identification of stimulant misuse and malingering of

165
University of Mississippi University of Mississippi eGrove eGrove Electronic Theses and Dissertations Graduate School 2017 Identification Of Stimulant Misuse And Malingering Of Symptoms Identification Of Stimulant Misuse And Malingering Of Symptoms Of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Sujith Ramachandran University of Mississippi Follow this and additional works at: https://egrove.olemiss.edu/etd Part of the Public Health Commons Recommended Citation Recommended Citation Ramachandran, Sujith, "Identification Of Stimulant Misuse And Malingering Of Symptoms Of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder" (2017). Electronic Theses and Dissertations. 741. https://egrove.olemiss.edu/etd/741 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at eGrove. It has been accepted for inclusion in Electronic Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of eGrove. For more information, please contact [email protected].

Upload: others

Post on 31-Mar-2022

3 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

University of Mississippi University of Mississippi

eGrove eGrove

Electronic Theses and Dissertations Graduate School

2017

Identification Of Stimulant Misuse And Malingering Of Symptoms Identification Of Stimulant Misuse And Malingering Of Symptoms

Of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

Sujith Ramachandran University of Mississippi

Follow this and additional works at: https://egrove.olemiss.edu/etd

Part of the Public Health Commons

Recommended Citation Recommended Citation Ramachandran, Sujith, "Identification Of Stimulant Misuse And Malingering Of Symptoms Of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder" (2017). Electronic Theses and Dissertations. 741. https://egrove.olemiss.edu/etd/741

This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at eGrove. It has been accepted for inclusion in Electronic Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of eGrove. For more information, please contact [email protected].

IDENTIFICATION OF STIMULANT MISUSE AND MALINGERING OF SYMPTOMS OF

ATTENTION DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER

A Dissertation

presented in partial fulfillment of requirements

for the degree of the Doctor of Philosophy

in the Department of Pharmacy Administration

The University of Mississippi

By

Sujith Ramachandran

May 2017

Copyright © 2017 by Sujith Ramachandran

All rights reserved

ii

ABSTRACT

It was estimated in 2013 that 54.3 million individuals reported overall lifetime prevalence

of prescription drug abuse, and 16.7 million individuals reported misusing or abusing

prescription drugs in the past year. This study focuses on the abuse of psychostimulants,

popularly used for treatment of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). It is estimated

that 10 to 30% of college students might be abusing stimulants, mostly for academic purposes.

The incidence of stimulant related ER visits has nearly doubled in the last decade. It is also

estimated that 10 to 50% of ADHD evaluations in a University setting might be exaggerated or

malingered. This study, in three parts, explored the feasibility of identification of prescription

stimulant abuse using large databases, developed a subtle behavioral self-reported scale, the

Subtle ADHD Malingering Screener (SAMS), for use in the primary care setting to identify

malingering among individuals reporting symptoms of ADHD, and compared the sensitivity of

the SAMS to other existing scales. The first paper identified two latent classes in the stimulant

user population based on risk factors for abuse identified from literature. The second paper

developed a 10-item 2-factor screener instrument, the SAMS, with satisfactory reliability and

factorial validity. The third paper calculated a cut-off score for the SAMS, and estimated a

sensitivity of 90.3% and a specificity of 80.1% toward malingering of ADHD symptoms. This

dissertation pursued innovative methods to help in the early identification of prescription

stimulant abusers and malingerers, in order to reduce overdiagnosis of ADHD, and abuse of

prescription stimulants.

iii

LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS AND SYMBOLS

ADHD Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

SAMS Subtle ADHD Malingering Screener

PAI Personality Assessment Inventory – Adult

LCA Latent Class Analysis

PDA Prescription Drug Abuse

iv

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This dissertation was a project in process from the summer of 2014 to the spring of 2017.

Over these two and a half years, it took a small village to complete this project, and I would be

amiss if I failed to acknowledge all the help I have had. I will begin with my beloved advisor, Dr.

John P. Bentley, who has been my mentor, both personally and professionally, and has been the

person I would find refuge in, for all my problems. He has also been my strongest advocate and

my champion while I found my profession, and for this, I will always be thankful to him. The

rest of my committee, Dr. Benjamin F. Banahan, Dr. Erin Holmes, Dr. Meagen Rosenthal, and

Dr. John Young. When this project was merely an idea during the summer and fall of 2014, it

seemed more like a wild dream than a realistic research project, and they gave me the confidence

to pursue it, and most importantly, drove me in a direction which was realistic and achievable.

Without their ideas, support, and help, this dissertation would not have been feasible. I need to

mention a special word of thanks to my mentor & thesis advisor, Dr. Benjamin Banahan, who

has helped me grow into my own self, and find my career. I have much to learn from you, Dr.

Banahan, and I thank you for being a role model for me and several other students.

During these two years, Dr. Amit S. Patel, Dr. Donna West-Strum, Dr. Dwight Wadell,

Nancy Jones, Sheree Jones, and several of my fellow graduate students – Dr. Amod Athavale,

Dr. Manasi Datar, Dr. Zainab Shahpurwala, Dr. Ruchit Shah, Dr. Tristen Jackson, Dr. Nagendra

Punyamurthula, Kaustuv Bhattacharya, Sasi Kiran Nunna, Nilesh Gangan, and Nick Keeling –

have been the sounding board for my ideas, always ready to listen, and bouncing forth ideas that

v

have sometimes changed the very scope of this project, molding it several times over until it

reached its final form, presented here. I look forward to their friendship for several years to

come.

The toughest part of this dissertation were the several challenges we had to face with

regard to feasibility, data collection, resources, and logistics. The person who was most

instrumental throughout this process was Dr. John Young. He has been an incredible resource,

often sharing ideas that have made the seemingly impossible, incredibly reachable. Several

professors, instructors, and graduate students on the campus of the University of Mississippi –

Dr. Tiffany Bensen, Dr. Anne Benson, Dr. Robert Doctor, Dr. Conrad Puozza, Dr. Ahmed

Rashed, Dr. Jason Ritchie, Dr. Matthew Shifflett, Dr. John Wiginton, Amy Beel, Anna Black,

Marie Darden, Kristen Johnson, Brooklee Lightsey, Kelly Peck, Sarah Scott, Alex Sivvopolous,

Lauren Weathers, and Sam Wilson – also helped me during the data collection and item

development phase of this dissertation. Without their assistance, this project would have never

seen the light of day. I sincerely thank them for their willingness to help, and wish them the best

for all future endeavors.

Finally, I would like to my family for their support during the last 5 years. My mom,

Radhika, my dad, Ramachandran, my sister, Sindhu, and her family, have all played bigger roles

in the completion of this project than they will admit. I have much to learn from each of them,

and will always be indebted to them.

vi

TABLE OF CONTENTS

ABSTRACT ........................................................................................................................ ii

LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS AND SYMBOLS .............................................................. iii

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ............................................................................................... iv

LIST OF TABLES ............................................................................................................ xii

CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION TO PRESCRIPTION STIMULANT ABUSE: A REVIEW OF

PREVALENCE, SOCIETAL IMPACT, SOURCES, AND EFFECT OF ABUSE ...... 1

INTRODUCTION .............................................................................................................. 2

Introduction to prescription drug abuse .............................................................. 2

Prevalence of PDA .............................................................................................. 4

Prescription Stimulants ....................................................................................... 5

Epidemiology of stimulant abuse........................................................................ 8

Feigning or malingering of ADHD ................................................................... 13

RESEARCH OBJECTIVES ............................................................................................. 15

BIBLIOGRAPHY ............................................................................................................. 16

CHAPTER 2: PAPER 1: IDENTIFICATION OF PATTERNS OF STIMULANT USE: AN

ANALYSIS OF MEDICAID AND PRESCRIPTION MONITORING PROGRAM DATA

FROM THE STATE OF MISSISSIPPI ....................................................................... 30

ABSTRACT ...................................................................................................................... 32

INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................................ 34

Introduction ....................................................................................................... 34

vii

Characteristics of stimulant misuse .................................................................. 35

Objectives ......................................................................................................... 36

METHODS ....................................................................................................................... 37

Materials & Methods ........................................................................................ 37

Study Design ..................................................................................................... 37

Data Source ....................................................................................................... 37

Timeline ............................................................................................................ 38

Inclusion/Exclusion........................................................................................... 38

Latent Class Indicators & Their Operationalization ......................................... 38

Outcome variables ............................................................................................ 39

Analysis............................................................................................................. 40

RESULTS ......................................................................................................................... 42

Results ............................................................................................................... 42

Demographics ................................................................................................... 42

Latent Classes ................................................................................................... 42

Prediction of Stimulant Abuse .......................................................................... 44

DISCUSSION ................................................................................................................... 47

Discussion ......................................................................................................... 47

Limitations & Conclusions ............................................................................... 48

BIBLIOGRAPHY ............................................................................................................. 50

viii

CHAPTER 3: PAPER 2: DEVELOPMENT OF THE SUBTLE ADHD MALINGERING

SCREENER (SAMS) FOR IDENTIFICATION OF INDIVIDUALS MALINGERING

ATTENTION DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER ......................................... 56

ABSTRACT ...................................................................................................................... 58

INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................................ 59

Introduction ....................................................................................................... 59

Malingering of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) ................. 59

Detection of Malingering .................................................................................. 60

Objective ........................................................................................................... 61

METHODS ....................................................................................................................... 62

Methods............................................................................................................. 62

Study Design ..................................................................................................... 62

Item Development ............................................................................................. 62

Experimental Design ......................................................................................... 63

Pretest ................................................................................................................ 64

Participants & Study Recruitment .................................................................... 65

ANALYSIS ....................................................................................................................... 66

Analysis............................................................................................................. 66

RESULTS ......................................................................................................................... 68

Results ............................................................................................................... 68

Sample Characteristics ...................................................................................... 68

ix

Manipulation Checks ........................................................................................ 71

SAMS Items ...................................................................................................... 72

Psychometric Items ........................................................................................... 73

DISCUSSION ................................................................................................................... 75

