mahwah, american longitudinal studies of developmentpr2... · 366 p. rogers for a variety of...

13
27 The contributions of North American longitudinal studies of writing in higher education to our understanding of writing development Paul Rogers George Mason University For many decades researchers have investigated the development of writing abilities (Britton, 1975; Applebee, 2000). While this research has included attention to children's acquisition of print literacy prior to schooling as well as writing in the workplace and the professions, a good deal of this research interest has been sustained by the need for an evidence-based foundation for the teaching and learning of writing in schools. Longitudi- nal studies in particular with their emphasis on change over time and across contexts have proven a particularly appropriate method in under- standing writing development. As Emig (1971, p. 95) noted in her pioneer- ing work Longitudinal case studies of a given sample of students, following them from the time they begin to write in the earliest elementary grades throughout their school careers, up to and including graduate school . . . would make better known the developmental dimensions of the writing process, both for the individual and for members of various chronological and ability age groups. To date, no studies of the scope for which Emig called have been con- ducted; yet, in North America a number of longitudinal studies of writing have been conducted, particularly in higher education settings. Methodologically longitudinal approaches have proven to have incorpo- rated four out of the six levels of inquiry in research on the composing process proposed by Bereiter and Scardamalia (1987, p. 34): reflective inquiry, empirical variable testing, text analysis, and process description. Rather than comparing cohorts to control groups, as in theory-embedded experimentation, findings for these studies were derived by examining changes in the performances, attitudes, beliefs, and values of the particip- ants. Moreover, they were based on viewing events and their impact on the cohorts or participants as they occurred over time, and did not include experimental interventions.

Upload: doanbao

Post on 24-Nov-2018

215 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Jes. Mahwah,

istics. Annual

• 70, 151-167. : genre. In A.

(pp. 67-78).

24). Retrieved

!terate activity

'!!ms. Tubarao, Jlinguagem/cd!

From reported 766. Retrieved • ?cookieSet= 1. activity theory

workplaces: A i, Culture, and

ricular history.

:;riting of activ-

me historians": 3azerman & D. ~ctivity perspec­use and Mind,

J(2), 200-234. ial reality. The

C. Walsh and

d. Evanston, IL:

rsity Press. A sociocultural .. ~. W. Rieber & ~/ems of general

ozch to mediated

[he challenge of (Ed.), Teaching

27 The contributions of North American longitudinal studies of writing in higher education to our understanding of writing development

Paul Rogers George Mason University

For many decades researchers have investigated the development of writing abilities (Britton, 1975; Applebee, 2000). While this research has included attention to children's acquisition of print literacy prior to schooling as well as writing in the workplace and the professions, a good deal of this research interest has been sustained by the need for an evidence-based foundation for the teaching and learning of writing in schools. Longitudi­nal studies in particular with their emphasis on change over time and across contexts have proven a particularly appropriate method in under­standing writing development. As Emig (1971, p. 95) noted in her pioneer­ing work

Longitudinal case studies of a given sample of students, following them from the time they begin to write in the earliest elementary grades throughout their school careers, up to and including graduate school . . . would make better known the developmental dimensions of the writing process, both for the individual and for members of various chronological and ability age groups.

To date, no studies of the scope for which Emig called have been con­ducted; yet, in North America a number of longitudinal studies of writing have been conducted, particularly in higher education settings.

Methodologically longitudinal approaches have proven to have incorpo­rated four out of the six levels of inquiry in research on the composing process proposed by Bereiter and Scardamalia (1987, p. 34): reflective inquiry, empirical variable testing, text analysis, and process description. Rather than comparing cohorts to control groups, as in theory-embedded experimentation, findings for these studies were derived by examining changes in the performances, attitudes, beliefs, and values of the particip­ants. Moreover, they were based on viewing events and their impact on the cohorts or participants as they occurred over time, and did not include experimental interventions.

366 P. Rogers

For a variety of reasons these studies do not, however, allow for direct causal arguments: the high number of variables bearing upon different units of analysis; the small incremental nature of change; the complex interrelationships between observed phenomena; and the near impossibility of determining the extent to which an individual variable contributed to an overall perceived change. However, the studies reviewed here have added much to our understanding of writing development; in particular, they have contributed to the generation of valuable hypotheses concerning how people learn to write and the impact of writing on learning, and they have provided a rich description of the multiple variables that contribute to the development of writing abilities.

