., 1999; Dickson et al., 2000; Mannand Paulsen, 2005). In contrast to the hippocampus, much less is known about which in-terneuron types are involved in generating and sustaining carbachol-induced oscillationsin the neocortex. For kainate-induced gamma oscillations, that may depend on differentmechanisms and are in vivo probably present during a different behavioral state (Figure1.5) than carbachol-induced oscillations, some studies have been performed to study thecellular mechanism (Cunningham et al., 2003; Cunningham et al., 2004; Roopun et al.,2006). Fast-spiking interneurons also seem to play a role in kainate-induced networkoscillations in somatosensory cortex (Roopun et al., 2006). In addition, fast rhythmicbursting pyramidal neurons, also described as chattering cells in layer 2/3 of the auditoryand visual cortex that are capable of high frequency firing, are suggested to be involvedin the generation of gamma band oscillations (Gray and McCormick, 1996; Cunninghamet al., 2004). However, it is not know at what phase of the oscillations cycle these neuronsare firing.

In the prefrontal cortex interneurons have been subdivided in three main cell classesbased on electrophysiological properties: fast spiking, late spiking and non-fast spikingcells (Figure 1.9) (Kawaguchi and Kondo, 2002; Couey et al., 2007). Further subdivisionscan be made based on neurochemical markers and morphology. Like in the hippocampus,perisomatic or basket cells can be positive for parvalbumin or cholecystokinin (CCK), thefirst coinciding with a fast spiking electrophysiological profile, and the latter with a non-fast spiking profile (Kawaguchi and Kondo, 2002). How these interneurons participatein fast network oscillations in the prefrontal cortex is not known.

Aims of this thesis

Oscillations are highly dynamic: followed in time, their frequency and amplitude changescontinuously (Figure 1.1). Even within frequency bands oscillations fluctuate in ampli-tude and frequency (Steriade et al., 1993; Bragin et al., 1995; Tallon-Baudry et al., 1998;Palva et al., 2005). The frequency fluctuations most likely reflect changing states ofneuronal network activity, as brain oscillations arise from the correlated synchronizedactivity of large numbers of neurons (Steriade et al., 1990; Lopes da Silva, 1991; Steriadeet al., 1993). However, the dynamic nature of brain oscillations within frequency bandshas been largely ignored, particularly with respect to frequency shifts. The research inthis thesis focuses on three aspects of neuronal mechanisms underlying the dynamic na-ture of fast network oscillations in the neocortex. Most of the studies were performed inthe prefrontal cortex, but some of the studies were also performed in the visual cortexto assess whether mechanisms extended to other neocortical areas as well. The followingresearch questions were addressed:

1. What are the neuronal mechanisms that govern the dynamic behavior of betaoscillations? Do single pyramidal neurons participate in multiple oscillations fre-quencies within the same frequency band, or do separate neuronal networks giverise to different oscillation frequencies (chapter 2)?

2. Since inhibitory transmission is crucial for sustaining fast network oscillations andlikely to determine oscillation frequency (Whittington et al., 1995; Fisahn et al.,1998; Traub et al., 1998; Palhalmi et al., 2004; Cope et al., 2005; Mann et al., 2005),

Page 18: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Introduction 17

Figure 1.9: Interneuron types in prefrontal cortex. Schematic view of known axon terminaldistributions on pyramidal cell surfaces of four GABA cell subtypes in the rat frontal cortex: parval-bumin local/horizontal arbor basket cells; parvalbumin chandelier cells; somatostatin Martinotti cells;and CCK large basket cells. Protrusions from dendrites from pyramidal cells represent spines, butexcitatory synapses on the spine heads are omitted. (adapted from Kawaguchi et al., 2002)

how will different types of interneurons phase-lock their firing during dynamicfrequency shifts (chapter 3)?

3. How does the connectivity of two neighboring and connected medial prefrontalcortex areas that play distinct roles in cognition and both receive cholinergic input(Jones et al., 2005; Hoover and Vertes, 2007; Ragozzino, 2007), translate itself infast network oscillations (chapter 4)?

To address these questions, we used a multi-disciplinary approach using EEG recordingsfrom humans as well as awake, freely moving rats, and recordings from acute brain slicesfrom rat brain. To address the spatial distribution of oscillations in the cortical networks,we made use of multi-electrode arrays that enable simultaneous field potential recordingsacross cortical layers, and in different medial prefrontal cortex areas, to monitor theeffects of carbachol on the whole cortical microcircuit (Figure 1.10). We combined theserecordings with single cell electrophysiology to establish the role of different cell types infast network oscillations.

Page 19: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

18

b c PrL

IL

PrL

IL

a

100 ms 2 µV

100 ms 2 µV

control + 25 µM carbachol

e g100

50

0

Sp

ike

s/b

in

20100Spike frequency (Hz)

L1

L2

L3

L5

d

150 µm

f

Figure 1.10: General set-up of experiments. (a) Picture of coronal slice containing medial pre-frontal areas prelimbic cortex (PrL) and infralimbic cortex (IL). From The Rat Brain in StereotaxticCoordinates by G. Paxinos and C. Watson, 2005. (b) Slice placed on 8 × 8 multi-electrode array. (c)Simultaneous recorded field potentials in prelimbic (top) and infralimbic (bottom) cortex before (left)and after (right) induction of fast network oscillations by carbachol. (d) Slice on multi-electrode arraywith patch pipette. (e) Reconstructed layer 5 pyramidal neuron. (f) Simultaneous field (top) andsingle cell (bottom) recordings. (g) Spike frequency distribution after carbachol application. Inset:Response after hyper- and depolarizing step current injections of –125 pA and +175 pA (scale bar 20mV, 200 ms).

Page 20: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Chapter 2

Existence of twoSubnetworks

Submitted as a part of

Two independent cortical subnetworks control spike timing in L5 neu-rons during dynamic oscillation shifts

Karlijn I. van Aerde1, Edward O. Mann3, Cathrin B. Canto1,3, Klaus Linkenkaer-Hansen1,4,Marcel van der Roest2, Antonius B. Mulder2, Ole Paulsen1,3, Arjen B. Brussaard1, Huib-ert D. Mansvelder1

1Dept. of Integrative Neurophysiology and 2Dept. of Anatomy and Neurosciences,Center for Neurogenomics and Cognitive Research (CNCR), VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.3The Neuronal Oscillations Group, Dept of Physiology, Anatomy and Genetics, Oxford University, Parks Road, Oxford OX13PT, United Kingdom.4BioMag laboratory, HUSLAB, Hospital district of Helsinki and Uusimaa, P.O. Box 340, FIN-00029HUS, Finland.

Human brain oscillations occur in different frequency bands that have been

linked to different behaviors and cognitive processes. Even within specific

frequency bands oscillations fluctuate in frequency and amplitude. Such fre-

quency fluctuations most likely reflect changes in the correlated synchronized

activity of large numbers of neurons. However, the neuronal mechanisms gov-

erning the dynamic nature of amplitude and frequency fluctuations within

frequency bands have not been investigated. Here we show that frequency

fluctuations are mediated by two distinct cortical networks controlling spike

timing in pyramidal neurons of output layers 5 and 6. Layer 5, but not

layer 6, pyramidal neurons participate in both oscillations. Frequency and

phase information is encoded and relayed between these networks through

timed excitatory and inhibitory synaptic transmission. Our data indicate

that frequency fluctuations reflect synchronized activity in distinct neuronal

networks, and suggest that cortical subnetworks can process information in

a parallel fashion.

19

Page 21: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

20 Chapter 2

Introduction

Brain oscillations occur in different frequency bands that have been linked to differentbehaviors and cognitive processes (Gray et al., 1989; Bragin et al., 1995; Tallon-Baudryet al., 1998; Fries et al., 2001; Pesaran et al., 2002; Buzsaki and Draguhn, 2004; Kahana,2006; Fan et al., 2007). Even so, brain oscillations in multiple frequency bands can occursimultaneously. Human EEG can display theta, alpha and beta oscillations within thesame session, and rats show gamma oscillations on top of theta waves during exploratorybehavior (Bragin et al., 1995; Palva et al., 2005; Kahana, 2006). Single neurons have beenreported to fire phase-locked to oscillations in multiple frequency bands, such as thetaand gamma oscillations (Bragin et al., 1995; Jacobs et al., 2007). In these cases the higherfrequency oscillation is typically some multitude of the lower frequency oscillations, andhence is likely to show phase synchrony (Palva et al., 2005).

Followed in time, oscillations also fluctuate in frequency and amplitude even withinspecific frequency bands (Steriade et al., 1993; Bragin et al., 1995; Tallon-Baudry etal., 1998; Palva et al., 2005). These frequency fluctuations are small, and frequencyshifts occur between non-harmonic frequencies. The frequency fluctuations most likelyreflect changing states of neuronal network activity, as brain oscillations arise from thecorrelated synchronized activity of large numbers of neurons (Steriade et al., 1990; Lopesda Silva, 1991; Steriade et al., 1993). However, the dynamic nature of brain oscillationswithin frequency bands has been largely ignored, particularly with respect to frequencyshifts. Whether such small frequency fluctuations are the result of variation in the sameneuronal network, or whether separate neuronal networks give rise to different oscillationfrequencies is not known.

Here, we investigate the dynamic nature of high-frequency oscillations in recordingsfrom rat brain slices, and address the question whether individual cortical output neuronscan synchronize their firing to these different frequencies. High-frequency oscillations inthe beta (15–30 Hz) and gamma range (30–80 Hz) have been linked to cognitive pro-cessing and working memory in humans (Tallon-Baudry, Bertrand et al., 1998; Nikolaev,Ivanitsky et al., 2001; Howard, Rizzuto et al., 2003; Fan, Byrne et al., 2007) and animals(Fries, Reynolds et al., 2001; Pesaran, Pezaris et al., 2002). Studies in awake animalsshow that during working memory acetylcholine levels increase in prelimbic and infralim-bic prefrontal cortex, and that these increased cholinergic levels are necessary for accurateperformance (Passetti, Dalley et al., 2000; McGaughy, Dalley et al., 2002; Kozak, Brunoet al., 2006; Parikh, Kozak et al., 2007). In vitro, cholinergic agonists such as carbachol,induce fast network oscillations in acute slices of rodent cortex (Buhl, Tamas et al., 1998;Chrobak and Buzsaki 1998; Csicsvari, Jamieson et al., 2003; Mann, Suckling et al., 2005;Bartos, Vida et al., 2007). We find that two independent networks in superficial and deepcortical layers generate oscillations that differ only a couple of Herz in frequency. Singlelayer 5, but not layer 6 pyramidal neurons phase-lock their action potential firing to bothoscillations. Timed inhibitory and excitatory synaptic transmission received by layer 5pyramidal neurons relays information on the phase of these oscillations and pyramidalneurons adjust the timing of firing accordingly.

Page 22: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Existence of two Subnetworks 21

Methods summary

Slice preparation and electrophysiology

Prefrontal coronal cortical slices (400 µm) were prepared from P14−28 Wistar rats,in accordance with Dutch license procedures. After recovery, slices were mounted on8 × 8 arrays of planar micro-electrodes (Tensor Biosciences, Irvine, CA). Pyramidalcells located in L5 and L6 were selected on the basis of their morphology and firingpattern (Table B.2). Excitatory and inhibitory postsynaptic currents (EPSCs and IPSCs)were recorded in whole-cell voltage clamp at −70 mV and +20 mV, respectively. Allexperiments were performed at 25 ◦C.

Human EEG recordingsOngoing electrical brain activity in sessions of 20 min was measured with electroen-

cephalography (EEG) from eight normal subjects (aged 20–30 years). The study wasapproved by the Ethics Committee of the Department of Radiology of the Helsinki Uni-versity Central Hospital.

Intracranial local field recordings in the awake ratFive adult male Wistar rats (350–450 g) were anaesthetized and mounted in a Kopf

stereotaxic frame. After skull exposure, a burr hole was drilled to allow mPFC tetrodeplacement (AP: +3.2 mm; ML +0.7 mm and DV 2.5–3.5 mm with respect to bregmaand cortical surface). Recordings of the mPFC local field potentials were made in thehome cage under video surveillance. For each animal 5–6 minute periods of quiet restwere selected for further analysis. All experiments were conducted in accordance withDutch license procedures.

Data analysis

Electrophysiological data was analyzed using custom-written procedures in Igor Pro(Wavemetrics, OR, USA).

Spike, EPSC and IPSC timing relative to fieldThe spike, EPSC and IPSC timing in single cells was analyses relative to the ongoing

field oscillation. Spikes were detected using simple threshold detection. Synaptic eventswere detected using the Mini Analysis Program (Synaptosoft Inc.). Circular statisticswere used to test whether individual cells showed phase locking to the network oscillation,and cells not significantly coupled to the network were excluded from further analysis(Rayleigh test, p < 0.05).

CSDAll signals were peak-to-peak averaged relative to a reference recording from super-

ficial layer 5 or layer 6. CSD plots are shown using an inverted color scale, with warmcolors corresponding to current sinks (i.e., neuronal membrane inward currents) and coolcolors corresponding to current sources.

StatisticsData are represented as mean ± s.e.m. Statistical analysis used either the Student’s

t test (paired or unpaired) or an ANOVA with Student Newman Keuls post-hoc test,as appropriate. Correlation analysis was performed calculating Pearson correlation co-efficients. For nonparametric data the Mann-Whitney test was used. Circular statisticsused the Rayleigh test, and the Watson-Williams test. Asterisks represent p < 0.05.

Page 23: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

22 Chapter 2

Results

Distinct beta oscillations in human EEG fluctuate in time

Brain oscillations observed in human EEGs are not stable over time and show fre-quency and amplitude fluctuations, even within single frequency bands (Figure 2.1b).EEG recordings were obtained from electrodes at frontal and occipital locations of eighthealthy human subjects who were resting with their eyes closed for 20 minutes (Figure2.1a). Within the beta-band, distinct slow and fast frequencies appeared intermittentlyat frontal electrodes of all subjects and at occipital electrodes in 5 out of 8 subjects (Fig-ure 2.1b,c, occipital data: Table B.1). Slow beta oscillations at frontal electrodes had apeak in the power spectrum at a frequency of 17.7 ± 0.8 Hz (mean ± s.e.m.), whereas fastbeta oscillations had a peak frequency of 22.9 ± 0.8 Hz (Figure 2.1d). Wavelet analysis(Torrence and Compo, 1998) showed that these beta activities fluctuated in magnitudeover time, showing periods of clearly elevated or reduced magnitude (Figure 2.1e). Thepower of slow and fast beta oscillations was significantly above the 1/f noise only in shortepisodes. The duration of these episodes was similar for both frequencies (Figure 2.1f;slow beta 143 ± 10 ms; fast beta 149 ± 11 ms; p = 0.63; mean of medians ± s.e.m.),but fast beta oscillation episodes were more frequent than slow episodes (fast beta 3.6± 1.9 Hz, slow beta 3.0 ± 0.1 Hz; p = 0.01). Slow and fast beta oscillations occurredsimultaneously for 44.7 ± 4.1% of the total recording time (Figure 2.1g), whereas only asingle frequency was present for 36.7 ± 3.2% of the total recording time. In about 20%of the total recording time no significant beta oscillation was observed (Figure 2.1g). Totest whether the occurrence of slow and fast beta oscillation frequencies are independentof each other or show interdependence, we tested whether the magnitude of differentfrequencies within the beta band are correlated, anti-correlated or uncorrelated in time.The magnitude of slow and fast beta oscillations showed only a weak positive correlation(r = 0.43 ± 0.08; p < 0.01 in all subjects). The magnitude of beta oscillations was onlyvery weakly correlated to the magnitude of the alpha oscillation (slow beta r = 0.15 ±0.03, fast beta r = 0.03 ± 0.02, p < 0.01 in all subjects), suggesting that the occurrenceof beta oscillations did not depend on the presence of oscillations in the alpha band.Thus, fast and slow beta oscillations appear to be only weakly correlated and occur bothsimultaneous and separate from each other, which could indicate that they are generatedby distinct neuronal networks that synchronize at different frequencies.

Fast and slow beta oscillations in the prefrontal cortex of awake rats

To investigate whether fast and slow beta frequencies are generated by distinct neuronalnetworks we turned to the rodent brain. We first examined if brain oscillations in thebeta band show similar frequency fluctuations in awake, freely moving rats (Figure 2.2).Recordings were obtained from the medial prefrontal cortex when animals were at quietrest in the homecage. In three out of five animals, frequency fluctuations occurred in thebeta band (Figure 2.2b). Slow beta oscillations had a peak in the power spectrum at afrequency of 15.8 ± 0.3 Hz, whereas fast beta oscillations had a peak frequency of 22.0± 1.7 Hz. As in the human EEG data, the power of slow and fast beta oscillations wassignificantly above the 1/f noise only in short episodes (Figure 2.2c). Both the frequencyand the duration of these episodes were comparable to the human EEG data (frequencyepisodes: 2.5 ± 0.1 Hz (slow), 3.7 ± 0.4 Hz (fast), n = 3, p = 0.03; duration episodes:Figure 2.2d, 150 ± 5 ms (slow), 110 ± 9 ms (fast), n = 3, p = 0.09). Slow and fast

Page 24: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Existence of two Subnetworks 23

a

f

dc

e

g10

5

0

% E

pis

od

es

1.00.50.0

Duration (s)

slow

fast

fast slow

4

2

0

Po

we

r (p

V2)

806040200

Frequency (Hz)

2520

20 fV2

b

30

20

10Fre

qu

en

cy

(H

z)

slow fast

250 ms 10 µV

30

20

10

Fre

qu

en

cy

(H

z)

1 s 10 µV

Fz

Oz

50 ms 2 µV

fast

slow

100 ms

10 µV

60

40

20

0

% T

ime

slow fast both none

Oscillations

Figure 2.1: Human EEG contains slow and fast β oscillations. (a) EEG scalp recording set-up. (b) Example traces from frontal electrode. Left: 1-sec traces. Right: slow and fast β oscillationepisodes. (c) Power spectrum of recording in (b). Right: double Gaussian fit (red) on expanded trace.(d) Frequency of β oscillations. (e) Frequency fluctuations of EEG recording (bottom) analyzed usingwavelet transform (top) with warmer colors representing increasing magnitude. Right: expanded timescale from boxed region in left. Arrows point to slow and fast episodes. (f) Duration of episodes. (g)Occurrence of slow and fast β oscillations.

beta oscillations occurred simultaneously for 29.9 ± 1.7% of the total recording time(Figure 2.2e), whereas only a single frequency was present for 41.1 ± 5.0% of the totalrecording time. In about 30% of the total recording time no significant beta oscillationwas observed (Figure 2.2e). As in the human EEG data, the magnitude of slow and fastbeta oscillations in the awake rat showed only a weak positive correlation (r = 0.42 ±0.14; p < 0.01 in all rats). This shows that fast and slow beta frequency oscillations havesimilar characteristics in human and rat brain, and suggests that also in rat prefrontal

Page 25: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

24 Chapter 2

a

c

d e

Fre

qu

ency

(H

z)

3020

10

1 s50 µV

b

250 ms 50 µV

40

20

0

% T

ime

slow fast both none

15

10

5

0

% E

pis

od

es

1.00.50.0Duration (s)

slow

fast

250 ms 100 µV

fast slow

Oscillations

Figure 2.2: Intracranial field recordings in the awake rat contain slow and fast β oscillations.(a) Intracranial field recording set-up. (b) Band-pass (10–30 Hz) filtered field recordings. Boxesindicate slow and fast β oscillation episodes. (c) Frequency fluctuations of field recording analyzedusing wavelet transform (top) with warmer colors representing increasing magnitude. For clarity band-pass filtered field potential are shown at the bottom. Right: expanded time scale from boxed regionin left. Arrows point to slow and fast episodes. (d) Duration of episodes. (e) Occurrence of slow andfast β oscillations.

cortex distinct beta frequencies may be generated by synchronization of distinct neuronalnetworks.

Slow and fast oscillations are distributed over superficial and deep cor-tical layers

To investigate whether slow and fast beta oscillations are generated by the synchroniza-tion of neuronal activity in distinct neuronal networks, we studied the spatial distri-bution of oscillations in rat medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC). Acute mPFC slices weremounted on planar 8 × 8 multi-electrode arrays and oscillations were induced by appli-cation of the cholinergic agonist carbachol (Figure 2.3a,b) (Shimono et al., 2000; Mannet al., 2005). These oscillations were dependent on intact glutamatergic and GABAergicsynaptic transmission (Figure B.1, Appendix B). Fourier analysis revealed that two non-harmonic frequency components were present in 20 out of 38 mPFC slices (Figure 2.3b,c).

Page 26: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Existence of two Subnetworks 25

In 18 out of 38 mPFC slices only one oscillation peak was present, which displayed max-imal power most often in L3/5 (prelimbic PFC (PrL): 8/11 max in L3/5, 3/11 max inL6; infralimbic PFC (IL): 7/7 max in L3/5). The more superficial L3/5 always oscillatedat a higher frequency than the deep layer 6 (Figure 2.3d; PrL: 16.6 ± 1.0 Hz (L3/5),11.2 ± 0.5 Hz (L6), n = 14, p < 0.01; IL: 14.7 ± 0.7 Hz (L3/5), 10.6 ± 0.5 Hz (L6), n= 6, p < 0.01). Since both power and frequency of oscillations in acute brain slices aretemperature-dependent (Figure B.1e)(Dickinson et al., 2003), the experiments were per-formed at 25 ◦C. Linear extrapolation suggests that at physiological temperatures, bothoscillation frequencies would fall within 25–40 Hz (Figure B.1e). Current-source-density(CSD) analysis (Shimono et al., 2001) showed that two sink-source pairs were presentin the mPFC: one between L1/2 and L5, and the other between superficial L5 and L6(Figure 2.3e), indicating that the two oscillation frequencies are generated by distinctsubnetworks.