Discussion ......................................................................................................... 75

Limitations ........................................................................................................ 77

Clinical Implications and Future Research ....................................................... 77

BIBLIOGRAPHY ............................................................................................................. 79

APPENDIX ....................................................................................................................... 88

APPENDIX – A: Malingering Group Instructions ........................................... 89

CHAPTER 4: PAPER 3: PSYCHOMETRIC VALIDATION OF THE SUBTLE ADHD

MALINGERING SCALE (SAMS) AND COMPARISON OF DIAGNOSTIC ACCURACY

WITH THE PERSONALITY ASSESSMENT INVENTORY (PAI) ......................... 90

ABSTRACT ...................................................................................................................... 92

INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................................ 94

Introduction ....................................................................................................... 94

Challenges in Detection of ADHD Malingering .............................................. 94

Subtle Scales ..................................................................................................... 95

Objective ........................................................................................................... 96

METHODS ....................................................................................................................... 97

Methods............................................................................................................. 97

x

Study Design ..................................................................................................... 97

Study Participants and Data Collection ............................................................ 97

Survey Design ................................................................................................... 98

Data Analysis .................................................................................................... 98

Selecting a Cut-off Point ................................................................................... 99

Comparison with Pre-existing Scales ............................................................. 100

RESULTS ....................................................................................................................... 101

Results ............................................................................................................. 101

Demographics Characteristics ........................................................................ 101

Estimation of a Cut-off Score & Psychometric Indices .................................. 103

Comparator Scale ............................................................................................ 106

DISCUSSION ................................................................................................................. 108

Discussion ....................................................................................................... 108

Limitations ...................................................................................................... 109

Clinical Implications & Future Research ........................................................ 110

BIBLIOGRAPHY ........................................................................................................... 111

CHAPTER 5: SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, AND FUTURE RESEARCH ............. 116

CONCLUSIONS............................................................................................................. 117

Concluding Comments on Paper One ............................................................. 118

Concluding Comments on Paper Two ............................................................ 119

Concluding Comments on Paper Three .......................................................... 120

xi

IMPLICATIONS ............................................................................................................ 121

Implications for Research & Practice ............................................................. 121

BIBLIOGRAPHY ........................................................................................................... 123

CURRICULUM VITAE ................................................................................................. 145

xii

LIST OF TABLES

Table A1: Demographics of the sample, and distribution across latent classes ............... 43

Table A2: Prediction of dependent/nondependent prescription stimulant abuse.............. 45

Table B1: Demographics of the sample ............................................................................ 68

Table B2: Differences between the three groups among the items used in the

Manipulation ..................................................................................................................... 72

Table B3: Mean sum scale scores on each factor of the Subtle ADHD Malingering

Screener (SAMS) in each study group.............................................................................. 73

Table B4: Means, standard deviations, and effect sizes for each of the three study

groups on each item in the Subtle ADHD Malingering Scale (SAMS) ........................... 74

Table C1: Demographics of the sample .......................................................................... 102

Table C2: Classification accuracy of the SAMS and PAI in each study group .............. 106

1

CHAPTER 1

INTRODUCTION TO PRESCRIPTION STIMULANT ABUSE: A REVIEW OF

PREVALENCE, SOCIETAL IMPACT, SOURCES, AND EFFECT OF ABUSE

2

INTRODUCTION

Introduction to prescription drug abuse

The United States’ War on Drugs is an extensive legal, economic, and sociocultural effort

to change the fact that a nation that comprises only 5% of the world’s population consumes over

25% of its illicit drugs (Zedido & Wheeler, 2012). The war on the use of illicit drugs has been

mostly inefficient and ineffective, and has overshadowed the problem of abuse of prescription

drugs (Zedido & Wheeler, 2012). An initial spotlight on prescription drugs was brought by the

White House Conference on Prescription Drug Abuse in 1980 that pointed out the high rates of

illness and death associated with misuse and abuse of prescription drugs (Wilford et al., 1994).

While some efforts were made to address this issue by the White House and the American

Medical Association (AMA), the problem has only grown worse (Wilford et al., 1994). In 2011,

the Office of National Drug Control Policy declared prescription drug abuse a public health crisis

in the US (ONDCP, 2011).

To facilitate a discussion of prevalence and to explore ways to mitigate this growing

problem of Prescription Drug Abuse (PDA), a thorough understanding of the terms used to

describe PDA is necessary. According to the Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services

Administration (2007), nonmedical use of a psychotherapeutic drug is defined as “use of a

prescription medication that was not prescribed for the user or taking a drug only for the

experience or feeling it may cause.” Compton & Volkow (2006) defined prescription drug abuse

as, “any intentional use of a medication with intoxicating properties outside of a physician’s

3

prescription for a bonafide medical condition, excluding accidental use.” By using the term

“intoxicating properties” Compton & Volkow (2006) excluded classes of medications abused for

reasons other than euphoria, such as prescription stimulants, which the Division of Population

Surveys had included with its “experience or feeling” definition. These two definitions

demonstrate the range of definitions for PDA in extant literature and the disagreements between

them. Boyd & McCabe (2008) explain that defining abuse can be challenging because misuse of

controlled prescription medications can happen by people who misuse someone else’s

prescription and also by those who misuse their own prescriptions without a physician’s

knowledge. Recognizing the problem of defining such diverse behavior, Smith et al. (2013)

conducted a systematic review of definitions of all forms of “misuse, abuse and related events”

and proposed their own definitions for each type of activity. They clarify that a misuse event

signifies any intentional therapeutic use of a drug product in an inappropriate way; and an abuse

event signifies any intentional, non-therapeutic use of a drug product, even once, for the purpose

of achieving a desirable psychological or physiological effect. This definition of misuse is broad,

it allows researchers to capture various forms of misuse, based on the type of medication being

misused, the motives and techniques for misuse, and the methods of acquiring the medication.

According to Boyd & McCabe (2008), there are four types of PDA possible depending on

whether the prescription belong to the abuser or someone else, and whether the motivation for

use was for the purpose of self-treatment or euphoria.

The focus of this study is on the abuse or misuse of psychostimulants commonly

prescribed for diagnoses such as Attention Deficit Disorder (ADD) and Attention Deficit

Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Stimulant abuse can occur in multiple forms such as the abuse

of Schedule II stimulants such as cocaine – which are mostly illegally manufactured, rather than

4

diverted from legal prescriptions – or the abuse of Schedule I stimulants such as ecstasy or 3, 4 –

methylenedioxy – methamphetamine (MDMA) – which are not available legally. This study will

focus on abuse of drugs which are available legally and are mostly acquired through prescription

diversion. Any further mention of prescription stimulants in this document will refer to the drugs

used to treat ADHD, unless specifically mentioned. The specific goals of this dissertation will be

to help develop strategies to prevent abuse of prescription psychostimulants by identifying

individuals who attempt to acquire prescriptions for the purpose of misuse, abuse or drug

diversion. Over the course of chapters 2 to 4, this dissertation will attempt to identify individuals

misusing prescription stimulants using a latent class analysis of known risk factors from

administrative claims data, and by developing a subtle scale that can be administered in the

physician’s office during the diagnosis of ADHD or ADD. A combination of both of these

techniques, refined by thorough careful study, will help curb misuse or abuse of prescription

stimulants. The rest of this chapter will deal with the impact of prescription drug abuse (with a

special focus on stimulant abuse), the prevalence of stimulant abuse, intentions of stimulant

abuse, a review of their impact on academic performance, and commonly available sources of

stimulants for abuse.

Prevalence of PDA

It was estimated in 2013 that 54.3 million individuals reported overall lifetime prevalence

of PDA, and 16.7 million individuals reported misusing or abusing prescription drugs in the past

year. Of the 16.7 million individuals misusing prescription drugs, over 12.5 million were using

opioids, 6 million were using tranquilizers and 3.3 million were using stimulants. The number

individuals misusing prescription drugs was greater than the number of illicit drug users

5

combined (such as heroin, cocaine, ecstasy, and hallucinogens), and is second only to the

prevalence of marijuana use (SAMHSA, 2013a). PDA cost the system $181 billion in 2002

(Manchikanti, 2006; ONDCP, 2004). The prevalence of PDA has increased by more than 250%

in the last 2 decades. Between 1992 and 2003, there was an 94% increase in the number of adults

abusing controlled substances (Bollinger et al., 2005). The impact of this increase can be seen in

the change in frequency of hospitalizations and emergency room visits attributed to PDA.

According to the Drug Abuse Warning Network (DAWN), between the years 2004 and 2008,

medical emergencies due to PDA increased 81% and emergency room visits increased 97%. In

2008 alone, 54% of all drug related ER visits were caused by PDA (SAMHSA, 2011).

In recent years, a new group of individuals, i.e., young adults enrolled in colleges and

schools, has emerged as a prominent abuse population. Between 1992 and 2003, there was a

212% increase in the number of 12 to 17 years olds abusing controlled substances, whereas the

prevalence among adults grew by only 85% (Bollinger et al., 2005). About 13% of 18 to 25 year

olds engage in PDA, compared to 7% in the 12 to 17 year age group (NIDA, 2013).

Prescription Stimulants

Stimulants are a class of psychoactive drugs that can improve cognition. They include

illegal substances such as cocaine, and crystal meth; and legal prescription drugs such as

Adderall (dextrompehtammine), Ritalin (methylphenidate), and Concerta (methyphenidate).

They are commonly prescribed to treat symptoms of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

(ADHD). Stimulants are known to reduce hyperactivity, impulsivity, inattentiveness, socially

aggressive behavior, and increase vigilance, reaction time, task persistence, productivity,

6

working memory, handwriting, fine motor speed, energy levels, alertness, self-esteem,

coordination, elevated mood, reduced appetite, and extended wakefulness among those who truly

have ADHD (Barkley et al., 2003; Zullig & Divin, 2012). After a thorough review of the effect

of stimulants on various characteristics related to academic performance, Smith & Farah (2011)

conclude that stimulants enhance declarative learning, working memory, cognitive control in

some individuals and might possibly cause cognitive enhancement in some others, but these

effects were too small to actually make a difference. However, most of these conclusions stem

from studies involving students with ADHD, meaning these results cannot be extended to

students without ADHD who might be misusing stimulants.

