learning disability quarterly 2014 orosco 45 53

Upload: meily-zoel

Post on 02-Mar-2018

216 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 7/26/2019 Learning Disability Quarterly 2014 Orosco 45 53

    1/9

    Learning Disability Quarterly

    2014, Vol 37(1) 4553 Hammill Institute on Disabilities 2013

    Reprints and permissions:sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

    DOI: 10.1177/0731948713504206ldq.sagepub.com

    Article

    Although publications aimed at improving instruction for

    English language learners (ELLs) has grown within the past

    decade (e.g., Crdenas-Hagan, Carlson, & Pollard-Durodola,

    2007; Mathes, Pollard-Durodola, Crdenas-Hagan, Linan-Thompson, & Vaughn, 2007; Pollard-Durodola & Simmons,

    2009), to date there is surprisingly little research on assistive

    instruction with teachers of ELLs on English language

    development in content areas such as math (Janzen, 2008).

    Further instructional support that improves math compre-

    hension for ELLs is still needed. However, lower oral lan-

    guage proficiency in English (e.g., English vocabulary skills

    and content knowledge) is often overlooked as a part of this

    added instruction (Goldenberg, 2011). Research indicates

    that although basic math skills such as computation are

    taught well enough for ELLs to perform as well as their

    native English-speaking peers, teachers are not showing

    ELLs how to grasp specific oral language development (i.e.,math vocabulary and concepts) that impacts word-problem-

    solving comprehension, at proficiency levels equivalent to

    native English speakers (Orosco, Swanson, OConnor, &

    Lussier, 2013). Well-developed oral proficiency in English

    may be a critical step to improving word-problem-solving

    skills for ELLs (Orosco et al., 2013). Specifically, English

    vocabulary knowledge, listening comprehension, and the

    ability to manage contextual aspects of language (i.e., pro-

    viding direct and explicit instruction with math terminology

    and concepts) may need to be linked to improving compre-

    hension (Rupley & Nichols, 2005), especially when devel-

    oping word-problem-solving skills.

    Also, elementary school teachers are particularly chal-lenged and need to summon extra resources because of the

    range of instructional needs of ELLs for whom math con-

    tent in a second language is more arduous due to limited (a)

    experiences in vocabulary development, (b) prior math con-

    tent knowledge, and (c) strategies to improve word-problem-

    solving skills. In addition, students at risk for math

    disabilities (MD) may need more intensive, individualized,

    or small-group instruction that is highly structured and

    explicit to mediate word-problem-solving content success-

    fully. Furthermore, given the multistep process of the word-

    problem-solving process, strategy instruction continues to

    be an important intervention approach to improve solution

    accuracy.The literature has proposed a set of instructional math

    practices that have been validated by research on English

    native speakers with or at risk for MD. First, math instruction

    206 LDQXXX10.1177/0731948713504206Learning Disability QuarterlyOrosco

    1University of California, Riverside, USA

    Corresponding Author:

    Michael J. Orosco, Area of Special Education, Graduate School of

    Education, University of California, Riverside, CA 92521, USA.

    Email: [email protected]

    Word Problem Strategy for Latino EnglishLanguage Learners at Risk for MathDisabilities

    Michael J. Orosco, PhD1

    Abstract

    English Language Learners(ELLs) at risk for math disabilities(MD) are challenged in solving word problems for numerous

    reasons such as (a) learning English as a second language, (b) limited experience using math vocabulary, and (c) lack of

    strategies to improve word-problem-solving skills. As a result of these difficulties, ELLs may not only need math supportbut also oral language and reading development assistance. The purpose of this study was to assess the effectiveness

    of a math comprehension strategy procedure based on a dynamic assessment (DA) framework. The strategy provided

    scaffolding support based on the students reading and language comprehension levels. A multiple baseline was used to

    assess 6 third-grade Latino ELLs at risk for MD. As compared with baseline, the strategy increased problem-solving abilityfor all participants. All students level of performance was maintained during follow-up sessions. Results suggest that a focus

    on comprehension strategies may help facilitate math skills development for ELLs at risk for MD.

    Keywords

    content-area instruction, instructional strategies, single-subject methods, research design or utilization, at risk, thinking/

    cognition, education, Teacher

    at Massey University Library on April 8, 2015ldq.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://ldq.sagepub.com/http://ldq.sagepub.com/http://ldq.sagepub.com/http://ldq.sagepub.com/
  • 7/26/2019 Learning Disability Quarterly 2014 Orosco 45 53

    2/9

    46 Learning Disability Quarterly 37(1)

    should include (a) methods of explicit and direct instruction

    that teaches conceptual understanding of math concepts and

    principles of a word problem (e.g., Fuchs, Fuchs, Finelli,

    Courey, & Hamlett, 2004; Griffin & Jitendra, 2008; Jitendra,

    DiPipi, & Perron-Jones, 2002; Jitendra, Griffin, Deatline-

    Buchman, & Sczesniak, 2007; Swanson, Hoskyn, & Lee,

    1999), (b) visual representation techniques designed to bridge

    a connection from verbal information to symbolic under-

    standing by creating a mental model (e.g., Jitendra et al.,

    2007; Jitendra & Xin, 1997; Van Garderen & Montague,

    2003), (c) using instructional feedback with peer-assisted

    learning strategies during instruction (e.g., Fuchs, Compton,

    et al., 2008; Fuchs, Fuchs, Yazdin, & Powell, 2002; Fuchs,

    Seethaler, et al., 2008), and (d) small group instruction,

    instructional modeling, corrective feedback, and student ver-

    balizations (Baker, Gersten, & Lee, 2002; Gersten et al.,

    2009; Swanson et al., 1999). Although this effective instruc-

    tion with English only students may serve as a foundation for

    teaching ELLs, however, this general effective instruction isprobably not sufficient to promote accelerated math learning

    among ELLs at risk for MD. Nevertheless, it is most likely a

    necessary basis.