As in humans and awake rats, both slow and fast oscillations appeared in shortepisodes (Figure 2.3f,g). The length of the episodes was somewhat longer than theepisode lengths observed in the human and awake rat recordings. In the rat mPFCslices the slow oscillation episodes tended to last longer than the fast oscillation episodes(Figure 2.3g, PrL: slow 251 ± 33 ms, fast 161 ± 17 ms, p < 0.01, IL: slow 415 ± 36 ms;fast 316 ± 31 ms; p < 0.01). Fast oscillation episodes appeared more often than slowoscillation episodes (PrL: slow 1.4 ± 0.1 Hz, fast 2.4 ± 0.1 Hz, p < 0.01; IL: slow 1.4 ±0.1 Hz, fast 1.9 ± 0.2 Hz, p < 0.01). Similar to the human and rat brain recordings, slowand fast oscillations in the slice appeared simultaneously for a substantial fraction of thetotal recording time (Figure 2.3h; PrL 28.7 ± 5.9%; IL 65.4 ± 4.9%), but also occurredseparate from each other (Figure 2.3h; PrL 37.42.2

The distribution of slow and fast oscillations over cortical layers was not unique toprelimbic and infralimbic PFC. Carbachol-induced oscillations showed very similar spatialand temporal distributions of slow and fast oscillation frequencies in visual cortex areasV1 and V2 (Figure B.2). Thus, frequency fluctuations in brain oscillations resulting fromdistributed neuronal networks may well be a general characteristic of cortical activity.

Layer 5, but not layer 6, pyramidal cells fire phase-locked to both slowand fast oscillations

Since both slow and fast oscillations were recorded in L5, we asked whether individualneocortical L5 pyramidal neurons fire phase-locked to both these frequencies. We madecell-attached and whole-cell recordings from visually identified L5 pyramidal neurons inPrL and IL PFC areas (Table B.2) while recording slow and fast oscillations with theelectrode grid (Figure 2.4). The overall firing rate of L5 pyramidal neurons during theseconditions was ∼ 3 Hz (PrL 3.19 ± 0.41 Hz, IL 3.14 ± 0.60 Hz, n = 17). In slices thatshowed two distinct oscillation frequencies, 11 out of 15 pyramidal neurons fired phase-locked to both frequencies with the highest discharge probability during the trough of thelocal oscillation (Figure 2.4d,f). Of the other cells, 2 fired phase-locked only to the fastoscillation, and 2 cells were not phase-locked. The average phase of firing was similarfor slow and fast oscillations both in PrL and IL PFC (Figure B.3a; PrL: slow 2.42± 0.15 rad, fast 2.79 ± 0.03 rad, n.s., n = 4/6; IL slow 3.51 ± 0.14 rad, fast 3.50 ±0.10 rad, n.s., n = 5/5; For phase of firing and phase-coupling analysis only cells thatshowed significant phase-locking with both oscillations at the same reference electrodein L3/5 were included). Due to jitter in action potential timing, phase-coupling betweenspikes and the oscillations was not very tight (Figure B.3b). L6 pyramidal neurons

Page 27: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

26 Chapter 2

30

20

10

Fre

qu

en

cy (

Hz)

2 s 5 µV

b c d

e

g

5 µV

5 µV

200 ms

5 µV

L3/5

L5

L6

2010

Frequency (Hz)

50 fV2

L3/5

L6

L5

h

PrL

IL

a

20

10

Fre

qu

en

cy

(H

z)

L3/5 L6L3/5 L6

* *PrL IL

f

L1L2

L3

L5

L6

36 ms 1 µV

source

sink

L2

L3

L5

L6

36 ms 60 ms

60 ms 500 nV

10

5

0

%E

pis

od

es

1.00.50.0Duration (s)

PrLslowfast

6

3

0

1.00.0

IL

slow

fast

400 ms 5 µV

L3/5 L5 L6L5

300 µm

50

0

% T

ime

slow fast both none

Oscillations above threshold

PrLIL

Figure 2.3: Distribution of slow and fast oscillations over cortical layers in rat brain slices.(a) Mini-slice from prelimbic (PrL) cortex (top,31) placed on a 8 × 8 multi-electrode grid (bottom). (b)Field potentials were simultaneously recorded across cortical layers. Band-pass filtered traces shownin dark colors. (c) Power spectra of recordings in (b). (d) Frequency of oscillations in prelimbic andinfralimbic (IL) cortex. (e) Current-Source-Density analysis (bottom) of peak-to-peak cycle averagedfield potentials (top). Two oscillation cycles are shown for clarity. The white rectangle in (a) marksthe electrodes that were used for CSD analysis. Field potentials are averaged with reference to L5(left, red trace) or to L6 (right, red trace). Black traces autoscaled for comparison. (f) Frequencyfluctuations of field potentials (bottom) analyzed using wavelet transform (top) with warmer colorsrepresenting increasing magnitude. Right: expanded time scale from boxed region in left. (g) Durationof episodes. (h) Occurrence of slow and fast oscillations.

Page 28: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Existence of two Subnetworks 27

b

c

d

100

50

0

Sp

ike

s/b

in

20100

Spike frequency (Hz)

1 µV

50 ms 5 pA

L1

L2

L3

L5

a

e

e’

f

f’

Phase (rad)

10

5

Sp

ike

s (

%)

0 2π

1 µV

64

Phase (rad)0 2π

150 µm

slow fast

slow fast

100 ms

5 pA

2 µV

Sp

ike

s (

%)

Figure 2.4: L5 pyramidal cell firing is controlled by slow and fast oscillations. (a) Slice onmulti-electrode array with patch pipette. (b) Reconstructed L5 pyramidal cell. (c) Spike frequencydistribution after carbachol application. Inset: Response after hyper- and depolarizing step currentinjections of –125 pA and +175 pA (scale bar 20 mV, 200 ms). (d) Field potential in L5 (top) andsimultaneous cell attached recording (bottom). (e–f) Spike averaged field potential (top) and cellularrecording (bottom) of spikes that occurred during slow (e) or fast (f) oscillation episodes. (e–f) Spikeprobability histogram (bottom) of spikes in (e–f) aligned with peak-to-peak cycle average of L5 (top).

Phase (rad)

10

5

Sp

ike

s (

%)

0 2π

500 nV

Phase (rad)

10

5

0 2π

a bslow fast a’ b’

500 nV

50 ms

20 mV

ns

slow fast

Figure 2.5: L6 pyramidal cell firing is only controlled by slow oscillations. (a–b) Spikeaveraged field potential of L6 (a, top) or L5 (b, top) and cellular recording (bottom) of spikes thatoccurred during slow (a) or fast (b) oscillation episodes. (a–b) Spike probability histogram (bottom)of spikes in (a–b) aligned with peak-to-peak cycle average of L6 (a) or L5 (b).

(Table B.2) fired phase-locked only with the slow oscillation when present, and not withthe fast oscillation (Figures 2.5b (n = 3) and B.4). These data suggest that actionpotential discharge in mPFC L5 pyramidal neurons is controlled by both the slow andfast oscillation, whereas L6 cells are controlled only by the slow oscillation.

Page 29: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

28 Chapter 2

Synaptic inputs to layer 5 pyramidal cells carry phase information onboth slow and fast oscillations

Since L5 pyramidal neurons fire phase-locked to both slow and fast oscillations, they mustreceive information about the oscillation phase from the surrounding network for bothfrequencies. Is this information present in the synaptic input to these neurons? To answerthis question, we recorded excitatory and inhibitory postsynaptic currents (EPSCs andIPSCs) received by L5 pyramidal neurons, and found that both EPSCs and IPSCs werephase-locked to the oscillations (Figures 2.6–7). EPSCs had the highest probability ofoccurring shortly after the trough of the local field oscillation for both the slow and fastoscillation (Figure 2.6a–c). The average phase of the EPSC was similar for slow and fastoscillations (Figure B.5a; slow 3.7 ± 0.2 rad, fast 3.7 ± 0.2 rad, p = 0.65, n = 6/7). Thefrequency of EPSCs during fast oscillation episodes was significantly higher than duringslow oscillation episodes (Figure 2.6d; p < 0.05 in 3 out of 3 pyramidal neurons, slow11.9 ± 1.6 Hz, fast 14.9 ± 1.3 Hz).

IPSCs were also phase-locked to both slow and fast oscillations (Figure 2.7a–c), andoccurred with the highest probability during the rising phase of the field at L5 electrodes(Figure 2.7b,c; Figure B.6a; slow 4.9 ± 0.3 rad, fast 4.9 ± 0.2 rad, p = 0.83, n = 6/7).Similar to the EPSC frequency, the frequency of IPSCs was higher during episodes offast oscillations than during episodes of slow oscillations (Figure 2.7d; p < 0.05 in 5 outof 7 pyramidal neurons, slow 10.0 ± 1.3 Hz, fast 12.4 ± 0.7 Hz). IPSCs showed lessjitter than EPSCs relative to the phase of the oscillations (Figure 2.8a,d). For both slowand fast oscillations, the probability distributions of action potentials and EPSCs weresimilar, indicating that action potentials and EPSCs occurred almost simultaneously(Figure 2.8a,b: combined data of slow and fast oscillations). In contrast, IPSCs occurred18.2 ± 2.0 ms after pyramidal neuron spiking (Figure 2.8c). Taken together, these datashow that phase information on both fast and slow oscillation frequencies is relayed toL5 output neurons through excitatory and inhibitory synaptic transmission.

Discussion

Brain oscillations in human EEGs contain many different frequencies that are contin-uously changing over time (Steriade et al., 1993; Bragin et al., 1995; Tallon-Baudry etal., 1998; Palva et al., 2005). Both amplitude and frequency of these oscillatory signalsfluctuate in time. Even within a single frequency band, oscillation frequencies fluctuate(Steriade et al., 1993; Bragin et al., 1995; Tallon-Baudry et al., 1998; Palva et al., 2005).Brain oscillations are the result of correlated synchronized activity of large numbers ofneurons (Steriade et al., 1990; Lopes da Silva, 1991; Steriade et al., 1993) and reflect thecommunication between the neuronal elements involved (Salinas and Sejnowski, 2001;Fries, 2005; Sejnowski and Paulsen, 2006). However, the neuronal mechanisms under-lying frequency fluctuations have not investigated. In this study, we characterized thetime structure of frequency and amplitude fluctuations of oscillations in the beta band(14–30 Hz) and addressed the neuronal mechanisms. We find that both in human EEGand in freely moving rat EEG, non-harmonic beta frequency components appear in shortepisodes of 100 to 200 ms and occur independently from each other. In brain slices,such distinct frequency components are generated by separate cortical subnetworks insuperficial and deep layers of the cortex. Layer 5 pyramidal neurons can synchronize

Page 30: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Existence of two Subnetworks 29

a

b c

b’ c’

8

4

Phase (rad)0 2π

50 ms

500 nV

20 pA

6

2EP

SC

's (

%)

Phase (rad)0 2π

1 µV

slow fast

100 ms

50 pA

2 µV

slow fast

20

10

Fre

qu

en

cy

(H

z)

slow fast

*

**

d

EP

SC

's (

%)

Figure 2.6: L5 pyramidal neurons receive phase information from slow and fast oscillationsthrough EPSCs. (a) Field potential in L5 (top) and simultaneously recorded EPSCs (bottom).(b–c) EPSC averaged field potential (top) and cellular recording (bottom) during slow (b) or fast(c) oscillation episodes. (b–c) EPSC probability histogram (bottom) of EPSCs in (b–c) aligned withaveraged field potential (top). (d) EPSC frequency during episodes of slow and fast oscillations.

their spiking activity to both oscillation frequencies generated in superficial and deepnetworks. Layer 6 pyramidal neurons only synchronize their spiking activity to oscilla-tions generated by deep cortical networks. Information on the phase of fast and slow betaoscillations is received by layer 5 pyramidal neurons through the timing and frequencyof synaptic transmission.

The occurrence of two non-harmonic network oscillations within the same corticalcolumn was not restricted to the prelimbic and infralimbic medial prefrontal cortex ar-eas. We observed a similar columnar distribution of non-harmonic frequencies in networkoscillations generated in superficial and deep layers of visual cortex areas V1 and V2, sug-gesting that this is a general phenomenon in the cortex. In the somatosensory cortex,kainite-induced oscillations show two distinct oscillation frequencies that fall into thebeta and gamma-band (Roopun et al., 2006). In this cortical area gamma frequencyoscillations were present in supragranular layers, and beta frequency oscillations werepresent in subgranular layers. Granular layer 4 displayed both frequencies in the powerspectrum. It is not know what the cellular mechanisms underlying these somatosensorynetwork oscillations are, but since they were induced by kainite instead of cholinergicagonists, they most likely depend on different mechanisms (Fisahn et al., 2004; Bartoset al., 2007). For instance, the kainate-induced beta oscillations in layer 5 of the so-

Page 31: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

30 Chapter 2

a slow fast

b c

b’ c’

1 µV

10

0IPS

Cs

(%

)

Phase (rad)0 2π

10

5

Phase (rad)0 2π

2 µV

100 ms

20 pA

50 ms

20 pA

1 µV

slow fast

15

10

5

Fre

qu

en

cy

(H

z)

slow fast

**

***

d

IPS

Cs

(%

)

Figure 2.7: L5 pyramidal neurons receive phase information from slow and fast oscillationsthrough IPSCs. (a) Field potential in L5 (top) and simultaneously recorded IPSCs (bottom). (b–c)IPSC averaged field potential (top) and cellular recording (bottom) during slow (b) or fast (c) oscillationepisodes. (b–c) IPSC probability histogram (bottom) of IPSCs in (b–c) aligned with averaged fieldpotential (top). (d) IPSC frequency during episodes of slow and fast oscillations.

matosensory cortex did not depend on synaptic transmission. It is not known whetherlayer 4 neurons fired phase-locked to one or both oscillation frequencies (Roopun et al.,2006). In contrast, the cholinergic agonist-induced fast network oscillations in PFC aredepended on intact GABAergic and glutamatergic transmission, and layer 5 pyramidalneurons showed timed spiking activity correlating with the phase information providedby GABAergic and glutamatergic inputs.

The order of pyramidal cell firing and excitatory and inhibitory inputs are in linewith the synaptic feedback model for hippocampal oscillations, in which pyramidal cellsare connected to other pyramidal cells as well as perisomatic interneurons (Fisahn et al.,1998; Mann and Paulsen, 2005; Mann et al., 2005). In this model pyramidal cell spikingis followed shortly by excitatory inputs from other synchronized firing pyramidal cellsand after a disynaptic delay by inhibitory inputs through a negative feedback loop. As inthe hippocampus, excitatory inputs received by PFC layer 5 pyramidal cells are phase-locked and arrive at the trough of the local field oscillation, coincident with cell firing,whereas inhibitory inputs are phase-locked to the rising phase of the local field oscillationsand arrive with some delay after cell firing. These data support a cellular model ofnetwork oscillations in which an inhibitory feedback loop plays a crucial role in the timingof the oscillation cycle. In the neocortex, distinct feedback loops exist in superficialand deep layers. Inhibitory feedback loops in superficial layers could synchronize local

Page 32: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Existence of two Subnetworks 31

Spike

EPSC

IPSC

Field

a b c

d

20

0

-20

Tim

e a

fte

r

sp

ike

(m

s)

EPSC IPSC

*2.0 π

1.0 π

Ph

as

e (

rad

)

EPSC IPSCSpike

ns

*

0.6

0.3

0.0Ph

as

e-c

ou

plin

g

EPSC IPSCSpike

ns

*

500

Time (ms)

5%

1 µV

Figure 2.8: IPSCs occur later on the oscillation cycle and are more precisely timed thanEPSCs and spikes. (a) Probability histograms of spikes, EPSCs and IPSCs recorded from the samecell, aligned with averaged field potential. (b) Phase of firing for spikes (n = 8), EPSCs (n = 11) andIPSCs (n = 9). (c) Phase-coupling of spikes, EPSCs and IPSCs. (d) Time after spike for EPSCs (n =8) and IPSCs (n = 6).

neuronal activity to a higher frequency than inhibitory feedback loops in deep layers.Layer 5 pyramidal neurons with their extended morphology receive phase informationfrom both these networks, and synchronize its firing according to which network displaysthe strongest synchronized synaptic activity.

Slow and fast oscillations occurred independently, sometimes one of these frequencieswas present, other times both were present. Since layer 5 pyramidal neurons synchronizedtheir firing to both fast and slow oscillations, whereas layer 6 pyramidal cells synchro-nized their firing only to slow oscillations, these output pathways may function partlyin parallel. During fast oscillation episodes cell spiking of pyramidal neurons in layer 5and 6 is uncorrelated, but during slow oscillation episodes both layer 5 and layer 6 cellsfire phase-locked to the oscillation cycle and their synchronized output contains a similartime structure. Possibly, differences in synchronization between the cortical output layersmay create alternative patterns in which target areas are engaged.

Labeling studies show that in the rat medial PFC layer 5 neurons primarily projectto the striatum and the lateral hypothalamus, which are involved in the organizationand guidance of complex motor functions and the control of behavioral arousal and shiftsof attention, respectively (Purves et al., 2001; Gabbott et al., 2005). Layer 6 neuronsproject mainly to the mediodorsal thalamus (Gabbott et al., 2005). The mediodorsalthalamus receives processed information from the primary sensory and motor cortices,and in its turn provides a mayor input to the frontal cortex (Purves et al., 2001). Hence,in a simplified theoretical scheme, activation of the PFC subnetwork that generates fastoscillations could function to modulate levels of alertness. Whereas activation of the PFCsubnetwork that generates slow oscillation episodes could additionally alter the thalamic

Page 33: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

32 Chapter 2

input to the prefrontal cortex. Episodes during which both oscillation frequencies arepresent may reflect ongoing parallel processing of information in both these networks.This would allow for a highly flexible neuronal code involving parallel processing incortical subnetworks.

Page 34: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Chapter 3

Participation of Interneurons

Submitted as a part of

Two independent cortical subnetworks control spike timing in L5 neu-rons during dynamic oscillation shifts

Karlijn I. van Aerde1, Edward O. Mann3, Cathrin B. Canto1,3, Klaus Linkenkaer-Hansen1,4,Marcel van der Roest2, Antonius B. Mulder2, Ole Paulsen1,3, Arjen B. Brussaard1, Huib-ert D. Mansvelder1

1Dept. of Integrative Neurophysiology and 2Dept. of Anatomy and Neurosciences,Center for Neurogenomics and Cognitive Research (CNCR), VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.3The Neuronal Oscillations Group, Dept of Physiology, Anatomy and Genetics, Oxford University, Parks Road, Oxford OX13PT, United Kingdom.4BioMag laboratory, HUSLAB, Hospital district of Helsinki and Uusimaa, P.O. Box 340, FIN-00029HUS, Finland.

Human brain oscillations occur in different frequency bands that have been

linked to different behaviors and cognitive processes. Even within specific

frequency bands oscillations fluctuate in frequency and amplitude. In ro-

dent brain slices such frequency fluctuations are mediated by two distinct

networks controlling spike timing in layer 5 pyramidal neurons. Frequency

information is encoded and relayed between these networks through excita-

tory and inhibitory synaptic transmission. Both theoretical and experimen-

tal studies indicate that IPSC kinetics influence oscillation frequency. How

different interneuron types are involved in the frequency fluctuations seen

in network oscillations is not known. Here we show that fast-spiking and

non-fast spiking interneurons fire phase-locked to both slow and fast oscil-

lations. This implies that when the same interneuron fires, the subsequent

oscillation cycle can either be short or long. In addition, the different IPSC

kinetics of fast spiking and non-fast spiking interneurons did not result in

a different involvement in either slow or fast oscillations. Our data indicate

that the oscillation frequency is not determined by the kinetics of inhibitory

transmission of layer 5 interneurons.

33

Page 35: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

34 Chapter 3

Introduction

Human brain oscillations occur in different frequency bands that have been linked todifferent behaviors and cognitive processes(Gray et al., 1989; Bragin et al., 1995; Tallon-Baudry et al., 1998; Fries et al., 2001; Pesaran et al., 2002; Buzsaki and Draguhn, 2004;Kahana, 2006; Fan et al., 2007). Even within specific frequency bands oscillations fluctu-ate in frequency and amplitude (Steriade et al., 1993; Bragin et al., 1995; Tallon-Baudryet al., 1998; Palva et al., 2005). Such frequency fluctuations most likely reflect chang-ing states of neuronal network activity, as brain oscillations arise from the correlatedsynchronized activity of large numbers of neurons (Steriade et al., 1990; Lopes da Silva,1991; Steriade et al., 1993). Previous findings indicate that frequency fluctuations inacute slices from rat prefrontal cortex are mediated by two distinct networks controllingspike timing in pyramidal neurons of output layers 5 and 6 (see Chapter 2). Actionpotential discharges in layer 5 pyramidal neurons are controlled by both the slow andfast oscillations, whereas L6 pyramidal cell spiking is controlled only by the slow oscil-lation. At the synaptic level, L5 pyramidal cells receive phase information from slowand fast oscillations through both EPSCs and IPSCs, of which the timing of IPSCs ismore precise than the timing of EPSCs (Chapter 2). This is in line with findings inthe hippocampus where IPSCs received by pyramidal cells are also more precisely timedthan EPSCs (Oren et al., 2006). Interneuron spiking thus appears to play an importantrole in sustaining fast network oscillations (Hajos et al., 2004; Mann et al., 2005; Orenet al., 2006; Tukker et al., 2007). Both theoretical and experimental studies indicatethat IPSC kinetics influence oscillation frequency (Whittington et al., 1995; Fisahn etal., 1998; Traub et al., 1998; Palhalmi et al., 2004; Cope et al., 2005). Increasing IPSCduration with benzodiazepines or barbiturates reduces the oscillation frequency in hip-pocampal slices (Fisahn et al., 1998; Palhalmi et al., 2004; Cope et al., 2005). Fastspiking, palvalbumin-positive interneurons are thought to control spike timing of pyra-midal cells and network oscillations since they target the pyramidal cell soma or axoninitial segment forming synapses with predominantly GABA-receptors containing alpha1subunits (Hajos et al., 2004; Mann et al., 2005; Freund, 2007). Intrinsic properties makefast spiking cells suitable for sustaining high frequency firing (Pike et al., 2000). Non-fastspiking CCK-positive perisomatic interneurons with synapses dominated by alpha2 sub-units (Kawaguchi and Kondo, 2002; Bacci et al., 2003) are thought to be more involvedin modulation of fast network oscillations and in shaping the activity of subgroups andassemblies of pyramidal cells (Freund, 2003; Klausberger et al., 2005).