While the positive effects of stimulants on academic performance, in individuals with or

without ADHD, lacks substantial evidence, stimulants are known to cause side-effects. The

commonly mentioned side-effects include insomnia, headaches, irritability, nervousness, tics,

significant weight loss, anxiety, anorexia, gastrointestinal distress, hallucinations (in the form of

amphetamine psychosis), talkativeness, agitation, anger, paranoia, delusions, personality

changes, mood swings, cardiovascular complications, and hypertension (Barkley et al., 2003;

Hamilton, 2009; White, Becker-Blease & Grace-Bishop, 2006). Charach, Ickowicz & Schachar

(2006) conducted a 5-year cohort study and found that while stimulants can improve symptoms

of ADHD for up to 5 years in children, adverse effects tend to persist over the longer term. The

FDA has also issued a blackbox warning for cardiovascular complications for prescription

stimulants. The use of stimulants, even if taken legitimately, can result in increased Emergency

Room (ER) visits (Miller et al., 2004). The number of stimulant-related ER visits have actually

more than doubled between 2005 and 2010, and are responsible for approximately 40,000 ER

visits per year (SAMHSA, 2013b).

7

While long-term data on effects of stimulant use is scarce, two effects have been studied

by many researchers. The first is the effect of stimulant use on future substance abuse. Some

studies have shown that use of stimulants is correlated with future substance abuse (Barkley et al,

2003). Other studies show that ADHD medication actually has a protective effect against future

substance abuse (Biederman, 1999), while yet others have found no relationship between the two

(Molina et al., 2013; Kaloyanides et al., 2007). The problem with most of these studies is that

they cannot account for the bias of the abuse liability of the ADHD patient independent from the

ADHD medication. In other words, they cannot assess how likely the subject would be to engage

in substance abuse had he not received the ADHD prescription. Therefore, while available

evidence raises some concerns about future effect of stimulant use, it is mostly inconclusive.

The second long-term effect of stimulants concerning younger patients is that of effect on

growth rates. Several researchers have found that long-term use of stimulants can cause stunted

growth rates among children (Toomey et al., 2003; Swanson et al., 2006; Charach et al., 2006;

Swanson et al., 2007). In addition to the negative effect on growth rates, it was also found that

children taking stimulants do not show rebound growth after drug use is discontinued (Swanson

et al., 2007). The only study that did not find a significant effect of stimulant use on long-term

growth rates was limited by a small sample size (Zachor et al., 2006). Despite these side-effects,

White, Becker-Blease & Grace-Bishop (2006) report that about 79% of those taking stimulants

report that they have no concern about stimulant abuse.

Abuse of stimulants can also cause depression (Teter et al., 2010) and addiction or drug

dependence (White, Becker-Blease, & Grace-Bishop, 2006). Prescription stimulants used for

treatment of ADHD are classified as a Schedule II controlled substance because they have a high

abuse liability, and cause withdrawal effects such as depression, anxiety, and severe fatigue

8

(Cohen, 2013). Manchikanti (2006) summarizes that prescription stimulants like

methylphenidate have a lot in common with cocaine. They both bind to the similar sites in the

brain and cause an increase in dopamine levels which leads to euphoria. While oral doses of

stimulants might not cause a rush of dopamine equivalent to that of cocaine, it is reported that

high doses of stimulants, taken by oral or intranasal routes, can cause high risk of addiction. Co-

ingestion with other drugs can also increase abuse liability (White, Becker-Blease & Grace-

Bishop, 2006; Volkow & Swanson, 2003). Smith & Farah (2011) found that 1 in 20 users of

stimulants actually meet the criteria for being drug dependent. Whiteside et al. (2015) found that

21.8% of those taking stimulants had a strong desire to misuse on a weekly/daily basis and 8.6%

had health, social, legal or financial problems which they would consider secondary to stimulant

abuse.

Epidemiology of stimulant abuse

The National Institute on Drug Abuse identifies individuals between the ages of 18 and

25 years as a high risk group for abuse of prescription drugs (NIDA, 2013). While opioids,

marijuana and stimulants are all commonly abused in the college population, research suggests

that stimulants are the most frequently abused drugs (Babcock & Bryne, 2000). McCabe et al.

(2014) provide evidence that the problem of stimulant abuse is actually of greater concern in the

young adult age group than other drugs. In the decade from 2003 to 2013, the use of opioids

decreased among college-aged individuals and use of stimulants and frequency of use of

stimulants increased in the same time period. They also found that high frequency use (3 or more

occasions) was normative among stimulants whereas low frequency use (1 or 2 occasions) was

normative among sedatives and opioids.

9

The true prevalence of prescription stimulant abuse in college and university settings is

difficult to measure. A 2012 systematic review concluded that most studies found less than 5%

stimulant abuse in the 12 to 17 year old age group (Young, Glover & Havens, 2012). In the 18

to 25 year old age group, many studies report a prevalence of 4 to 8% (Frankenberger, 1990;

Zullig & Divin, 2012; Poulin, 2001; Teter et al., 2005; Johnston et al., 2014; Whiteside et al.,

2015; Novak et al., 2007); while others found a prevalence of 10% or higher in the same

population (LeFever & Dawson, 1999; Rowland et al., 2002; Johnston et al., 2005; Marsh et al.,

2000; McCabe & West, 2013; White et al. 2006; Babcock & Bryne, 2000; Hall et al., 2005;

Bavarian et al., 2013). Two longitudinal studies were conducted in this population and both

reported higher rates than any of these other studies. Garnier-Dysktra et al. (2012) found that by

the end of four years in college, 61.8% of students were offered stimulants and over 31% of them

accepted. As part of a 10-year longitudinal study, Wilens et al. (2006) found that only 56% of

students who were taking prescription stimulants actually had ADHD.

Most of these estimates of prevalence of misuse or abuse of stimulants are obtained from

direct online surveys where participants are asked to disclose any illicit use behavior. Because

such use is considered unacceptable and, many times, these drugs are obtained illegally by

prescription diversion, individuals may not be willing to disclose their behavior. Dietz et al.

(2013) found that direct online surveys can only present an underestimate of true abuse

prevalence. They administered a survey in Germany using an innovative technique that ensures

respondent privacy, called Randomized Response Estimates, and found that the true prevalence

of using stimulants and other cognitive-enhancing drugs is closer to 20%.

The recent reports of increased levels of stimulant abuse are supported by the fact that the

number of prescriptions being written for stimulants has also grown steadily (Fortuna et al.,

10

2010; Paulozzi, Mack & Hockenberg, 2014). The prevalence of ADHD, usually estimated at 3 to

7%, has also increased in the past few years (Visser et al., 2010). The CDC reports that

legitimate use of stimulants for children under 18 has increased 5 times in the period from 1988-

1994 to 2007–2010 (National Center for Health Statistics, 2014). And while 75% to 97% of

actual ADHD patients respond positively to stimulant treatment (Barkley et al., 2003), ADHD

may also be over-diagnosed (Kube, 2002). Angold et al. (2000) found that only 3.4% of their

student sample actually had ADHD, but about 7.3% of them had prescriptions to treat ADHD.

With a mean score of 3.54 on a scale of 1 to 5, a college student population surveyed by Hall et

al. (2005) agrees that ADHD is over-diagnosed in the college population. Zgierska, Miller and

Rabago (2012) explain that the rise in rate of prescriptions for stimulants might also be because

patients are becoming more aware of stimulants and want to experiment with them, while

physicians are under increased pressure to prescribe these medications to keep their patients

satisfied.

The most common reason for misuse of stimulants is to improve academic performance

or concentration. Manchikanti (2006) reports that 43% of students use stimulants to help with

their school work, 31% use them to deal with other problems, and 22% use them to get high.

Hamilton (2009) reports that the most common motives were concentration and alertness. In a

longitudinal study across four years of college, Garnier-Dysktra et al. (2012) found that curiosity

was the primary motive in early years, but academic performance becomes the primary motive

later in college. Teter et al. (2005) also found concentration to be the most common motive for

stimulant misuse. Interestingly, they found that the respondents who had reported using

stimulants “only to study” also reported use of alcohol and other drugs, used to get high, as often

as those who used stimulants “to get high”, suggesting that very few individuals might actually

11

be using stimulants solely for the purpose of academic improvement. Other motives for abuse or

misuse as reported by Teter et al. (2005), in their systematic review, include use as a study-aid

for all-nighters, euphoria, recreational tool in combination with alcohol, and as a reinforcement

tool. The academic motives also explain why young adults enrolled in colleges were more likely

to abuse stimulants than those not enrolled in educational institutions (Teter et al., 2005). Only

14% of stimulant abusers thought that stimulants lead to long-term academic achievements (Hall

et al., 2005).

Hall et al. (2005) found that 27% of students took stimulants during finals week, 15.5%

took them before tests, and 12% took them when they “partied”. A 2010 National Survey of

Counselling Centers found that 91% of college counselling directors believe that many students

are facing severe psychological problems on campus. The prevalence of mental health illnesses

on campus is rising and the odds of abusing stimulants among students who reported feelings of

hopelessness, sadness, depression, and suicide attempts were 1.2 to 1.4 times that of students

who did not report these incidents (Zullig & Divin, 2012). In fact, Arria & Dupont (2010)

recommend that stimulant abuse can be used as a marker for academic difficulties and mental

health problems.