    In addition, another discrepancy that arises from the

    math literature is the high dependency on standardized

    measures (e.g., administered pre- and post-test) which test a

    students word-problem-solving performance by presenting

    scripted tasks that require the student to access previous

    learned knowledge with little teacher input (e.g., Grigorenko,

    2009; Haywood & Lidz, 2007; Swanson & Lussier, 2001).

    Because of this, static assessments have not been able to

    incorporate teacherstudent interaction as part of the testing

    process, where such feedback can help alleviate studentproblem-solving difficulty. As a result of this, there is an

    opportunity in the math literature in developing an assess-

    ment model that can be used to identify word-problem-

    solving challenges, make diagnostic decisions, and propose

    instructional strategies that address learning difficulties.

    The purpose of this study was to assess the effectiveness of

    a math strategy procedure based on a dynamic assessment

    (DA) framework.

    DA diverges with traditional models of assessment and

    instruction, in which only the students current competen-

    cies are measured, and the tester does not intervene so as

    not to influence the results (Orosco et al., 2013). In con-

    trast, DA determines whether substantive changes that

    occur in student performance were due to instructional scaf-

    folding across an array of tasks. During DA, a teacher facil-

    itates a students ability to build on prior knowledge through

    studentteacher interaction, and uses this mediation process

    as a way to help the student internalize new information

    (Vygotsky, 1962, 1978). The teacher uses a standardized

    protocol to measure learning potential. The learning poten-

    tial is a higher level of academic ability that a student can

    achieve after new knowledge is presented instructionally.

    Specifically, learning potential is measured in terms of the

    difference between, and/or change from a students level of

    performance when unassisted versus their achievable level

    of performance with assistance (i.e., Zone of Proximal

    Development[ZPD]; Sternberg & Grigorenko, 2002). Thus,DA allows for the measurement of students developing

    skills in determining which of these abilities are modifiable,

    and what interventions or mediational supports may be

    developed to improve their cognitive processes (Englert &

    Mariage, 2013).

    Although DA has been suggested as an alternative to tra-

    ditional math assessment (e.g., Fuchs, Compton, et al.,

    2008; Seethaler, Fuchs, Fuchs, & Compton, 2012), there are

    few published studies with Latino ELLs at risk for MD. The

    purpose of this study was to investigate a word-problem-

    solving strategy called Dynamic Strategic Math (DSM).

    DSM was operationally defined throughout this study as the

    tester modifying word-problem solving via a four-levelvocabulary modification procedure (Table 1) to the stu-

    dents level of word-problem-solving cognition, and then

    providing intervention with probes that assessed students

    word-problem-solving ability. This study addressed two

    research questions with Latino ELLs.

    Research Question 1:To what level does DA improve

    students word-problem-solving skills as measured by

    Table 1. Four-Level Math Vocabulary Modification Procedure.

    Level Description Example

    Basic (Level 1) Math terms used in everydayconversation

    before, after, combine, extra together, more, greater than,total, fewer, sort, fewer than, take away

    Intermediate (Level 2) Math terms not directly associatedwith a specific math content area

    addition, digits, division, multiplication, factor, factors,subtraction, sum

    Advance intermediate (Level 3) Math terms directly associated with aspecific content area

    quotient, divisor, divisible by, dividend, least commondenominator, least common multiple

    Technical vocabulary (Level 4) Math terms associated with a specificmath terminology

    perimeter, area, cylinder, inch, meter, centimeter, mile,rectangle, square, triangle, cube, right triangle

    Source.Adapted from Ernst-Slavit and Slavit (2007); Orosco, Swanson, OConnor, and Lussier (2013).

    at Massey University Library on April 8, 2015ldq.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://ldq.sagepub.com/http://ldq.sagepub.com/http://ldq.sagepub.com/http://ldq.sagepub.com/
  • 7/26/2019 Learning Disability Quarterly 2014 Orosco 45 53

    3/9

    Orosco 47

    word-problem-solving achievement (compared with

    baseline level)?

    Research Question 2:To what degree does DA main-

    tain students word-problem-solving skills in generaliza-

    tion sessions?

    Method

    Setting and Participants

    Six third-grade Latino ELLs at risk for MD from a southern

    California (English/Spanish) elementary classroom partici-

    pated in this study. This study defines Latino ELLs as stu-

    dents who speak Spanish as their native language, are

    identified as coming from Latin American descendants

    (e.g., Mexican, Mexican American), and are in the process

    of acquiring English as a second language, and who have

    not achieved full English proficiency. The schools popula-

    tion consisted of 453 students (55% Hispanic [39% Latino

    ELLs], 22% Black/African Americans, 14% White (non-Hispanic), 5% Asian, and 4% Other [categories created

    based on the U.S. Census]). According to district informa-

    tion, 75% of the schools population was in the free or

    reduced-price lunch program.