Much less is known about which interneuron types are involved in generating andsustaining fast network oscillations in the neocortex. However fast-spiking interneuronsalso seem to play a role in kainate induced network oscillations in somatosensory cortex(Roopun et al., 2006). Prefrontal L5 pyramidal neurons connect to several interneu-ron classes, including interneurons located in L5 and L6 (Gabbott et al., 1997). Howdifferent interneuron types are involved in the frequency fluctuations seen in prefrontalfast network oscillations is not known. We hypothesize that the IPSCs measured inlayer 5 pyramidal neurons during slow and fast network oscillations come from differentsources: from fast-spiking interneurons that have faster kinetics during the fast oscilla-tion episodes, and from non-fast spiking interneurons that have slower kinetics duringslow oscillation episodes. To test this we induced network oscillations in rat prefrontalcortex slices while recording from electrophysiologically identified layer 5 interneurons.We found that both fast-spiking and non-fast spiking interneurons fire phase-locked toboth slow and fast oscillations.

Page 36: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Participation of Interneurons 35

Method summary

Slice preparation and electrophysiology

Prefrontal (IL) coronal cortical slices (350 µm) were prepared from P14–28 Wistar rats,in accordance with Dutch license procedures. After recovery, slices were mounted on 8 ×8 arrays of planar microelectrodes (Tensor Biosciences, Irvine, CA). Interneurons locatedin L5 were selected on the basis of their morphology and firing pattern (Table 1). Allexperiments were performed at 25 ◦C.

Data analysis

Electrophysiological data was analyzed using custom-written procedures in Igor Pro(Wavemetrics, OR, USA).

Spike timing relative to fieldThe spike timing in single cells was analyzed relative to the ongoing field oscillation.

Spikes were detected using simple threshold detection. Circular statistics were used totest whether individual cells showed phase locking to the network oscillation, and cellsnot significantly coupled to the network were excluded from further analysis (Rayleightest, p < 0.05).

StatisticsData are represented as mean ± s.e.m. Statistical analysis used either the Student’s

t test (paired or unpaired) or an ANOVA with Student Newman Keuls post-hoc test, asappropriate. Circular statistics used the Rayleigh test, and the Watson-Williams test.Asterisks represent p < 0.05.

Results

In acute brain slices from rat infralimbic cortex, the acetylcholinergic agonist carbachol(25 µm) induces fast network oscillations (Figure 3.1a). We recorded these oscillationsusing a 8 × 8 multi-electrode grid (Shimono et al., 2000; Mann et al., 2005) from iso-lated infralimbic cortex slices to avoid interference from prelimbic cortex (see Chapter 4).Carbachol-induced oscillations were dependent on intact glutamatergic and GABAergictransmission (Figure B.1). Slow and fast network oscillations were differentially dis-tributed over cortical layers (Figure 3.1b–d). More superficial layer 3/5 displayed fieldpotential oscillations at a higher frequency than deep layer 6 (Figure 3.1d; L3/5 13.5 ±0.8 Hz; L6 10.3 ± 0.5 Hz; n = 18; p < 0.01). Wavelet analysis (Torrence and Compo,1998) showed that the fast network oscillations fluctuated in magnitude over time, show-ing clear episodes of increased or reduced magnitude (Figure 3.1e). The magnitude ofthe oscillations was only significantly above the 1/f noise level for short episodes (seemethods). Slow oscillation episodes lasted longer and occurred less frequent than fastoscillation episodes (Figure 3.1f; duration episodes: slow 371.3 ± 34.6 ms; fast 301.0 ±28.4 ms; n = 12; p < 0.01; frequency epiodes: slow 1.4 ± 0.1 Hz; fast 1.9 ± 0.1 Hz; n

Page 37: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

36 Chapter 3

b c d

e

g

IL

a

f

2010Frequency (Hz)

10 fV2

20 fV2

50 fV2

L3/5

L5

L6

100 ms

2 µV

L3/5

L5

L6

30

20

10

400 ms 2 µV

L3/5 L5 L6

30

20

10

Fre

qu

en

cy

(H

z)

2 s

L3/5

6

4

2

0% E

pis

od

es

1.00.50.0

Duration (s)

slowfast

20

15

10Fre

qu

en

cy

(H

z)

L3/5 L6

*

50

0

% T

ime

slow fast both noneOscillations above threshold

Figure 3.1: Distribution of slow and fast oscillations over cortical layers in rat brain slices.(A) Slices were cut from infralimbic prefrontal areas (right: picture from Paxinos and Watson, 2005)and placed on a 8 × 8 multi-electrode grid (bottom). (B) Field potentials were simultaneously recordedacross cortical layers. Bandpass-filtered signals in dark colors. (C) Power spectra of recordings in (B).(D) Frequency of oscillations in L3/5 and L6 (n = 18). (E) Frequency fluctuations of field potentials(right, bottom) in time as analyzed using wavelet transform (top) with warmer colors representingincreasing magnitude. Right: expanded time scale in different layers. (F) Duration of episodes of slowand fast oscillations (n = 12). (G) Occurrence of slow and fast oscillations (n = 12).

= 12; p < 0.01). Slow and fast oscillation episodes occurred simultaneously in ∼ 50% ofthe time and separate from each other in ∼ 30% time (Figure 3.1g). The magnitude ofslow and fast oscillations was only weakly correlated in time (r = 0.29 ± 0.07; p < 0.01in 12 slices).

We previously showed that L5 pyramidal neurons in mPFC can phase-lock their firingto both fast and slow oscillations (Chapter 2). To study whether mPFC L5 interneuronsfire phase-locked to slow or fast oscillations or both we made cell-attached recordingsfrom L5 interneurons, while recording slow and fast field potential oscillations. After5–10 minutes of cell-attached recording, patches were broken through to whole-cell con-figuration to allow electrophysiological identification for post-hoc analysis We classifiedinterneurons as fast spiking (FS) or non-fast spiking (non-FS) based on their spikingpattern and action potential characteristics (Table 3.1, Figure 3.2) (Bacci et al., 2003).Following a current step protocol, FS interneurons started spiking at a later current stepthan non-FS interneurons (Figure 3.2a), but rapidly increased their spike frequency with

Page 38: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Participation of Interneurons 37

20 mV

200 ms

+10 pA

FSa

a’

b

b’

non-FS

200 ms

+50 pA

20 mV

-50 pA; +140 pA 200 ms

-50 pA; +140 pA

20 mV

200 ms

60

40

20

0

Fir

ing

fre

q. a

t

14

0p

A (

Hz)

FS non-FS

*d

c

60

40

20

0Sp

ike

fre

q.

(Hz)

150100500-50Inj. current (pA)

FS

non-FS

Figure 3.2: Electrophysiological properties of fast-spiking (FS) and non fast-spiking (non-FS) cells. (a–a) Example current-clamp recording of FS cell. Shown is first depolarizing step at whichthe cell fired (a) and responses after –50 pA and +140 pA current injection (a). (b–b) as (a–a) fornon-FS cell. Note the difference with FS cell in action potential shape as well as firing frequencies.(c) Plot of mean firing frequencies in response to injected currents of different amplitudes for FS cells(gray, n = 6) and non-FS cells (black, n = 18). (d) Mean firing frequency after 140 pA current injectionfor FS cells (n = 5) and non-FS cells (n = 13).

increasing current (Figure 3.2a–c; slope f-I plot between 0–140 pA: FS 0.45 ± 0.04 Hz/pA,n = 5; non-FS 0.16 ± 0.02 Hz/pA, n = 11; p < 0.01). With 140 pA current injection fastspiking interneurons displayed a two-fold increase in spiking frequency compared to non-FS cells (Figure 3.2d; FS 40.6 ± 5.9 Hz, n = 5; non-FS 21.7 ± 2.7 Hz, n = 11; p < 0.01).Fast spiking and non-fast spiking cells also differed in action potential characteristics ashalf-width and afterhyperpolarzation (Table 1).

Upon carbachol application 20 out of 30 interneurons started spiking (Figure 3.3a–d). The overall firing frequency for fast spiking and non-spiking interneurons was in therange of ∼ 3–4 Hz (Figure 3.3e; FS 4.5 ± 2.1 Hz, n = 5; non-FS 3.3 ± 0.5 Hz, n = 11;p = 0.46). The variation in firing frequency was considerable, especially for fast spikinginterneurons (Figure 3.3e). Surprisingly, interneurons did not fire at a higher rate thanlayer 5 pyramidal cells (pyramidal cells 3.1 ± 0.6 Hz, n = 8; ANOVA p = 0.62) as hasbeen reported for carbachol-induced oscillations in the hippocampus (Oren et al., 2006).

In slices that showed two distinct oscillation frequencies, 8 out of 13 interneurons firedphase-locked to both slow and fast oscillations with the highest discharge probabilityduring the rising phase of the local oscillation (Figures 3.4 and 3.5). Of the other cells,two cells fired only phase-locked to the fast oscillation, and three cells did not fire phase-locked. FS and non-FS interneurons did not differ in phase of firing (Figure B.7a; meanphase of all phase-locked cells; FS (n = 4): 4.55 ± 0.21 rad, non-FS (n = 4) 4.71 ± 0.37rad, p = 0.72) or amount of jitter (Figure B.7b; FS: 0.28 ± 0.06; non-FS 0.12 ± 0.04, p =0.07). The phase of firing was similar for slow and fast oscillations (Figure B.8a; slow 4.76± 0.44 rad, fast 5.00 ± 0.34 rad, n = 5, p = 0.19). Also the strength of phase-couplingwas similar for slow and fast oscillation (Figure B.8b; slow r = 0.21 ± 0.02, fast r = 0.30± 0.07, n = 6, p = 0.10). These data suggest that action potential firing in fast-spikingand non fast-spiking interneurons is controlled by both slow and fast oscillations.

Page 39: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

38 Chapter 3

100 s

50 pA

+ carbachola b

c d

e

500 ms

50 pA

30

20

10

0

Sp

ike

fre

q.

(Hz)

6004002000

Time (s)

100

50

0Sp

ike

s/b

in

20100Spike freq. (Hz)

15

10

5

0

Sp

ike

fre

q.

(Hz)

FS non-FS pyr

ns

Figure 3.3: Carbachol application increases spike frequencies in fast-spiking (FS) and non-FS interneurons. (a) Example 10 min. cell-attached recording of FS cell during carbachol application(arrow). (b) as (a) with expanded time scale. (c) Spike frequency of recording in (a) in time. (d) Spikefrequency histogram of recording in (a). (e) Median spike frequency after carbachol application for FS(n = 5) and non-FS (n = 11) interneurons and L5 pyramidal cells (n = 8).

In previous experiments we recorded excitatory and inhibitory postsynaptic currents(EPSCs and IPSCs) received by L5 pyramidal neurons, and found that both EPSCs andIPSCs were phase-locked to the oscillations (Figures 2.6,, 2.7, and 3.6). Interneuron firingand IPSC arrival at the pyramidal cell occurred at the rising phase of the local oscillationcycle and were preceded by EPSC arrival at the pyramidal cell and pyramidal cell spikingthat both occurred at the trough of the oscillation cycle (Figure 3.6a–i; ANOVA, p <

Table 3.1: Firing properties of FS and non-FS interneurons. All values are means ± s.e.m.; ** p <0.01; n = 6 (FS); n = 18 (non-FS). Note that for AP properties only cells with a minimum spike heightof 20 mV were included, leaving n = 5 (FS) and n = 16 (non-FS). aAHP, Afterhyperpolarization;calculated as the difference between action potential threshold and the most negative level reachedduring the repolarizing phase. bCalculated from total spikes elicited by 1-sec-long current injections of140 pA. cf-i slope, firing frequency versus injected current slope, with current injections ranging from0 to 140 pA. dSpike frequency adaptation was calculated as the ratio of the instantaneous frequencyat the second and eight spike intervals in an action potential train.

FS non-FS

Action potential

- threshold (mV) –38.8 ± 2.6 –36.9 ± 1.2

- height (mV) 39.1 ± 1.8 45.3 ± 2.1

- half-width (ms) 0.8 ± 0.1 1.8 ± 0.1 **

- AHP (mV)a 22.4 ± 1.7 16.0 ± 1.0 **

mean firing frequency (Hz)b 40.6 ± 5.9 21.7 ± 2.7 **

f-i slope (Hz/pA)c 0.45 ± 0.04 0.16 ± 0.02 **

adaptationd 1.00 ± 0.04 0.82 ± 0.04 **

Page 40: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Participation of Interneurons 39

b

c da L1

L2

L3

L5

slow fast

50 ms 50 pA

200 nV

50 ms 50 pA

200 nV

e f

10

5

0Sp

ike

s (

%)

Phase (rad)

500 nV

0 2π

10

5

0Sp

ike

s (

%)

Phase (rad)

500 nV

0 2π

100 ms 100 pA

2 µV

slow fast

slow fast

150 µm

Figure 3.4: Fast-spiking interneurons fire phase-locked to slow and fast network oscilla-tions. (a) Slice on multi-electrode array with patch pipette on top. (b) Simultaneously recorded fieldpotential in L5 (top, bandpass-filtered signal in black) and FS cell action potentials in cell-attachedmode (bottom). Boxes are drawn around a slow (left) and fast (right) episode of field oscillations. (c,d) Spike averaged field potential (top) and cellular recording (bottom) of spikes that occurred duringslow (c) or fast (d) oscillation episodes. (e, f) Spike probability histogram (bottom) of spikes duringslow (e) or fast (f) oscillation episodes, aligned with peak-to-peak cycle average of field potential (top).

0.01; IPSCs (n = 9) 4.68 ± 0.19 rad; interneurons (n = 8) 4.46 ± 0.16 rad; p = 0.36.EPSCs (n = 10) 3.77 ± 0.16 rad; pyramidal cells (n = 10) 3.48 ± 0.14 rad; p = 0.20). Thestrength of phase-coupling of interneurons was in between pyramidal cells and EPSCs onthe one hand and IPSCs on the other hand, and did not differ significantly with any othergroup. IPSCs showed stronger phase-coupling than EPSCs and pyramidal cell spiking(Figure 3.6a–h, J; ANOVA, p < 0.05; IPSCs r = 0.37 ± 0.07; interneurons r = 0.25 ±0.04, p = 0.07; EPSCs r = 0.21 ± 0.03, p = 0.04; pyramidal cells r = 0.21 ± 0.02, p =0.02).

These results show that fast spiking and non-fast spiking interneurons in L5 of ratprefrontal cortex fire phase-locked to both slow and fast oscillation episodes. Interneuronspiking coincides with IPSC arrival on L5 pyramidal cells and occurs later at the localoscillation cycle than pyramidal cell spiking and EPSC arrival.

Page 41: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

40 Chapter 3

500 nV

50 ms

10 pA

50 ms

10 pA

500 nV c d

e f

a

10

5

0Sp

ike

s (

%)

0Phase (rad)

500 nV

10

5

0Sp

ike

s (

%)

0Phase (rad)

500 nV

slow fast

200

100Po

we

r (f

V2

)

2010

Frequency (Hz)

b

100 ms 10 pA

5 µV

slow fast

slow fast

Figure 3.5: Non-FS interneurons spike phase-locked to slow and fast network oscillations.(a) Simultaneously recorded field potential in L5 (top, bandpass-filtered signal in black) and non-FScell action potentials in cell-attached mode (bottom). Boxes are drawn around a slow (left) and fast(right) episode of field oscillations. (b) Power spectrum of field recording in L5. (c, d) Spike averagedfield potential (top) and cellular recording (bottom) of spikes that occurred during slow (c) or fast (d)oscillation episodes. (e, f) Spike probability histogram (bottom) of spikes during slow (e) or fast (f)oscillation episodes, aligned with peak-to-peak cycle average of field potential (top).

Discussion

Frequency fluctuations in rodent brain slices are generated by distinct neocortical net-works (Chapter 2). In line with previous work, our data shows that slow and fast os-cillations are distrubuted over superficial and deep cotical layers. Fast oscillations wereprimarily found in more superficial layer 3/5, whereas slow oscillations were dominant indeep layer 6. In cortical layer 5 episodes of both slow and fast oscillation were present.Pyramidal cells in layer 5 fire phase-locked to both slow and fast oscillations, and receiveIPSCs that are phase-locked to slow and fast oscillations (Chapter 2). Since IPSC kinet-ics are thought to determine oscillation frequency (Whittington et al., 1995; Fisahn etal., 1998; Traub et al., 1998; Palhalmi et al., 2004; Cope et al., 2005), we hypothesizedthat two classes of interneurons are differentially involved in slow and fast oscillations:fast spiking parvalbumin positive interneurons with fast kinetics in sustaining fast oscil-lations, and non-fast spiking CCK positive interneurons with slower kinetics in sustainingslow oscillations. To examine whether IPSCs during slow and fast oscillation episodesoriginated from the same or different interneurons, we recorded L5 interneurons while in-ducing network oscillations. We found, contrary to our hypothesis, that fast spiking andnon-fast spiking interneurons could fire phase-locked to both slow and fast oscillations.Also, there was no difference at what phase of the oscillation cycle interneurons fired

Page 42: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Participation of Interneurons 41

g

h

a

b

e

f

c

d

Pyramidal cells InterneuronsEPSCs IPSCs

i j

50 ms

10 pA

0.5 µV

8

4

Sp

ike

s (

%)

2π0Phase (rad)

1 µV

50 ms 10 pA

1 µV

8

4

EP

SC

s (

%)

2π0Phase (rad)

2 µV

50 ms 20 pA

2 µV

15105

IPS

Cs

(%

)

2π0Phase (rad)

2 µV

50 ms 20 pA

0.5 µV

8

4

Sp

ike

s (

%)

2π0Phase (rad)

0.5 µV

Ph

as

e (

rad

)

EPSC InterIPSCPyr

ns ns

*

0.6

0.4

0.2

0.0Ph

as

e c

ou

plin

g

EPSC InterIPSCPyr

**

Figure 3.6: Interneuron spiking and IPSC arrival on pyramidal cells occur at the samephase of the oscillation cycle. (A) Spike averaged field potential (top) and cellular recording(bottom) of pyramidal cell spiking during field oscillations. (b) Spike probability histogram (bottom)of spikes aligned with peak-to-peak cycle average (top) of field potential. (c–d) as (a–b) for pyramidalEPSCs. (e–f) as (a–b) for pyramidal IPSCs. (g–h) as (a–b) for interneuron spikes. (i) Phase ofoscillation cycle of pyramidal cell firing, EPSC and IPSC arrival and interneuron firing (pyramidalcells, n = 10; EPSCs, n = 10; IPSCs, n = 9; interneurons, n = 8). The trough of the oscillation cycleis defined as π radians. (j) Phase-coupling (pyramidal cells, n = 10; EPSCs, n = 10; IPSCs, n = 9;interneurons, n = 8). Non significant comparisons are not shown for clarity.

between slow and fast oscillation episodes. This implies that when the same interneuronfires the subsequent oscillation cycle can either be short or long.

In hippocampus, fast spiking and non-fast spiking interneurons make synapses withdifferent GABAA-receptor subunit composition, resulting in differences in IPSC kinetics(Bacci et al., 2003; Freund, 2003). It is not known whether GABAergic synapses made byfast spiking and non-fast spiking interneurons in the PFC display different IPSC kinetics.If this would be the case, oscillation frequencies in the PFC are not determined by singleIPSCs, since we did not observe differences in action potential phase-locking betweenthe interneuron types. Alternatively, the oscillation frequency could be affected by thekinetics of summated IPSCs. These summated IPSCs could show different kinetics ifdifferent amounts of IPSCs are summated, or if there is a differential contribution ofslow and fast IPSCs. Fast spiking and non-fast spiking interneurons showed a similaramount of phase-locking during slow and fast oscillation episodes. Although fast spikinginterneurons showed a trend towards stronger phase-locking compared to non-fast spikinginterneurons, this was the case for both slow and fast oscillations. Thus, it seems unlikely

Page 43: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

42 Chapter 3

that fast spiking and non-fast spiking cells would contribute to a different extend to IPSCsummation.

Apart from IPSC kinetics, interneurons other than layer 5 fast spiking and non-fast spiking interneurons could be involved in setting the oscillation frequency. Thephase-coupling of IPSCs received by the pyramidal is stronger than the phase-couplingof interneuron action potentials, implying that IPSC arrival at pyramidal cells is moreprecisely timed than layer 5 interneuron firing. This could result from the contributionof other interneuron types to spontaneous IPSCs received by layer 5 pyramidal neurons.These other interneuron types could be located in layer 3 or layer 6 (Gabbott et al.,1997).

In the hippocampus more than fifteen interneuron types have been identified based onaxonal and dendritic morpholgy and chemical markers (Somogyi and Klausberger, 2005).Both in vivo and in vitro studies show that different interneuron types have distinctroles in sustaining hippocampal network oscillations (Hajos et al., 2004; Somogyi andKlausberger, 2005; Oren et al., 2006; Tukker et al., 2007). Some interneurons participatein theta, gamma and ripple oscillations, whereas other types do not (Klausberger et al.,2003). Perisomatic interneurons show a stronger involvement in fast network oscillationsthan dendritic targeting interneurons, although bistratified interneurons are an exceptionto this general rule (Hajos et al., 2004; Tukker et al., 2007). Interneurons can also differin the relative timing to the local oscillation cycle: CCK positive cells in the CA1 regionare found to fire even before pyramidal cell firing, in contrast to all other interneurontypes that fire on the ascending phase of the oscillation cycle (Tukker et al., 2007). Thiscould result from feed-forward inhibition as in the hippocampal circuitry fast networkoscillations are generated in CA3 and then relayed to the CA1 area (Fisahn et al., 1998).

In the prefrontal cortex interneurons have been subdivided in three main cell classesbased on electrophysiological properties: fast spiking, late spiking and non-fast spik-ing cells (Kawaguchi and Kondo, 2002, Couey et al., 2007). Further subdivisions canbe made based on neurochemical markers and morphology. Like in the hippocampus,perisomatic or basket cells can be positive for parvalbumin or cholecystokinin (CCK),the first coinciding with a fast spiking electrophysiological profile, and the latter with anon-fast spiking profile (Kawaguchi and Kondo, 2002). In contrast to their hippocampalcounterparts layer 5 fast spiking and non-fast spiking interneurons show a similar timingof firing to the field oscillations, and they fire phase-locked to slow and fast oscillationepisodes. This implies that the oscillation frequency does not rely on the kinetics ofinhibitory transmission of these interneurons.