The earliest longitudinal studies of writing focused on children in their early years of schooling and the development of print literacy (Tierney & Sheehy, 2005). Loban (1963), for example, undertook a longitudinal study of 338 urban students during the entire course of their K-12 schooling showing that socioeconomic factors played a significant role in the devel­opment of their reading, speech, and writing. Rental and King (1981, 1983) followed 36 children stratified by sex, socioeconomic status, dialect, and school at intervals of four months over the children's first four years of schooling. Primarily using an analysis of student narratives, this study looked at the transition to writing from earlier forms of literacy. The scar­city of such studies attests to the many challenges of longitudinal research, including high attrition rates, the need for significant human and financial resources to cope with logistics and large quantities of data, and the desire for quick results by funding sources. In spite of these difficulties and because of the urgent need for evidence regarding the need for and efficacy of writing instruction in higher education, a number of researchers have conducted longitudinal studies of writing in North American colleges and universities. In the rest of this chapter I summarize the developmental find­ings of these studies, looking first at single-subject longitudinal case studies, and then the multi-subject longitudinal studies.

The earliest longitudinal case study of a college writer was Lucille McCarthy's (1987) dissertation study. Conducted at a private liberal arts university between 1983 and 1985, McCarthy used classroom observa­tions, informal discussion, textual analysis, think-aloud protocols, as well as retrospective and semi-structured interviews, with teachers and her subject to investigate how the student came to understand the requirements of different disciplines and teachers, and how the student produced the writing to meet those demands. McCarthy found a great similarity among assigned writing tasks; yet, the students attentional focus was different on each task, and he was unable to identify the connections among the tasks, i.e., the student interpreted each writing situation as being totally different from other writing tasks. McCarthy's research highlighted the dominant role the immediate environment of the classroom played in the student's learning, and described other factors which influenced the student's writing success: the role the writing served for the student personally, the impor-

tance of ethnic at dent's in

Haas whom s: sity. Th: views, c underst<: course c nized "t Haas fc support texts ar the exp ment of ific wri1 of disci work vo agency operate way sh what h. versa tic opmen­pre-exl observ• factorSi social• of prac

Unc 1993, deveb opmer furthe: analySi Beaufc charac writin histor­compl ment · in on~ comn: priate writir: ment~

~:lirect ferent cnplex

1 'b'l' ;} 1 tty I to an I added r, they rg how ~y have ~to the I

in their ~rney & al study :hooling ~e devel­~ (1981, ~. dialect, )Ur years )lis study The scar­research,

l financial the desire

~lties and 1d efficacy :hers have ~lieges and 1ental find­lase studies,

rras Lucille liberal arts

m observa­tols, as well ;:rs and her requirements •roduced the larity among ~ different on mg the tasks, tally different rhe dominant

1

the student's dent's writing ly, the impar­l

Longitudinal studies and writing development 367

tance of teacher-student relations including differences and similarities in ethnic and class backgrounds, and the learning that resulted from the stu­dent's interactions with peers and their writing.

Haas (1994) described the rhetorical development of one biology major whom she followed between 1986 and 1990 at a private research univer­sity. Through extended, minimally structured, and discourse-based inter­views, classroom observations, and think-aloud protocols, Haas sought to understand "if students' views of academic discourse change over the course of their college careers" (p. 46), and more specifically if they recog­nized "the rhetorical nature of scientific action and scientific texts" (p. 50). Haas found in her subject a growing awareness of the rhetorical frame supporting written discourse, as the student began to see "behind scientific texts are human authors with motives" (p. 32). Haas claimed it was not the explicit introduction of theories of discourse which led to the develop­ment of her subject's growing awareness of the social dimensions of scient­ific writing; rather, the student developed new ideas concerning the nature of disciplinary discourse through interaction and activity. In particular, work with scientists in the lab helped her to see more clearly the human agency involved in text production and the social context in which texts operated. Changes to her perception of academic discourse influenced the way she approached her writing, i.e., she moved from merely reporting what her textbooks said to understanding that authors are having a con­versation. Haas's research suggests that in order to understand the devel­opment of student writing abilities researchers must investigate student's pre-existing conceptions of texts at the beginning of their college career, observe ways in which these conceptions change, and note the variety of factors influencing their change, especially in relationship to a rhetorical or social understanding of the ways texts operate within specific communities of practice.

Undertaken at a major private research university between 1990 and 1993, Beaufort's (2004, 2007) study was not concerned with the general development of writing abilities; instead, it attempted to describe the devel­opment of one college student in learning to write history in order to further a model of disciplinary writing expertise. Using interviews, textual analysis, and consultation with disciplinary experts on history writing, Beaufort aimed to understand "how expertise in history writing might be characterized; what developmental changes occurred that were related to writing; and what factors contributed to the development of the subject's history writing skills" (2004, p. 137). The results of her study show the complex, simultaneous, interweaving of multiple factors in the develop­ment of discipline-specific writing expertise, and how a writer may develop in one area but not another. Beaufort argues that mature members of the community of professional historians choose appropriate genres for appro­priate purposes, but in the student's case the genres and purposes for the writing in his history classes were unclear: the subject described assign­ments as either the regurgitation of content or general explorations of

368 P. Rogers

broad subject matter. Hence, his texts approximated features of several genres, and often exhibited different purposes within the same document (2004, p. 162). However, genre awareness gains did include a movement toward the formal register of history writing, and some of his texts approx­imated aspects of authentic history writing.

