jitter‐index of the fundamental frequency of infant cry as a possible diagnostic tool to predict...

9
This article was downloaded by: [Joh Gutenberg Universitaet] On: 24 October 2014, At: 15:28 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Early Child Development and Care Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/gecd20 Jitterindex of the fundamental frequency of infant cry as a possible diagnostic tool to predict future development problems E. Ludwig Grauel a , Sabine Hock a & Hartmut Rothg#auanger b a Pediatric Clinic, Department of Neonatology , Univ.Hospital [Charite], HumboldtUniv., Berlin, GDR b Institute of Anthropology , Univ.Hospital [Charite], HumboldtUniv., Berlin, GDR. Published online: 07 Jul 2006. To cite this article: E. Ludwig Grauel , Sabine Hock & Hartmut Rothg#auanger (1990) Jitterindex of the fundamental frequency of infant cry as a possible diagnostic tool to predict future development problems, Early Child Development and Care, 65:1, 23-29, DOI: 10.1080/0300443900650104 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0300443900650104 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-

Upload: hartmut

Post on 28-Feb-2017

217 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

This article was downloaded by: [Joh Gutenberg Universitaet]On: 24 October 2014, At: 15:28Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Early Child Development and CarePublication details, including instructions for authors andsubscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/gecd20

Jitter‐index of the fundamentalfrequency of infant cry as apossible diagnostic tool to predictfuture development problemsE. Ludwig Grauel a , Sabine Hock a & HartmutRothg#auanger ba Pediatric Clinic, Department of Neonatology ,Univ.‐Hospital [Charite], Humboldt‐Univ., Berlin, GDRb Institute of Anthropology , Univ.‐Hospital [Charite],Humboldt‐Univ., Berlin, GDR.Published online: 07 Jul 2006.

To cite this article: E. Ludwig Grauel , Sabine Hock & Hartmut Rothg#auanger (1990)Jitter‐index of the fundamental frequency of infant cry as a possible diagnostic tool topredict future development problems, Early Child Development and Care, 65:1, 23-29, DOI:10.1080/0300443900650104

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0300443900650104

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information(the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor& Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warrantieswhatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purposeof the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are theopinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor& Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should beindependently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francisshall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs,expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arisingdirectly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use ofthe Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes.Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-

licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expresslyforbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Joh

Gut

enbe

rg U

nive

rsita

et]

at 1

5:28

24

Oct

ober

201

4

Early Child Development and Care, 65, p. 23-29 © 1990 Gordon and Breach Science Publishers S.A.Reprints available directly from the publisher Printed in the United KingdomPhotocopying permitted by license only

Jitter-index of the fundamental frequency ofinfant cry as a possible diagnostic tool topredict future development problems

Part 2: Clinical considerations

E. LUDWIG GRAUEL, SABINE HOCK

Pediatric Clinic, Department of Neonatology, Univ.-Hospital(Charite), Humboldt-Univ., Berlin, GDR.

HARTMUT ROTHGÄNGER

Institute of Anthropology, Univ.-Hospital (Charite),Humboldt-Univ., Berlin, GDR.

In 36 newborn babies, 22 of them from a high risk population, cry analyses wereperformed at the age of up to 4 weeks. The jitter-index, a parameter derived fromthe microfluctuation of the fundamental frequency, showed a good correlation tothe developmental outcome of the children at the age of 2 years. A low jitter-index of pain cry (jP) predicts an unfavourable outcome. The results, however, areso far preliminary, since the number of patients is too small and the group tooheterogeneous to draw final conclusions. The authors, however, are hopeful thatcry analysis will become a supplement to the battery of noninvasive methods,which permit a prognosis for newborns at risk concerning possible futuredevelopmental problems.