A study of the sources of drugs for abusers of stimulants found that 70% of abusers began

use after starting college. Of the students abusing stimulants in college, 87.1% acquired drugs

from friends who had prescriptions, 30.4% obtained them from acquaintances and 26.4% abused

their own medications without the knowledge of their prescriber (Bavarian et al., 2013). Maxwell

(2011) also found that obtaining drugs from a friend or relative was the most common way to

acquire the drug (55%). They found that 18% of individuals abused their own prescription, 5%

obtained drugs from a drug dealer or a stranger and 0.4% used the Internet to order their

12

medications. Other researchers have also found that obtaining drugs from a friend or a relative is

by far the most common way for obtaining prescription drugs for the purpose of abuse

(Lessenger & Feinberg, 2008; McCabe et al., 2014). More than half of college students thought it

was easy or somewhat easy to acquire stimulants for illicit use and they obtained it at a cost of $1

to $5 per pill (White, Becker-Blease and Grace-Bishop, 2006; Hall et al., 2005). Medication

diversion, or the practice of using prescription medications for purposes other than their intended

use, such as for selling or giving away, is incredibly common with stimulants. Anywhere from

25% to 54% of individuals with abuse-liable prescriptions are approached for diversion of their

medication (McHugh, Neilsen & Weiss, 2015; McCabe et al., 2014; McCabe et al., 2004, 2006;

Poulin, 2001; Moline & Frankenberger, 2001). Wilens et al. (2006) found that 11% of

individuals with legitimate ADHD prescriptions diverted their meds. Levine & Coupey (2009)

found that 25% of students with a legitimate prescription gave away their medications.

In a longitudinal study of stimulant misuse behaviors, Garnier-Dykstra et al. (2012) found

that rates of misuse of one’s own prescription increased in later years in college. They suggest

that this effect was possible because “upper classmen might seek out a diagnosis of ADHD as a

means of ensuring a steady supply of prescription stimulants.” When Angold et al. (2000) found

that more individuals had been prescribed stimulants than those who actually had ADHD, they

explained that it was due to over-diagnosis, while it might very well also be explained by

individuals who were faking ADHD for the sake of a prescription. White, Becker-Blease and

Grace-Bishop (2006) explain that in order to curb stimulant abuse, attention should be paid to

“whether students might fake ADHD or ADD symptoms to obtain stimulants for planned misuse

or abuse or for resale.”

13

Feigning or malingering of ADHD

Drug seeking behavior among stimulant abusers often manifests itself as students

attempting to malinger attention deficit disorders in order to obtain legal prescriptions for

stimulants. Hence, the ability of health practitioner to identify drug seeking behavior is crucial to

decreasing drug abuse. However, in a study of opioid abuse, Weiner et al. (2013) found that

physicians in the emergency room had a sensitivity of 63.2%, specificity of 72.7%, and positive

predictive value of 41.2% for identifying drug seeking behavior. It is reported that only 19% of

physicians were trained in school to identify drug diversion and only 40% of them were trained

to identify PDA and addiction. In contrast, about 50% of pharmacists are trained to identify drug

diversion or PDA and addiction. In addition, 43% of physicians do not ask about drug abuse

when taking a patient’s history (Bollinger et al., 2005).

Several studies show that 10% to 50% of students evaluated for ADHD in a University

setting might be exaggerating or feigning their symptoms (Suhr et al., 2008; Sullivan, May &

Galbally, 2007; Jasinksi & Ranseen, 2011; Harrison & Edwards, 2010; Pella et al., 2011). The

promise of incentives such as access to stimulants, which can be misused or sold, and academic

accommodations, provided to students diagnosed with learning disorder, and the decreasing

stigma associated with rising awareness of the condition, has caused increasingly more students

to feign or exaggerate their symptoms in order to be diagnosed with ADHD (Slick, Sherman &

Iverson, 1999). There exists no standardized, statistically valid method for detection of

malingering of ADHD (Harrison, Edwards & Parker, 2007), making it very easy to fake

responses to appear ADHD-like on most ADHD diagnostic scales (Booksh et al., 2010; Harrison,

& Edwards, 2010; Quinn, 2003). Jasinksi & Ranseen (2011) compiled evidence to show that

ADHD can be faked very easily during physician interviews, on self-report inventories, observer

14

symptom ratings, cognitive measures and even tests of memory, executive function and

attention. Symptom Validity Tests (SVT) provide the best opportunity at identifying malingerers

among existing methods. However, even they suffer from poor sensitivity (35% to 63%,

depending on the test) and are time intensive, and expensive (Jasinksi et al., 2011; Marshall et

al., 2010; Jasinksi & Ranseen, 2011).

15

RESEARCH OBJECTIVES

In order to better control the abuse of prescription stimulants among young adults

enrolled in colleges, there exists a need to understand the stimulant use behavior, and to identify

the individuals who attempt to feign symptoms of ADHD in order to gain access to the

medication. The goal of this diisertation is to attempt to identify individuals misusing stimulants

and prevent stimulant abuse by developing an innovative methodology to identify early abusers.

The specific aims of this dissertation are

1. To explore the feasibility of identification of prescription stimulant abuse using large

databases (Paper 1).

2. To develop a subtle behavioral self-reported scale, the Subtle ADHD Malingering Screener

(SAMS), for use in the primary care setting to identify malingering among individuals

diagnosed with ADHD (Paper 2).

3. To test the ability of the newly developed scale, SAMS, to detect faking among adults

diagnosed with ADHD and compare it to other existing scales (Paper 3).

16

BIBLIOGRAPHY

17

Angold, A, Erkanli, A., Egger, H. L., Costello, J. (2000). Stimulant treatment for children: a

community perspective. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent

Psychiatry, 39, 975–984.

Arria, A. M., & DuPont, R. L. (2010). Nonmedical prescription stimulant use among college

students: Why we need to do something and what we need to do. Journal of Addictive

Diseases, 29, 417–426.

Babcock, Q., Byrne, T. (2000). Student perceptions of methylphenidate abuse at a public liberal

arts college. Journal of American College Health, 49, 143–148.

Barkley, R. A., Fischer, M., Smallish, L., & Fletcher, K. (2003). Does the treatment of attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder with stimulants contribute to drug use/abuse? A 13-year

prospective study. Pediatrics, 111(1), 97-109.

Bavarian, N., Flay, B. R., Ketcham, P. L., & Smith, E. (2013). Illicit use of prescription

stimulants in a college student sample: A theory-guided analysis. Drug and alcohol

dependence, 132(3), 665-673.

Biederman, J., Wilens, T., Mick, E., Spencer, T. J., Faraone, S. V. (1999). Pharmacotherapy of

attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder reduces risk for substance use disorder. Pediatrics,

104:e20.

18

Birnbaum, H. G., White, A. G., Schiller, M., Waldman, T., Cleveland, J. M., & Roland, C. L.

(2011). Societal costs of prescription opioid abuse, dependence, and misuse in the United

States. Pain Medicine, 12(4), 657-667.

Bollinger, L. C., Bush, C., Califano, J. A., et al. (2005). Under the counter: The diversion and

abuse of controlled prescription drugs in the U.S. The National Center on Addiction and

Substance Abuse at Columbia University (CASA). Available at:

http://www.casacolumbia.org/addiction-research/reports/under-the-counter-diversion-

abuse-controlled-perscription-drugs. Accessed on: July 9th 2015.

Booksh, R. L., Pella, R. D., Singh, A. N., & Gouvier, W. D. (2010). Ability of college students to

simulate ADHD on objective measures of attention. Journal of Attention Disorders.

13(4), 325-338.

Boyd, C. J., & McCabe, S. E. (2008). Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and

Policy. Substance abuse treatment, prevention, and policy, 3, 22.

Charach, A., Figueroa, M., Chen, S., Ickowicz, A., & Schachar, R. (2006). Stimulant treatment

over 5 years: effects on growth. Journal of the American Academy of Child &

Adolescent Psychiatry, 45(4), 415-421.

Cohen, M. J. (2013). Off-Label: Combating the Dangerous Overprescription of Amphetamines

to Children. The George Washington Law Review, 82, 174.

Compton, W. M., & Volkow, N. D. (2006). Abuse of prescription drugs and the risk of

addiction. Drug and alcohol dependence, 83, S4-S7.

19

Dietz, P., Striegel, H., Franke, A.G., Lieb, K., Simon, P., & Ulrich, R. (2013). Randomized

response estimates for the 12‐month prevalence of cognitive‐enhancing drug use in

university students. Pharmacotherapy: The Journal of Human Pharmacology and Drug

Therapy, 33(1), 44-50.

Fortuna, R. J., Robbins, B. W., Caiola, E., Joynt, M., & Halterman, J. S. (2010). Prescribing of

controlled medications to adolescents and young adults in the United States. Pediatrics,

126(6), 1108–1116, Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2010-0791. Accessed on

July 9th 2015.

Frankenberger, W., Lozar, B., Dallas, P. (1990). The use of stimulant medication to treat

attention deficit hyperactive disorder (ADHD) in elementary school children. Develop

Disabilities Bulletin, 18, 1–13.

Garnier-Dykstra, L. M., Caldeira, K. M., Vincent, K. B., O’Grady, K. E., & Arria, A. M. (2012).

Nonmedical use of prescription stimulants during college: Four-year trends in exposure

opportunity, use, motives, and sources. Journal of American College Health, 60(3), 226-

234.

Hall, K. M., Irwin, M. M., Bowman, K. A., Frankenberger, W., & Jewett, D. C. (2005). Illicit use

of prescribed stimulant medication among college students. Journal of American College

Health, 53(4), 167-174.

Hamilton, G. J. (2009). Prescription drug abuse. Psychology in the Schools, 46(9), 892-898.

Hansen, R. N., Oster, G., Edelsberg, J., Woody, G. E., & Sullivan, S. D. (2011). Economic costs

of nonmedical use of prescription opioids. The Clinical journal of pain, 27(3), 194-202.

20

Harrison, A. G., & Edwards, M. J. (2010). Symptom exaggeration in post-secondary students:

Preliminary base rates in a Canadian sample. Applied Neuropsychology, 17(2), 135-143.

Harrison, A. G., Edwards, M. J., & Parker, K. C. H. (2007). Identifying students faking ADHD:

Preliminary strategies for detection. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22, 577-588.

Jasinski, L. J., & Ranseen, J. D. (2011). Malingered ADHD evaluations: A further complication

for accommodations reviews. The Bar Examiner, 79, 6-16.