    While there is controversy over the definition of learn-

    ing disabilities, this study adhered to the growing consen-

    sus among researchers that it is best to use an absolute

    definition of learning disabilities (cutoff score on achieve-

    ment) rather than a discrepancy between achievement and

    IQ (e.g., Fletcher et al., 1989). In determining the criteria at

    risk for MD, we first considered (a) teacher recommenda-

    tion for intervention based on students being exposed to at

    least 3 years of math instruction; (b) students who had con-tinued to experience word-problem-solving difficulties in

    English; (c) students who had performed in the lower 25th

    percentile on district math tests previously; (d) Spanish

    spoken as their native language, as determined by the

    schools home language survey; (e) the California English

    Language Development Test (CELDT; Marr, Rodden, &

    Woods, 2009) was used to define ELL status; (f) Woodcock

    Johnson NU Tests of Achievement 3rd Edition, Achievement

    Test: Applied Problems (WJ NU III-ACH Test 10;

    Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2007; students who per-

    formed in the lower 25th percentile were included in the

    sample); and (g) parent consent. The WJ NU III has a

    reported internal reliability coefficient of .85 for ACH Test

    10 ages 8 to 10 (Woodcock et al., 2007). The test was

    administered at pre- and post-test, and the test data were

    compared with multiple baseline data, in determining

    whether the math intervention positively mediated stu-

    dents word-problem-solving skills. Finally, the CELDT

    measures English proficiency (listening, speaking, reading,

    and writing), and reliability scores for this test are between

    .73 and .94 across grade levels (Marr et al., 2009).

    Instrument

    DSM is built on a conceptual foundation of reciprocal

    teaching (Palincsar & Brown, 1984) and many features

    associated with effective instruction (e.g., collaborative

    group work, interactive dialogue, and explicit teaching

    strategies; for example, Baker et al., 2002; National

    Mathematics Advisory Panel, 2008; National ResearchCouncil, 2001). DSM includes instructional practices asso-

    ciated with improved reading comprehension: (a) building

    vocabulary so that students can contextualize and bring

    meaning to math language; (b) teaching students to moni-

    tor their comprehension and procedures for adjusting when

    word-problem-solving difficulties arise; (c) using cooper-

    ating learning practices so that students not only practice

    their English language skills, but also their problem-solving

    skills; (d) providing support for questioning strategies that

    assist students in answering critical questions about the

    word problem, feedback to students regarding their answers

    to questions, and opportunities for students to ask andanswer questions about the word problem; and (e) teaching

    students to write down and reflect on important ideas in the

    problem-solving process.

    Finally, DSM has shown preliminary evidence as a

    research-validated practice. The initial research was con-

    ducted with 6 second-grade Latino ELLs (Orosco et al.,

    2013). In this single-subject study, students were taught by

    the homeroom teacher to use the DSM with school math cur-

    riculum. Students made significant improvements in their

    word-problem-solving and demonstrated high levels of aca-

    demic engagement. They assisted each other with word

    meanings, main ideas, and understanding word problem

    text. Teacher feedback indicated that students word-problem-solving comprehension gains were associated with the qual-

    ity and quantity of DSM training and implementation.

    Experimental Design

    A changing criterion multiple baseline across subjects

    design (Kennedy, 2005) was used for evaluating the effects

    of a problem-solving intervention that aimed to change

    (accelerate)word-problem-solving efficiency in a sys-

    tematic stepwise method. In this study, each intervention

    session is associated with a stepwise criterion that targets

    a word-problem-solving level of difficulty (four levels or

    steps). The student advances to a higher level of difficulty

    after solving a set of four word problems correctly at their

    ZPD level. To prevent selection bias (Kratochwill &

    Levin, 2010), participants were selected and categorized

    based on teacher recommendation (all students had low

    math and reading scores and a need for intervention), and

    a list was randomly generated (based on Woodcock

    Johnson [WCJ] scores and reading rank) so as not to place

    the student with the most serious word-problem-solving

    at Massey University Library on April 8, 2015ldq.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://ldq.sagepub.com/http://ldq.sagepub.com/http://ldq.sagepub.com/
  • 7/26/2019 Learning Disability Quarterly 2014 Orosco 45 53

    4/9

    48 Learning Disability Quarterly 37(1)

    difficulties (e.g., WCJ math scores) first (e.g., Natasha

    went first). After students math performance was stable in

    the baseline phase, the independent variable was staggered

    across subjects and the number of sessions necessary to

    establish response stability (minimum of three sessions

    above the baseline mean). All participants were individu-

    ally administered four word problems per session similar

    to those used during the baseline phase. This study was

    conducted as a pullout program for 17 sessions (average

    2025 min per session) over a 5-week period and was a

    supplementary intervention to the general education math

    curriculum students received (50 min/day).

    Word problems. In this study, word problems applied were

    similar to those used in daily instruction. These word prob-

    lems were linguistically modified based on a scaffolding

    ladder that premised the language of math into four one-

    dimensional language levels, each providing a scaffold that

    supported the next higher level of word-problem-solving

    development. Level 1 word problems were embedded inbasic math terminology used in everyday discourse (high-

    frequency words), Level 2 word problems incorporated

    math terms not directly associated with a specific math con-

    tent (general math words), Level 3 word problems included

    math words directly associated with a specific math content

    area (specialized math vocabulary), and Level 4 incorpo-

    rated math vocabulary associated with a specific math

    content area topic (technical vocabulary). As an example of

    this scaffolding (Table 2), a Level 2 word problem may

    have asked the following: Luis likes to spend time at theme

    parks. He spent three hours in Disneyland and four hours at

    Universal Studies. What was the sum of time spent at theme

    parks in all? In this example, the word problem was made

    less linguistically complex by taking the Level 2 math term

    (sum), and teaching a Level 1 meaning (total) without alter-

    ing the math concept being taught.