Page 44: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Chapter 4

Cortical Interaction

PLoS ONE. 2008; 3(7): e2725.

Prelimbic and Infralimbic Prefrontal Cortex Interact During Fast Net-work Oscillations

Karlijn I. van Aerde, Tim S. Heistek, and Huibert D. Mansvelder

Department of Intergrative Neurophysiology, Center for Neurogenomics and Cognitive Research (CNCR), VU University Ams-terdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands.

The medial prefrontal cortex has been implicated in a variety of cognitive

and executive processes such as decision making and working memory. The

medial prefrontal cortex of rodents consists of several areas including the

prelimbic and infralimbic cortex that are thought to be involved in different

aspects of cognitive performance. Despite the distinct roles in cognitive be-

havior that have been attributed to prelimbic and infralimbic cortex, little

is known about neuronal network functioning of these areas, and whether

these networks show any interaction during fast network oscillations. Here

we show that fast network oscillations in rat infralimbic cortex slices oscillate

at higher frequencies and with higher power than oscillations in prelimbic

cortex. The difference in oscillation frequency disappeared when prelimbic

and infralimbic cortex were disconnected. Also, the power of oscillations in-

creased in both areas when disconnected from each other. Our data indicate

that neuronal networks of prelimbic and infralimbic cortex can sustain fast

network oscillations independent of each other, but suggest that neuronal

networks of prelimbic and infralimbic cortex are interacting during these

oscillations.

43

Page 45: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

44 Chapter 4

Introduction

The medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) of rodents consists of several areas including theprelimbic and infralimbic cortex (Van Eden and Uylings, 1985; Uylings et al., 2003;Hoover and Vertes, 2007). These adjacent cortical areas have a different cytoarchitecture(Van Eden and Uylings, 1985) and partly differ in their connections with other brainareas (Hoover and Vertes, 2007). Although many studies have addressed the role ofprelimbic and infralimbic cortex without distinguishing between the two areas, otherstudies show specific involvement of prelimbic or infralimbic cortex in cognitive behavior(Dalley et al., 2004; Marquis et al., 2007; Ragozzino, 2007). Injection and lesion studiesin the awake animal suggest that the prelimbic cortex is involved in behavioral flexibility(Marquis et al., 2007; Ragozzino, 2007), whereas the infralimbic cortex seems to beinvolved in impulsive behavior and habit formation (Chudasama et al., 2003; Killcrossand Coutureau, 2003; Murphy et al., 2005).

High-frequency oscillations in the beta (14–30 Hz) and gamma range (30–80 Hz) havebeen linked to cognitive processing and working memory in humans (Tallon-Baudry et al.,1998; Howard et al., 2003) and animals (Fries et al., 2001; Pesaran et al., 2002). Studiesin the awake animal show that during working memory acetylcholine levels increase inprelimbic and infralimbic cortex, and that these increased cholinergic levels are necessaryfor accurate performance (Passetti et al., 2000; McGaughy et al., 2002; Kozak et al.,2006; Parikh et al., 2007). In vitro, cholinergic agonists such as carbachol, induce fastnetwork oscillations in acute slices of rodent cortex (Buhl et al., 1998; Chrobak andBuzsaki, 1998; Csicsvari et al., 2003; Mann et al., 2005; Bartos et al., 2007). Despite thedistinct roles in cognitive behavior that have been attributed to prelimbic and infralimbiccortex, little is known about neuronal network functioning of these areas, and whetherthese networks show any interaction during fast network oscillations.

To address these questions, we induced carbachol-induced fast network oscillations inacute brain slices of rat prelimbic and infralimbic cortex, while they were connected witheach other, or in isolation. We find that fast network oscillations in infralimbic cortexoscillate at higher frequencies and with higher power than oscillations in prelimbic cor-tex. The difference in oscillation frequency disappeared when prelimbic and infralimbiccortex were disconnected. Thus, although neuronal networks of prelimbic and infralimbiccortex can sustain fast network oscillations independent of each other, our data suggestthat neuronal networks of prelimbic and infralimbic cortex are interacting during theseoscillations.

Method summary

Slice preparation and electrophysiology

Prefrontal coronal cortical slices (400 µm) were prepared from P14–28 Wistar rats, inaccordance with Dutch license procedures. Slices were left to recover for 1 h. Mini slices,containing either the prelimbic or infralimbic cortex, were cut using surgical blades. Inother slices, prelimbic and infralimbic cortex were disconnected by making a cut betweenthese areas. Electrical stimulation experiments confirmed the functional disconnection.Slices were mounted on 8 × 8 arrays of planar microelectrodes (Tensor Biosciences, Irvine,CA). All experiments were performed at 25 ◦C.

Page 46: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Cortical Interaction 45

Data analysis

Electrophysiological data was analyzed using custom-written procedures in Igor Pro(Wavemetrics, OR, USA).

CSDAll signals were peak-to-peak averaged relative to a reference recording from prelimbic

or infralimbic cortex. CSD plots are shown using an inverted color scale, with warm colorscorresponding to current sinks (i.e., neuronal membrane inward currents) and cool colorscorresponding to current sources.

StatisticsData are represented as mean ± s.e.m. Statistical analysis used either the Student’s

t test (paired or unpaired) or an ANOVA with Student Newman Keuls post-hoc test, asappropriate. Asterisks represent p < 0.05.

Results

To record from prelimbic and infralimbic cortex simultaneously, we placed acute ratprefrontal cortex slices on a planar 8 × 8 multi-electrode grid with an interelectrodedistance of 300 µm, covering an area of 4.4 mm2 (Shimono et al., 2000; Mann et al.,2005) (Figure 4.1a–c). Bath application of 25 µm carbachol (CCh) induced fast networkoscillations that were dependent on glutamatergic and GABAergic transmission (FigureB.1). Fourier analysis revealed that field oscillations in the infralimbic cortex oscillatedat a higher frequency compared to the prelimbic cortex (Figure 4.1c–e; mean ± s.e.m.;prelimbic (PrL) 12.7 ± 0.7 Hz; infralimbic (IL) 14.7 ± 0.9 Hz; p = 0.02, n = 12).Oscillation power in infralimbic cortex was greater than prelimbic cortex in 13 out of 15slices, and varied greatly between experiments (Figure 4.1f; area power spectrum 5–35Hz; PrL 1.5 ± 0.3 µV2; IL 2.1 ± 0.5 µV2; p = 0.02, n = 15). To calculate the relativepower of prelimbic to infralimbic oscillations, we normalized the power in prelimbic cortexto the power in infralimbic cortex per experiment. On average the power in prelimbiccortex was reduced with ∼ 25% (Figure 4.1g; PrL 73.7 ± 6.8% of IL, p < 0.01, n = 15).Thus, fast network oscillations in prelimbic and infralimbic cortex differ in frequency andpower.

The power of the fast network oscillations strongly fluctuated in time, both in prelim-bic and infralimbic cortex. Since fast network oscillations reflect synchronized activity oflarge groups of neurons (Steriade et al., 1990; Lopes da Silva, 1991; Steriade et al., 1993),these power fluctuations most likely reflect episodes of increased and reduced synchronic-ity in neuronal activity, which is a property of neuronal networks (Paulsen and Sejnowski,2006). To determine whether prelimbic and infralimbic cortex neuronal networks showdifferences in power fluctuations, these fluctuations in oscillation power were quantifiedusing time resolved wavelet analysis (Torrence and Compo, 1998) (Figure 4.2a–d). Inboth prelimbic and infralimbic cortex, the episodes during which the power of oscilla-tions was significantly above threshold (see Materials and Methods, Figure B.2) occurredaround 2 Hz (PrL 1.9 ± 0.2 Hz, n = 5; IL 1.7 ± 0.1 Hz, n = 7; p = 0.59) and lasted about225 ms (Figure 4.2e; mean of medians ± s.e.m.; PrL 212.9 ± 25.6 ms, n = 5; IL 237.6± 20.4 ms, n = 7; p = 0.46). The oscillation episodes occurred both simultaneously andseparately in prelimbic and infralimbic cortex (Figure 4.2f; only PrL 18.1 ± 2.3%; only

Page 47: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

46 Chapter 4

b c

d e

PrL

IL

PrL

IL

a

302010

Frequency (Hz)

50 fV2

PrL

IL f g

6

4

2

Po

we

r (p

V2)

PrL IL

*

100

0

Po

we

r (%

IL

)

PrL

*20

15

10Fre

qu

en

cy

(H

z)

PrL IL

*

100 ms 2 µV

100 ms 2 µV

control + 25 µM carbachol

Figure 4.1: Fast network oscillations are bigger and faster in infralimbic cortex comparedto prelimbic cortex. (a) Picture of coronal slice containing medial prefrontal areas prelimbic cortex(PrL) and infralimbic cortex (IL). From The Rat Brain in Stereotaxtic Coordinates by G. Paxinosand C. Watson, 2005. (b) Slice placed on 8 × 8 multi-electrode array. (c) Simultaneous recorded fieldpotentials in prelimbic (top) and infralimbic (bottom) cortex before (left) and after (right) induction offast network oscillations by carbachol. Bandpass-filtered traces in darker colors. (d) Power spectrumof recordings in (c). (e) Frequency of oscillations in prelimbic and infralimbic cortex (n = 12, p =0.02). (f) Absolute oscillation power (taken as area below 5–35 Hz from power spectrum) (n = 15, p= 0.02). (g) Oscillation power in prelimbic cortex as percentage of power in infralimbic cortex (n =15, p < 0.01).

IL 25.2 ± 2.1%; both 33.1 ± 4.7%; neither 23.7 ± 4.3%, n = 5). These findings suggestthat fast network oscillations in prelimbic and infralimbic cortex can occur independentlyfrom each other.

To further investigate whether separate neuronal networks generate the fast networkoscillation in prelimbic and infralimbic cortex, we analyzed the underlying current sinksand sources that generated the field oscillations with two dimensional current-source-density (CSD) analysis (Shimono et al., 2001) (Figure 4.3). When an electrode in layer 5from the prelimbic cortex served as reference electrode, a sink-source pair between layer 5and the superficial layers 1/2 was revealed (Figure 4.3c). In 6 out of 12 slices the currentsink-source pair was restricted to the prelimbic cortex and did not involve the infralimbiccortex (Figure 4.3b). When in the same experiment an electrode in layer 5 from theinfralimbic cortex served as reference electrode, a current sink-source pair between thedeep and superficial layers of infralimbic cortex was revealed (Figure 4.3f). This sink-source pair did not extend to the prelimbic cortex and was completely restricted to theinfralimbic cortex (Figure 4.3e). These results show that the fast network oscillations aregenerated by separate neuronal networks in prelimbic and infralimbic cortex, and can berestricted to that area.

Page 48: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Cortical Interaction 47

30

20

10

Fre

qu

en

cy

(H

z)

100 ms

5 µV

30

20

10

Fre

qu

en

cy

(H

z)

2 sa b

c d

e f

PrL

PrL

30

20

10

Fre

qu

en

cy

(H

z)

100 ms

5 µV

IL

30

20

10

Fre

qu

en

cy

(H

z)

2 s

IL

40

20

0

% T

ime

PrL IL both none

Oscillations > threshold

15

10

5

0

% E

pis

od

es

1.00.50.0

Duration episodes (s)

PrL

IL

Figure 4.2: Oscillations in prelimbic and infralimbic cortex can be present simultaneouslyor separate from each other. (a–b) Frequency fluctuations in time as analyzed with wavelet anal-ysis from simultaneous recordings in prelimbic (a) and infralimbic (b) cortex. Warmer colors repre-sent increasing oscillation magnitudes. (c–d) Expanded time-scale from boxed region in (a–b) withbandpass-filtered (red) field recording (bottom). (e) Duration of oscillation episodes in prelimbic (red)and infralimbic (blue) cortex (PrL, n = 5; IL, n = 7). (f) Relative amount of time when oscillationswere present in prelimbic and infralimbic cortex simultaneous or separate from each other, or bothabsent (n = 5).

Since fast network oscillations in prelimbic and infralimbic are generated by their ownneuronal networks, this could suggest that they may exist independent from each other.To test whether neuronal networks from prelimbic and infralimbic cortex can generateand sustain fast network oscillations independent of each other, we cut out mini-slicesthat included either the prelimbic or the infralimbic cortex (Figure 4.4a). Application of

Page 49: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

48 Chapter 4

e

39.4 ms 1 µV

a b c

d

t = 0 ms 15.8 ms 31.5 ms 47.3 ms 63.1 ms

f

29.1 ms 2 µV

t = 0 ms 1.7 ms 23.3 ms 35.0 ms 46.6 ms

PrL

IL

sink

source

deep sup.

Figure 4.3: Current source density analysis of prelimbic and infralimbic electrodes revealstwo sink-source pairs of oscillations. (a) Brain slice with 2D-CSD plot as calculated from peak-to-peak cycle averaged field potentials from 8 × 8 multi-electrode array. (b) Peak-to-peak cycle averagedfield potentials, using a prelimbic field recording as reference oscillation (red trace). Two oscillationcycles are shown for clarity. The white rectangle in (a) marks the column of 8 electrodes, spanningprelimbic and infralimbic cortex, that are displayed. (c) 2D-CSD plots of 8 × 8 electrodes at differenttime points. The CSD plots display alternating sink (red) and source (blue) pairs between deep andsuperficial layers of prelimbic cortex. Note that sink-source pairs are restricted to prelimbic cortex.(d–f) as (a–c) using an infralimbic field recording (red trace in (e) as reference oscillation. Note thatsink-source pairs are restricted to infralimbic cortex.

carbachol to these isolated slice parts induced fast network oscillations in both prelimbicslices and infralimbic slices (Figure 4.4). The power of oscillations was much larger inboth prelimbic and infralimbic isolated slices compared to the slices that contained bothareas (Figure 4.5b; PrL: isolated 4.0 ± 0.4 pV2, n = 17, connected 1.5 ± 0.3 pV2, n =15, p < 0.01; IL: isolated 7.9 ± 1.8 pV2, n = 12, connected 2.1 ± 0.5 pV2, n = 15, p <0.01). This was due to an increase in the median and maximum magnitude of oscillationsin isolated slices (Figure 4.5c, d; median magnitude: isolated-PrL 5.9 ± 0.2 µV2, n =4, connected-PrL 2.5 ± 0.4 µV2, n = 5, p < 0.01; isolated-IL 9.5 ± 1.7 µV2, n = 5,connected-IL 3.2 ± 0.6 µV2, n = 7, p < 0.01; maximum magnitude: isolated-PrL 22.1± 2.8 µV2, n = 4, connected-PrL 10.1 ± 0.8 µV2, n = 5, p < 0.01; isolated-IL 28.8 ±2.6 µV2, n = 5, connected-IL 13.4 ± 2.0 µV2, n = 7, p = 0.01). Also, the duration ofepisodes showed a two-fold increase in isolated slices (Figure 4.5e, f; PrL: isolated 419.6 ±29.4 ms, n = 4, connected 212.9 ± 25.6 ms, n = 5, p < 0.01; IL: isolated 576.0 ± 55.6 ms,n = 5, connected 237.6 ± 20.4 ms, n = 7, p < 0.01). The power of the field oscillationsin the isolated infralimbic slices was larger than the power in isolated prelimbic slices(Figure 4.4c; isolated-PrL 4.0 ± 0.4 pV2, n = 17; isolated-IL 7.9 ± 1.8 pV2, n = 12; p= 0.02), similarly to when the areas were connected (Figure 4.1f). This suggests thatthe difference in oscillation power between these areas results from properties within theprelimbic and infralimbic neuronal networks. In contrast, the frequency of oscillations inisolated prelimbic and isolated infralimbic slices was not different (Figure 4.4b; isolated-PrL 13.8 ± 0.9 Hz, n = 17; isolated-IL 13.8 ± 1.0 Hz, n = 12; p = 0.97). This wassurprising since the oscillation frequencies were different in prelimbic and infralimbiccortex when these areas were connected (Figure 4.1d). This could suggest that duringfast network oscillations, prelimbic and infralimbic cortical neuronal networks affect each

Page 50: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Cortical Interaction 49

b ca

10

5

0

Po

we

r (p

V2)

PrL IL

27

*

20

10Fre

qu

en

cy

(H

z)

ILPrL

ns

Figure 4.4: Oscillations in infralimbic mini-slices are bigger but not faster than in prelimbicmini-slices. (a) Mini-slices were cut from hemisphere-slices and placed on a 8 × 8 multi-electrodearray. Drawn lines indicate boundaries of prelimbic and infralimbic cortex (adapted from The RatBrain in Stereotaxic Coordinates, by G. Paxinos and C. Watson, 2005). (b) Oscillation frequency ofprelimbic and infralimbic mini-slices (PrL, n = 17; IL, n = 12). (c) Oscillation power of prelimbic andinfralimbic mini-slices (PrL, n = 17; IL, n = 12).

other, giving rise to differences in oscillation frequency, which disappear when these areasare isolated from each other (Figure 4.5a; PrL: isolated 13.8 ± 0.9 Hz, n = 17; connected12.7 ± 0.7 Hz, n = 12; p = 0.39. IL: isolated 13.8 ± Hz, n = 12; connected 14.7 ± 0.9Hz, n = 12; p = 0.51).

To investigate whether a direct connection between prelimbic and infralimbic cortexmodulates the oscillation frequency to be different between these areas, we made a cutin whole coronal slices between prelimbic and infralimbic cortex (Figure 4.6a). Indeed,after the cut was made there no longer was a difference in oscillation frequency betweenprelimbic and infralimbic cortex (Figure 4.6b; PrL 13.5 ± 1.3 Hz; IL 13.5 ± 1.1 Hz; n= 7; p = 0.94). Surprisingly, there was also no difference in oscillation power betweenprelimbic and infralimbic cortex (Figure 4.6c; PrL 3.1 ± 1.5 pV2; IL 3.0 ± 1.3 pV2; n =7; p = 0.84), which could result from the large variation in oscillation power.

The above results suggest that prelimbic and infralimbic cortex are interacting duringoscillations since connected prelimbic and infralimbic cortex show differences in frequencythat disappear when the connection between these areas is cut either in whole coronalslices or in isolated mini-slices. Indeed, prelimbic and infralimbic cortex are known to bestrongly connected with each other (Jones et al., 2005). If these areas are affecting eachother during fast network oscillations, one would expect a larger correlation between thefield potentials in these areas when they are connected than in isolation. To that end, wecross-correlated the field potentials in connected prelimbic and infralimbic cortex slices,in disconnected whole coronal slices and in slices from isolated prelimbic and isolatedinfralimbic cortex placed on the same electrode grid (Figure 4.7). In slices of connectedprelimbic and infralimbic cortex there was a significantly higher correlation between thefield potentials than when these areas were disconnected or isolated (Figure 4.7b; cross-correlation at 900 µm; connected: r = 0.33 ± 0.03, n = 12; disconnected: r = 0.12± 0.01, n = 6 isolated: r = 0.12 ± 0.02, n = 5; ANOVA p < 0.01; Newman-Keulsp < 0.01). This suggests that although infralimbic and prelimbic cortex can generateand sustain fast network oscillations, they do interact and affect synchronization of eachothers neuronal networks.

Page 51: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

50 Chapter 4

a

b

30

20

10

0

% T

ime

3020100

Magnitude (µV2)

isolated PrL

connected PrL

20

10

0

% T

ime

3020100

Magnitude (µV2)

isolated IL

connected IL

c

d

15

10

5

0

% E

pis

od

es

210

Duration (s)

isolated PrL

connected PrL

15

10

5

0

% E

pis

od

es

210

Duration (s)

isolated IL

connected IL

e

f

IL

PrL20

15

10

5

Fre

qu

en

cy

(H

z)

isolated connected

ns

10

5

0

connectedisolated

27*

Po

we

r (p

V2)

IL

PrL

Figure 4.5: Oscillations are bigger in isolated slices. (a) Frequency of oscillations in isolatedslices (PrL n = 17; IL n = 12) and connected slices (n = 12). (b) Power of oscillations in isolated slices(PrL n = 17; IL n = 12) and connected slices (n = 16). (c, e) Magnitude distribution and episodeduration of oscillations in isolated (n = 4) and connected prelimbic cortex slices (n = 5). (d, f) as (c,e) for infralimbic cortex (isolated n = 5, connected n = 7).

b ca

20

15

10

5

Fre

qu

en

cy

(H

z)

PrL IL

ns

10

5

0

Po

we

r (p

V2)

PrL IL

ns

Figure 4.6: Oscillation frequency difference disappears after disconnecting prelimbic andinfralimbic cortex. (a) A cut was made between prelimbic and infralimbic cortex. Drawn linesindicate boundaries of prelimbic and infralimbic cortex (adapted from “The Rat Brain in StereotaxicCoordinates”, by G. Paxinos and C. Watson, 2005). (b) Oscillation frequency of disconnected prelimbicand infralimbic cortex (n = 7, p = 0.94). (c) Oscillation power of disconnected prelimbic and infralimbiccortex (n = 7, p = 0.84).

Page 52: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Cortical Interaction 51

ba c isolatedconnected

0.2 s

0.2

0.2 s

0.2

0.2 s

0.2

disconnected d

0.4

0.2

0.0

Ma

x c

orre

lati

on

con dis iso

*

ns

Figure 4.7: Cross-correlation between prelimbic and infralimbic cortex.(a–c) Cross-correlationbetween prelimbic and infralimbic cortex in a connected slice (a) disconnected slice (b) and betweentwo isolated mini-slices. Vertical dashed line at t = 0 s. (d) Maximal cross-correlation (connected n =12; disconnected n = 6; isolated slices n = 5, p < 0.01).