Jasinski, L. J., Harp, J. P., Berry, D. T., Shandera-Ochsner, A. L., Mason, L. H., & Ranseen, J.

D. (2011). Using symptom validity tests to detect malingered ADHD in college

students. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 25(8), 1415-1428.

Johnston, L. D., O’Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., Schulenberg, J. E. (2005). Monitoring the

Future National Survey Results on Drug Use, 1975–2004, vol. I: Secondary School

Students (NIH Publication No. 05-5727). National Institute on Drug Abuse, Bethesda,

MA, 680 pp.

Johnston, L. D., O’Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., & Schulenberg, J. E. (2011). Monitoring the

Future National Survey Results on Drug Use, 1975-2012: Volume 2, College Students

and Adults Ages 19-50. Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research, The University of

Michigan.

Johnston, L. D., O'Malley, P. M., Miech, R. A., Bachman, J. G., & Schulenberg, J. E. (2014).

Monitoring the future national results on adolescents drug use: 1975–2013 overview, key

findings on adolescent drug use. Ann Arbor: Institute for Social Research, The University

of Michigan.

21

Kaloyanides, K. B., McCabe, S. E., Cranford, J. A., & Teter, C. J. (2007). Prevalence of illicit

use and abuse of prescription stimulants, alcohol, and other drugs among college

students: relationship with age at initiation of prescription stimulants. Pharmacotherapy:

The Journal of Human Pharmacology and Drug Therapy, 27(5), 666-674.

Kube, D. A., Peterson, M. C., Palmer, F. B. (2002). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder:

comorbidity and medication use. Clinical Pediatrics, 41, 461–470.

LeFever, G., Dawson, K. (1999). The extent of drug therapy for attention deficit hyperactivity

disorder among children in public schools. American Journal of Public Health, 89, 1359–

1364.

Lessenger, J. E., & Feinberg, S. D. (2008). Abuse of prescription and over-the-counter

medications. Journal of the American Board of Family Medicine, 21(1), 45 – 54.

Levine, S. B., Coupey, S. M. (2009). Nonmedical use of prescription medications: An emerging

risk behavior among rural adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 44, 407–9.

Manchikanti, L. (2006). Prescription drug abuse: what is being done to address this new drug

epidemic? Testimony before the Subcommittee on Criminal Justice, Drug Policy and

Human Resources. Pain Physician, 9(4), 287.

Marsh, L. D., Key, J. D., Payne, T. P. (2000). Methylphenidate misuse in substance abusing

adolescents. Journal of Child & Adolescent Substance Abuse, 9, 1-14.

Marshall, P., Schroeder, R., O’Brien, J., et al. (2010) Effectiveness of symptom validity

measures in identifying cognitive and behavioral symptom exaggeration in adult attention

deficit hyperactivity disorder. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 24(7), 1204-1237.

22

Maxwell, J. C. (2011). The prescription drug epidemic in the United States: a perfect

storm. Drug and alcohol review, 30(3), 264-270.

McCabe, S. E., Teter, C. J., & Boyd, C. J. (2004). The use, misuse and diversion of prescription

stimulants among middle and high school students. Substance Use & Misuse, 39(7), 1095

– 1116.

McCabe, S. E., Teter, C. J., & Boyd, C. J. (2006). Medical use, illicit use and diversion of

prescription stimulant medication. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 38(1), 43 – 56.

McCabe, S. E., & West, B. T. (2013). Medical and nonmedical use of prescription stimulants:

Results from a national multicohort study. Journal of the American Academy of Child

and Adolescent Psychiatry, 52, 1272–1280, Available at:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2013.09.005. Accessed on: July 9th 2015.

McCabe, S. E., West, B. T., Teter, C. J., & Boyd, C. J. (2014). Trends in medical use, diversion,

and nonmedical use of prescription medications among college students from 2003 to

2013: Connecting the dots. Addictive behaviors, 39(7), 1176-1182.

McHugh, R. K., Nielsen, S., & Weiss, R, D. (2015) Prescription drug abuse: from epidemiology

to public policy. Journal of substance abuse treatment, 48(1), 1-7.

Miller, A. R., Brehaut, J. C., Raina, P., McGrail, K. M., & Armstrong, R. W. (2004). Use of

medical services by methylphenidate-treated children in the general population.

Ambulatory Pediatrics, 4(2), 174–180, Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1367/A03-

031R1.1. Accessed on: July 9th 2015.

23

Miller, T., Novak, S. P., Galvin, D. M., Spicer, R. S., Cluff, L., & Kasat, S. (2015). School and

Work Status, Drug-Free Workplace Protections, and Prescription Drug Misuse Among

Americans Ages 15–25. Journal of studies on alcohol and drugs, 76(2), 195–203.

Molina, B. S., Hinshaw, S. P., Arnold, L. E., et al. (2013) Adolescent substance use in the

multimodal treatment study of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (MTA) as

a function of childhood ADHD, random assignment to childhood treatments, and

subsequent medication. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent

Psychiatry, 52(3), 250-263.

Moline, S., Frankenberger, W. (2001) Use of stimulant medication for treatment of attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A survey of middle and high school students’ attitudes.

Psychology in the School, 38, 1–16.

National Center for Health Statistics. (2014). Health, United States, 2014: With Special Feature

on Prescription Drugs. Hyattsville, MD. 2014. Available at:

http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/hus/hus14.pdf. Accessed on: July 9th 2015.

National Institute on Drug Abuse. (2013). Abuse of prescription (Rx) drugs affects young adults

most. Available at: http://www.drugabuse.gov/related-topics/trends-

statistics/infographics/abuse-prescription-rx-drugs-affects-young-adults-most. Accessed

on: July 9th 2015.

Novak, S. P., Kroutil, L. A., Williams, R. L., & Van Brunt, D. L. (2007). The nonmedical use of

prescription ADHD medications: results from a national Internet panel. Substance abuse

treatment, prevention, and policy, 2(1), 32.

24

Office of National Drug Control Policy. (2004). The Economic Costs of Drug Abuse in the

United States, 1992- 2002, Pub. No. 207303, at vi (Dec. 2004).

Office of National Drug Control Policy. (2011). Epidemic: Responding to America’s

prescription drug abuse crisis. Washington, DC: Author.

Paulozzi, L. J., Budnitz, D. S., Yongli, X. (2006). Increasing deaths from opioid analgesics in the

United States. Pharmacoepidemiology and Drug Safety, 15, 618-627.

Paulozzi, L. J., Mack, K. A., & Hockenberry, J. M. (2014). Vital signs: Variation among states in

prescribing opioid pain relievers and benzodiazepines. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly

Report, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 63, 563–568.

Pella, R. D., Hill, B. D., Shelton, J. T., Elliott, E., & Gouvier, W. D. (2011). Evaluation of

embedded malingering indices in a non-litigating clinical sample using control, clinical,

and derived groups. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 090.

Poulin, C. (2001). Medical and nonmedical stimulant use among adolescents: from sanctioned to

unsanctioned use. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 165(8), 1039-1044.

Quinn, C. A. (2003). Detection of malingering in assessment of adult ADHD. Archives of

Clinical Neuropsychology, 18(4), 379-395.

Rowland, A. S., Umbach, D. M., Stallone, L., Naftel, A. J., Bohlig, E. M., Sandler, D. P. (2002).

Prevalence of medication treatment for attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder among

elementary school children in Johnston County, North Carolina. American Journal of

Public Health, 92, 231–234.

25

Slick, D. J., Sherman, E. M., & Iverson, G. L. (1999). Diagnostic criteria for malingered

neurocognitive dysfunction: Proposed standards for clinical practice and research. The

Clinical Neuropsychologist, 13(4), 545-561.

Smith, M. E., & Farah, M. J. (2011). Are prescription stimulants “smart pills”? The

epidemiology and cognitive neuroscience of prescription stimulant use by normal healthy

individuals. Psychological bulletin, 137(5), 717.

Smith, S. M., Dart, R. C., Katz, N. P., et al. (2013). Classification and definition of misuse,

abuse, and related events in clinical trials: ACTTION systematic review and

recommendations. PAIN, 154(11), 2287-2296.

Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. (2007). Results from the 2006

National Survey on Drug Use and Health: National Findings. Department of Health and

Human Services: Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. Available

at: https://www.asipp.org/documents/2006NSDUH.pdf. Accessed on: December 20th

2016.

Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA). (2011). Drug abuse

warning network, 2008: National estimates of drug-related emergency department visits.

HHS publication no. SMA 11-4618. Rockville, MD.

Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. (2013a). Results from the 2012

National Survey on Drug Use and Health: Detailed tables. Available at:

http://www.samhsa.gov/data/NSDUH/2012SummNatFindDetTables/DetTabs/NSDUH-

DetTabsTOC2012.htm. Accessed on: July 9th 2015.

26

Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. (2013b). Emergency department

visits involving attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder stimulant mediations. Available

at: http://www.samhsa.gov/data/2k13/DAWN073/sr073-ADD-ADHD-medications.htm.

Accessed on: July 9th 2015.

Suhr, J., Hammers, D., Dobbins-Buckland, K., Zimak, E., & Hughes, C. (2008). The

Relationship of Malingering Test Failure to Self-Reported Symptoms and

Neuropsychological Findings in Adults Referred for ADHD Evaluations, Archives of

Clinical Neuropsychology, 23, 521–530.

Sullivan, B. K., May, K., & Galbally, L. (2007). Symptom Exaggeration by College Adults in

Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and Learning Disorder Assessments. Applied

Neuropsychology, 14, 189–207.

Swanson, J., Greenhill, L., Wigal, T. I. M., et al. (2006). Stimulant-related reductions of growth

rates in the PATS. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent

Psychiatry, 45(11), 1304-1313.

Swanson, J. M., Elliott, G. R., Greenhill, L. L., et al. (2007). Effects of stimulant medication on

growth rates across 3 years in the MTA follow-up. Journal of the American Academy of

Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 46(8), 1015-1027.