    Probing. In this study, a probing procedure was developed

    by the researcher (see Table 3), in which the dependent vari-

    able was word-problem level achieved with the strategy

    intervention. The tool was designed to scaffold differing

    levels of students word-problem-solving skills through the

    application of five prompts in determining a students abil-

    ity with and without assistance. Scoring of the five levels

    involved the assignment of points at each prompt (0 =

    incorrect response, 1 = correct response). As part of the

    probing procedure, each student was asked to solve four lin-guistically modified word problems based on their problem-

    solving level. After 3-min duration, if the student was

    having difficulty solving the problem, the student was given

    the prompts with 1 min to answer each prompt. The admin-

    istration of prompts averaged 4- to 5-min duration, and the

    number of prompts administered to solve the problem

    established the type of intervention students received.

    Table 2. Dynamic Strategic Math Cue Sheet (Abbreviated Example).

    Word problem example: Luis likes to spend time at theme parks. He spent three hours in Disneyland and four hours at UniversalStudies. What was the sum of time spent in theme parks in all?

    Examiner, A word problem asks a question (point to the question): What was the sum of time spent in theme parks in all? Next, Iwill underline the important words in the question.

    Examiner, I know that the word sum means an amount obtained as a result of adding numbers. What does the concept sum of timemean? Sum of time means to add (+) time together. Yes, this makes sense. Sum of time can also mean to add (+) time together.

    Examiner, What was the sum of time spent in theme parks in all? The word problem states: Luis likes to spend time at themeparks. He spent three hours in Disneyland and four hours at Universal Studies. I am going to circle these numbers, as these are thenumbers I need to solve this problem. Okay, lets solve the problem. He needs to sum or add time, writing 3 hours + 4 hours = 7hours. My answer is 7 hours. Luis spent 7 hours at theme parks.

    Examiner, Okay, I need to check my answer. In the ones place 3 plus 4 equals 7. Luis spent 7 hours at theme parks. Now it is your turn.

    Table 3. Dynamic Strategic Math Probe (DSMAP).

    Examiner, A word problem asks a question. Can you find the question in the following word problem?

    Examiner, In each question there are always important words. Can you underline words in this question that you think are importantto solving this problem?

    Examiner, In each math problem there are always numbers that you need to solve the problem. Can you circle the numbers that you

    need to solve this problem?Examiner, Numbers are used to set-up and solve a math problem. Can you use these numbers to set-up the problem so that you cansolve the word problem?

    Examiner, After solving the math problem, you need to check your answer. Can you check your answer?

    at Massey University Library on April 8, 2015ldq.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://ldq.sagepub.com/http://ldq.sagepub.com/http://ldq.sagepub.com/http://ldq.sagepub.com/
  • 7/26/2019 Learning Disability Quarterly 2014 Orosco 45 53

    5/9

    Orosco 49

    Procedure

    Baseline phase. At the baseline level, each participant was

    individually administered four grade-level word problems

    that contained four progressive levels of word problem dif-

    ficulty. Students were told to do their best, and given as

    much time as possible to solve the problems. None of the

    participants required more than 10 min in attempting tosolve the problems. This established the baseline level as

    the word-problem level each student could accurately solve

    without assistance. This also created the entry level/starting

    point for the word problems for the intervention. All six

    participants started at word-problem-solving Level 1.

    Intervention Phase 1: Preteaching concepts and vocabu-

    lary. ELLs were first pre-taught specific math concepts,

    vocabulary, and terminology for the word-problem-solving

    lesson by direct and explicit modeling (see the appendix for

    description).

    Intervention Phase 2: Teaching the strategies. At this level,

    DSM integrated five common problem-solving strategies

    (What Do I Know, What Can I Find, What Is The Set-Up,

    Solve It, and Check For Understanding) that showed stu-

    dents how to solve the word problem that were modeled

    through direct and explicit instruction (see appendix for

    description).

    Intervention Phase 3: Co-operative learning and/or student pair-

    ing. Once students were knowledgeable in strategy usage,

    they were provided a collaborative approach (in pairs one

    student and one teacher), which allowed the students to

    practice these strategy methods (see appendix for descrip-tion). If word-problem-solving difficulties persisted in this

    stage, the teacher re-taught specific strategies by reciprocal

    teaching again until the students understood them.

    Social Validity

    At the conclusion of the study the social validity of the

    intervention was assessed using a three-question interview

    protocol. During this interview, participants were asked

    questions regarding their satisfaction with DSM (e.g., Do

    you think the strategy helped you understand word prob-

    lems better? Please explain.)

    Interobserver Agreement and Treatment

    Integrity

    An ELL/bilingual trained classroom teacher and researcher

    alternated sessions administering the intervention. To check

    on the degree to which intervention techniques were being

    applied in teacher interactions with students, a treatment

    integrity checklist based on the sequence of probe state-

    ments (e.g., pacing, quality of instruction, and scaffolding)

    for each intervention was applied. The checklist was com-

    pleted by a classroom observer (the researcher alternated

    with the ELL teacher as implementer/observer) for a total of

    33% of all phases, including 33% for baseline, 33% for

    intervention, and 33% for maintenance. The observer would

    code for fidelity via a checklist and score yes or no for

    each probed observed. A total agreement calculation method

    for each session (i.e., dividing the number of agreements

    between the probe responses by the number of disagree-

    ments and then multiplying by 100) indicated the consistent

    presence of intervention behaviors for all sessions by the

    two observers at baseline (85%), intervention (100%), and

    maintenance behaviors (90%).