Discussion

Prelimbic and infralimbic cortex are both part of the medial prefrontal cortex (Van Edenand Uylings, 1985) and are associated with different aspects of working memory (Chu-dasama et al., 2003; Killcross and Coutureau, 2003; Dalley et al., 2004; Murphy et al.,2005; Marquis et al., 2007; Ragozzino, 2007). Despite the distinct roles in cognitive be-havior little is known about neuronal network functioning of these areas, and whetherthese networks show much interaction during fast network oscillations. We investigatedcarbachol-induced fast network oscillations in acute rat slices of prelimbic and infral-imbic cortex. We performed simultaneous field recordings of connected prelimbic andinfralimbic cortex, and of isolated prelimbic or infralimbic mini-slices. We found thatneuronal networks of prelimbic and infralimbic cortex can sustain fast network oscilla-tions independent of each other in isolated mini-slices. When connected, fast networkoscillations in prelimbic and infralimbic cortex remain restricted to their own area in50% of the slices. Fast network oscillations in the infralimbic cortex displayed a higherpower than oscillations in prelimbic cortex, in both connected and isolated slices. Indisconnected slices there was no difference in oscillation power, but this could be maskedby the large extend of variation. The difference in oscillation power suggests a differencein internal network properties between prelimbic and infralimbic cortex. The architec-ture of the microcircuit could play a role in this. Hippocampal oscillations display fastnetwork oscillations with much higher power, than oscillations generated by neocorticalnetworks (Buhl et al., 1998; Chrobak and Buzsaki, 1998; Csicsvari et al., 2003; Mannet al., 2005; Bartos et al., 2007). It is generally assumed that this results from the onelayered pyramidal cell structure of the hippocampal circuit (Bartos et al., 2007). Themicrocircuit layout of prelimbic and infralimbic cortex is very alike: both consisting ofa typical neocortical multi-layered structure, both lacking layer 4, which is typical ofthe rodent medial prefrontal cortex (Van Eden and Uylings, 1985; Uylings et al., 2003).However, there are two striking differences between infralimbic and prelimbic corticalarchitecture: 1– the lamination in general, and especially of layer 2 and layer 3, is lessclear in infralimbic cortex (Van Eden and Uylings, 1985; Gabbott et al., 1997); 2– theprelimbic cortex is thicker and contains a larger number of cells per column than infral-imbic cortex (Van Eden and Uylings, 1985; Gabbott et al., 1997). Since fast networkoscillations result from the synchronized activity of large groups of neurons (Steriadeet al., 1990; Lopes da Silva, 1991; Steriade et al., 1993), the increased number of cells

Page 53: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

52 Chapter 4

available in prelimbic cortex would seem an advantage, assuming an equal proportionof cells that participate in fast network oscillations. On the other hand, the thinnercortical layers in the infralimbic cortex could also lead to more alignment of the neuronsgenerating the fast network oscillations, and hence to a greater summation of currents.However, the general decreased lamination of the infralimbic cortex would reduce thiseffect. If it is not through differences in cell number or lamination, the infralimbic cortexcould be more tuned to generate fast network oscillations than prelimbic cortex throughother properties, such as possible differences in cell types, intralaminar connectivity orsensitivity to carbachol.

The frequency of fast network oscillations seems to be partly dependent on the inter-action between prelimbic and infralimbic cortices. When connected, infralimbic cortexoscillated at a higher frequency than prelimbic cortex. This frequency difference dis-appeared when prelimbic and infralimbic cortices were disconnected, as in the isolatedmini-slices experiments. Cross-correlation analysis of the field potential in prelimbic cor-tex with the field potential in infralimbic cortex confirmed the interaction between thetwo areas. Also the power of fast network oscillations was reduced in connected slicescompared to isolated slices for both areas, which suggest that the two oscillations couldinhibit each other. Prelimbic and infralimbic cortex are intrinsically connected (Jones etal., 2005; Hoover and Vertes, 2007). Prelimbic layer 5/6 projects to infralimbic layer 5/6,and infralimbic layers 1–6 project to primarily to prelimbic layers 1, 3 and 5 (Jones et al.,2005). Thus it seems likely that pyramidal cells that fire phase-locked to the local fastnetwork oscillations in one area influence pyramidal cell firing in the other area. How theinteraction at the macrocircuit level influences the local fast network oscillations remainsan intriguing question. We conclude that neuronal networks of prelimbic and infralimbiccortex can sustain fast network oscillations independent of each other, but do interactduring these oscillations and affect synchronization of each others neuronal networks.

The present results suggest that the increase in acetylcholine levels seen in the awakeanimal during working memory tests and cue detection (Passetti et al., 2000; McGaughyet al., 2002; Kozak et al., 2006; Parikh et al., 2007), have a profound and parallel effecton the distinct network activity in prelimbic and infralimbic cortex. Although, until now,most emphasis has been placed on the role of the prelimbic cortex in behavioral flexibility,the greater response of the infralimbic network to carbachol application would justifymore attention for this area. In addition, the connectivity (Jones et al., 2005; Hooverand Vertes, 2007) and interaction between prelimbic and infralimbic cortex during fastnetwork oscillations presented here, suggests that these areas could function in concertwith each other during high acetylcholine levels.

Page 54: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Chapter 5

Discussion

The main findings of the research presented in this thesis are:

1. Frequency fluctuations in human and rat brain oscillations reflect synchronizedactivity in distinct neuronal networks in superficial and deep cortical layers, whichsuggests that cortical subnetworks can process information in a parallel fashion(Chapter 2).

2. Fast spiking and non-fast spiking interneurons participate similarly in nonharmonicfrequency fluctuations, suggesting that the oscillation frequency is not determinedby the kinetics of inhibitory transmission of layer 5 interneurons (Chapter 3).

3. Neuronal networks of prelimbic and infralimbic cortex can sustain fast networkoscillations independent of each other, but neuronal networks of prelimbic andinfralimbic prefrontal cortex are interacting during these oscillations (Chapter 4).

The goal of the research was to get better insight into the functioning of neuronal net-works involved in working memory and attention. Fast network oscillations in the beta-and gamma-band (15–30 Hz resp. 30–100 Hz) of the human EEG are associated withattention and working memory (Tallon-Baudry et al., 1998; Fries et al., 2001; Howard etal., 2003). We studied brain oscillations in human EEG recordings and recordings fromawake, freely moving rats, and characterized the timing of fast and slow oscillations inthe beta band. These fast and slow beta oscillations occurred in short episodes lasting100 to 400 ms and occurred independent of each other. To understand what the synaptic,cellular and network mechanisms underlying these fast and slow oscillations are and howshifts in oscillation frequencies are generated in the neocortex, we turned to the rat brain.Fast and slow network oscillations were induced in acute brain slices by the muscarinicacetylcholine agonist carbachol. We found that fast and slow oscillations were distributedover different cortical layers. Layer 5 pyramidal neurons and interneurons experiencedboth fast and slow frequencies and timed their action potential firing to both frequen-cies. In this chapter, we discuss the implications of these findings for cortical informationprocessing and suggest experiments that could help to understand the function of slowand fast network oscillations during attention behavior.

53

Page 55: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

54 Chapter 5

Cellular mechanisms of neocortical network oscil-

lations

In contrast to carbachol-induced oscillations in hippocampus (Fisahn et al., 1998; Mannet al., 2005), two frequency components were present in ∼ 50% of prefrontal cortexslices after bath application of carbachol. The more superficial layer 3/5 consistentlyoscillated at a higher frequency than deep layer 6 (Figure 2.3b–d). This appears tobe a general phenomenon in the neocortex since a similar distribution of oscillationfrequencies was observed in visual cortex areas V1 and V2 (Figure B.2). Fluctuations inamplitude and frequency of brain oscillations have been observed before, ever since thefirst recordings by Hans Berger in 1924. However, thorough analyses of the dynamicsand the underlying neuronal mechanisms have not been presented before (Steriade et al.,1993; Bragin et al., 1995; Tallon-Baudry et al., 1998; Palva et al., 2005). In Chapter2, we showed in acute brain slices that such frequency fluctuations are mediated by twodistinct networks controlling spike timing in pyramidal neurons of output layers 5 and6. Layer 5 pyramidal neurons participated in both oscillations, but layer 6 pyramidalneurons fired only phase-locked to the slow oscillations (Figures 2.4, 2.5). This impliesthat during slow oscillations layer 5 and layer 6 pyramidal cell firing is tuned. Layer5 pyramidal cells received excitatory and inhibitory post synaptic currents (EPSCs andIPSCs) that were phase-locked to the slow and fast oscillation episodes (Figures 2.6, 2.7).The frequency of the EPSCs and IPSCs was higher during fast oscillation episodes whencompared to slow oscillation episodes (Figures 2.6d, 2.7d).

In chapter 3 we showed that both fast spiking and non-fast spiking interneurons fromlayer 5 fired phase-locked to slow and fast oscillations (Figures 3.4, 3.5). This came asa surprise, since synapses between pyramidal cells and fast spiking and non-fast spikinginterneurons most likely differ in IPSC kinetics (Bacci et al., 2003; Freund, 2003) andexperimental and model studies of hippocampus suggest that IPSC kinetics determinethe oscillation frequency (Whittington et al., 1995; Fisahn et al., 1998; Traub et al.,1998; Palhalmi et al., 2004; Cope et al., 2005). Nonetheless, in PFC these two types ofinterneurons participated in both slow and fast network oscillations.

The order of pyramidal cell firing and excitatory and inhibitory inputs we found inPFC are in line with the synaptic feedback model for hippocampal oscillations, in whichpyramidal cells are connected to other pyramidal cells as well as perisomatic interneurons(Fisahn et al., 1998; Mann and Paulsen, 2005; Mann et al., 2005) (see Chapter 1).Similarly, excitatory inputs to PFC layer 5 pyramidal cells were phase-locked and arrivedat the trough of the local field oscillation, coincident with pyramidal cell firing, whereasinhibitory inputs and interneuron firing were phase-locked to the rising phase of the localfield oscillations and arrived with some delay after cell firing. The most parsimoniousextension of the hippocampal cellular model for network oscillations to the multi-layeredneocortex, which could support two distinct oscillation frequencies, is to connect twolocal feedback networks that oscillate each at their own frequency. Given the spatialdistribution of different oscillation frequencies over superficial and deep cortical layers,with the faster oscillation frequency present in layer 3 and the slower frequency presentin layer 6 with an overlap region in layer 5, these two local feedback networks could belocated in layer 3 and layer 6. Layer 5 pyramidal neurons receive timed synaptic inputfrom both these networks relaying frequency and phase information.

The arrival of inhibitory IPSCs at the layer 5 pyramidal neurons is more preciselytimed than the arrival of excitatory EPSCs, both for fast and slow oscillations. Therefore,

Page 56: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Discussion 55

inhibitory transmission could be more important in determining the firing of pyramidalneurons during oscillations. Since there are interneurons in layer 6 that extent to layer 5and vice versa (Gabbott et al., 1997), these interneurons could innervate both layer 5 andlayer 6 pyramidal cells and determine spike timing during slow oscillation episodes. Dur-ing fast oscillation episodes a different set of interneurons could be active that innervateslayer 3 and layer 5 pyramidal neurons.

Thus, a theoretical model for prefrontal network oscillations would constitute of layer3 and layer 6 pyramidal cells that are innervated by local interneurons. Activation ofthese subnetworks would lead to slow oscillation episodes in layer 6 and fast oscillationsin layer 3. Layer 5 pyramidal cells receive most precise phase and frequency informationthrough both excitatory and inhibitory transmission. Recurrent activation of layer 5interneurons could serve to further improve the timing of firing of the layer 5 pyramidalneuron network, depending which of the two oscillation frequencies is dominant. Futureexperiments should be aimed at testing this model, and resolving the phase-locking oflayer 3 pyramidal neurons, as well as targeting interneurons in layer 3 and 6 to determinethe phase relation of their activity to the different frequencies of oscillations.

Macrocircuit modulation of network oscillations

Differences in the power and frequency of fast network oscillations between prelimbicand infralimbic cortex were apparent despite the fact that these cortical areas have avery similar microcircuit (Figure 4.1). In both areas pyramidal layer 5 neurons firedphase-locked to the oscillation cycle (Figure B.3). And in both areas slow and fastoscillations were observed (Chapter 3). When disconnected, the difference in power wasstill preserved, while the difference in frequency disappeared (Figure 4.4). The latterobservation suggests that modulation at the macrocircuit level rather than differences inthe microcircuit could be responsible for determining the oscillation frequency. A similarmodulation might play a role in the hippocampal system. There, the entorhinal cortexthat provides and receives input from the hippocampal formation, oscillates at a lowerfrequency than the hippocampus (van Der Linden et al., 1999).

Labeling studies show that prelimbic and infralimbic cortex are intrinsically connected(Jones et al., 2005; Hoover and Vertes, 2007). Prelimbic layer 5/6 projects to infralimbiclayer 5/6, and infralimbic layers 1–6 project to primarily to prelimbic layers 1, 3 and 5(Jones et al., 2005). Thus it seems likely that pyramidal cells that fire phase-locked to thelocal fast network oscillations in one area influence pyramidal cell firing in the other area.It would be interesting to know exactly which cells in prelimbic and infralimbic cortexare connected: are for example the output pyramidal cells in layer 5 and 6 from one areatargeting pyramidal cells or interneurons in the other area? Modulation of the oscillationfrequency could result from selective activation or depression of specific interneurons,as IPSC kinetics are thought to determine the oscillation frequency (Whittington et al.,1995; Fisahn et al., 1998; Traub et al., 1998; Palhalmi et al., 2004; Cope et al., 2005).

Page 57: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

56 Chapter 5

Function of slow and fast oscillations

Parallel processing

The muscarinic acetylcholine agonist carbachol induced two distinct field oscillations thatinvolved different subnetworks. The fact that the episodes of slow and fast oscillationsoccurred both simultaneously and separately from each other suggests that these oscilla-tions are independently generated and/or modulated. This is in conflict with a classicalview of subcortical information processing, in which information processing is serial andbegins with cortical input to the superficial layers where it is combined with thalamicinput that has been processed by layer 4, and feedback inputs from layer 5. The result-ing activity is then relayed to output layer 5 neurons, which drive subcortical structuresinvolved in action, like the basal ganglia. Layer 5 neurons also innervate output layer 6neurons that feedback to the thalamus, and thus influence the ongoing input (Douglas andMartin, 2004; Grillner et al., 2005). Instead, I observed that slow and fast oscillationsoccurred independently and determined spike timing of layer 5 and layer 6 pyramidalcells, suggesting that these output pathways function in partly in parallel: during fastoscillation episodes cell spiking of pyramidal neurons in layer 5 and 6 is uncorrelated,but during slow oscillation episodes cell spiking is synchronized, when both layer 5 andlayer 6 cells fire phase-locked to the oscillation cycle.

Extensive labeling studies by Gabbott et al. show that in the rat medial prefrontalcortex layer 5 neurons primarily project to the striatum and the lateral hypothalamus(together ∼ 45%), and furthermore to the amygdala, spinal cord and VTA (Gabbott etal., 1997). The striatum participates in the organization and guidance of complex motorfunctions (Purves et al., 2001). The lateral hypothalamus is involved in the control ofbehavioral arousal and shifts of attention (Purves et al., 2001). GABAergic cells in theventral lateral preoptic nucleus (VLPO) of the hypothalamus innervate thalamocorticalneurons. When these GABAergic cells hyperpolarize the thalamocortical neurons thisenhances the oscillatory state of these neurons via activation of low-threshold calciumchannels. During this oscillatory state, the neurons in the thalamus become synchronizedwith cortical neurons. In this way, the cortex does not receive any information from theoutside world anymore. This is strongest during slow-wave sleep. In contrast, during theawake state thalamocortical neurons are depolarized and in a tonically active state, inwhich they do transmit information from the peripheral stimuli to the cortex (Purves etal., 2001). Hence pyramidal cell spiking in layer 5 of the prefrontal cortex, could influencethe amount of alertness.

In contrast, ∼ 35% of layer 6 neurons project to the mediodorsal thalamus (Gabbottet al., 1997). Of note is that for layer 6 a large proportion its efferents is still unknown.The mediodorsal thalamus receives (processed) information from the primary sensoryand motor cortices, and in its turn provides a mayor input to the frontal cortex (Purveset al., 2001). Thus, pyramidal cell firing in layer 6 of the prefrontal cortex could alterthe thalamic input it is receiving. Hence, in a simplified theoretical scheme, activationof the cortical subnetwork that generates fast oscillations could function to modulate theamount of alertness. Whereas activation of the subnetwork that generates slow oscillationepisodes could alter the thalamic input to the prefrontal cortex in addition. Episodesduring which both frequencies are present could reflect ongoing parallel processing ofinformation in both these networks.

Page 58: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Discussion 57

Relevance for behavior

A first step to understand the relevance of slow and fast oscillations in the prefrontalcortex for behavior could be to simultaneously record field potentials in superficial anddeep layers during a relevant behavioral task. Since acetylcholine levels increase duringtasks that require cue detection and alertness, such as the five-choice-serial-reaction-timetask (Passetti et al., 2000; Parikh et al., 2007), this type of task could be suitable to studythe function of slow and fast oscillations. In chapter 2 we demonstrate that frequencyfluctuations can be observed in the awake, freely moving rat even during quiet rest whendelta/theta oscillations are prominent and power in the beta frequency is low (Figure 2.2).With increased power in the beta-band and fine spatial resolution of recording electrodes,it could be possible to elucidate the differential role of slow and fast oscillation episodes.However, this might turn out to be not so straight forward if we take into account theshort duration (150–400 ms) of these episodes, even during constant bath applicationof carbachol (Figure 2.3f,g). In vivo the duration of episodes is even shorter (100–200ms), both in the human EEG and in the awake rats (Figures 2.1e–f, 2.2c–d), althoughboth these experiments were performed during quiet rest, when acetylcholine levels areexpected to be low. And since slow and fast episodes in the rat brain slice both are verydynamic, it might be difficult to make the connection to a specific aspect of the task.Thus, another way to investigate the function of slow and fast oscillations would be tointerfere with them.

Although layer 5 interneurons fire phase-locked to both slow and fast oscillationepisodes (Figure 3.4, 3.5), this need not be the case for interneurons in deep layer 3, orlayer 6, where the network oscillations are less mixed than in layer 5. Layer 6 pyramidalcells showed only phase-locking to the slow oscillations when present (Figure 2.5), thismay also be true for layer 6 interneurons. If interneurons that are specific for one typeof oscillation can be found and characterized, this may open possibilities to selectivelyenhance or decrease one type of oscillation using specific drugs. Or, if these interneuronsare restricted to a specific area, local infusion with synaptic transmission blockers couldalso selectively decrease the activity of one type of oscillation. In this way the effect ofdecreased or increased slow or fast oscillations could be studied in the behaving animal.

To study the function of slow and fast network oscillations in humans a similarapproach could be used as described by Marshall et al (Marshall et al., 2006). Applyingan electrical field of a certain frequency has been shown to increase the power and shiftthe frequency of on going oscillations (Deans et al., 2007). In this way, it may be possibleto force the prefrontal cortex to display only slow or only fast oscillations. In addition,a more detailed analysis of human EEG data during tasks where high frequencies areenhanced may already shed some light on a possible function. A comparison with patientssuffering from attention disorders might also be insightful.

Implications for working memory and attention

To get a better understanding of the neuronal networks underlying working memory andattention, it would be worthwhile to investigate the effect of known modulators of workingmemory and attention on the generation and stability of network oscillations. Dopamineand nicotine are known to play distinct roles in working memory and attention and canpartly diminish the symptoms of ADHD and schizophrenia patients (Lohr and Flynn,1992; Glassman, 1993; Goldman-Rakic, 1995; Levin and Simon, 1998; Sarter et al., 1999;Mansvelder et al., 2005). In hippocampus, high concentrations of dopamine can block

Page 59: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

58 Chapter 5

carbachol-induced fast network oscillations (Weiss et al., 2003). Preliminary results fromprefrontal cortex slices show that nicotine also reduces oscillation power (Mansvelderet al., 2005), which may result from increased inhibition of pyramidal cells (Couey etal., 2007). These results are encouraging experiments where network and cellular effectsare studied simultaneously. The fact that dopamine can have opposite effects on cellexcitability depending on the used concentration and time after application (Goldman-Rakic et al., 2000; Gulledge and Jaffe, 2001), really necessitates a systematic approach.

In addition to studying the modulation of fast network oscillations by dopamine andnicotine in normal wild-type animals, it would be worthwhile to do the same in animalswith attention and/or working memory deficits, such as the SNAP25 knock-out mouseor dopamine transporter (DAT) knock-out mouse (Russell, 2007; van der Kooij andGlennon, 2007). In addition, one could look at the extremes within a normal wild-typepopulation or use (recombinant) inbred strains (Pattij et al., 2006).

In conclusion

Despite extensive behavioral studies in humans and animals, with or without combinationof single cell recordings, prefrontal functioning and dysfunction is still poorly understood.Our approach has been to study the intermediate level between behavior and single cells,namely the neuronal network level. We have made a first step towards understandingthe cellular and synaptic mechanisms that underlie slow and fast network oscillations inthe prefrontal cortex. The uncovering of two separate subnetworks that partly functionin parallel proves that this is a promising approach. Future studies of the cellular effectsof modulators such as acetylcholine, dopamine and nicotine, should also encompass theeffects at the (sub)network level by recording oscillatory field potentials. On a higherlevel, network oscillations can be used to study the interaction between (related) brainareas such as the prelimbic and infralimbic cortex. In addition, our results can contributeto a more precise (clinical) interpretation of EEG data which can contribute to a betterunderstanding of prefrontal functioning under normal and pathological conditions.

Page 60: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Appendix A

Materials and Methods

Slice Preparation and Electrophysiology

Prefrontal coronal cortical slices (Paxinos and Watson 2005) (400 µm) were preparedfrom P14–28 Wistar rats, in accordance with Dutch license procedures. Brain slices wereprepared in ice-cold artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF), which contained: 125 mM NaCl,3 mM KCl, 1.25 mM NaH2PO4, 3 mM MgSO4, 1 mM CaCl2, 26 mM NaHCO3, and 10mM glucose (300 mOsm). Slices were then transferred to holding chambers in which theywere stored in ACSF containing: 125 mM NaCl, 3 mM KCl, 1.25 mM NaH2PO4, 2 mMMgSO4, 2 mM CaCl2, 26 mM NaHCO3, and 10 mM glucose, bubbled with carbogen gas(95% O2/5% CO2). Slices were left to recover at room temperature for one hour.