The student's history writing did improve across his freshman, sopho­more, and junior years: the student made gains in subject-matter knowledge including "some understanding of historical themes, but his key interpretive skills were still weak" (2004, p. 154). The subject's critical-thinking skills (what Beaufort refers to as the procedural aspect of subject-matter know­ledge) were not consistently applied. While the student's purpose for writing changed slightly from writing for self and grades toward writing for the dis­cipline of history, "The classroom discourse community took precedence and was, in fact, the only social context the student was aware of, rather than any sense of the larger disciplinary discourse community" (2004, p. 150). Beaufort reports that the subject was unable to make the kinds of writing-related rhetorical choices that would be considered an authentic part of the discourse of historians. There were, however, signs that the student was beginning to understand what historians do when they write, and that he was to some degree attempting to take on features of that iden­tity: he was beginning to understand how historians analyze historical texts and through these texts take on the analysis of broader social events. However, what the student learned about history writing seemed to happen inferentially rather than through explicit instruction.

The only longitudinal study dealing explicitly with second-language acquisition, Spack (1997) reported on a student she followed for two years using classroom observations, textual analysis, interviews, and informal conversations. She wanted to understand how her subject(s) drew on mul­tiple resources to succeed as readers and writers in a university setting, and what features of the subject's linguistic, educational, and cultural back­ground constrained and enabled her to manage coursework. Spack found ~hat the student matured as a reader and writer as she received meaningful mput from numerous classroom experiences; she linked the student's writing development to constant practice, subject-matter immersion, and "talking about her projects with those who could share their expertise" (p. 47). In the beginning of the student's experience, linguistic and cultural factors impacted the student's writing performance; however, in Spack's view the student's lack of background knowledge and difficulty with aca­demic argument in English posed the greatest challenges later on. As the student progressed through her course of study her literacy skills developed through active engagement in course activities, especially reading. While the student's sense of self-efficacy as a writer grew over time, she continued to perceive her most difficult writing problem as overcoming a "Japanese style of writing" (p. 46). Although the student benefited significantly from the ESL courses she took in her first year the learning did not always trans­fer to future courses.

Lon gil

Chiseri-Strater (1991) repc versity of New Hampshire _b following six. Her research u: interpreted the literacy dem~ the student's point of view tc upon interviews with student mal conversations, and textu; writing tasks, discipline speci for the students as they mo· academic life. Moreover, th. groups and in class modelitll never ~enerated totally in is· texts served as a foundatior: Furthermore, students came t is their attention moved awa! instead to the "sources and (p. 21). However, this learni1 differences found between c writing and classroom discoL Zawacki, 2006).

Herrington and Curtis (20 dents between 1989 and 199. (the original cohort was 18 interviews with the study pa• and classroom observations, the students' writing develc undergraduate years. The c writing across their college developed, more coherent, m• with more authority; their la sonal assurance and purpos• They also provide detailed c reflection, self-fashioning, ar personal developme~t, s~atin~ interests are closely tmphcate-

Herrington and Curtis poi ning with students pre-existi writing experiences, both I shaping student perceptions. related to and works in pare place in students' lives, in P· ities to revise, hard work ar writing topics also contribt: Emphasizing the way classro illustrate how supportive cla! growth, and provide examp

ts approximated features of several purposes within the same document .eness gains did include a movement •riting, and some of his texts approx­iting. :nprove across his freshman, sopho­de gains in subject-matter knowledge Jrical themes, but his key interpretive

The subject's critical-thinking skills lural aspect of subject-matter know­bile the student's purpose for writing nd grades toward writing for the dis­:course community took precedence :xt the student was aware of, rather try discourse community" (2004, p. :t was unable to make the kinds of

would be considered an authentic "here were, however, signs that the ;vhat historians do when they write, ting to take on features of that iden­JW historians analyze historical texts

analysis of broader social events. •Ut history writing seemed to happen· _t instruction. 1g explicitly with second-language 3. student she followed for two years

analysis, interviews, and informal :::~dhow her subject(s) drew on mul­d writers in a university setting, and ic, educational, and cultural back­J manage coursework. Spack found td writer as she received meaningful eriences; she linked the student's :ice, subject-matter immersion, and o~Vho could share their expertise" (p.

experience, linguistic and cultural performance; however, in Spack's knowledge and difficulty with aca­~reatest challenges later on. As the Jf study her literacy skills developed 3.Ctivities, especially reading. While ;riter grew over time, she continued roblem as overcoming a "Japanese student benefited significantly from r the learning did not always trans-