    Results

    Figure 1 displays word-problem level achieved for eachparticipant as a function of baseline, intervention, and main-

    tenance sessions. Visual analysis supports strong evidence

    of a causal relationship (Kratochwill et al., 2010) between

    the DSM intervention on word-problem-solving accuracy.

    In addition, there was a clear consistency of level, trend,

    and variability in baseline, intervention, and maintenance

    phases. Also shown are WJ NU III-ACH Test 10 pre and

    post-test scores (Table 4).

    Baseline Performance

    During baseline, all six participants started at a baseline

    Level 1. Although the participants performance on word

    problem solving demonstrated a pattern of stability at base-

    line, the low performance on more language complex and

    difficult word problems for all the participants indicated a

    need for intervention.

    Intervention

    As compared with baseline, the intervention condition pro-

    duced an increased trend (effect) in accuracy and level of

    word-problem difficulty solved for all students. After each

    session, each participant was administered a set of four

    word problems based on the intervention level received. Allstudents showed immediate effects from DSM intervention

    because they received learning opportunities that were open

    and constrained to their background knowledge, oral

    language development, vocabulary, and problem-solving

    needs. First, students were directly and explicitly taught

    math concepts and vocabulary that connected to everyday

    words. This helped to build their linguistic math register.

    at Massey University Library on April 8, 2015ldq.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://ldq.sagepub.com/http://ldq.sagepub.com/http://ldq.sagepub.com/
  • 7/26/2019 Learning Disability Quarterly 2014 Orosco 45 53

    6/9

    50 Learning Disability Quarterly 37(1)

    Next, students were taught comprehension strategies that

    involved building on students current knowledge for solv-

    ing word problems that depended on understanding the

    quantities involved in the problems and the procedural flu-

    ency required in solving these problems. Finally, students

    were given carefully directed practice, with feedback, to

    develop the intervention method through the use of probes.

    These probes built on students thinking and helped them

    practice their math and language skills, and also helped

    them attend to relationships between the problems and solu-

    tions to word problems.

    Maintenance

    Finally, to determine maintenance of treatment skills, all

    students were administered four math word problems simi-

    lar to those used during the baseline phase for three ses-

    sions. During this phase, all students sustained a consistent

    word-problem-solving accuracy level similar to the inter-

    vention phase. Arthur maintained the highest level of

    performance a Level 4, while, April, Natasha, Victoria,

    Tomas, and Rudy maintained a Level 3. Post-test scores on

    the WJ NU III-ACH Test 10 also indicated student gains as

    a result of intervention.

    Social Validity

    Interview data indicated that all the participants were in

    agreement (100%) that intervention procedures were rea-

    sonable and effective. Several students commented around

    the theme I like the teaching; we could talk about math.

    The teacher commented, I really liked the simplicity of the

    strategy, and how easily it integrated math content and

    vocabulary with ELL reading pedagogy. The students rec-

    ommended more collaboration and practice time. Also,

    the teacher would have liked more professional develop-

    ment and planning time to think about how to improve on

    her word-problem-solving instruction.

    Discussion

    The purpose of this study was to investigate the effects of

    a math intervention, DSM, on Latino ELLs word-prob-

    lem-solving achievement. Visual analysis of graphed data

    indicated a functional relationship between DSM and

    increased word-problem-solving ability. As students

    knowledge of DSM improved, their skills to solve word

    problems with increasingly complex vocabulary and con-

    tent improved during the intervention in comparison with

    that during the baseline phase. In addition, students could

    maintain knowledge of the DSM process, during three

    maintenance sessions after intervention and relevant to

    baseline phase. Overall, results provided support for the

    impact of DSM on the improvement of word-problem-

    solving skills.

    The evidence from this study lends support that DSM

    improved ELLs word-problem-solving ability during the

    treatment phase in comparison to baseline phase, and this

    performance was maintained during generalization sessions.

    The DA literature infers that scaffolding interventions can

    positively mediate word-problem-solving performance over

    time because it can give mediation support based on stu-

    dents actual developmental level as determined by indepen-

    dent problem-solving performance, and the highest level of

    potential development achieved through teacher assistanceor collaboration with more capable peers (Vygotsky, 1978).

    The results (e.g., increased word-problem-solving ability) of

    this study demonstrated a positive mediation trend during

    intervention. Although students in this project had the basic

    math skills to perform algorithmic computations adequately,

    while encountering word problems, the participants needed

    assistance in understanding math vocabulary and concepts

    to improve their word-problem-solving performance. The

    findings from this study were consistent with those of the

    Figure 1. Word-problem-solving level achieved per session.

    at Massey University Library on April 8, 2015ldq.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://ldq.sagepub.com/http://ldq.sagepub.com/http://ldq.sagepub.com/http://ldq.sagepub.com/
  • 7/26/2019 Learning Disability Quarterly 2014 Orosco 45 53

    7/9

    Orosco 51

    emerging literature in this and area that dynamic math

    assessment may be an effective model for improving ELLs

    word-problem-solving performance.

    Finally, the evidence from this study provides addi-

    tional support for the integration of math content with

    reading comprehension strategies as an effective peda-

    gogy for teaching ELLs word-problem-solving skills.