Teter, C. J., McCabe, S. E., Cranford, J. A., Boyd, C. J., & Guthrie, S. K. (2005). Prevalence and

motives for illicit use of prescription stimulants in an undergraduate student

sample. Journal of American College Health, 53(6), 253-262.

27

Teter, C. J., Falone, A. E., Cranford, J. A., Boyd, C. J., & McCabe, S. E. (2010). Nonmedical use

of prescription stimulants and depressed mood among college students: frequency and

route of administration. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 38, 292-298.

Toomey, R., Lyons, M. J., Eisen, S. A., et al. (2003). A twin study of the neuropsychological

consequences of stimulant abuse. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(3), 303-310.

Visser, S. N., Bitsko, R. H., Danielson, M. L., Perou, R., & Blumberg, S. J. (2010). Increasing

prevalence of parent-reported attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder among children—

United States, 2003 and 2007. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 59(44), 1439-

1443.

Volkow, N. D., Swanson, J. M. (2003). Variables that affect the clinical use and abuse of

methylphenidate in the treatment of ADHD. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 1909–

1918.

Weiner, S. G., Griggs, C. A., Mitchell, P. M., et al. (2013). Clinician impression versus

prescription drug monitoring program criteria in the assessment of drug-seeking behavior

in the emergency department. Annals of emergency medicine, 62(4), 281-289.

White, A. G., Birnbaum, H. G., Mareva, M. N., et al. (2005). Direct costs of opioid abuse in an

insured population in the United States. Journal of Managed Care Pharmacy, 11(6), 469.

White, B. P., Becker-Blease, K. A., & Grace-Bishop, K. (2006). Stimulant medication use,

misuse, and abuse in an undergraduate and graduate student sample. Journal of American

College Health, 54(5), 261-268.

28

Whiteside, L. K., Cunningham, R. M., Bonar, E. E., Blow, F., Ehrlich, P., & Walton, M. A.

(2015). Nonmedical prescription stimulant use among youth in the emergency

department: Prevalence, severity and correlates. Journal of substance abuse

treatment, 48(1), 21-27.

Wilens, T. E., Gignac, M., Swezey, A., Monuteaux, M. C., & Biederman, J. (2006).

Characteristics of adolescents and young adults with ADHD who divert or misuse their

prescribed medications. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent

Psychiatry, 45(4), 408-414.

Wilford, B. B., Finch, J., Czechowicz, D. J., & Warren, D. (1994). An overview of prescription

drug misuse and abuse: Defining the problem and seeking solutions. The Journal of Law,

Medicine & Ethics, 22(3), 197-203.

Young, A. M., Glover, N., & Havens, J. R. (2012). Nonmedical use of prescription medications

among adolescents in the United States: a systematic review. Journal of Adolescent

Health, 51(1), 6-17.

Zachor, D. A., Roberts, A. W., Hodgens, J. B., Isaacs, J. S., & Merrick, J. (2006). Effects of

long-term psychostimulant medication on growth of children with ADHD. Research in

developmental disabilities, 27(2), 162-174.

Zedillo, E., & Wheeler, H. (2012). Rethinking the “War on Drugs” Through the US-Mexico

Prism. Yale Center for the Study of Globalization, 33.

Zgierska, A., Miller, M., & Rabago, D. (2012). Patient satisfaction, prescription drug abuse, and

potential unintended consequences. Journal of the American Medical

Association, 307(13), 1377-1378.

29

Zullig, K. J., & Divin, A. L. (2012). The association between non-medical prescription drug use,

depressive symptoms, and suicidality among college students. Addictive

behaviors, 37(8), 890-899.

30

CHAPTER 2: PAPER 1

IDENTIFICATION OF PATTERNS OF STIMULANT USE: AN ANALYSIS OF MEDICAID

AND PRESCRIPTION MONITORING PROGRAM DATA FROM THE STATE OF

MISSISSIPPI

Formatted to the requirements of Drug and Alcohol Dependence

31

Title: Identification of Patterns of Stimulant Use: An Analysis of Medicaid and Prescription

Monitoring Program Data From the State of Mississippi

Sujith Ramachandran, Department of Pharmacy Administration, University of Mississippi,

University, MS, USA – 38655. Email: [email protected]; Phone: 6623803006; Fax:

6629155102; Address: 128 Faser Hall, University of Mississippi, University, MS

(Corresponding author)

Dr. John P. Bentley, Department of Pharmacy Administration, University of Mississippi,

University, MS, USA – 38677.

Dr. Erin R. Holmes, Department of Pharmacy Administration, University of Mississippi,

University, MS, USA – 38677.

Dr. Meagen Rosenthal, Department of Pharmacy Administration, University of Mississippi,

University, MS, USA – 38677.

Dr. Benjamin F. Banahan III, Department of Pharmacy Administration, University of

Mississippi, University, MS, USA – 38677.

Dr. John Young, Department of Psychology, University of Mississippi, University, MS, USA –

38677.

Running title: (60 characters) Development of the Subtle ADHD Malingering Screener

Word count – Abstract: 243/250; Main text: 3,473/4,000

32

ABSTRACT

Background

An increase in prevalence of prescription stimulant abuse has led to concerns about stimulant-

related addiction, emergency room visits, and other adverse effects. The aim of this study is to

explore the feasibility of the identification of prescription stimulant abuse by categorizing

stimulant users using Mississippi Medicaid administrative claims data and Mississippi

Prescription Monitoring Program data.

Methods

This study employed a retrospective design to follow a cohort of prescription stimulant users

using data from Mississippi Medicaid administrative claims and the Mississippi Prescription

Monitoring Program, for the years 2014 and 2015. Risk factors for stimulant abuse were used to

characterize latent classes among prescription stimulant users. Predicted latent class membership

was used to predict chances of being diagnosed with dependent or nondependent stimulant

abuse.

Results

A latent class model with 2 classes was selected as the best fit for this sample. The first latent

class, comprising 4.1% of the sample, was characterized by a significantly higher proportion of

cash prescriptions, new starts, and significantly lower prevalence of ADHD, and fewer unique

33

pharmacies visited. However, latent class membership was not significantly predictive of

diagnosis of stimulant abuse (p = 0.802).

Conclusions

This study shows the value of commonly used risk factors in identification of prescription

stimulant abuse. It also categorizes the various patterns of use of prescription stimulants and tests

its ability to predict stimulant abuse diagnoses. Further research is needed to test the validity of

these latent classes in the prediction of stimulant abuse.

34

INTRODUCTION

1.0 Introduction

Prescription stimulants such as amphetamine and methylphenidate are commonly prescribed

to treat symptoms of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). These drugs are

classified as schedule II controlled substances because they have a high abuse liability (Cohen,

2013). It is estimated that over 3.3 million individuals misused stimulants in 2013 alone

(SAMHSA, 2013). Between 2006 and 2011, the nonmedical use of prescription stimulants

among adults increased by 67%, and stimulant-related emergency room visits have more than

doubled (Chen et al., 2016). Repeated use of prescription stimulants can also lead to drug

dependence or addiction among individuals with and without ADHD (Bollinger et al., 2005;

National Center for Health Statistics, 2014). The prevalence of drug dependence is estimated to

be 1 in every 20 stimulant users, and is steadily increasing (Bollinger et al., 2005; National

Center for Health Statistics, 2014; Smith & Farah, 2011). Abuse of stimulants can lead to a

variety of side-effects such as insomnia, headaches, significant weight loss, anxiety, delusions,

mood swings, cardiovascular complications, and hypertension (Barkley et al., 2003; Charach et

al., 2006; Hamilton, 2009; Smith & Farah, 2011; White, Becker-Blease & Grace-Bishop, 2009).

An understanding of the patterns of use is essential to identification, treatment, and even

prevention of stimulant misuse. Research about characteristics and patterns of drug use have

been conducted in the area of alcohol abuse (Agrawal et al., 2006; Chiauzzi, DasMahapatra, &

Black, 2013), and substance abuse (Carlson et al., 2005; Falck et al., 2005; Kelly et al., 2015;

35

Morasco & Dobscha, 2008; Patra et al., 2009; Sherman et al., 2009). For example, Sullivan et al.

(2010) used data from Arkansas Medicaid and a commercial insurance plan to develop an opioid

misuse score that correlates to possible indicators of misuse. Chiauzzi et al. (2013) classified

college students into four different classes based on a combination of their alcohol and drug use

frequencies. However, such research is lacking in the area of prescription stimulant abuse.

1.1 Characteristics of stimulant misuse

Stimulant use patterns can be challenging to characterize because of the varying frequencies

of use among both legal and illegal users of stimulants. For example, individuals with legal

stimulant prescriptions might take their medication every day or choose to use it infrequently,

depending on their needs, and the severity of their symptoms (Caisley & Muller, 2012).

Individuals misusing stimulants might also have varying patterns of use, depending on whether

they are drug dependent, or if their motivation for misuse is for academic performance-related

reasons (Garnier-Dysktra, et al., 2012; Moore et al., 2014). This varying frequency of use is also

commonly seen among opioids; hence, several of the characteristics applicable to identification

of opioid misuse can be used to help drive research in the identification of prescription stimulant

misuse.

Risk factors commonly used to identify misuse of opioids include doctor shopping, high

frequency of prescriptions, familial availability of prescriptions, and early refills (Katz et al.,

2010; Morasco & Dobscha, 2008; Sansone & Sansone, 2012; Van den Bree et al., 1998). Several

of these risk factors have been shown to be relevant in misuse of prescription stimulants. For

example, Van den Bree et al. (1998) demonstrated the importance of familial substance abuse in

identifying stimulant misuse. High doses of oral stimulants taken non-medically are known to

cause drug dependence (NIDA, 2014). Doctor shopping has been demonstrated to be a

36

successful technique for acquiring prescription drugs for misuse or diversion, in cases of both

opioids and stimulants (Sansone & Sansone, 2012; Worley, 2012).

1.2 Objectives

The objective of the current study is to explore the feasibility of identification of prescription

stimulant misuse using, administrative claims data to identify and characterize latent classes of

prescription stimulant users, and study the relationship between the membership in each latent

class and the diagnosis of stimulant abuse.