Longitudinal studies and writing development 369

Chiseri-Strater (1991) reported on two students she followed at the Uni­versity of New Hampshire between 1987 and 1988-although she began following six. Her research interests included understanding "how students interpreted the literacy demands made on them and what it meant from the student's point of view to be literate in a university" (p. 184). Drawing upon interviews with students and teachers, classroom observations, infor­mal conversations, and textual analysis, Chiseri-Strater reported that texts, writing tasks, discipline specific knowledge and language provided support for the students as they moved toward greater levels of participation in academic life. Moreover, through class discussion, reading and writing groups, and in class modeling, students begin to see that their ideas were never generated totally in isolation, and that the reading and writing of texts served as a foundation from which writers make new knowledge. Furthermore, students came to a tacit understanding of intertextuality, that is their attention moved away from focusing on the lone writer and shifted instead to the "sources and social contexts from which discourse arises" (p. 21). However, this learning was fraught with difficulties because of the differences found between courses that even in the same discipline use writing and classroom discourse in very different ways (see also Thaiss & Zawacki, 2006).

Herrington and Curtis (2000) investigated the development of four stu­dents between 1989 and 1993 at the University of Massachusetts, Amherst (the original cohort was 18 students). In their case studies they integrate interviews with the study participants and their teachers, textual analysis, and classroom observations, and describe, using chronological narratives, the students' writing development and personal growth through their undergraduate years. The cases show positive change in the students writing across their college experience: "students writing became more developed, more coherent, more surely articulated; further, all were writing with more authority; their later writings conveyed more of a sense of per­sonal assurance and purpose in communicating with readers" (p. 357). They also provide detailed descriptions of the way that writing for self­reflection, self-fashioning, and participation contributed to the students' personal development, stating that "students' personal histories, goals, and interests are closely implicated in their learning" (p. 124).

Herrington and Curtis point to many influences on development begin­ning with students pre-existing abilities, cultural backgrounds, and prior writing experiences, both positive and negative, which contribute to shaping student perceptions. They posit that writing development is inter­related to and works in parallel with other forms of development taking place in students' lives, in particular development in reading. Opportun­ities to revise, hard work and effort, plus student choice and interest in writing topics also contribute to the development of writing abilities. Emphasizing the way classroom contexts dominate student concerns, they illustrate how supportive classmates and their texts contributed to student growth, and provide examples of the significant role teachers played in

370 P. Rogers

student writing development. For example, the ways that the presence or absence of "scaffolding that is specific to the writer" (2000, p. 93), clearly defined course and assignment expectations, and a responsive teacher atti­tude in relation to feedback can constrain or promote higher levels of literacy.

Sternglass (1997) followed 53 study participants between 1989 and 1995 at the City University of New York (CUNY), through their initial composition courses into their upper division courses-in her published accounts she reported on nine. Using interviews, classroom observations, surveys, and textual analysis of student writing and course materials such as syllabi and instructor comments, she sought to understand "the ways in which the development of complex reasoning strategies were fostered by writing, the role that writing plays in learning, and how the multifaceted social factors in students lives affected their academic progress" (p. xiv). She also looked beyond students' academic development and sought for "a full and rich picture of [student's] development as whole persons" (p. xiv). While all of the students in her study developed as writers their growth was "neither neat nor linear" (p. xiv). The role of writing in their overall growth was very important for "only through writing did they achieve the insights that moved them to complex reasoning about the topic under con­sideration" (p. 295). Specifically, all of her subjects "were able to move beyond automatic acceptance of facts to recognize that new insights could be provided that might alter some unexamined assumptions in their field" (p. 295). Students also grew in their ability to create new knowledge for themselves through the integration of their own experience and academic discourse: "The worldviews of all students impinge on their attempts to make sense of the academic perspective and to integrate their experience into it" (p. 296). She claims that the relationship of college material to stu­dent's experience, especially the issues of race, class, and gender on multi­cultural urban populations is significant.

Sternglass argued that early instruction in composition was critical to fostering critical reading and writing skills, and was especially important for students with poor academic preparation who "have the potential to develop the critical reasoning processes that they must bring to bear in aca­demic writing if they are given the time" (1997, p. 296). Her longitudinal perspective allowed her to frame composition as "only a single step in the progression of student writers" (p. 141). Qualifying the ways instruction influences writing development, Sternglass argued, "Writing instruction needs to be thought of from a long term perspective and issues of content and form should be pursued at every level, both in composition courses and in content level courses" (p. 160). Moreover, the types of tasks stu­dents are asked to perform both for writing and examinations, and institu­tional practices such as grading contribute greatly to creating "teaching facilitated learning" (p. 195), and "the enculturation of students into aca­demic cultures" (p. 24). The nature of assignments, whether they are chal­lenging or not, the amount of previous practice and experience students

brin1 also "per. centt men1

St diffe1 play( She~ tor a ment de vel serve

C< (200: dura1 kno" of se· video instn analy comp intera two-s our n take' throu egtes accon grow Even comp1 were, cerns lenges