    Evidence from this study indicates that reading compre-

    hension strategies (e.g., reciprocal teaching) may be an

    effective method because it provides a dialogic form of

    scaffolding instruction that integrates students reading

    prompts (e.g., consider their background knowledge on

    the topic they are reading, to summarize key ideas, and to

    self-question while they read) with math content. The

    results from this study indicate that DSM may be an

    effective word problem solving tool because it can

    improve ELLs word problem solving accuracy, promotes

    their academic language development, and provides equi-table access for them to general math education. In addi-

    tion, this method gives teachers a deeper understanding of

    the underlying processes, essential elements, develop-

    ment trajectory, skill levels, and potential of their stu-

    dents math achievement through improved instruction.

    Limitations

    While the findings from this study demonstrate that ELLs

    word-problem-solving skills can be positively mediated to

    higher development levels (e.g., criterion changing math

    terminology difficulty) by providing scaffolding support,

    there were limitations to this study. This was a small-scale

    study (N= 6) in which individual data were collected for

    duration of 17 sessions, and therefore, the extent to which

    the intervention may have positively mediated word-

    problem-solving skills in other ELLs for this duration of

    time is unknown. And because of this, generalizing inter-

    vention effectiveness to other ELL populations is limited.

    To date, there have been few math studies that combine DA

    with reading instruction with ELLs at the elementary in

    comparing intervention effectiveness.

    Implications

    The evidence from this study has implications for ELLs at

    risk for MD. These findings indicate that although partici-

    pants had number sense, school data collected prior to the

    study indicated that the participants skill level was limitedto number calculations with basic math problems and heav-

    ily influenced by the context in which the numbers

    appeared. This below basic grade-level performance indi-

    cated that DSM provided ELLs with (a) a differentiated

    math instruction, one that was matched to the students

    English language proficiency; (b) instructional approaches,

    which built on students pre-existing background knowl-

    edge and made a connection with prior learning experi-

    ences; (c) teacher and student joint collaborative activities;

    (d) an opportunity to use sentence frames and models to

    help students talk about math content; and (e) gave addi-

    tional math practice and/or math time for discussion of key

    concepts. Word-problem-solving data indicated that stu-

    dents could acquire higher-level word-problem-solving

    proficiency with scaffolding support in vocabulary and

    reading comprehension development.

    Appendix

    Intervention Procedure

    Baseline phase. The baseline level was established as the

    word problem level (4 possible levels) the student could

    accurately solve without assistance. This established the

    word problem level/difficulty starting point of the

    intervention.

    Intervention phase 1: Preteaching concepts and

    vocabulary.Each student was provided 3 5 index

    cards to practice concepts and vocabulary. The teacher

    modeled the activity by holding up a card, providing

    a definition, elaborating on the definition through

    contextualization, writing this vocabulary on chart

    paper/blackboard, and applying usage of the word in

    a math problem while solving it.

    Table 4. Demographic, School-Related Data, and WCJ Test 10 Pre- and Post-Test Scores.

    Student Gender AgeDistrict readingassessment level

    District mathassessment level

    WCJ pre-testpercentile (%)

    WCJ pre-teststandard score

    WCJ post-testpercentile (%)

    WCJ post-teststandard score

    Natasha F 8.7 2.4 Below basic 16 85 18 86

    Victoria F 8.6 2.7 Below basic 18 86 19 87

    April F 8.5 2.6 Below basic 14 84 16 85

    Tomas M 8.8 2.8 Below basic 18 86 19 87Rudy M 9.0 2.5 Below basic 16 85 18 86

    Arthur M 8.7 2.7 Below basic 18 86 21 88

    M 16.67 18.50

    SD 1.63 1.64

    Note.WCJ = WoodcockJohnson NU Tests of Achievement 3rd Edition, Achievement Test: Applied Problems; F = Female; M = Male.

    at Massey University Library on April 8, 2015ldq.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://ldq.sagepub.com/http://ldq.sagepub.com/http://ldq.sagepub.com/http://ldq.sagepub.com/
  • 7/26/2019 Learning Disability Quarterly 2014 Orosco 45 53

    8/9

    52 Learning Disability Quarterly 37(1)

    1. Definition:The teacher stated: This is the word sum.

    It means to combine, add, or total.[On the chart paper

    the teacher wrote these sentences and next to them,

    the math symbol +.]

    2. Contextualization: The teacher contextualized the

    vocabulary by stating: Luis likes to spend time at

    theme parks. He spent three hours in Disneyland and

    four hours at Universal Studies. What was the sum of

    time spent in theme parks in all? What does sum

    mean? [Referring to the chart paper.] Sum means to

    combine (+). Sum means to add (+). Sum means to

    total (+).

    3. Writing & Using: The teacher repeats reading the

    word problem, and then solves the problem on board.

    3 + 4 = 7, the sum of time spent at theme parks was

    seven. Now it is your turn. [Students repeat this

    process.]

    As students became familiar with this information, the

    teacher then began to integrate and embed comprehensionstrategy instruction.

    Intervention phase 2: Teaching the strategies.At this

    level, DSM integrated five common problem-solving

    strategies (Know, Find, Set-Up, Solve, and Check

    Understanding):

    1. Know -What do I know about the question occurred

    after reading the word problem and consisted of

    brainstorming (activating background knowledge)

    what was already known about the word problem,

    and predicting how it may be solved.