Multi-electrode Recordings

After recovery, slices were mounted on 8 × 8 arrays of planar microelectrodes (electrodesize: 50 µm × 50 µm; interpolar distance: 150 or 300 µm; Panasonic MED-P5155;Tensor Biosciences, Irvine, CA). To improve slice adhesion, the multi-electrode probeswere coated with 0.1% polyethylenimine (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO) in 10 mM boratebuffer (pH 8.4) for at least 6 hr before use. The multi-electrode probe was then placedin a chamber saturated with humidified carbogen gas for at least 1 hr. For recordings,slices were maintained in submerged conditions at 25 ◦C, and superfused with ACSF,bubbled with carbogen, at 4–5 ml/min. Spontaneous field potentials from all 64 recordingelectrodes were acquired simultaneously at 20 kHz, using the Panasonic MED64 system(Tensor Biosciences), and decimated off-line to 200 Hz or 2 kHz.

Single-Cell Electrophysiology

Pyramidal cells and interneurons located in layer 5 and 6 of the prelimbic and infralimbicpart of the medial PFC (Van Eden and Uylings 1985; Gabbott, Dickie et al. 1997),visualized using infrared differential interference contrast microscopy, were selected onthe basis of their morphology and firing pattern (Tables 3.1 and B.2). All experiments

59

Page 61: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

60 Appendix A

were performed at 25 ◦C. Basic cell passive and active properties were assessed by initialhyperpolarization, followed by stepped depolarization.

Recordings were made using a Multiclamp 700B amplifier (Axon Instruments, CA),sampled at intervals of 50 or 100 µs, digitized by pClamp software (Axon), and lateranalyzed off-line (Igor Pro software, Wavemetrics, Lake Oswego, OR). To ensure temporalalignment between the multi-electrode and single-cell electrophysiological signals, thecurrent or voltage signals from the patch-clamp amplifiers were recorded on one channelof the MED64 system, using a custom-made interface device (Tensor Biosciences).

Patch pipettes (3–5 MOhms) were pulled from standard-wall borosilicate capillariesand were filled with one of two intracellular solutions. For whole-cell current-clamprecordings, we used a solution containing 140 mM K-gluconate, 1 mM KCl, 10 mMHEPES, 4 mM K-phosphocreatine, 4 mM ATP-Mg, and 0.4 mM GTP and 5 mg/mlbiocytin (pH 7.3, pH adjusted to 7.3 with KOH; osmolarity, ∼ 300 mOsm/l). For whole-cell voltage-clamp recordings, potassium gluconate was replaced by equimolar cesiumgluconate. 2 mM QX-314 in cesium gluconate was added to the intracellular solutionprior to use to prevent cells from spiking. Series resistance was not compensated.

Human EEG Recordings

Ongoing electrical brain activity in sessions of 20 min was measured with electroen-cephalography (EEG) from eight normal subjects (aged 20–30 years). This data set hasbeen used in (Linkenkaer-Hansen, Nikouline et al. 2001). The study was approved bythe Ethics Committee of the Department of Radiology of the Helsinki University CentralHospital. The EEG cap had 60 electrodes. The subjects were seated in a magneticallyshielded room and instructed to relax and sit still with eyes closed during the recordingsessions. The data were sampled at 900 Hz and resampled off-line to 300 Hz with thepassband of 0.1–100 Hz (6th order Butterworth digital filters).

Intracranial local field recordings in the awake rat

Five adult male Wistar rats (350–450 g, Harlan, Horst, the Netherlands), kept underreverse day/light cycle (7 AM – 7 PM: lights off), were anaesthetized with 1.75% isoflu-rane in O2 and N2O and mounted in a Kopf stereotaxic frame. Body temperature wasmaintained at 36.5 ◦C using a temperature-feedback controlled heating pad. After skullexposure, a burr hole was drilled to allow mPFC tetrode placement (AP: +3.2 mm; ML+0.7 mm and DV 2.5–3.5 mm with respect to bregma and cortical surface). Tetrodesconsisted of four twisted insulated nichrom wires (Kanthal, Hallstahammar, Sweden). Inaddition, six titanium fixation screws and one ground screw penetrating the interpari-etal skull bone were placed after which the tetrode driver (Neuralynx, Bozeman, USA),holding four moveable tetrodes, was cemented in place. After implantation, the ratswere allowed to recover from surgery, on average for 10 days, before recordings started.Recordings of the mPFC local field potentials were made in the home cage under videosurveillance at the end of the animals night phase between 5 PM and 7 PM. Local fieldpotentials were bandpass filtered (0.7–170 Hz), amplified (2000 times) and digitized (1000Hz) using a Plexon system (Plexon Inc, Dallas, USA). For each animal 5–6 minute pe-riods of quiet rest were selected for further analysis. At the end of experimentation, asmall electrolytic lesion was made through each lead of each tetrode. Lesion sites weredetermined in Nissl-stained cryosections of paraformaldehyde (4% in buffered phosphate)perfused brains. All experiments were conducted in accordance with Dutch license pro-cedures.

Page 62: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Materials and Methods 61

300

200

100On

se

t o

sc

illa

tio

ns

(s

)

3020100

Time window (s)

slice 1 slice 2 slice 3

elec. 1 elec. 2

a b c

d

3020100Frequency (Hz)

10 µV2

time window (s) 2 4 8 16

e

10

0

302010Frequency (Hz)

global

95% fit

Ma

gn

itu

de (

µV

2)

30

20

10

4002000Time (s)

20 µV

5 Hz 17 Hz

Ma

gn

itu

de

V2)

8

6

4

2

0

151050

Time window (s)

slice 1 slice 2 slice 3

elec. 1 elec. 2

Co

nfi

de

nc

e i

nte

rva

l (µ

V2)

f

Ma

gn

itu

de (

µV

2)

30

20

10

302010

Frequency (Hz)

time (s) 0

128

256

384

Figure A.1: Calculation of significance field oscillations in rat brain slices. (a) Global wavelettransforms of 32 sec time windows at several time points after carbachol application. (b) Magnitudeof wavelet transform at 5 Hz (reference frequency) and 17 Hz (oscillation frequency) of field potential(bottom). Wavelet transforms were performed on 32 sec time windows. (c) Exponential fit with 95%confidence interval of wavelet transform. (d) Calculation of onset of oscillations for different timewindow sizes (n = 3 slices, n = 6 electrodes). (e) Example 95% confidence intervals of exponential fitsfor different time window sizes. (f) 95% Confidence intervals for different time window sizes (n = 3slices, n = 6 electrodes).

Data Analysis

Electrophysiological data was analyzed using custom-written procedures in Igor Pro(Wavemetrics, OR, USA).

Significance of oscillations

Brain SlicesAfter application of 25 µM muscarinic acetylcholine receptor agonist carbachol, neu-

ronal activity in the slice increases (Figures 2.1a,b and A.1a). Power spectrum analysisshows first an increase in red noise (1/f noise) during wash-in. Then, after some ∼ 3–5 minutes, oscillations occur that show a distinct peak in the power spectrum (FigureA.1a,b). Wavelet analysis (Torrence and Compo 1998) showed that when the oscilla-tions are clearly present, still the magnitude and frequency of oscillations fluctuates intime (Figure 2.3f). To identify and quantify episodes during which field oscillations arepresent, we compared the wavelet magnitude of ongoing field oscillations with the waveletmagnitude during wash-in period, when the red (1/f) noise was elevated, but no distinctoscillations were yet visible, i.e. just before the onset of oscillations (time point 128 secin Figure A.1a,b). This prevented false positive identification of oscillation episodes dueto overall increases in red (1/f) noise.

Page 63: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

62 Appendix A

a b25

20

15

10M

ag

nit

ud

e (

µV

2)

40302010

Frequency (Hz)

globals

95% fit25

20

15

10

Ma

gn

itu

de

V2)

40302010

Frequency (Hz)

Figure A.2: Calculation significance of oscillations for EEG data. (a) Example global wavelettransforms of 8 sec time windows (gray) with superposed 95% confidence fit (black) from (b). (b)Average of global wavelet transforms (gray) with exponential fit (black) from 12–15 Hz to 45–48 Hz(dashed lines).

The onset of oscillations was determined by averaging the wavelet magnitude in acertain time window (see below) for the whole frequency spectrum: a “global wavelet”(Figure A.1a). When followed in time, the global wavelet power spectrum first showedan increase in red (1/f) noise followed by the appearance of a distinct oscillation peak. Ineach experiment, the increase in magnitude at the oscillation frequency was compared tothe reference frequency of 5 Hz, the magnitude of which only increased as part of the red(1/f) noise. The time point of the onset of oscillations was determined as the intersectionof these curves (Figure A.1b). The 95% confidence interval of an exponential fit to theglobal wavelet power spectrum at the time window preceding the onset of oscillationswas taken as the threshold for oscillations during the entire recording (Figure A.1c).

To determine the impact of the time window size on the threshold, we investigatedthe effect of window size on the two parameters that determine the threshold. Firstly, thethreshold will depend on the time point used for constructing the global wavelet powerspectrum. Secondly, the threshold will depend on how well the power spectrum beforethe onset of oscillations was fitted by a mono-exponential function. When the onsettime of oscillations was calculated for different lengths of time windows (2–4–8–16–32s), there was a general trend towards an earlier onset for larger window sizes (FigureA.1d). However, the variation in onset time between experiments and between differentelectrodes from the same experiment by far exceeded the variation due to different windowsizes (average standard deviation of time windows within experiments: 6.7 ± 0.3 s;average standard deviation between experiments 37.3 ± 0.6 s; p < 0.05). Thus, timewindow sizes between 2 sec and 32 sec to calculate the ’global wavelet’ does not affectthe onset time point of oscillations.

In contrast, the goodness of fit of the global wavelet power spectrum was affected bythe time window size (Figure A.1e). The noise in the global wavelet power spectra ob-tained with short time window sizes below 8 seconds gives rise to a broad 95% confidenceinterval of the exponential fit. Plotting the 95% confidence interval of the fit againstdifferent time window sizes for different experiments and different electrodes showed thatthe confidence interval becomes narrower with increasing time window lengths (FigureA.1f). Time window sizes above 8 seconds did not result in narrower confidence intervals.To determine the onset of oscillations with sufficient time resolution, but at the sametime with sufficiently low noise levels to obtain a good exponential fit, a time window of8 seconds was used in all experiments.

In vivo recordingsFor the in vivo data, in which no acetylcholine-free period can be determined, we

fitted an exponential starting on the decay of the alpha-peak and ending at 48 Hz, butexcluding any present beta-band peaks, of the averaged wavelet power plot (Figure A.2).

Page 64: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Materials and Methods 63

In this way we obtained an estimation of the baseline values at beta-band frequencies.The 95% confidence interval of the exponential fit at the beta-band frequencies was takenas the threshold value.

Frequency specificity of wavelet analysis

Due to the lower frequency resolution that is inherent to the increased time-resolutionof wavelet analysis, two frequency peaks that are clearly separated in the Fourier powerspectrum will be less clearly separated using wavelet analysis. In our quantification ofslow and fast oscillation episodes this could potentially lead to an overestimation of thepercentage of co-occurrence of oscillations. We used multi-electrode recordings in whichslow and fast oscillations were present both separate and mixed at different electrodes(Figure 2.3b,c) to verify the capacity of the wavelet analysis to distinguish between twooscillations. The magnitude of slow and fast oscillations that was calculated from theelectrode with both slow and fast oscillations (L5) was positively correlated with themagnitude that was calculated from electrodes at which only the slow (L6) or only thefast oscillation (L3/5) was present (slow: r = 0.71, p < 0.01; fast: r = 0.64, p < 0.01).Moreover, the calculation of oscillation magnitude with wavelet analysis appeared to befrequency-specific: there was only a very weak correlation between the magnitude ofthe slow oscillation in L6 with the magnitude of the fast oscillation in L5 (r = 0.03, p< 0.01), and no correlation between the magnitude of the fast oscillation in L3/5 andthe slow oscillation in L5 (r = −0.02, p = 1). Subsequent quantification of episodeduration and frequency, although calculated from electrodes that differed in oscillationpower, was similar (duration: slow: 499.2 ms (L6), 433.0 ms (L5); fast: 291.2 ms (L3/5),221.2 ms (L5); frequency: slow: 1.0 Hz (L6), 1.2 Hz (L5); fast: 2.0 Hz (L3/5), 2.3 Hz(L5)). The amount of occurrence and co-occurrence did not seem to be over-estimatedin the L5 electrode (occurrence: slow: 23.5% (L5), 18.3% (L3/5–L6); fast: 18.6% (L5),18.3% (L3/5–L6); co-occurrence: 47.9% (L5), 58.6% (L3/5–L6); no oscillations: 10.0%(L5), 4.8% (L3/5–L6)). Thus we conclude that although wavelet analysis has a lowerfrequency resolution, this did not lead to false identification of oscillation episodes in ourstudies.

Separation of oscillation episodes

To determine if single cells fired correlated to one or both field oscillations, we classifiedthe recorded spikes or synaptic events, as occurring during episodes when the fast or slowoscillation was present, or during episodes when no significant oscillation was present.We used two different approaches to achieve this. In the first approach a spike or synapticevent was classified to oscillation A, if, firstly, oscillation A was significantly present atthat moment in time (using the maximal 1/f noise threshold, see above), and secondly,if the magnitude of oscillation A was greater than that of oscillation B.

The second approach used an objective threshold (see below) and classified a spike orsynaptic event to oscillation A, if the magnitude of oscillation A was above that thresholdat that moment in time, and the magnitude of oscillation B was below that threshold atthat moment in time. In this way, the timing of spikes of layer 5 pyramidal neurons couldbe phase related to the dominant oscillation frequency at a given time point in layer 5with the least amount of interference from the other oscillation at that time. To find outat which threshold value oscillation A and oscillation B would be maximally alternating,we calculated the percentage of recording time at which the oscillations were separatelypresent for a range of thresholds (Figure A.3). Starting at a threshold of zero, both os-cillations were above threshold for the entire recording (Figure A.3a), and therefore thepercentage of separate oscillations was zero (Figure A.3b). With increasing threshold,

Page 65: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

64 Appendix A

a b100

50

0T

ime

ab

ov

e

thre

sh

old

(%

)40200

Threshold (µV2)

100

50

0Oc

cu

ren

ce

(%

)

40200

Threshold (µV2)

simultaneous

separate

Figure A.3: Separating slow and fast oscillations maximally. (a) Percentage of recording timethat is above threshold shown for example experiment. (b) Percentage of recording time that slowand fast oscillation are present simultaneously (black) or separately (gray). Vertical dashed line atthreshold value where slow and fast oscillations are maximally separate.

the differentiation between both oscillations increased, as did the percentage of alternat-ing oscillations. Some moments in time oscillation A was above threshold, sometimes B,sometimes both. At high threshold levels the amount of recording time above thresholddecreased, as did the percentage of alternating oscillations. The threshold value at whichalternating oscillations were maximally present was determined for each experiment in-dividually. The threshold taken to determine whether an oscillation was above thresholdwas set at the maximum value of the “alternating” curve (Figure A.3b). Spikes thatoccurred when none of the oscillations was above threshold were excluded from phasedetermination.

Next, we compared a large subset of data with both methods (n = 10 cells; n = 30analyses, Figure A.4). The 1/f-thresholds were lower compared to the “max separate”-thresholds used by the second approach (Figure A.4a; slow: 1.22 × 10−5 ± 5.15 × 10−7

vs. 2.06 × 10−5 ± 2.39 × 10−6, p < 0.05; fast: 1.07 × 10−5 ± 4.50 × 10−7 vs. 1.72 ×10−5 ± 1.87 × 10−6, p < 0.05), and less spikes or synaptic events were included (FigureA.4b; slow: 547.0 ± 144.4 vs. 163.0 ± 50.3, p < 0.05; fast: 848.3 ± 249.8 vs. 543.4 ±183.7, p < 0.05). However, there were no differences in circular statistics between thetwo methods: mean phase (Figure A.4c; slow: 3.74 0.18 vs 3.87 0.23, p=0.45; fast: 3.68± 0.27 vs. 3.73 ± 0.24, p = 0.46) and phase-coupling (Figure A.4d; slow: 0.20 ± 0.03vs. 0.23 ± 0.03, p = 0.17; fast: 0.28 ± 0.05 vs. 0.28 ± 0.04, p = 0.52) were the same forboth methods. Significance (Rayleigh test; p < 0.05) was the same for 26/30 analyses(Figure A.4e). The different outcome in 4/30 analyses could partially (2/4) be explainedby the low number of included spikes (29 and 32), when the “max separate”-thresholdwas used. We conclude therefore that both approaches yield the same result. The 1/f-threshold method was used for final analysis, to guarantee a sufficient number of includedspikes and synaptic events.

Spike, EPSC and IPSC timing relative to field

The spike, EPSC and IPSC timing in single cells was analyzed relative to the ongoingfield oscillation. Spikes were detected using simple threshold detection. Synaptic eventswere detected using the Mini Analysis Program (see below). Spike/event probabilityhistograms were constructed by assigning each spike or event time to a phase bin of π/10radians, and dividing by the total number of spikes. Circular statistics were used totest whether individual cells showed phase locking to the network oscillation, and cellsnot significantly coupled to the network were excluded from further analysis (Rayleightest, p < 0.05). Different conditions within the same cell were tested using the Watson-Williams test. This, however, did not often give reliable results due to low concentration

Page 66: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Materials and Methods 65

a

25

20

15

10Th

res

ho

ld (

µV

2)

1/f separate

*

slow

fast

1000

500

0Nu

m in

cl. e

ve

nts

1/f separate

*

slow

fast

0.4

0.2

0.0Ph

ase-c

ou

plin

g

1/f separate

ns

slow

fast

ed

cb

0.04

0

P-v

alu

e

1/f separate 1/f separate

0.6

0.2

slow fast

1.5 π

1.0 π

Ph

ase

(ra

d)

1/f separate

ns

slow

fast

1.3 π

Figure A.4: Circular statistics of spikes and synaptic events using two different methodsto separate slow and fast oscillation episodes. (a) Threshold values to determine slow (black)and fast (gray) oscillation episodes, using the maximal 1/f noise method (“1/f”, Figure A.1) and themaximal separation method (“separate”, Figure A.3) (n = 10 cells, n = 30 analysis). Note that slowand fast oscillations also have a different threshold value using the “separate” method. Thresholds arenormalized for the wavelet transform that enhances low frequencies compared to high frequencies. (b)Number of included spikes or synaptic events when thresholds from (a) are used. (c) Phase of firingof spikes and synaptic events. (d) Phase-coupling of spikes and synaptic events. (e) Significance ascalculated with Rayleigh’s test. Note broken y-axis at p = 0.05. Individual analyses are shown ofwhich the outcome did (gray, n = 4) and did not (black, n = 26) change between used methods.

values. Therefore, differences between population means of phase and phase-couplingwere tested using Student’s paired t-test. Differences in phase and phase-coupling be-tween spikes, EPSCs, IPSCs and interneurons were tested using one-way ANOVA withpost-hoc Student Newman Keuls Test.

For comparison of spike timing and EPSC/IPSC arrival only data from IL cortex wasused, to ensure that spikes/events were referenced at the same cortical layer. As PrLand IL have different cortical thickness (Table B.2), this results in a slight phase shiftwhen spikes are referenced to a fixed position from layer 2. Furthermore, as there wereno differences in timing or phase-coupling between slow and fast oscillations, cells thatwere recorded when there was only one oscillation present were also included for the finalcomparison of pyramidal cells, EPSC’s, IPSC’s and interneurons.

Synaptic Events

We recorded excitatory and inhibitory postsynaptic currents (EPSCs and IPSCs) in L5pyramidal cells held in whole-cell voltage clamp at −70 mV and +20 mV, respectively.Synaptic events were detected by amplitude, rise time, and surface area criteria usingthe Mini Analysis Program software (Synaptosoft Inc.). After automated detection eachevent was visually inspected. Events for which the rise time was slower than the decaytime were excluded from further analysis.

Page 67: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

66 Appendix A

Synaptic event frequencies

For calculation of event frequencies during slow or fast oscillation episodes (Figure 2.6d,2.7d), cells were excluded if they had less than 100 inter-event-intervals. Event frequencieswere compared using the Mann-Whitney test.

CSD

All signals were peak-to-peak averaged relative to a reference recording from layer 3/5 orlayer 6. Signals were band-pass filtered between 5 and 35 Hz before detection of negativesignal peaks. Each peak-to-peak signal was interpolated to a 100-point wave, and thesewaves were averaged to provide the peak-to-peak average. Current source density (CSD)analysis was performed on the peak-to-peak average cycles. The one-dimensional CSDwas computed as the second spatial derivative of the voltage signals following spatialfiltering with a 3-point Gaussian filter. For two-dimensional CSD signals were passedthrough a 3 × 3 Gaussian spatial filter and convolved with a 3 × 3 Laplacian kernel (0−1 0, −1 4 −1, 0 −1 0), as previously described (Shimono, Taketani et al. 2001; Mann,Suckling et al. 2005). CSD signals from outer electrodes are not presented, as they maycontain edge artifacts. CSD plots are shown using an inverted color scale, with warmcolors corresponding to current sinks (i.e., neuronal membrane inward currents) and coolcolors corresponding to current sources.

Statistics

Data are represented as mean ± s.e.m. Statistical analysis used either the Student’s t test(paired or unpaired) or an ANOVA with Student Newman Keuls post-hoc test, as appro-priate. Correlation analysis was performed calculating Pearson correlation coefficients.For nonparametric data the Mann-Whitney test was used. Circular statistics used theRayleigh test, and the Watson-Williams test. All statistics were calculated in Igor exceptfor the Watson-Williams test that was calculated in Oriana (Kovach Computing Services,UK) and the Mann-Whitney test that was calculated using GraphPad InStat (version3.05, GraphPad Software, San Diego California USA, www.graphpad.com). Asterisksrepresent p < 0.05.