Sot Harva surve, paper~

a com; ument exposl chang~ report! that SG in wrij

1

:hat the presence or WOO, p. 93), clearly :JOnsive teacher atti­::>te higher levels of

between 19 8 9 and :hrough their initial :s-in her published :;room observations, ::>urse materials such ~rstand "the ways in .es were fostered by -DW the multifaceted : progress" (p. xiv). nt and sought for "a )le persons" (p. xiv). writers their growth iting in their overall did they achieve the the topic under con­"were able to move

tt new insights could ptions in their field" new knowledge for

rience and academic ()n their attempts to rate their experience •llege material to stu-3.nd gender on multi-

sition was critical to especially important

nave the potential to : bring to bear in aca­%). Her longitudinal ly a single step in the the ways instruction "Writing instruction and issues of content composition courses

1e types of tasks stu­l.inations, and institu­to creating "teaching of students into aca­

"Vhether they are chal­:1 experience students

Longitudinal studies and writing development 371

bring to an assignment, and the presence of new analytical demands were also important variables in student learning. Sternglass's cautions that "personal life situations might interfere with the amount of time or con­centration available to fulfill a task" (p. 3), reminding teachers that assign­ment design is only one factor in student writing performance.

Sternglass's analysis of teacher comments on student writings show how different kinds of comments, those focused on form and content, also played a role in creating a supportive environment for student learning. She also described how the student the degree of empathy between instruc­tor and student can be an important source of support for student develop­ment; as students progressed through their schooling, they began to develop professional relationships with their instructors who frequently served as strong role models and mentors for them.

Conducted at Pepperdine University between 1994 and 1998, Carroll (2002) followed 46 students over four years-with 20 staying through the duration of the study-to answer the question: "What do we actually know about how the writing of students develops over the course of several years of college?" Carroll's data (portfolios of student writing, videotaped focus groups, textual analysis of student writing including instructor comments and grades, written self-assessments, interviews, and analysis of course materials) showed that students did develop new and complex forms of literacy over their four years of college through multiple interactions with teachers, peers, and texts. Carroll argues that a one- or two-semester first-year course in writing cannot meet all the needs of even our more experienced writers, for literacy skills develop slowly "as writers take on new roles" (p. xii). Carroll suggests that as students progressed through their undergraduate years they internalized a language and strat­egies for approaching new reading and writing tasks. For the students to accomplish the advanced writing tasks required in college they needed to grow in multiple dimensions rather than develop a general writing ability. Even students who came to Pepperdine feeling prepared and who were competent in writing certain kinds of argumentative and research writing were challenged by the greater analytical demands and presentation con­cerns of college writing; however, it was in working through these chal­lenges that students developed as writers.

Sommers (2002) conducted a longitudinal study of 422 students at Harvard University between 1997 and 2002. Drawing upon interviews, surveys, and textual analysis-including writing assignments, student papers, and teacher comments, Sommers and her colleagues set out to gain a comprehensive view of the role writing plays in the college years, to doc­ument students' first-year writing experiences outside of their required expository writing courses, and the way students talk about writing changed throughout their undergraduate years. Although a complete report of the study has never been published, one finding to emerge was that students appeared to make the greatest gains in writing when they saw in writing a larger purpose than fulfilling an assignment (Sommers & Saltz,

372 P. Rogers

2004, p. 139). While Harvard students dearly valued writing their sense of interest needed to be sustained throughout their college career, by first accepting a novice role, then shedding it as they progress through their college careers. Sommers (2007) argues that feedback on writing plays an important role in undergraduate writing development "when, and only when, students and teachers create a partnership through feedback-a transaction in which teachers engage with their students by treating them as apprentice scholars and evolving writers, offering honest critique paired with instruction." Sommers concludes that when students have been taken seriously as apprentice writers, and when instructors model the role of an attentive reader, such comments function to anchor students in their aca­demic lives and, ultimately, make a vast difference in their college writing.

In addition, Sommers argues that growth in writing knowledge and skills is not always immediately evident in student texts. In fact, gaps between what students know about writing and what they can perform can be observed throughout the undergraduate years. This sleeper effect between what students report understanding and what they can consist­ently perform presents additional challenges for writing researchers, as several studies of writing at the college level suggest that students regress as well as progress as writers throughout their college experience (see, for example, Haswell, 2000, p. 310).

Haswell (2000) conducted a quantitative longitudinal study that used a direct comparison of writing samples written by the same 64 students during their first and third year of college. The goal of Haswell's study was to "document normal longitudinal changes in the writing of students during the first five semesters of college, changes associated with the per­formance of competent post graduate workplace writers" (p. 324). This empirical study of texts employed a theoretical frame that held the work of professional writers as the direction toward which students develop (p. 337).