1. INTRODUCTION

The peculiar cry of severely brain damaged infants has been known to physicians forcenturies. Sonagraphic studies on infants with cerebral diseases and 21-Trisomy(Wolff, 1969; Wasz-Hockert et al., 1968) as well as in infants with the Cri-du-chatsyndrome (Luchsinger et al., 1967, Schroeder et al., 1967; Wasz-Hockert et al., 1968)suggested the use of the infant cry as a diagnostic indicator (Lester & Boukydis, 1985;Murry & Murry, 1980).

To make a developmental prognosis for an individual newborn is even to-day, avery difficult and dubious problem. The Apgar score as well as different biochemicalparameters seem to be poor predictors of individual outcome (Ruth & Raivo, 1988;Menzel et al., 1974; Menzel et al., 1984; Bottcher et al., 1975); EEG and ultrasoundcontribute only in relatively severe cases to the developmental prognosis (Staudt et al.,1982; McMeramin et al., 1984).

Correspondence: Prof. Dr. sc. med. E.L. Grauel, Abt. Neonatologie, Univ.-Kinderklinik, Schumannstrasse20/21, Berlin, GDR-1040

23

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Joh

Gut

enbe

rg U

nive

rsita

et]

at 1

5:28

24

Oct

ober

201

4

24 E. L GRAUEL ETAL

It was out intention to use cry analysis in newborns to diagnose disturbances of theCNS, which cannot be detected properly by conventional methods. If it provesvaluable, cry analysis might be a supplementary noninvasive bedside method, easilyapplied with a minimum of disturbance for the infant. Some preliminary results of ourinvestigations will be presented.

2. METHOD AND STUDY DESIGN

Altogether 36 babies were included in the study; the 14 infants already mentioned inPart 1 of this publication and in addition, 22 infants from a high risk population.Their birth weights ranged from 930 to 3,900 g, their gestational age ranged between27 and 40 weeks and the 5-minute Apgar score between 4 and 10.

Since the majority of premature babies were unable to produce reliable cry signalsduring the first week of life, the investigations were carried out at about 4 weeks afterbirth. The condition of stimulation and recording of the cries were the same asdescribed in Part 1.

All babies were followed up by an experienced neuropediatrician and classified inthree groups after two years: normal, moderately impaired and severely impaired.Moderate impairment was defined as developmental delay of up to 3 months, severeimpairment was defined as developmental delay of more than 3 months, and/ordistinct neurological disturbance.

From a previous preliminary study on 5 healthy newborns and 5 newborns withmoderate adaptional disturbances, we had gained the impression that the jitter-indexof pain cry (jP) yielded the best discriminatory parameter for the characterization of"normal" and "disturbed". As normal, we defined indices above 7.0, as intermediate,

j P between 5.5 and 7.0 and as abnormal, j P less than 5.5. The 36 infants underinvestigation were classified accordingly.

In a second step, the cry groups were then classified according to the outcome.

3. RESULTS

Fifteen infants belonged to the group with normal j P , 12 infants belonged to theintermediate group and another 9 babies had an abnormal j P .b It can be seen (Table 1) that in contrast to the distinct discriminatory values of j P

the other two parameters (f0 and t) show no clear differences between the 3 groups.This holds true not only for pain cry but also for hunger and frustration cry.

It seems noteworthy that, if j is reduced in pain cries it is also reduced in hungerand frustration cries.

After the follow-up at age two years the 36 infants could be classified as 20 withoutdevelopmental problems, 9 with moderate developmental delay and/or minorneurological deviations and 6 severely handicapped ones.

The correlation of the grouping according to j P during the newborn period and thatof the clinical outcome after 2 years revealed statistical significance approaching the1%-level (Table 2).