Histological Procedures

After intracellular recording, the slices were fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 M or0.2 M phosphate buffer (PB; pH 7.4) for at least 24 hr, followed by several washoutswith PB. Then slices were treated with 1% H2O2 in PB for 10 min, to reduce endoge-nous peroxidase activity. Biocytin-filled cells were visualized using avidin-biotinylatedhorseradish peroxidase complex reaction (ABC, Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA)with 3,3’-diaminobenzidine (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO) as chromogen giving a darkreaction product. After dehydration and embedding in Entellan (Merck, Darmstadt, Ger-many) or Durcopan resin (Fluka, Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO), representative neuronswere reconstructed with the aid of a drawing tube at 40-63 × magnification.

Drugs and Chemicals

Carbamoylcholine chloride (carbachol, CCh), bicuculline-methiodide and atropine werefrom Sigma-Aldrich (St. Louis, MO); 6,7-dinitroquinoxaline-2,3(1H,4H)-dione (DNQX)from RBI (Natrick, MA, USA); QX-314 from Alomone Labs (Jerusalem, Israel); ε-biotinoyl-L-lysine (biocytin) from Invitrogen (Molecular Probes, Oregon, USA).

Page 68: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Appendix B

Supplemental Tables andFigures

67

Page 69: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

68 Appendix B

Table B.1: EEG characteristics at occipital electrode Oz. All values are means ± s.e.m.; ** p < 0.01;n = 5

slow β fast β

Frequency (Hz) 17.7 ± 1.2 21.5 ± 1.2 **

Duration episodes (ms) 111.2 ± 5.8 152.7 ± 16.0

Frequency episodes (Hz) 3.3 ± 0.3 3.3 ± 0.4

Correlation: - α - 0.25 ± 0.03 0.17 ± 0.03

- slow β - fast β 0.56 ± 0.11

β freq above threshold (%)

- slow β + fast β 39.0 ± 6.5

- only slow β 8.9 ± 2.8

- only fast β 26.6 ± 5.3

- none 25.7 ± 2.6

Table B.2: Location and Firing properties of L5 and L6 pyramidal cells in mPFC. All values aremeans ± s.e.m.; ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05; IL-L5: n = 5, PrL–L5: n = 11, PrL–L6: n = 5. a AHP,Afterhyperpolarization; calculated as the difference between action potential threshold and the mostnegative level reached during the repolarizing phase. b Spike frequency adaptation was calculated asthe ratio of the instantaneous frequency at the second and fourth spike interval in an action potentialtrain (IL–L5: n = 5, PrL–L5: n = 11, PrL–L6: n = 4).

Cortex IL PrL PrL

Layer L5 L5 L6

Distance to L2 (µm) 364.3 ± 47.5 * 504.5 ± 22.8 ** 825.0 ± 33.5

Action potential

- threshold (mV) -35.5 ± 2.3 -36.5 ± 1.4 -37.5 ± 2.0

- height (mV) 49.5 ± 6.2 47.4 ± 3.0 49.2 ± 1.8

- half-width (ms) 2.2 ± 0.2 1.9 ± 0.1 1.8 ± 0.1

- AHP (mV)a 10.5 ± 2.6 6.6 ± 1.5 ** 15.2 ± 2.3

adaptationb 0.86 ± 0.16 0.63 ± 0.05 * 0.85 ± 0.06

Page 70: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Supplemental Tables and Figures 69

a b

m

f g h

i j k

l

c100

50

0

Po

we

r (%

)

3020100

Time (min)

*d

100

50

0

Po

we

r (%

)

control CCh atropine

* *

100

50

0

Po

we

r (%

)

control CCh DNQX

* *

100 ms 10 µV

control

CCh

+ atropine

60 s 10 µV

200 ms 10 µV

+ carbachol

5 µM

25 µM

100 µM

100 ms 5 µV

control

CCh

+ DNQX

6

4

2

0

Po

we

r (p

V2)

302010Frequency (Hz)

controlCChatropine

100

50

0

Po

we

r (%

)

control CCh bic

* *

control

CCh

+ bic

300

200

100

0

Po

we

r (f

V2)

302010Frequency (Hz)

controlCChbic

400

200

0

Po

we

r (f

V2)

302010Frequency (Hz)

controlCChDNQX

e

n

Po

we

r (p

V2) 10

5

0

control CCh

*

20

10

0

Po

we

r (p

V2)

100500CCh concentration (µM)

*

*

ns

30

20

10

Fre

qu

en

cy

(H

z)

302520Temperature (°C)

L3/5L6

200 ms 10 µV

10 s

20 µV

200 ms 20 µV 100 ms 10 µV

Figure B.1: Carbachol (CCh) induces high-frequency network oscillations that are depen-dent on intact glutamatergic and GABAergic synaptic transmission. (a) Field recordingsafter application of 5, 25 and 100 µM carbachol. (b) Power (area power spectrum from 5–35 Hz) offield oscillations induced by 5 µM CCh (n =3 ), 25 µM (n = 19) and 100 µM (n = 3). (c) Powerwas significantly greater during carbachol conditions compared to control (n = 9). Note the variationin power in the carbachol condition. (d) Normalized power of oscillations remained stable over a 30min time period (n = 9). (e) The frequency of oscillations was dependent on recording temperature(Pearson correlation test: L3/5: r = 0.63, p < 0.01, n = 15; L6: r = 0.69, p < 0.05, n = 10). (f–h)Oscillations were blocked by 10 µM atropine (n = 4). (f) Field potentials. (g) Power spectrum. (h)Quantification. (i–k) as (f–h) for 20 µM Kainate/AMPA-R antagonist DNQX (n = 3). (l–n) as (f–h)for 20 µM GABA-R antagonist bicucullin (n = 2). Note the induction of low frequency seizures withbicucullin (l, bottom left), which were excluded from the power spectrum (m).

Page 71: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

70 Appendix B

d

2010Frequency (Hz)

50 fV2 L2/3

L4

L5/6

a b c

e

200 ms 5 µV

L2/3

L4

L5/6

15

10

5

0

%E

pis

od

es

1.00.50.0

Duration (s)

V1slowfast

10

5

0

1.00.50.0

V2slowfast

20

10

Fre

qu

en

cy

(H

z)

sup deep sup deep

* *V1 V2

40

20

0

% T

ime

both slow fast

Oscillations above threshold

V1

V2

-1

0

1

Co

rre

lati

on

V1 V2

f

Figure B.2: Distribution of slow and fast oscillations over cortical layers in rat visual cortexslices. (a) Field potentials were simultaneously recorded across cortical layers. (b) Power spectra ofrecordings in (a). (c) Frequency of oscillations in superficial and deep layers (V1 (n = 4): superficial17.8 ± 2.8 Hz, deep 12.5 ± 1.1 Hz, p < 0.05; V2 (n = 5): superficial 16.6 ± 1.1 Hz, deep 10.9 ± 1.0Hz, p < 0.01). (d) Duration of episodes of slow and fast oscillations (V1: slow 221.8 ± 11.0 ms, fast138.0 ± 21.0 ms, p = 0.07; V2: slow 277.0 ± 44.3 ms, fast 183.4 ± 34.7 ms, p < 0.05). (e) Slow andfast oscillations were present simultaneously and separate from each other (V1: both 31.9 ± 6.3%, slow22.3 ± 2.9%, fast 22.0 ± 2.4%, none 39.0 ± 16.0%; V2: both 36.9 ± 6.3% slow 22.0 ± 2.4%. fast 22.0± 1.6%, none 32.6 ± 14.2%). (f) Correlation magnitude slow and fast oscillations (V1: r = 0.12 ±0.08, p < 0.01 in 3/4; V2: r = 0.08 ± 0.03, p < 0.01 in 5/5).

a b

L5 pyramidal cells

Ph

as

e (

rad

)

slow fast

ns1.5 π

1.0 π

0.5 π

0.4

0.2

0.0

Ph

as

e-c

ou

plin

g

slow fast

ns

IL

PrL

IL

PrL

Figure B.3: Phase of firing and phase-coupling of L5 pyramidal cells is not different duringslow and fast oscillations. (a) Phase of firing for slow and fast oscillations. The trough of theoscillation cycle is defined as π radians. Note that the difference between prelimbic and infralimbiccells is due to cortical thickness differences (Table B.2), as the reference electrode was taken at a fixedlength from L2. (b) Phase-coupling to slow and fast oscillations.

Page 72: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Supplemental Tables and Figures 71

a b

0.5

0.0

Ph

as

e-c

ou

pli

ng

slow

Ph

as

e (

rad

)

slow

2.0 π

0.5 π

1.3 π

L6 pyramidal cells

Figure B.4: L6 pyramidal cell firing is only controlled by slow oscillations. (a) Phase of firingfor slow oscillations. The trough of the oscillation cycle is defined as π radians. (b) Phase-coupling toslow oscillations.

a b

2 π

1 πPh

as

e (

rad

)

slow fast

ns

0.4

0.2

0.0

Ph

as

e-c

ou

plin

g

slow fast

ns

EPSCs

Figure B.5: Phase and phase-coupling of EPSCs is not different during slow and fastoscillations. (a) Phase of EPSCs during slow and fast oscillations. The trough of the oscillation cycleis defined as π radians. (b) Phase-coupling to slow and fast oscillations.

a b

2 π

1 π

Ph

as

e (

rad

)

slow fast

ns1.0

0.5

0.0

Ph

as

e-c

ou

plin

g

slow fast

ns

IPSCs

Figure B.6: Phase and phase-coupling of IPSCs is not different during slow and fast oscil-lations. (a) Phase of IPSCs during slow and fast oscillations. The trough of the oscillation cycle isdefined as π radians. (b) Phase-coupling to slow and fast oscillations.

Page 73: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

72 Appendix B

a b2π

Ph

as

e (

rad

)

FS non-FS

ns

0.4

0.2

0.0

Ph

as

e c

ou

plin

g

FS non-FS

ns

Figure B.7: Fast-spiking (FS) and non-FS interneurons do not differ in spike timing (phase)or amount of phase coupling. (a) Phase of interneuron firing (FS n = 4; non-FS cells n = 4; p =0.72). The trough of the oscillation cycle is defined as π radians. (b) Phase coupling of interneurons(FS n = 4; non-FS n = 4; p = 0.07).

a b0.6

0.4

0.2

0.0

Ph

as

e-c

ou

plin

g

slow fast

ns

Ph

as

e (

rad

)

slow fast

ns

Figure B.8: Interneurons fire at rising phase of slow and fast oscillations. (a) Phase ofinterneuron firing during slow and fast oscillation episodes (n = 5; p = 0.19). The trough of the oscil-lation cycle is defined as π radians. (b) Phase coupling of interneurons with slow and fast oscillations(n = 6; p = 0.10).

Page 74: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

References

Bacci A, Rudolph U, Huguenard JR, Prince DA (2003) Major differences in inhibitory synaptictransmission onto two neocortical interneuron subclasses. J Neurosci 23:9664-9674.

Bartos M, Vida I, Jonas P (2007) Synaptic mechanisms of synchronized gamma oscillations ininhibitory interneuron networks. Nat Rev Neurosci 8:45-56.

Bouyer JJ, Montaron MF, Vahnee JM, Albert MP, Rougeul A (1987) Anatomical localization ofcortical beta rhythms in cat. Neuroscience 22:863-869.

Bragin A, Jando G, Nadasdy Z, Hetke J, Wise K, Buzsaki G (1995) Gamma (40-100 Hz) oscillationin the hippocampus of the behaving rat. J Neurosci 15:47-60.

Buhl EH, Tamas G, Fisahn A (1998) Cholinergic activation and tonic excitation induce persistentgamma oscillations in mouse somatosensory cortex in vitro. JPhysiol 513:117-126.

Buzsaki G, Draguhn A (2004) Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science 304:1926-1929.Buzsaki G, Leung LW, Vanderwolf CH (1983) Cellular bases of hippocampal EEG in the behavingrat. Brain Res 287:139-171.

Castellanos FX, Tannock R (2002) Neuroscience of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: thesearch for endophenotypes. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:617-628.

Chrobak JJ, Buzsaki G (1998) Gamma oscillations in the entorhinal cortex of the freely behavingrat. J Neurosci 18:388-398.

Chudasama Y, Passetti F, Rhodes SE, Lopian D, Desai A, Robbins TW (2003) Dissociable aspectsof performance on the 5-choice serial reaction time task following lesions of the dorsal anteriorcingulate, infralimbic and orbitofrontal cortex in the rat: differential effects on selectivity, im-pulsivity and compulsivity. Behav Brain Res 146:105-119.

Cope DW, Halbsguth C, Karayannis T, Wulff P, Ferraguti F, Hoeger H, Leppa E, Linden AM,Oberto A, Ogris W, Korpi ER, Sieghart W, Somogyi P, Wisden W, Capogna M (2005) Loss ofzolpidem efficacy in the hippocampus of mice with the GABAA receptor gamma2 F77I pointmutation. Eur J Neurosci 21:3002-3016.

Couey JJ, Meredith RM, Spijker S, Poorthuis RB, Smit AB, Brussaard AB, Mansvelder HD (2007)Distributed network actions by nicotine increase the threshold for spike-timing-dependent plas-ticity in prefrontal cortex. Neuron 54:73-87.

Csicsvari J, Jamieson B, Wise KD, Buzsaki G (2003) Mechanisms of gamma oscillations in thehippocampus of the behaving rat. Neuron 37:311-322.

Cunningham MO, Davies CH, Buhl EH, Kopell N, Whittington MA (2003) Gamma oscillationsinduced by kainate receptor activation in the entorhinal cortex in vitro. J Neurosci 23:9761-9769.

Cunningham MO, Whittington MA, Bibbig A, Roopun A, LeBeau FE, Vogt A, Monyer H, BuhlEH, Traub RD (2004) A role for fast rhythmic bursting neurons in cortical gamma oscillationsin vitro. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101:7152-7157.

Dalley JW, Cardinal RN, Robbins TW (2004) Prefrontal executive and cognitive functions in ro-dents: neural and neurochemical substrates. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 28:771-784.

Deans JK, Powell AD, Jefferys JG (2007) Sensitivity of coherent oscillations in rat hippocampusto AC electric fields. J Physiol 583:555-565.

Dickinson R, Awaiz S, Whittington MA, Lieb WR, Franks NP (2003) The effects of general anaes-thetics on carbachol-evoked gamma oscillations in the rat hippocampus in vitro. Neuropharma-cology 44:864-872.

Dickson CT, Biella G, de Curtis M (2000) Evidence for spatial modules mediated by temporalsynchronization of carbachol-induced gamma rhythm in medial entorhinal cortex. J Neurosci20:7846-7854.

Douglas RJ, Martin KA (2004) Neuronal circuits of the neocortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 27:419-451.Fan J, Byrne J, Worden MS, Guise KG, McCandliss BD, Fossella J, Posner MI (2007) The relationof brain oscillations to attentional networks. J Neurosci 27:6197-6206.

73

Page 75: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

74 References

Fisahn A, Pike FG, Buhl EH, Paulsen O (1998) Cholinergic induction of network oscillations at40 Hz in the hippocampus in vitro. Nature 394:186-189.

Ford JM, Krystal JH, Mathalon DH (2007) Neural synchrony in schizophrenia: from networks tonew treatments. Schizophr Bull 33:848-852.

Freund TF (2003) Interneuron Diversity series: Rhythm and mood in perisomatic inhibition.Trends Neurosci 26:489-495.

Freund TF (2007) Perisomatic inhibition. Neuron.Fries P, Reynolds JH, Rorie AE, Desimone R (2001) Modulation of oscillatory neuronal synchro-nization by selective visual attention. Science 291:1560-1563.

Gabbott PL, Dickie BG, Vaid RR, Headlam AJ, Bacon SJ (1997) Local-circuit neurones in themedial prefrontal cortex (areas 25, 32 and 24b) in the rat: morphology and quantitative distri-bution. J Comp Neurol 377:465-499.

Glassman AH (1993) Cigarette smoking: implications for psychiatric illness. AmJPsychiatry150:546-553.

Goldman-Rakic PS (1995) Cellular basis of working memory. Neuron 14:477-485.Goldman-Rakic PS, Muly EC, III, Williams GV (2000) D(1) receptors in prefrontal cells and cir-cuits. Brain ResBrain ResRev 31:295-301.

Gray CM, Konig P, Engel AK, Singer W (1989) Oscillatory responses in cat visual cortex exhibitinter-columnar synchronization which reflects global stimulus properties. Nature 338:334-337.

Gray CM, McCormick DA (1996) Chattering cells: superficial pyramidal neurons contributing tothe generation of synchronous oscillations in the visual cortex. Science 274:109-113.

Grillner S, Markram H, De Schutter E, Silberberg G, LeBeau FE (2005) Microcircuits in action–from CPGs to neocortex. Trends Neurosci 28:525-533.

Gulledge AT, Jaffe DB (2001) Multiple effects of dopamine on layer v pyramidal cell excitabilityin rat prefrontal cortex. JNeurophysiol 86:586-595.

Hajos N, Katona I, Naiem SS, MacKie K, Ledent C, Mody I, Freund TF (2000) Cannabinoidsinhibit hippocampal GABAergic transmission and network oscillations. Eur J Neurosci 12:3239-3249.

Hajos N, Palhalmi J, Mann EO, Nemeth B, Paulsen O, Freund TF (2004) Spike timing of distincttypes of GABAergic interneuron during hippocampal gamma oscillations in vitro. J Neurosci24:9127-9137.

Hoover WB, Vertes RP (2007) Anatomical analysis of afferent projections to the medial prefrontalcortex in the rat. Brain Struct Funct 212:149-179.

Howard MW, Rizzuto DS, Caplan JB, Madsen JR, Lisman J, Aschenbrenner-Scheibe R, Schulze-Bonhage A, Kahana MJ (2003) Gamma oscillations correlate with working memory load inhumans. Cereb Cortex 13:1369-1374.

Huxter J, Burgess N, O’Keefe J (2003) Independent rate and temporal coding in hippocampalpyramidal cells. Nature 425:828-832.

Jacobs J, Kahana MJ, Ekstrom AD, Fried I (2007) Brain oscillations control timing of single-neuron activity in humans. J Neurosci 27:3839-3844.

Jones BF, Groenewegen HJ, Witter MP (2005) Intrinsic connections of the cingulate cortex in therat suggest the existence of multiple functionally segregated networks. Neuroscience 133:193-207.

Kahana MJ (2006) The cognitive correlates of human brain oscillations. J Neurosci 26:1669-1672.Kawaguchi Y, Kondo S (2002) Parvalbumin, somatostatin and cholecystokinin as chemical mark-ers for specific GABAergic interneuron types in the rat frontal cortex. J Neurocytol 31:277-287.

Killcross S, Coutureau E (2003) Coordination of actions and habits in the medial prefrontal cortexof rats. Cereb Cortex 13:400-408.

Klausberger T, Magill PJ, Marton LF, Roberts JD, Cobden PM, Buzsaki G, Somogyi P (2003)Brain-state- and cell-type-specific firing of hippocampal interneurons in vivo. Nature 421:844-848.

Klausberger T, Marton LF, O’Neill J, Huck JH, Dalezios Y, Fuentealba P, Suen WY, Papp E,Kaneko T, Watanabe M, Csicsvari J, Somogyi P (2005) Complementary roles of cholecystokinin-and parvalbumin-expressing GABAergic neurons in hippocampal network oscillations. J Neu-

Page 76: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

References 75

rosci 25:9782-9793.Kozak R, Bruno JP, Sarter M (2006) Augmented prefrontal acetylcholine release during challengedattentional performance. Cereb Cortex 16:9-17.

Levin ED, Simon BB (1998) Nicotinic acetylcholine involvement in cognitive function in animals.Psychopharmacology (Berl) 138:217-230.

Lohr JB, Flynn K (1992) Smoking and schizophrenia. SchizophrRes 8:93-102.Lopes da Silva F (1991) Neural mechanisms underlying brain waves: from neural membranes tonetworks. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 79:81-93.

Mann EO, Paulsen O (2005) Mechanisms underlying gamma (’40 Hz’) network oscillations in thehippocampus–a mini-review. Prog Biophys Mol Biol 87:67-76.

Mann EO, Suckling JM, Hajos N, Greenfield SA, Paulsen O (2005) Perisomatic feedback inhibi-tion underlies cholinergically induced fast network oscillations in the rat hippocampus in vitro.Neuron 45:105-117.

Mansvelder HD, van Aerde KI, Couey JJ, Brussaard AB (2005) Nicotinic modulation of neuronalnetworks: from receptors to cognition. Psychopharmacology (Berl):1-14.

Marquis JP, Killcross S, Haddon JE (2007) Inactivation of the prelimbic, but not infralimbic,prefrontal cortex impairs the contextual control of response conflict in rats. Eur J Neurosci25:559-566.

Marshall L, Helgadottir H, Molle M, Born J (2006) Boosting slow oscillations during sleep poten-tiates memory. Nature 444:610-613.

McGaughy J, Dalley JW, Morrison CH, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW (2002) Selective behavioral andneurochemical effects of cholinergic lesions produced by intrabasalis infusions of 192 IgG-saporinon attentional performance in a five-choice serial reaction time task. J Neurosci 22:1905-1913.

Miller EK (2000) The prefrontal cortex and cognitive control. Nat Rev Neurosci 1:59-65.Murphy ER, Dalley JW, Robbins TW (2005) Local glutamate receptor antagonism in the ratprefrontal cortex disrupts response inhibition in a visuospatial attentional task. Psychopharma-cology (Berl) 179:99-107.

Olejniczak P (2006) Neurophysiologic basis of EEG. J Clin Neurophysiol 23:186-189.Oren I, Mann EO, Paulsen O, Hajos N (2006) Synaptic currents in anatomically identified CA3neurons during hippocampal gamma oscillations in vitro. J Neurosci 26:9923-9934.

Palhalmi J, Paulsen O, Freund TF, Hajos N (2004) Distinct properties of carbachol- and DHPG-induced network oscillations in hippocampal slices. Neuropharmacology 47:381-389.

Palhalmi J, Paulsen O, Freund TF, Hajos N (2004) Distinct properties of carbachol- and DHPG-induced network oscillations in hippocampal slices. Neuropharmacology 47:381-389.

Palva JM, Palva S, Kaila K (2005) Phase synchrony among neuronal oscillations in the humancortex. J Neurosci 25:3962-3972.

Parikh V, Kozak R, Martinez V, Sarter M (2007) Prefrontal acetylcholine release controls cuedetection on multiple timescales. Neuron 56:141-154.