Writing samples were drawn from each participant when they entered college and in the first semester of their junior year. In order to combat the attrition associated with longitudinal studies, Haswell drew his random sample after the students had completed the writing in their junior year. Analysis of the texts was conducted using a group of eight measures con­densed and refined from Haswell's earlier studies. No significant differ­ences in the random sample in terms of pre-existing abilities (p. 330) or gender emerged; also, neither the order of the prompt nor the rhetorical frames from which students could answer showed any difference. Eight of the nine measures showed statically significant change for the group; still few of the students in the sample show "wholesale advance across all eight measures and some record hardly any change from freshman to junior year" (p. 333). In the same way the focus on individual cases in the other longitudinal studies reviewed here prohibits the generalization of effects, Haswell's shows how conversely focusing on "group norms hides indi­vidual differences" (p. 333). While this study provides clear empirical evid-

J t

c v. e ~

fi ti di oi lq

p~

ot of: th\ to~ lm

sense of by first

gh their plays an _nd only back-a ng them

1e paired en taken Jle of an ::1e1r aca­nriting. dge and _ct, gaps perform er effect consist-

:hers, as s regress (see, for

at used a students

:udy was students the per-

~4). This work of develop

·entered mbat the random

1or year. Jres con­lt differ-

330) or hetorical Eight of

nup; still all eight

:o JUniOr :he other f effects, des indi­ical evid-

Longitudinal studies and writing development 373

ence for a general advance in college writing abilities from the freshman to the junior year, many questions remain.

Lunsford and her colleagues conducted a longitudinal study of writing between 2001 and 2006 at Stanford University (ssw.stanford.edu), which examined the writing practices and development of a random sample of 12 percent of Stanford's incoming class of 2001 (n = 189), through their undergraduate years and into their first year beyond college. Using semi­structured interviews, textual analysis, and surveys, the Stanford Study of Writing was the first longitudinal study of writing to collect student writing through an online database and to pay systematic attention to students' extracurricular writing. Additionally, the collection of data in the students' first year post-graduation provided insight into the development of college students' writing abilities as they entered graduate school or began profes­sional careers, and helped to address issues of learning transfer across mul­tiple contexts.

The study had several major goals: "to provide an overview of student writing at Stanford; to trace student development in writing across a five­year period; and to use findings to inform writing instruction at the institu­tional level." In addition, the study consciously sought to make useful contributions to longitudinal studies of writing and to the teaching of writing across the disciplines. Stanford students reported being deeply engaged with writing in and out of class; for these students, extracurricular writing was very often more important than in-class writing. Across the five years of the study, participants' understanding of the scope and func­tion of writing also changed considerably, from fairly instrumentalist definitions of writing to writing as a way of managing and making sense of enormous amounts of information and as a way of creating new knowledge.

Rogers (2008) has reported on 40 of the Stanford Study of Writing participants, addressing the processes and products of their writing devel­opment through an analysis of interviews and a criterion referenced scoring of students' academic writing across the five years of the study. Analysis of this data indicates that students developed through social interactions that took place at two levels: first, through interactions with the college curricu­lum itself; second, and more importantly, students grew through dialogic interactions with teachers and peers, that is, writing focused conversations (written or spoken) that were ongoing, invited active response, and which were addressed to the individual writer's thinking and ideas. The actual mechanism of development was fine tuning in which writers adjusted, syn­chronized, and coordinated their writing with readers and teachers, as well as disciplinary and professional communities of practice. As students moved through the undergraduate curriculum and pursued disciplinary specializations their writing showed statistically significant growth in a number of important knowledge domains: rhetorical knowledge, genre knowledge, and domain-specific content knowledge, including critical thinking and argumentation. Moreover, student writers developed across

374 P. Rogers

these multiple dimensions in a nonlinear fashion in the direction of increased, specialized, social participation. At the time of this writing, data analysis continues on the Stanford Study and is focused on issues of audi­ence, agency, and collaboration.

Discussion

While these studies demonstrate that writers develop across their college years in multiple ways, defining writing development remains a difficult task, as writing is a complex-cognitive and situated-social activity. Writing development therefore must always be seen as highly contextual. More­over, theories and standards of development are cultural inventions rooted in changing social practices which are also in flux; therefore, tracing what counts as competence and development means invoking theoretical frames, not merely reporting empirical data.

The studies reviewed here provide evidence that writing develops in multidimensional and nonlinear ways in higher education, supporting Kel­logg's (2006) model of development, which suggests that important aspects of writing development take place in and beyond the college years. This evidence directly challenges the idea that writing is a single general skill, which, like riding a bike, once learned is never lost (Russell, 2002). These studies also show that college students developed as writers through growth in multiple knowledge domains and by becoming enculturated into disciplinary practices (Rogers, 2008; Beaufort, 2004, 2007). As Haas (1994) noted, "at the college level to become literate is in many ways to learn the patterns of knowing about and behaving toward, texts within a disciplinary field" (p. 43). Thus, while growth in writing abilities is inti­mately connected to social interactions and is related to other forms of psychological and emotional change related to identity and self-efficacy, the bulk of the detectable changes exhibited by developing writers are arguably best viewed as movement toward greater levels of participation in particular communities of practice.