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Joh

Gut

enbe

rg U

nive

rsita

et]

at 1

5:28

24

Oct

ober

201

4

EARLY CNS DIAGNOSIS BY CRY ANALYSIS 25

Table 1 Jitter-index (j), fundamental frequency (f0) and duration of cry (t) in different groups ofnewborn according to jitter-index in pain cry

Type of cry

1. Hungerjfo(Hz)t(ms)

2. Painjfo(Hz)t(ms)

3. Frustrationjfo(Hz)t(ms)

X

5,224441170

8,675053470

9,965431550

normalJP>7,0n = 15

SD

0,6646380

1,1464

1050

2,66100400

intermediateJi

X

4,794561470

6,205063660

7,095281650

P 5,5-7,0n = 12

SD

0,6646390

0,3245

1000

0,7053200

abnormaljP<5,5n = 9

X

4,604091550

4,724653510

5,884621550

SD

0,7557200

0,6055

1700

1,7480350

Table 2 X2-correlation of jitter-index groups (jp) and outcome at 2 years of age (n = 36)

follow-up (2 years of life)jitter-index 1 2 3

(normal) (moderate (severe controlimpairment) impairment) n X2

1 (normal)z 12 3 0 15

X2 1,61 0,33 2,5 4,44

2 (intermediate)z 4 6 2 12

X2 1,07 2,13 0 3,20

3 (abnormal)z 4 1 4 9

X2 0,20 0,9 4,17 5,27

2,88 3,36 6,67 12,91

X2 table, f =4 <x 5%= 9,49oc 1% = 13,28

4. DISCUSSION

Minimal changes in the fundamental frequency of infant cries and other vocalinformations, "jitters", seem to belong to a kind of signal comparable to themicrofluctuation, which is found in different other biological signals, such as thecardiotachogram. The loss of such microfluctuations usually indicates the loss ofstability in a system. Therefore, we suppose that a reduction of the vocal jitter in

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Joh

Gut

enbe

rg U

nive

rsita

et]

at 1

5:28

24

Oct

ober

201

4

26 E. L. GRAUEL ETAL.

infant cry may indicate certain abnormalities of the central nervous activities, whichregulate and control the function of the cry producing organs.

In healthy infants there is some parallelity in the course of j , fo and t of cries after acry stimulation (see Part 1). There seems to be no parallel modification of fo and t if jdecreases in cases of infants at risk of cerebral dysfunction (Table 1). This indicatesthat the jitter is either controlled in a different way than fo and t, or at least is moresensitive to disturbances of the system.b The good correlation between abnormality in j P and outcome at age 2 years seemsto prove our hypothesis concerning the possibility of drawing prognostic conclusionsfrom the jitter-index of pain cry (Table 2). There are, however, some reservations tobe made:

1. The number of patients is small, the prevalence of infants with later developmen-tal problems is high. This might lead to apparently more significant results.2. The result is greatly influenced by the fact that all infants with severeimpairments after follow-up could be correctly diagnosed by intermediate orabnormal j (Table 3). Such infants, however, are usually easily recognized early ininfancy by simple clinical observation. Only seven often with moderate impairments,(a group which we believe is of special interest, because the diagnosis in these infantsis frequently missed or made too late for efficient therapy), are correctly diagnosed byan intermediate or abnormal j . And finally, only 12 out of 20 infants who later onproved to be normally developed, had a normal j . Thus, if we would calculate — thesmall number, however, forbids this — the diagnostic sensitivity would be relativelyhigh, but the specificity would be very low.3. The clinical data alone (Table 4 and 5) lead to expect some developmentalproblems: the frequency of low birth weight and immaturity as well as the necessity ofrespiratory assistance, were higher in the groups of infants, who proved to beimpaired later on.4. Last but not least (Table 4, last line) the postconceptional age at which the cryanalysis was performed differed considerably between the groups with normal andimpaired development. It is possible that the differences are due to maturationalfactors. So far we lack sufficient data on ontogenesis of pain cry jitter, particularly atdefined gestational ages.