Passetti F, Dalley JW, O’Connell MT, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW (2000) Increased acetylcholinerelease in the rat medial prefrontal cortex during performance of a visual attentional task. Eur-JNeurosci 12:3051-3058.

Pattij T, Janssen MC, Loos M, Smit AB, Schoffelmeer AN, van Gaalen MM (2006) Strain speci-ficity and cholinergic modulation of visuospatial attention in three inbred mouse strains. GenesBrain Behav.

Paulsen O, Sejnowski TJ (2006) From invertebrate olfaction to human cognition: emerging com-putational functions of synchronized oscillatory activity. J Neurosci 26:1661-1662.

Pesaran B, Pezaris JS, Sahani M, Mitra PP, Andersen RA (2002) Temporal structure in neuronalactivity during working memory in macaque parietal cortex. Nat Neurosci 5:805-811.

Pike FG, Goddard RS, Suckling JM, Ganter P, Kasthuri N, Paulsen O (2000) Distinct frequencypreferences of different types of rat hippocampal neurones in response to oscillatory input cur-rents. JPhysiol 529 Pt 1:205-213.

Purves D, Augustine GJ, Fitzpatrick D, Katz LC, LaMantia A, McNamara JO, Williams SM(2001) Neuroscience, 2nd Edition. Sunderland, Massachusetts, USA: Sinauer Associates.

Page 77: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

76 References

Radman T, Datta A, Peterchev AV (2007) In vitro modulation of endogenous rhythms by ACelectric fields: Syncing with clinical brain stimulation. J Physiol 584:369-370.

Ragozzino M (2007) The Contribution of the Medial Prefrontal Cortex, Orbitofrontal Cortex andDorsomedial Striatum to Behavioral Flexibility. Ann N Y Acad Sci.

Roopun AK, Middleton SJ, Cunningham MO, LeBeau FE, Bibbig A, Whittington MA, TraubRD (2006) A beta2-frequency (20-30 Hz) oscillation in nonsynaptic networks of somatosensorycortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103:15646-15650.

Russell VA (2007) Reprint of ”Neurobiology of animal models of attention-deficit hyperactivitydisorder”. J Neurosci Methods 166:I-XIV.

Sarter M, Bruno JP, Turchi J (1999) Basal forebrain afferent projections modulating cortical acetyl-choline, attention, and implications for neuropsychiatric disorders. AnnNYAcadSci 877:368-382.

Shimono K, Brucher F, Granger R, Lynch G, Taketani M (2000) Origins and distribution of cholin-ergically induced beta rhythms in hippocampal slices. JNeurosci 20:8462-8473.

Shimono K, Taketani M, Brucher F, Kubota D, Colgin L, Robertson S, Granger R, Lynch G(2001) Continuous two-dimensional current source density analyses of electrophysiological activ-ity in hippocampal slices. Neurocomputing 38-40:899-905.

Somogyi P, Klausberger T (2005) Defined types of cortical interneurone structure space and spiketiming in the hippocampus. J Physiol 562:9-26.

Steriade M, McCormick DA, Sejnowski TJ (1993) Thalamocortical oscillations in the sleeping andaroused brain. Science 262:679-685.

Steriade M, Pare D, Datta S, Oakson G, Curro Dossi R (1990) Different cellular types in meso-pontine cholinergic nuclei related to ponto-geniculo-occipital waves. J Neurosci 10:2560-2579.

Tallon-Baudry C, Bertrand O, Peronnet F, Pernier J (1998) Induced gamma-band activity duringthe delay of a visual short-term memory task in humans. J Neurosci 18:4244-4254.

Torrence C, Compo GP (1998) A Practical Guide to Wavelet Analysis. Bull Amer Meteor Soc79:61-78.

Traub RD, Spruston N, Soltesz I, Konnerth A, Whittington MA, Jefferys GR (1998) Gamma-frequency oscillations: a neuronal population phenomenon, regulated by synaptic and intrinsiccellular processes, and inducing synaptic plasticity. ProgNeurobiol 55:563-575.

Tukker JJ, Fuentealba P, Hartwich K, Somogyi P, Klausberger T (2007) Cell type-specific tuningof hippocampal interneuron firing during gamma oscillations in vivo. J Neurosci 27:8184-8189.

Uylings HB, Groenewegen HJ, Kolb B (2003) Do rats have a prefrontal cortex? Behav Brain Res146:3-17.

Uylings HB, van Eden CG (1990) Qualitative and quantitative comparison of the prefrontal cortexin rat and in primates, including humans. Prog Brain Res 85:31-62.

van der Kooij MA, Glennon JC (2007) Animal models concerning the role of dopamine in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 31:597-618.

van Der Linden S, Panzica F, de Curtis M (1999) Carbachol induces fast oscillations in the medialbut not in the lateral entorhinal cortex of the isolated guinea pig brain. J Neurophysiol 82:2441-2450.

Van Eden CG, Uylings HB (1985) Cytoarchitectonic development of the prefrontal cortex in therat. J Comp Neurol 241:253-267.

Viswanathan A, Freeman RD (2007) Neurometabolic coupling in cerebral cortex reflects synapticmore than spiking activity. Nat Neurosci 10:1308-1312.

Weiss T, Veh RW, Heinemann U (2003) Dopamine depresses cholinergic oscillatory network ac-tivity in rat hippocampus. Eur J Neurosci 18:2573-2580.

Whittington MA, Traub RD, Jefferys JG (1995) Synchronized oscillations in interneuron networksdriven by metabotropic glutamate receptor activation. Nature 373:612-615.

Yordanova J, Banaschewski T, Kolev V, Woerner W, Rothenberger A (2001) Abnormal early stagesof task stimulus processing in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder–evidence fromevent-related gamma oscillations. Clin Neurophysiol 112:1096-1108.

Page 78: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Summary

This thesis describes the research that was performed in the past five years concerning thegeneration of fast brain waves, and the cellular mechanisms that are involved. These fast brainwaves, in the so called beta (15-30 Hz) and gamma (30-100 Hz) frequencies are mostly prominentwhile people are engaged in a task that requires working memory and attention. Therefore ourrationale has been that the better understanding of the generation of these fast brain oscillations(which cortical area, which cortical layer, which cell types are involved, etc), will eventually leadto improved treatment of patients suffering from abnormal “attention”, such as children andadults with AD(H)D and patients suffering from schizophrenia.

We have approached this by inducing fast brain waves in brain slices from rats. This can donein very simple and direct way, namely via the application of the neurotransmitter acetylcholine,or a substitute such as carbachol. This same neurotransmitter is also released in the brain duringtasks that require attention. Nicotine acts partly in a similar way. It is probably not by chancethat ADHD and schizophrenic patients are severe smokers.

In chapter 2 and 3 we have investigated whether the small amplitude and frequency variationsthat can be seen in human EEG data (Figure 2.1b) are the result of the “normal” variation inthe underlying system or network, or that these variations are in fact the result from multipleactive networks. We investigated this by measuring the brain activity using multi-electrode gridsof 8 × 8 electrodes with a high spatial resolution: the distance between electrodes was only 150µm, and the total area 1.1 mm2. Thus we could measure the activity in all layers of the cortex(Figure 2.3a,b). From these experiments it became clear that there are in fact two subnetworksthat are active at the same time. One subnetwork gives rise to activity seen in layer 2 andlayer 3/5, and the other to activity seen in layer 5 and 6 (Figure 2.3e). These two subnetworksgive rise to oscillations that differ in frequency, layer 3/5 always being a bit faster than layer 6.Interestingly, both oscillations were present in layer 5. We wondered therefore if the nerve cellsin layer 5 were divided in two subpopulations, one population involved in the slower and theother population involved in the faster oscillations, or that individual cells were contributing toboth oscillations. To answer this question we recorded the activity from individual cells (Figure2.4a,b). These measurements showed that the same cell was active and firing action potentialsduring the slow and fast oscillations (Figure 2.4d, 3.4, 3.5), due to the fact that these cells receivedtimed synaptic input, both excitatory and inhibitory, during both oscillations (Figure 2.6, 2.7).This was in contrast to cells in layer 6, which only fired action potentials related to the slowoscillations (Figure 2.5). These results implicate that cells from layer 5 and 6 fire synchronouslyduring the slow oscillation episodes, but not during the fast episodes. Layer 5 and layer 6 projectto different brain areas: layer 5 primarily to a brain region called the hypothalamus that regulatesthe level of alertness. Layer 6 contacts the mediodorsal thalamus, from which the sensory inputis send to the prefrontal cortex. Therefore, activation of the subnetwork that generates the fastoscillations could change alertness, while activation of the subnetwork that generated the slowoscillations could influence the input to the prefrontal cortex. The simultaneous activation ofboth subnetworks (Figure 2.3f,h) may reflect parallel information processing.

In chapter 4 we studies the interaction between two brain areas, namely between the prelimbicand the infralimbic cortex. Both brain areas are part of the prefrontal cortex and are importantduring tasks that require attention and/or working memory, but each in a different way. Whenprelimbic cortex functioning is disturbed, rats will make more mistakes in a test where a lightindicates which of five holes contains a reward. The difficulty of the task being that there is adelay in time between when the light is shown and when the reward can be obtained. However,the disturbed function of the prelimbic cortex does not affect the number of premature responses:when the rat tries out a hole too soon after the presentation of the light, or before a light is evenshown. This is reversed when the infralimbic cortex is affected: now the rats also perform worse

77

Page 79: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

78 Summary

than normal but due to an increase in impulsive responses. Given the fact that these brainregions are important in different aspects of attention behavior, we wondered whether this wouldresult in different characteristics of fast brain oscillations, and whether a possible interactionbetween these connected areas could be revealed during these oscillations. We therefore used amulti-electrode grid of 8 × 8 electrodes covering an area of 4.4 mm2, so that we could measure theelectrical activity in both areas simultaneously. These experiments showed that the prelimbicand the infralimbic cortex generate their own specific oscillation in about 50% of the brainslices. The oscillations in the infralimbic cortex are faster (14.7 ± 0.9 Hz) than those in theprelimbic cortex (12.7 ± 0.7 Hz). The amplitude of the oscillations was also different betweenthe two areas (Figure 4.1). To investigate whether these areas can really generate oscillation bythemselves, we made so called mini slices: brain slices that contained only the prelimbic or onlythe infralimbic area. The measurements from these mini slices are surprising: they confirmedthat both areas can generate oscillations independent from each other, but interestingly thereis no longer a difference in oscillation frequency (in contrast: the difference in amplitude is stillexisting, Figure 4.4). Apparently it is the interaction between these areas that is causing thesubtle difference in oscillation frequency. Interestingly, it is these kinds of interactions betweenbrain areas that are abnormal in schizophrenic patients.

Page 80: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Samenvatting

Dit proefschrift beschrijft het onderzoek van de afgelopen vijf jaar naar de cellulaire processenen netwerkmechanismen die betrokken zijn bij het doen ontstaan van snelle hersengolven. Dezesnelle hersengolven, in de zogenoemde beta- of gammafrequentie (15–30 respectievelijk 30–100 Hz), zijn vooral prominent wanneer mensen een taak moeten uitvoeren waarbij werkge-heugen en attentie onontbeerlijk zijn. Vandaar dat onze gedachte is dat als we beter kunnenbegrijpen hoe deze hersenoscillaties worden opgewekt (in welk hersengebied, in welke laag vande hersenschors, welke betrokkken celtypen, etc), dit uiteindelijk zal leiden tot betere behandel-ingsmogelijkheden bij patienten die attentiestoornissen hebben, zoals kinderen of volwassenenmet AD(H)D en patienten die lijden aan schizofrenie.

We hebben dit gedaan door deze snelle hersengolven te induceren in hersenplakjes van ratten.Dit kan op een heel directe en simpele manier, namelijk door de neurotransmitter acetylcholine,of een stof die hier op lijkt zoals carbachol, toe te voegen. Deze neurotransmitter komt ook vrijtijdens attentietaken. Nicotine werkt deels op dezelfde manier, en het is waarschijnlijk niet voorniets dat patienten met ADHD of schizofrenie vaak roken.

In hoofdstuk 2 en 3 hebben we onderzocht, of de kleine variaties in amplitude en frequentievan netwerkoscillaties, zoals je die bijvoorbeeld ziet in EEG-studies bij mensen (Figuur 2.1b),het resultaat zijn van een “normale”variatie van het onderliggende systeem c.q. netwerk, ofdat deze in feite de activiteit laten zien van twee verschillende netwerken. We onderzochten ditdoor de hersenactiviteit te meten met hoge resolutie multi-elektrode grids van 8 × 8 elektroden,met slechts een afstand van 150 µm tussen de elektroden en een totaal oppervlakte van 1,1mm2. Door deze hoge resolutie waren wij in staat om de activiteit te meten in alle lagen vande hersenschors (Figuur 2.3a,b). Hieruit kwam naar voren dat er twee subnetwerken actief zijn:het ene subnetwerk leidt tot activiteit in laag 2 en laag 3/5, en het andere subnetwerk genereertoscillaties in laag 5 en laag 6 (Figuur 2.3e). Deze twee netwerkoscillaties verschilden van elkaarin oscillatiefrequentie, waarbij laag 3/5 een hogere frequentie laat zien dan laag 6. Interessantwas dat beide oscillaties zichtbaar waren in laag 5. We vroegen ons daarom af, of de cellen inlaag 5 verdeeld waren in twee subpopulaties, waarbij de ene subpopulatie betrokken was bij delangzame, en de andere bij de snelle oscillaties, of dat dezelfde cellen bij beide oscillaties warenbetrokken. Om deze vraag te beantwoorden, maakten we afleidingen van individuele zenuwcellen(Figuur 2.4a,b). Het bleek dat cellen uit laag 5 zowel actiepotentialen vuurden, of te wel actiefwasren tijdens de langzame als tijdens de snelle oscillaties (Figuur 2.4d, 3.4, 3.5), mede doordatdeze cellen tijdens beide oscillaties exciterende en remmende synaptische input kregen (Figuur2.6, 2.7). Dit was in tegenstelling tot cellen uit laag 6, die alleen gerelateerd aan de langzameoscillaties vuurden (Figuur 2.5). De implicaties hiervan zijn dat tijdens de langzame oscillatieepisoden de cellen in laag 5 en laag 6 simultaan vuren, maar niet tijdens de snelle oscillatieepisoden. Beide hersenlagen projecteren naar verschillende hersengebieden: laag 5 primair naareen hersengebied, de hypothalamus, waar alertheid wordt gereguleerd. Laag 6 heeft vooralprojecties naar de mediodorsale thalamus, van waaruit de sensorische input naar de prefrontalehersenschors komt. Activering van het subnetwerk dat snelle oscillaties genereert zou dus totverandering kunnen leiden in de mate van alertheid, terwijl activering van het subnetwerk datde langzamere oscillatie episoden genereert, de input naar de prefrontale hersenschors benvloedt.Beide subnetwerken kunnen ook tegelijk actief zijn (Figuur 2.3f,h), dit zou impliceren dat erparallelle informatieverwerking plaatsvindt.

In hoofdstuk 4 wordt de interactie tussen twee hersengebieden bestudeerd, namelijk tussen deprelimbische en de infralimbische hersenschors. Deze hersengebieden, die beide tot de prefrontalehersenschors behoren, zijn belangrijk in attentietaken, maar op een andere manier. Wanneer defunctie van de prelimbische hersenschors verstoord is, maken ratten meer fouten in een test waarineen lichtje aangeeft welke van vijf gaten een beloning bevat. De moeilijkheid van deze test schuilt

79

Page 81: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

80 Samenvatting

in de tijd die tussen de presentatie van het lichtje en de beloning zit. De dysfunctie van de pre-limbische hersenschors heeft echter geen effect op het aantal premature responsen: wanneer derat te snel na de presentatie van het lichtje al naar het gat gaat, of zelfs al wanneer er nog nieteens een lichtje is geweest. Bij een verstoring van de infralimbische hersenschors is dit preciesomgekeerd: de ratten presteren ook slechter dan normaal, maar nu omdat ze meer impulsieveacties verrichten. Deze hersengebieden hebben dus andere functies, en wij vroegen ons af, of dittot verschillen zou leiden in de eigenschappen van netwerk oscillaties, en of potentiele interactiestussen deze twee gebieden te meten zouden zijn tijdens de snelle netwerk oscillaties. Met be-hulp van een multi-elektrode grid van 8 × 8 elektrodes met een totale oppervlakte van 4,4 mm2

hebben we tegelijkertijd de hersenactiviteit gemeten in beide gebieden. Uit deze experimentenblijkt dat de prelimbische hersenschors en de infralimbische hersenschors in ongeveer de helft vande gevallen allebei een eigen oscillatie vertonen. De oscillaties in de infralimbische hersenschorszijn iets sneller (gemiddeld 14,7 ± 0,9 Hz) dan die in de prelimbische hersenschors (gemiddeld12,7 ± 0,7 Hz), en ook was de amplitude van de oscillaties in de infralimbische hersenschors groter(Figuur 4.1). Om te onderzoeken of deze twee gebieden dus echt zelfstandig netwerk oscillatieskunnen genereren, maakten we zogenoemde mini-plakjes: hersenplakjes van alleen het prelim-bische of alleen het infralimbische gebied. De metingen van deze mini-plakjes gaven verrassenderesultaten. Ze bevestigen dat deze gebieden inderdaad zelfstandig oscillaties kunnen genereren.Maar het interessantste gegeven is dat er nu niet langer een verschil in oscillatie frequentie was(ter vergelijking: het verschil in oscillatie amplitude bleef wel bestaan; zie Figuur 4.4). Blijkbaarontstaat dat subtiele verschil in oscillatie frequentie als gevolg van de interactie tussen deze tweegebieden. En het is juist dit soort interacties tussen hersengebieden dat verstoord lijkt te zijn inschizofrenie patienten.

Page 82: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Acknowledgements

The author of this thesis would like to thank the following persons for their contributions to thework.

All the colleagues of the department Integrative Neurophysiology (formally Experimental Neu-rophysiology) and the rest of the CNCR.

The co-authors of the papers. Especially Ed Mann and Ole Paulsen for teaching me the techniqueof multi-electode recordings and the analysis of the data.

Matthijs Verhage for giving the right directions.

Cyriel Pennartz, Pieter Roelfsema, and Kees Stam for evaluating this thesis.

Tonny Mulder, Raymond van Ee, and Pascal Fries for discussing earlier drafts of the manuscripts.

Arjen Brussaard and Huib Mansvelder, my promotors, for the supervision and the opportunities.

81

Page 83: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

List of Publications

Publications related to this Thesis

Karlijn I. van Aerde, Tim S. Heistek, and Huibert D. Mansvelder, Prelimbic and InfralimbicPrefrontal Cortex Interact During Fast Network Oscillations, PLoS ONE. 2008; 3(7): e2725.

Karlijn I. van Aerde, Edward O. Mann, Cathrin B. Canto, Klaus Linkenkaer-Hansen, Marcelvan der Roest, Antonius B. Mulder, Ole Paulsen, Arjen B. Brussaard, Huibert D. Mansvelder,Two independent cortical subnetworks control spike timing in layer 5 neurons during dynamicoscillation shifts, submitted

Other Publications

Joost H. Heeroma, Martijn Roelandse, Keimpe Wierda, Karlijn I. van Aerde, Ruud F. G. Toonen,Robert A. Hensbroek, Arjen Brussaard, Andrew Matus and Matthijs Verhage, Trophic supportdelays but does not prevent cell-intrinsic degeneration of neurons deficient for munc18-1, Eur JNeurosci. 2004 Aug;20(3):623-34.

Huibert D. Mansvelder, Karlijn I. van Aerde, Jonathan J. Couey and Arjen B. Brussaard, Nico-tinic modulation of neuronal networks: from receptors to cognition, Psychopharmacology (Berl).2006 Mar;184(3-4):292-305. Epub 2005 Jul 2. Review.

Gonul Dilaver, Rinske van de Vorstenbosch, Celine Tarrega, Pablo Ros, Rafael Pulido, Karlijnvan Aerde, Jack Fransen and Wiljan Hendriks, Proteolytic processing of the receptor-type proteintyrosine phosphatase PTPBR7, FEBS J. 2007 Jan;274(1):96-108. Epub 2006 Nov 27.

Page 84: Network Oscillations in the Prefrontal Cortex...re ect reduced functioning of the prefrontal cortex neuronal networks that are involved in processing information necessary for attention

Curriculum Vitae

Karlijn van Aerde was born on June 9th, 1978 in Tilburg, the Netherlands. She attended thePauluslyceum in Tilburg from 1990 and obtained her gymnasium β certificate in 1996. Laterthat year she started the study of Medical Biology at Utrecht University that was completedwith a Master of Science title five years later in 2001.

In the period May 1999 – February 2000 she stayed in Manchester (UK) for an internship atthe Dept. Clinical Genetics at St. Mary’s Hospital of the University of Manchester. This lead tothe thesis “Genetic and Physical Mapping of The Wagner Disease Locus (WGN-1)” with whichshe won the Dr F.P. Fischer-prize Ophthalmology on 14 December 2000.

From May 2000 until December 2001 she did an internship in the field of Medical Pharma-cology at the Rudolf Magnus Institute of Neuroscience, Utrecht University (the Netherlands).The work was supervised by prof.dr. Matthijs Verhage and lead to the thesis “The Role of Reg-ulated Secretion of Synaptic Vesicles in Synapse Formation and Stabilisation in the Absence ofMunc18”. She won the Best Student Presentation Prize in March 2001.

During her studies she had various teaching jobs as student assistant. She wrote for theweekly issued Utrecht University magazine U-Blad and the Dutch national newspaper de Volk-skrant as a columnist and a free-lance writer. In 2007 she contributed to the “de VolkskrantBetacanon”.

From March 2002 until July 2003 she worked as a junior researcher at the departmentFunctional Genomics of the Center for Neurogenomics and Cognitive Research (CNCR), VUUniversity Amsterdam (the Netherlands) under the supervision of prof.dr. Matthijs Verhage.She performed electrophysiological and behavioral experiments on a mouse model of ADHD.

She started her PhD-research project at the Dept. of Experimental Neurophysiology of theCNCR in August 2003. The work was finished in 2008. The results obtained in this period aredescribed in this thesis.

83