Additionally, the longitudinal studies reviewed here show unequivocally that students develop as writers and people throughout their college experi­ence through interactions with a variety of sociocultural inputs. These studies also provide a comprehensive view of the curricular and extracur­ricular factors that contribute to the development of writers and writing abilities (see Table 27.1). While non-classroom factors influence consider­ably student writing development, of particular interest are those factors that relate to students' classroom experiences; it is in the classroom where the students' background and pre-existing abilities, teacher behaviors and talk, assignment design, writing tasks, peers, feedback, texts, and assess­ment practices influence students' writing performance; moreover; these factors may be directly influenced by teachers and curriculum designers.

The studies reviewed here attend differently to these variables, as some of the studies deliberately focused upon out-of-class variables within their

Table 27.1 Factors contributir

Non-classroom-related factors

Students' lives outside of the cl or school context

Psychological factors such as s esteem, confidence, or anxiety

Time -Natural development (; maturity, and development)

Pre-existing abilities and writir experiences; cultural backgrou gender

Student engagement

Institutional context, includin§ assessment regimes

Mentoring (in sociocultural se-

Teacher behaviors

Teacher expectations

Responsive teacher attitude in to feedback

Immediate rhetorical context, classroom and grades

Time to draft, revise, and refle

Mentoring (by teachers)

Repeat performance opportuu practice

Nature of tasks

Teacher supportiveness, acces~ outside class

research design. Sternglass­stand the ways in which c formance; Chiseri-Strater ::: intermingling of personal visible the complex interpL and development of the wh non-schooling factors. On • est was in student growth ~

:he direction of his writing, data n issues of audi-

oss their college :nains a difficult activity. Writing •ntextual. More­J.ventions rooted •re, tracing what eoretical frames,

:ing develops in supporting Kel­

nportant aspects liege years. This gle general skill, ~n, 2002). These writers through ~nculturated into ~007). As Haas n many ways to :l, texts within a

abilities is inti­other forms of

md self-efficacy, ?ing writers are ~participation in

w unequivocally ir college experi-11 inputs. These :1r and extracur­ters and writing 1uence consider­tre those factors :lassroom where ~r behaviors and ~xts, and assess­moreover; these .1m designers. _riables, as some bles within their

Longitudinal studies and writing development 375

Table 27.1 Factors contributing to the development of writing abilities

Non-classroom-related factors

Students' lives outside of the classroom or school context

Psychological factors such as self­esteem, confidence, or anxiety

Time- Natural development (growth, maturity, and development)

Pre-existing abilities and writing experiences; cultural backgrounds, and gender

Student engagement

Institutional context, including assessment regimes

Mentoring (in sociocultural settings)

Teacher behaviors

Teacher expectations

Responsive teacher attitude in relation to feedback

Immediate rhetorical context, e.g., classroom and grades

Time to draft, revise, and reflect

Mentoring (by teachers)

Repeat performance opportunities, i.e. practice

Nature of tasks

Teacher supportiveness, accessibility outside class

Classroom discourse

What teachers say about writing in the classroom, including direct instruction

Peer to peer talk, reading and writing groups

Whole-class discussion

Conversation with teachers

Classroom genres

Teacher written response to writing, i.e., ongoing, performance-specific feedback

Model texts

Access to other student texts

Reading

General instructional supports: handouts, graphic organizers, assignments, and rubrics

Increased domain knowledge

research design. Sternglass, for example, wanted policy makers to under­stand the ways in which outside financial pressures affected student per­formance; Chiseri-Strater and Herrington and Curtis wanted to show the intermingling of personal literacies with academic literacies, and make visible the complex interplay between the development of writing abilities and development of the whole person. These studies give more attention to non-schooling factors. On the other hand, Beaufort and Haas, whose inter­est was in student growth within a single discipline, focused almost entirely

376 P. Rogers

on school-based factors. In this regard, the Lunsford study is the first longitudinal study to pay systematic attention to both school and extra­curricular writing.

These findings confirm Emig's notion that "longitudinal case studies ... make better known the developmental dimensions of the writing process" (1971, p. 95); furthermore, they suggest the importance of constructing rich learning environments where these variables are present in ways that promote rather than constrain student learning. Finally, these factors provide us with clear variables for future inquiry, where they can be tested for correlations, their relative importance in contributing to development, as well as the varying degrees in which individual elements contribute to the varying dimensions of writing development.

References

Applebee, A. (2000). Alternative models of writing development. In R. Indrisano & J. Squire (Eds.), Writing: Research!Theory/Practice. Newark, DE: IRA.