Table 3 Distributiongroups)

normal jintermediate jabnormal j

of jitter-indices (j)

normal(n = 20)

602020

in the newborns followed up to 2 years (%

moderateimpairment

(n = 10)

306010

of the follow-up-

severeimpairment

(n = 6)

03366

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Joh

Gut

enbe

rg U

nive

rsita

et]

at 1

5:28

24

Oct

ober

201

4

EARLY CNS DIAGNOSIS BY CRY ANALYSIS 27

Table 4 Birth weight and maturity of the infants followed up at age 2 years (median, range)

birth weight

%<2000g

gestationalage%<32w.

gestationalage at cryrecord

normal(n = 20)

3390(1035-3900)

15

39(31-41)

5

40(35-40)

moderateimpairment

(n = 10)

1880(930-3500)

60

32(27-40)

40

36(33-43)

severeimpairment

(n = 6)

1630(1260-1850)

100

31(29-33)

66

34(33-35)

Table 5 Clinical data of newborns followed up to 2 years (% of the follow-up-groups)

c-section rate

umbilical pH^7,2

Apgar 5' s=7

respiratory assistance

normal(n = 20)

30

44

25

5

moderateimpairment

(n = 10)

33

75

22

22

severeimpairment

(n = 6)

33

25

50

66

Thus, there is no doubt that the results presented here are only preliminary. We are,however, convinced that it is worthwhile to continue the investigations and tocomplete the data and — if the jitter-index should prove not sufficiently predictive —to look for other features hidden in the cry signals.

ReferencesAinsworth, M.D.S. & Wittig, B.A. (1969) Attachment and exploratory behavior of one-year-olds in a

strange situation. In: Determinants of Infant Behaviour, ed. by B.M. Foss, London: Methuen.Bisping, R. (1986) Der Schrei des Neugeborenen: Struktur und Wirkung. In: Lehr- und Forschungstexte

Psychologie, 22, Berlin, Heidelberg, New York, London, Paris, Tokyo: Springer-Verlag, 1-171.Böttcher, M., Grauel, EX., Gross, J. & Syllm-Rapoport, I. (1975) Die Dichte roter Blutzellen als

möglicher Index für die fetale Hypoxie. Ergebn. exp. Med., 19, 267-272.Brazelton, T.B. (1962) Crying in infancy. Pediat., 29, 575-588.Creutzfeldt, O.D. (1983) Cortex cerebri: Leistung, strukturelle und funktionelle Organisation der Hirnrinde. Berlin,

Heidelberg, New York, Tokyo: Springer-Verlag.Fisichelli, V.R. & Karelitz, S. (1963) The cry latencies of normal infants and those with brain damage.J.

Pediat. 62, 724-734.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Joh

Gut

enbe

rg U

nive

rsita

et]

at 1

5:28

24

Oct

ober

201

4

28 E. L. GRÄUEL ET AL.

Gardosik, Th., Ross, P.J. & Singh, S. (1980) Acoustic characteristics of the first cries of infant. In: Infantcommunication: cry and early speech, T. Murry & J. Murry, Houston: College-Hill Press.

Gleiss, J . & Höhn, W. (1968) Das Verhalten beim Schreien nach konstanter Schmerzreizung atemge-sunder und atemgestörter Neugeborener. Dt. Zeitsch. Nervenheilk., 194, 311-317.

Golub, H.L. (1979) A physioacoustic model of the infant cry and its use for medical diagnosis andprognosis. In: Speech communication papers presented at the 97th Meeting of the Acoustical Society ofAmerica, Wolf, J J . & D.H. Klatt, Cambridge, Mass.: Acoustical Soc. of Amer.

Golub, H.L. & Corwin, M.J. (1982) Infant cry: a clue to diagnosis. Pediat., 69, 197-201.Golub, H.L. & Corwin, M.J. (1985) A physioacoustic model of the infant cry. In: Infant crying, B.M. Lester

& C.F.Z. Boukydis, New York, London: Plenum Press.Hollien, H. (1980) Developmental aspects of neonatal vocalizations. In: Infant communication: cry and early

speech, T. Murry & J . Murry, Houston: College-Hill Press.Illingworth, R.S. (1955) Crying in infants and children. Brit. Med. J., 1, 75-77.Irwin, O.C. (1974) Infant speech: consonant sounds according to place of articulation. J. of Speech Dis., 12,

397-401.Karelitz, S. & Fisichelli, V.R. (1962) The cry thresholds of normal infants and those with brain damage. J .