37

METHODS

2.0 Materials & Methods

2.1 Study design

This study employed a retrospective data analysis to follow a cohort of prescription

stimulant users using data from the Mississippi Medicaid administrative claims and the

Mississippi Prescription Monitoring Program, for the years 2014 and 2015. Risk factors for

stimulant misuse were identified from the data and used to characterize latent classes of

prescription stimulant users. Predicted latent class membership was then used to predict a distal

outcome, i.e, the likelihood of being diagnosed with dependent or nondependent stimulant abuse.

Approval was obtained from the University of Mississippi’s Institutional Review Board before

researchers had access to the data.

2.2 Data source

The Mississippi Medicaid administrative claims data contains all claims paid for by

Mississippi Division of Medicaid for beneficiaries enrolled in its fee-for-service or managed care

plans. It contains separate datasets for pharmacy claims, inpatient claims, outpatient claims, and

a beneficiary master file – that contains demographic and eligibility information of enrollees.

Claims for Medicaid beneficiaries paid for with cash are not captured in this dataset. The

Mississippi Prescription Monitoring Program (PMP) data were used in this study to capture the

cash prescriptions of Medicaid beneficiaries. The patient ID in the PMP data was linked to the

38

beneficiary ID in the Medicaid data, offering the researchers comprehensive prescription

information, hospital or outpatient use data, and diagnosis information.

2.3 Timeline

The study used data from July 1st 2014 to June 30th 2015 to identify Mississippi Medicaid

beneficiaries who were filling prescriptions for stimulants within the state of Mississippi. The

first dispensing event for prescription stimulants during this period was labelled as the index

prescription. The period ranging from January 1st 2014 to June 30th 2014 was used as a washout

period to check for medication and diagnostic history. Individuals identified during the study

period were followed up to December 31st 2015 in order to ensure that every participant included

in the study had at least 6 months of follow up from their index date.

2.4 Inclusion/exclusion

All Medicaid beneficiaries needed to be continuously enrolled during the study period

(January 2014 to December 2015). Participants were eligible if they were over 18 years of age at

the beginning of the study period, could not have dual eligibility in Medicare, and could not be

enrolled in long term care. Participants were also required to have at least 3 refills for stimulants

during the study period (July 1st 2014 to December 31st 2015).

2.5 Latent Class Indicators & Their Operationalization

Risk factors for drug misuse identified from literature were used as indicators for the

latent class analysis. The risk factors included in this study were the number of refills for

prescription stimulants during the study period, percentage of early refills, percentage of refills

paid for with cash (i.e., not paid for by Medicaid), excess distance travelled to obtain stimulants,

average number of stimulants per enrollee in a given zipcode, and the number of unique doctors

39

and pharmacies who have prescribed and dispensed stimulants, respectively. In addition to these

variables, two categorical variables: whether or not the patient had a new start on stimulant

therapy, and the presence of an ADHD diagnosis in the data were also included.

Total number of refills, number of early refills, and percentage of prescriptions paid for in

cash indicate intentions of misuse or abuse. Early refills were operationalized as obtaining a refill

2 or more days before the end of the supply from the previous prescription. Prescriptions paid for

with cash were identified using the payment type variable found in the PMP database. Excess

distance travelled to obtain stimulants indicates the number of times the patient travelled further

than necessary to a doctor appointment for his/her stimulant prescriptions. It was operationalized

as the number of times the participant has travelled more than the average distance travelled by

all the Medicaid enrollees in his/her zipcode to obtain a stimulant prescription. Geographic

variations were accounted for using the average number of stimulant prescriptions per eligible

Medicaid enrollee in the zipcode in which the participant resides. Zipcode information for each

beneficiary was obtained from the beneficiary master file. The number of unique pharmacies and

doctors visited by each patient is used as a measure of doctor shopping, and was calculated using

each of the patient’s visits during the follow up period. New therapy on prescription stimulants

were defined as those patients who have no fills for stimulants during the 6 month washout

period before their index prescription. Diagnosis for ADHD was identified using ICD9 and

ICD10 codes present in the outpatient and inpatient Medicaid claims records of participating

beneficiaries.

2.6 Outcome Variables

Medicaid beneficiaries who were found to be receiving treatment for dependent or

nondependent stimulant abuse were used as the group of interest in this study. These patients

40

were identified from the ICD9 and ICD10 diagnosis codes found in the inpatient and outpatient

claims records. These diagnosis codes are the closest approximation to a gold standard for

identification of prescription stimulant abuse available in administrative claims data. Other

demographic variables such as age, race, and gender were used as control variables in the

analysis.

2.7 Analysis

Data management and analysis were conducted using MPlus (Muthen & Muthen, 1998)

and IBM SPSS (Chicago, IL). Latent Class Analysis (LCA), using MPlus, was used to identify

subgroups of individuals that were similar to each other based on chosen indicator variables, i.e.,

the risk factors for misuse (Lazarsfeld & Henry, 1968). LCA was chosen because it uses model-

based posterior probabilities to obtain subgroups, instead of a measure of distance in multi-

dimensional space, such as that used in cluster analysis. It allows for latent classes with unequal

variances and provides more interpretable results, while also being as reliable at prediction as

discriminant analysis when using both categorical and continuous indicators (Carlson et al.,

2005; Magidson & Vermunt, 2002). Similar approaches of identifying latent classes among drug

abusers have been applied in the past to better understand patterns and behaviors of individuals

dependent on other drugs (Agrawal et al., 2006; Carlson et al., 2005; Chiauzzi et al., 2013; Falck

et al., 2005; Kelly et al., 2015; Morasco & Dobscha, 2008; Patra et al., 2009; Sherman et al.,

2009).

Decisions about model fit were based on fit statistics such as the Bayesian Information

Criterion (BIC), the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC), the Adjusted BIC (aBIC), and the

entropy statistic. In general, lower values of AIC, BIC or ABIC and higher values of entropy

indicate greater class separation and higher precision in class assignments. The Lo-Mendel-

41

Rubin Likelihood Ratio (LMR LR test) was used to choose the model with the appropriate

number of latent classes (Carlson et al., 2005; Kelly et al., 2015). Most likely class membership

for each observation was saved, and a logistic regression was performed, using SPSS, to identify

the relationship between class membership (treated as an observed variable) and a combined

endpoint of a diagnosis of diagnosis of dependent or nondependent stimulant abuse. Available

demographic variables were used as covariates in the logistic regression. This regression was

conducted in SPSS, instead of using the distal outcome option (DCATEGORICAL) in MPlus,

because despite using true probabilities of class membership, the DCATEGORICAL feature

does not provide an option to control for covariates (Asparouvhov & Muthen, 2014). The use of

most likely class membership as an observed variable is considered an acceptable approach when

covariates are present in the analysis (Clark & Muthen, 2009). However, the DCATEGORICAL

option in MPlus was also performed as a sensitivity analysis.

42

RESULTS

3.0 Results

3.1 Demographics

A total of 1022 Medicaid beneficiaries were eligible for the study. They were mostly

female (70%), Caucasian (75%), and 31.7 years (SD: 10.99) old, on average. Most of the sample

had a diagnosis of ADHD (83.6%), and 27.3% were identified as new starts on stimulant

therapy. On average, 4% of the prescriptions were paid for in cash, and nearly 37% of refills

were filled early. The average Medicaid beneficiary visited 2 unique pharmacies and 1.7 unique

physicians for stimulant during the study period. Demographics of the study sample are provided

in Table A1.

3.2 Latent classes

A latent class model with the 2 classes was chosen as the final solution in this study. The

3-class model had a significant LMR LR test, and lower values of AIC, and BIC, indicating

satisfactory fit. However, the 3-class model provided a highly skewed class distribution with

95.2% in Class 1, 4.1% in Class 2, and 0.5% in Class 3. The 3-class model also suffered from a

non-positive definite first-order derivative product matrix due to model nonidentification,

resulting in a lack of replicability in the best loglikelihood value, despite increasing the number

of random starts. In contrast, the 2-class solution, despite having slightly higher values of AIC

(41970.03 vs 40226.28), and BIC (42098.19 vs 40403.74), showed a significant LMR LR test,

43

Table A1: Demographics of the sample, and distribution across latent classes

Characteristic

n (%)/Mean[SD]

Overall

(N = 1022)

Class 1

(N = 42)

Class 2

(N = 980)

ADHD* 854 (83.6) 28 (66.7) 826 (84.3)

New prescriptions* 279 (27.3) 19 (45.2) 260 (26.5)

Race

Caucasian 768 (75.1) 33 (78.6) 735 (75.0)

African American 139 (13.6) 8 (19.0) 131 (13.4)

Other 114 (11.2) 1 (2.4) 113 (11.5)

Female** 723 (70.7) 40 (95.2) 683 (69.7)

Dependent abuse of stimulants 28 (2.7) 0 (0) 28 (2.9)

Nondependent abuse of stimulants 32 (3.1) 3 (7.1) 29 (3.0)

Composite endpoint 54 (5.3) 3 (7.1) 51 (5.2)

Total number of prescriptions 11.8 [6.3] 11.1 [7.1] 11.8 [6.3]

Number of pharmacies** 2.0 [1.4] 0.7 [1.0] 2.03 [1.4]

Number of physicians 1.71 [1.0] 1.6 [1.1] 1.7 [1.0]

Percent of cash prescriptions** 4.4 [17.7] 84.6 [19.6] 0.9 [4.7]

Percent of early refills 36.9 [23.6] 41.4 [18.2] 36.7 [23.8]

Average stimulants per beneficiary in a

zipcode

0.55 [1.4] 0.4 [0.3] 0.6 [1.4]

Age* 31.7 [11.0] 35.9 [10.5] 31.6 [11.0]

Note: * – p < 0.05; ** – p < 0.0001

44

and demonstrated satisfactory replicability in the best loglikelihood value. The 2-class model

also had a high entropy value of 0.999. As stated by Carlson et al., (2005) “LCA classification

should be driven by both statistics, and theory”, and classes must be “interpretable, and have a

meaningful number of observations.” Therefore, for reasons of interpretability, model

identification, and theoretical considerations, the 2-class model was chosen as the final solution

(Geiser, 2013).