Beaufort, A. (2004). Developmental gains of a history major: A case for building a theory of disciplinary writing expertise. Research in the Teaching of English, 39(2), 136-185.

Beaufort, A. (2007). College writing and beyond: A new framework for university writing instruction. Logan, UT: Utah State Press.

Bereiter, C., & Scardamalia, M. (1987). The psychology of written composition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Britton, ]., Burgess, T., Martin, N., McLeod, A., & Rosen, H. (1975). The devel­opment of writing abilities (11-18). London: Macmillan Education.

Caroll, L. (2002). Rehearsing new roles. Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois Univer­sity Press.

Chiseri-Strater, E. (1991). Academic literacies: The public and private discourse of university students. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.

Emig, J. (1971). The composing process of twelfth graders. Urbana, IL: NCTE. Haas, C. (1994). Learning to read biology: One student's rhetorical development in

college. Written Communication, 11, 43-84. Haswell, R. H. (1991). Gaining ground in college writing: Tales of development

and interpretation. Dallas, TX: Southern Methodist University Press. Haswell, R. H. (2000). Documenting improvement in college writing: A longitudi­

nal approach. Written Communication, 17, 307-352. Herrington,]., & Curtis, M. (2000). Persons in process: Four stories of writing and

personal development in College. Urbana, IL: NCTE. Kellogg, R. T. (2006). Professional writing expertise. In K. A. Ericsson, N. Char­

ness, P. J. Feltovich, & R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), Expertise and expert performance. New York: Cambridge University Press.

King, M., & Rente!, V. (1982). Transition to writing. Columbus, OH: Research Foundation.

Loban, W. (1967). Language development: Kindergarten through grade twelve. NCTE Research Report No. 18, Urbana, IL: NCTE.

McCarthy, L. (1987). A stranger in strange lands: A college student writing across the curriculum. Research in the Teaching of English, 21,233-265.

Longitudina~

Rente!, V., & King, M. (1983). A lm written narrative. Columbus, OH: Rc

Rogers, P. M. (2008). The developme11 dina/ study across and beyond the cc. of California, Santa Barbara.

Russell, D. R. (2002). Writing in th~ ac ed.). Carbondale, IL: Southern Illmo

Sommers, N. (2002). The Harvard stuo

20 2006 from www.fas.harvard.ed-Som~ers, N. (2007). Across the drafts Sommers, N., & Saltz, L. (2004). The_

College Composition and Commun~ Spack, R. (1997). The acquisition of a

gitudinal case study. Written Comm Sternglass, M. S. (1997). Time to kna

learning at the college level. Mahwa Tierney, R. J., & Sheehy, M. (2005).

development/What literacy develoJ Flood, D. Lapp, J. R. Squire, & ] teaching the English language arts.

nsford study is the first • both school and extra-

_gitudinal case studies ... :; of the writing process" :)Ortance of constructing are present in ways that ~· Finally, these factors where they can be tested ~ibuting to development, J elements contribute to

•elopment. In R. Indrisano Newark, DE: IRA.

tajor: A case for building a the Teaching of English,

.J framework for university

:y of written composition.

:;en, H. (1975). The devel­m Education. -: Southern Illinois Univer-

!c and private discourse of

-s. Urbana, IL: NCTE. rhetorical development in

'ng: Tales of development oiversity Press. Jlege writing: A longitudi-

~our stories of writing and

K. A. Ericsson, N. Char­"! and expert performance.

::::olumbus, OH: Research

en through grade twelve.

~ge student writing across '233-265.

Longitudinal studies and writing development 3 77

Rente!, V., & King, M. (1983). A longitudinal study of coherence in children's written narrative. Columbus, OH: Research Foundation.

Rogers, P. M. (2008). The development of writers and writing abilities: A longitu­dinal study across and beyond the college-span. Doctoral dissertation, University of California, Santa Barbara.

Russell, D. R. (2002). Writing in the academic disciplines: A curricular history (2nd ed.). Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois University Press.

Sommers, N. (2002). The Harvard study of undergraduate writing. Retrieved April 20, 2006, from www.fas.harvard.edu/-exposlindex.cgi?section=study.

Sommers, N. (2007). Across the drafts. Unpublished manuscript. Sommers, N., & Saltz, L. (2004). The novice as expert: Writing the freshman year.

College Composition and Communication, 56(1), 124-149. Spack, R. (1997). The acquisition of academic literacy in a second language: A lon­

gitudinal case study. Written Communication, 10,235-261. Sternglass, M. S. (1997). Time to know them: A longitudinal study of writing and

learning at the college level. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Tierney, R. ]., & Sheehy, M. (2005). What longitudinal studies say about literacy

development/What literacy development says about longitudinal studies. In J. Flood, D. Lapp, ]. R. Squire, & ]. M. Jensen (Eds.), Methods of research on teaching the English language arts. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.