    2. Find -Find the important vocabulary and numbers

    occurred during reading and referred to the process

    of finding key information for meaning and

    understanding.

    At this point, students were taught to use strategies to

    help them figure out unknown words or concepts.

    3. Set Up -Students also began to set it up during read-

    ing by stopping after each sentence to find the main

    idea and to check to see if this information was rele-

    vant to solving the problem.

    4. Solve -Finally, solve it and check it took place after

    reading the word problem.5. Check Understanding -Next, students checked their

    understanding by generating and answering questions

    about what they had already read and reviewed what

    they had learned by summarizing the key ideas pre-

    sented in the word problem, solving it, and checking

    it to see if solved correctly.

    Intervention phase 3: Cooperative learning and/or

    student pairing.Once students were knowledgeable

    in strategy usage, they were provided a collaborative

    approach (in pairs), which allowed the students to

    practice this method. In this stage, the student assumed

    the leadership role and imitated the teachers role.

    Within this process:

    1. The student generated and asked questions to check

    for understanding.

    2. The student solved the problem and checked to see if it

    was answered correctly. If answered incorrectly, the prob-

    lem-solving process was repeated between the teacher

    and student again, to see where mistakes were made.

    Within this process:

    1. The teacher monitored the students effectiveness by

    providing probes as needed (e.g., reading words, clar-

    ifying math concepts, or reminding students of a strat-

    egy skipped). (See Table 2 for probes and Table 3 for

    strategy cues.)2. If word-problem-solving difficulties persisted, the

    teacher then re-taught specific strategies by recipro-

    cal teaching again until the student understood them.

    Declaration of Conflicting Interest

    The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect

    to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

    Funding

    The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support

    for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This

    paper is based on a study funded by the U.S. Department ofEducation, Cognition and Student Learning in Special Education

    (USDE R324A090002), Institute of Education Sciences, awarded

    to the author.

    References

    Baker, S., Gersten, R., & Lee, D. (2002). A synthesis of empirical

    research on teaching mathematics to low-achieving students. The

    Elementary School Journal, 103, 5173. doi:10.1086/499715

    Crdenas-Hagan, E., Carlson, C. D., & Pollard-Durodola, S. D.

    (2007). The cross-linguistic transfer of early literacy skills: The

    role of initial l1 & l2 skills and language of instruction.Language

    Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 38, 249259.

    Englert, C. S., & Mariage, T. (2013). The sociocultural model

    as a framework in instructional intervention research. In

    H. Lee Swanson, S. Graham, & K. R. Harris (Eds.),Handbook

    of learning disabilities(pp. 545564). New York, NY: Guilford.

    Ernst-Slavit, G., & Slavit, D. (2007). Educational reform, math-

    ematics, & diverse learners: Meeting the needs of all students.

    Multicultural Education, 14(4), 2027.

    Fletcher, J. M., Epsy, K. A., Francis, P. J., Davidson, K. C.,

    Rourke, B. P., & Shaywitz, S. E. (1989). Comparison of

    cutoff and regression-based definitions of reading dis-

    abilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 334338.

    doi:10.1177/002221948902200603

    at Massey University Library on April 8, 2015ldq.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://ldq.sagepub.com/http://ldq.sagepub.com/http://ldq.sagepub.com/http://ldq.sagepub.com/
  • 7/26/2019 Learning Disability Quarterly 2014 Orosco 45 53

    9/9

    Orosco 53

    Fuchs, L. S., Compton, D. L., Fuchs, D., Hollenbeck, N., Craddock,

    C. F., & Hamlett, C. L. (2008). Dynamic assessment of alge-

    braic learning in predicating third graders development

    of mathematical problem solving. Journal of Educational

    Psychology, 100, 829850. doi:10.1037/a0012657

    Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Finelli, R., Courey, S. J., & Hamlett, C.

    L. (2004). Expanding schema-based transfer instruction to

    help third graders solve real-life mathematical problems.American Educational Research Journal, 41, 419445.

    doi:10.3102/00028312041002419

    Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Yazdin, L., & Powell, S. R. (2002).

    Enhancing first-grade childrens mathematical development

    with peer-assisted learning strategies. School Psychology

    Review, 31, 569583.

    Fuchs, L. S., Seethaler, P. S., Powell, S. R., Fuchs, D., Hamlett, C. L.,

    & Fletcher, J. M. (2008). Effects of preventative tutoring on the

    mathematical problem solving of third-grade students with math

    and reading difficulties.Exceptional Children, 74, 155173.

    Gersten, R., Chard, D. J., Jayanthi, M., Baker, S. K., Morphy, P.,

    & Flojo, J. (2009). Mathematics instruction for students with

    learning disabilities: A meta-analysis of instructional com-

    ponents. Review of Educational Research, 79, 12021242.doi:10.3102/0034654309334431

    Goldenberg, C. (2011). Reading instruction for English language

    learners. In M. L. Kamil, P. D. Pearson, E. B. Moje, & P. P.

    Afflerbach (Eds.),Handbook of reading research volume IV

    (pp. 684710). New York, NY: Routledge.