Pediat., 61, 679-685.Kittel, G. (1978) Diagnostische Möglichkeiten durch die frühkindliche Stimme. Union d. Europ. Phoniater,

VII. Kongress 8, 9-12.Lenneberg, E.H., Rebelsky, F.F. & Nichols, I.A. (1965) The vocalizations of infants born to deaf and

hearing parents. Human Develop., 8, 23—37.Lester, B.M. (1976) Spectrum analysis of the cry sounds of well-nourished and malnourished infants. Child

Devel., 47, 237-241.Lester, B.M. (1984) A biosocial model of infant crying. In: Advances in infancy research, ed. by L.P. Lipsitt,

New York: Academic Press.Lester, B.M. & Boukydis, C.F.Z. (1985) Infant crying, New York, London: Plenum Press.Lester, B.M. & Zeskind, P.S. (1978) Brazelton scale and physical size correlates of neonatal cry features.

Infant Behav. Developm., 1, 393-402.Lieberman, Ph., Harris, K.S., Wolff, P. & Russell, L.H. (1971) Newborn infant cry and nonhuman

primate vocalization. J. Speech and Hearing Res., 14, 718—727.Lind, J . (1965) Newborn infant cry. Acta Paediatr. Scand., Suppl. 163, 1-128.Luchsinger, R., Dubois, C , Vassella, F., Joss, E., Gloor, R. & Wiesmann, U. (1967) Spektralanalyse des

"Miauens" bei Cri-du-Chat-Syndrom. Folia phoniat., 19, 27—33.Lüdge, W. & Gips, P. (1989) Microcomputer-aided studies of cry jitter uttered by newborn children based

upon high-resolution analysis of fundamental frequencies. Computer Methods and Programs in Biomed., 28,151-156.

Lüdge, W. & Rothgänger, H. (1990) Computer-aided method to use the fundamental frequency jitter for adiagnosis of central nervous functional disturbance in the perenatal period. Early Child Development andCare (in press).

McMenamin, J.B., Shackelford, G.D. & Volpe, J.J. (1984) Outcome of neonatal intraventricularhemorrhage with periventricular echodense lesions. Ann. Neurol., 15, 285-290.

Menzel, K. & Braun, W. (1974) Serumaktivitäten der LDH, CPK, GOT, GPT und LAP beiasphyktischen Neugeborenen. Acta Paediatr. Acad. Sci. Hung., 15, 241-254.

Menzel, K., Linke, M. & Wiedemann, Gudrun (1984) Kreatinkinase und Kreatinkinase-Isoenzyme imSerum Neugeborener: 1. Einfluss des Geburtsmodus auf Gesamt-CK und CK-B. Zbl. Gynäkol., 106,114-119.

Michelsson, K. (1971) Cry analyses of symptomless low birth weight neonates and of asphyxiated newborninfants. Ada Paediatr. Scand., Suppl. 216, 1-45.

Michelsson, K., Järvenpää, A.-L. & Rinne, A. (1983) Sound spectrographic analysis of pain cry in preterminfants. Early Human Developm., 8, 141-149.

Murry, T. & Murry, J . (1980) Infant Communication: Cry and early speech. Houston: College-Hill Press.Ostwald, P.F., Phibbs, R. & Fox, S. (1968) Diagnostic use of infant cry. Biol. Neonate, 13, 68-82.Papousek, H., Papousek, M. & Giese, R. (1984) Die Anfänge der Eltern-Kind-Beziehung. In:

Psychosomatische Probleme in der Gynäkologie und Geburtshilfe. V. Frick-Bruder & P. Plate, Berlin:Springer-Verlag.