    Griffin, C., & Jitendra, A. K. (2008). Word problem-solving

    instruction in inclusive third-grade mathematics class-

    rooms. The Journal of Educational Research, 102, 187201.

    doi:10.3200/JOER.102.3.187-202

    Grigorenko, E. L. (2009). Dynamic assessment and response to

    intervention: Two sides of one coin. Journal of Learning

    Disabilities, 42, 111132. doi:10.1177/0022219408326207

    Haywood, C. K., & Lidz, C. S. (2007). Dynamic assessment in

    practice: Clinical and educational applications. New York,

    NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Janzen, J. (2008). Teaching English language learners in the con-

    tent areas.Review of Educational Research, 78, 10101038.

    doi:10.3102/0034654308325580

    Jitendra, A. K., DiPipi, C. M., & Perron-Jones, N. (2002). An

    exploratory study of schema-based word-problem solving

    instruction for middle school students with learning disabili-

    ties: An emphasis on conceptual and procedural understand-

    ing. The Journal of Special Education, 36, 2238. doi:10.117

    7/00224669020360010301

    Jitendra, A. K., Griffin, C., Deatline-Buchman, A., & Sczesniak,

    E. (2007). Mathematical word problem-solving in third grade

    classrooms: Lessons, learned from design experiments. TheJournal of Educational Research, 100, 283302.doi:10.3200/

    JOER.100.5.283-302

    Jitendra, A., & Xin, Y. P. (1997). Mathematical word-problem-

    solving instruction for students with mild disabilities

    and students at risk for math failure a research synthe-

    sis. The Journal of Special Education, 30, 412438.

    doi:10.1177/002246699703000404

    Kennedy, C. (2005). Single-case designs for educational

    researcher. Boston, MA. Allyn & Bacon.

    Kratochwill, T. R., Hitchcock, J., Horner, R. H., Levin, J. R.,

    Odom, S. L., Rindskopf, D. M., & Shadish, W. R. (2010).

    Single-case designs technical documentation. Retrieved from

    http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/pdf/wwc_scd.pdf

    Kratochwill, T. R., & Levin, J. R. (2010). Enhancing the scientific

    credibility of single-case intervention research: Randomization

    to the rescue.Psychological Methods, 15, 124144.

    Marr, D., Rodden, L., & Woods, A. (2009). Technical report for

    the California English Language Development Test (CELDT)

    (California Department of Education). Monterrey, CA: CTB/McGraw-Hill.

    Mathes, P. G., Pollard-Durodola, S. D., Crdenas-Hagan, E., Linan-

    Thompson, S., & Vaughn, S. (2007). Teaching struggling readers

    who are native Spanish speakers: What do we know?Language,

    Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 38, 260271.

    National Mathematics Advisory Panel. (2008). Foundations

    for success: The final report of the National Mathematics

    Advisory Panel. Retrieved from http://www.ed.gov/about/

    bdscomm/list/mathpanel/report/final-report.pdf

    National Research Council. (2001). Adding it up: Helping chil-

    dren learn mathematics ((J. Kilpatrick, J. Swafford, & B.

    Findell, Eds. Mathematics Learning Study Committee, Center

    for Education, Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences

    and Education). Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Orosco, M. J., Swanson, H. L., OConnor, R., & Lussier, C.

    (2013). The effects of dynamic strategic math on English lan-

    guage learners word problem solving. The Journal of Special

    Education, 47, 96107.

    Palincsar, A. S., & Brown, A. L. (1984). The reciprocal teaching

    of comprehension-fostering and comprehension-monitoring

    activities. Cognition and Instruction, 1, 117175.

    Pollard-Durodola, S. D., & Simmons, D. C. (2009). The role

    of explicit instruction and instructional design in promot-

    ing awareness development and transfer from Spanish

    to English. Reading & Writing Quarterly: Overcoming

    Learning Diff iculties, 25, 139161.

    Rupley, W. H., & Nichols, W. D. (2005). Vocabulary instruc-

    tion for the struggling reader. Reading & Writing Quarterly:

    Overcoming Learning Difficulties, 21, 239260.

    Seethaler, P. M., Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., & Compton, D. L. (2012).

    Predicting first graders development of calculation versus word-

    problem performance: The role of dynamic assessment.Journal

    of Educational Psychology, 104, 224234. doi:10.1037/a0024968

    Sternberg, R. J., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2002).Dynamic testing: The

    nature and measurement of learning potential. New York,

    NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Swanson, H. L., Hoskyn, M., & Lee, C. M. (1999).Interventions for

    students with learning disabilities. New York, NY: Guilford.

    Swanson, H. L., & Lussier, C. (2001). A selective synthesis of the exper-

    imental literature on dynamic assessment.Review of Educational

    Research, 71, 321363. doi:10.3102/00346543071002321Van Garderen, D., & Montague, M. (2003). Visual-spatial rep-

    resentation, mathematical problem solving, and students of

    varying abilities.Learning Disabilities Research & Practice,

    18, 246254. doi:10.1111/1540-5826.00079

    Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA:

    MIT Press.

    Vygotsky, L.S. (1978). Mind and society. Cambridge, MA:

    Harvard University Press.

    Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, J. (2007). Woodcock

    Johnson NU tests of achievement(3rd ed.). Rolling Meadows,

    IL: Riverside Publishing.

    at Massey University Library on April 8, 2015ldq.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://www.ed.gov/about/bdscomm/list/mathpanel/report/final-report.pdfhttp://www.ed.gov/about/bdscomm/list/mathpanel/report/final-report.pdfhttp://www.ed.gov/about/bdscomm/list/mathpanel/report/final-report.pdfhttp://ldq.sagepub.com/http://ldq.sagepub.com/http://ldq.sagepub.com/http://ldq.sagepub.com/http://www.ed.gov/about/bdscomm/list/mathpanel/report/final-report.pdf