Papousek, M. & Papousek, H. (1989) Stimmliche Kommunikation im frühen Säuglingsalter alsWegbereiter der Sprachentwicklung. In: Handbuch der Kleinkindforschung, H. Keller, Berlin:Springer-Verlag.

Peiper, A. (1963) Die Eigenart der kindlichen Hirntätigkeit. Ed. Leipzig.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Joh

Gut

enbe

rg U

nive

rsita

et]

at 1

5:28

24

Oct

ober

201

4

EARLY CNS DIAGNOSIS BY CRY ANALYSIS 29

Prescott, R. (1975) Infant cry sound: developmental features. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 57, 1186-1191.Ringel, R.L. & Kluppel, D.D. (1964) Neonatal crying: a normative study. Folia Phoniat., 16, 1-9.Rothgänger, H. & Schindler, S. (1988) Analyse des Schmerzschreies von term und preterm geborenen

Säuglingen in Abhängigkeit vom Lebensalter und vom Reifegrad. Wiss. Z. Humboldt-Univ. Berlin, R.Math ./Nat. wiss., 37, 312-315.

Rothgänger, H., Lüdge, W. & Gräuel, E.L. (1989) A new method for infant cry analysis for computer-aided measurement of central neural stimuli and disturbances. Akad. d. Wiss. d. DDR, Mitt, des Zentrums f.wiss. Gerätebau.

Ruth, V.J. & Raivo, K.O. (1988) Perinatal brain damage predictive value of metabolic acidosis and theApgar score. Br. Med.J., 297, 24-27.

Schroeder, H.-J., Schleiermacher, E., Schroeder, T.M. & Bauer, H. (1967) Zur klinischenDifferentialdiagnostik des Cri-du-Chat-Syndroms: Ergebnisse akustischer Analysen des "Katzensch-reies" und ähnlich klingender Säuglingsschreie. Humangenetik, 4, 294-304.

Sedlackovä, E. (1967) Development of the acoustic pattern of the voice and speech in the newborn andinfant. Praha: Academia Ved., 1—111.

Staudt, F., Howieson, J., Benda, G.J., Engel, R.C. (1982) EEG bei Neugeborenen mit intrakraniellenBlutungen: Ein Vergleich mit klinischen Befunden und Computertomogramm. Z. EEG-EMG, 13, 143-147.

Thóden, C.-J. & Koivisto, M. (1980) Acoustic analysis of the normal pain cry. In: Infant communication:cry and early speech. T. Murry & J. Murry, Houston: College-Hill Press.

Tenold, J.L., Crowell, D.H.Jones, R.H., Daniel, T.H., McPherson, D.F. & Popper, A.N. (1974) Cepstraland stationarity analysis of full term and premature infant cries. J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 56, 975—980.

Wasz-Höckert, O., Lind, J., Vuorenkoski, V., Partanen, T. & Valanne, E. (1968) The infant cry. Clin. inDevelop. Med., 29, Lavenham: Spastics International Medical Publ. in Association with WilliamHeinemann Medical Books.

Wermke, K. (1987) Begründung und Nachweis der Eignung des Säuglingsschreies als Indikator fürzentralnervöse Funktionsstörungen des Neugeborenen — Fallstudie unter Einsatz eines speziellenComputerverfahrens. Diss., Berlin, Humboldt-Univ.

Wolff, P.H. (1969) The natural history of crying and other vocalizations in early infancy. In: Determinants ofInfant Behaviour, ed. by B.M. Foss, London: Methuen.

Zeskind, Ph.S. (1985) A developmental perspective of infant crying. In: Infant crying. B.M. Lester & C.F.Z.Boukydis, New York, London: Plenum Press.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Joh

Gut

enbe

rg U

nive

rsita

et]

at 1

5:28

24

Oct

ober

201

4