Of the 1022 subjects included in the latent class analysis, 4.1% (42) were included in the

Class 1, and 95.9% (980) were included in Class 2. The distribution of each of the latent class

indicators and the demographic variables across the two classes is provided in Table 1. Class 1,

which includes only 42 individuals, was significantly older, with more females, less prevalence

of ADHD, higher prevalence of cash prescriptions, and a fewer number of pharmacies visited.

The prevalence of cash prescriptions in Class 1 was significantly higher than that in Class 2

(84.6% versus 0.9%). The percentage of early refills, though not statistically significantly, was

also found to be higher in Class 1 than Class 2 (41.4% versus 36.7%). The smaller size of Class

1, along with the distribution of risk factors, suggest that Class 1 might be representative of

stimulant misuse population. Individuals classified into Class 2 might be representative of

regular/legal stimulant use behavior.

3.3 Prediction of stimulant abuse

The results of the 2-class solution indicate that Class 1 might be comprised of individuals

likely to be misusing prescription stimulants. A logistic regression examining the effect of most

likely class membership on dependent/nondependent prescription stimulant abuse, after

controlling for demographic variables was not found to be significant (p = 0.802). The

45

demographic variable, race was dropped from the model because the distribution of race across

the levels of the outcome variable were highly skewed leading to large standard errors.

Individuals in Class 1 had 1.17 times the odds of being diagnosed with prescription

stimulant abuse, when compared with those in Class 2; however this increase in odds was not

found to be significant (95% CI = 0.63 – 2.17; p = 0.802). When compared to males, females had

1.55 times the odds of being diagnosed with prescription stimulant abuse, but this difference was

not statistically significant (95% CI = 0.07 – 2.24; p = 0.239). Subject age was found to be

significantly related to the outcome with an odds ratio of 1.03 (95% CI = 1.01 – 1.04; p = 0.047;

Table A2).

Table A2: Prediction of dependent/nondependent prescription stimulant abuse

Variable Odds ratio 95% CI

Class 1 1.2 0.63 – 2.17

Female 1.6 0.07 – 2.24

Age* 1.0 1.01 – 1.04

Note: Reference categories: Class 1 – Class 2; Female – Male.

Note: * – p < 0.05

In addition to the logistic regression conducted using SPSS and most likely class

membership as the focal independent variable, the distal outcome analysis was also conducted

using the DCATEGORICAL option in MPlus without any covariates (Asparouhov & Muthen,

2014). This approach takes into account measurement error and uncertainty of classification; the

use of most likely class membership does not do so, which is an inherent limitation (Clark &

Muthen, 2009). The DCATEGORICAL option has been suggested as the preferred method for

analyzing categorical distal outcomes (Asparouhouv & Muthen, 2014). This analysis revealed no

46

significant relationship between class membership and the combined endpoint (chi square =

0.226; p = 0.634), a finding consistent with the previous analysis.

47

DISCUSSION

4.0 Discussion

This study attempts to develop a deeper understanding of the differences in stimulant use

behaviors between abusers and non-abusers, using a latent class analysis to first classify

individuals according to their behaviors. It is one of the first studies to attempt to exclusively

characterize stimulant use behaviors, and validate the developed latent classes using stimulant

abuse diagnoses identified from administrative claims data. The use of administrative claims data

to identify misusers of prescription stimulants revealed critical insights that can help future

research in this area. This study was also the first to use a combination of Medicaid

administrative claims data and PMP data to comprehensively capture all use of stimulants while

estimating the risk behaviors.

A latent class model with 2 classes was selected as the best fit in this study. Over 95.9%

of the sample was classified in Class 2, with only 4.1% classified into Class 1. This distribution,

while skewed, was in line with the expectation of identifying a small proportion of misusers

present in the population. Class 1 (N = 42) was expected to be related to abuse behavior because

individuals classified in this class also had a lower proportion of ADHD diagnoses, and a

significantly higher proportion of cash refills, an indicator expected to be correlated with abuse

(Inciardi et al., 2009). In contrast, other indicators expected to be positively correlated with

abuse, such as the number of unique pharmacies visited was significantly lower in this class

(Sansone & Sansone, 2012; Worley, 2012). The other doctor shopping indicator variable,

48

number of unique physicians visited, was not significantly different between the two classes.

Perhaps this difference in the distribution of risk factors contributed to the non-significant

prediction of stimulant misuse.

The odds ratio from the validation procedures corresponding to latent class membership

was not found to be statistically significant, indicating that behavioral risk factors of misuse were

not predictive of stimulant misuse in administrative claims data. The practical significance of the

latent classes needs to be judged within the context of the data used in this study. The diagnosis

of dependent or nondependent stimulant abuse obtained from the medical records was the best

available indicator for stimulant misuse. However, these diagnostic codes are used to identify

abuse of both prescription stimulants and non-prescription illegal stimulants such as cocaine and

heroin, leading to inaccurate capture of the outcome variable. There is also a possibility that

stimulant abuse was underdiagnosed in the Medicaid population. Furthermore, individuals

associated with risk behaviors such as cash refills, early refills, or doctor shopping might be

diverting their prescriptions, and not misusing the medications themselves, and therefore will

never be diagnosed with stimulant abuse. Finally, while the direction of relationship between

stimulant abuse and latent class membership was in the right direction, perhaps the sample size

of abusers was too small for a significant finding. It is possible that any combination of these

reasons was responsible for the non-significant finding obtained in this study. The latent classes

obtained in this study might still be indicative of stimulant misuse, but further research into the

stimulant use behavior, perhaps using alternative validation techniques, is needed.

4.1 Limitations & Conclusions

While this study was one of the first to characterize latent classes of stimulant users, it

carries limitations derived from the datasets used. Administrative claims data, even combined

49

with PMP data often do not provide a complete picture of the patient’s healthcare use. It is

possible that stimulant abusers filled prescriptions outside of state limits or obtained the

medications illegally, both of which are not captured by available data. Other latent class

indicators, such as presence of an ADHD diagnosis, prescription history for identification of new

prescriptions were limited by the fact that only 6 months of historical data were available. The

datasets used in this study were merged based on an encrypted beneficiary identification number,

and it is possible that some claims were wrongly attributed to other patients. Thorough quality

checks were conducted on the PMP data prior to this study to ensure they were linked to the

correct beneficiary identification number, and this error, if existing, is expected to be minimal.

The limitations of the outcome variable could have influenced the validation of the latent

classes, and the prediction of stimulant abuse status. It is possible that many individuals

classified in Class 1 were in fact abusing prescription stimulants, but had not been diagnosed; but

there was no way to know their true misuse status from the data. Hence the findings of the

prediction of stimulant misuse using latent classes need to be treated with caution and probably

represent an underestimation. It is possible that alternative validation mechanisms with more

reliable identification of misusers might have discovered a correlation with the latent classes

obtained in this study.

This study demonstrates the value of latent class techniques to help identify variations in

patterns of stimulant use, while also displaying the limitations of administrative claims databases

in capturing true misuse status. Prescription stimulant misuse has increased in recent years (Chen

et al., 2016), and techniques to identify this misuse need to be further researched to help address

this problem.

50

BIBLIOGRAPHY

51

Agrawal, A., Lynskey, M. T., Madden, P. A., Bucholz, K. K., & Heath, A. C. (2006). A latent

class analysis of illicit drug abuse/dependence: results from the National Epidemiological

Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Addiction, 102(1), 94-104.

Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. (2014). Auxiliary variables in mixture modeling: Three-step

approaches using M plus. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary

Journal, 21(3), 329-341.

Barkley, R. A., Fischer, M., Smallish, L., & Fletcher, K. (2003). Does the treatment of attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder with stimulants contribute to drug use/abuse? A 13-year

prospective study. Pediatrics, 111(1), 97-109.

Bollinger, L. C., Bush, C., Califano, J. A., et al. (2005). Under the counter: The diversion and

abuse of controlled prescription drugs in the U.S. The National Center on Addiction and

Substance Abuse at Columbia University (CASA). Available at:

http://www.casacolumbia.org/addiction-research/reports/under-the-counter-diversion-

abuse-controlled-perscription-drugs. Accessed on: July 9th 2015.

Caisley, H., & Müller, U. (2012). Adherence to medication in adults with attention deficit

hyperactivity disorder and pro re nata dosing of psychostimulants: a systematic review.

Eur. Psychiatry, 27(5), 343-349.

52

Carlson, R. G., Wang, J., Falck, R. S., & Siegal, H. A. (2005). Drug use practices among

MDMA/ecstasy users in Ohio: a latent class analysis. Drug Alcohol Depend., 79(2), 167-

179.

Charach, A., Figueroa, M., Chen, S., Ickowicz, A., & Schachar, R. (2006). Stimulant treatment

over 5 years: effects on growth. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry., 45(4), 415-421.

Chen, L. Y., Crum, R. M., Strain, E. C., Alexander, G. C., Kaufmann, C., & Mojtabai, R. (2016).

Prescriptions, nonmedical use, and emergency department visits involving prescription

stimulants. J Clin Psychiatry., 77(3), 297-304.

Chiauzzi, E., DasMahapatra, P., & Black, R. A. (2013). Risk behaviors and drug use: A latent

class analysis of heavy episodic drinking in first-year college students. Psychol Addict

Behav., 27(4), 974.

Clark, S.L. & Muthen, B. (2009). Relating Latent Class Analysis Results to Variables not

Included in the Analysis. Mplus Web Notes. Available at:

http://www.statmodel.com/download/relatinglca.pdf Accessed on: December 20th, 2016.”

Cohen, M. J. (2013). Off-Label: Combating the Dangerous Overprescription of Amphetamines

to Children. George Washington Law Rev., 82, 174.

Falck, R. S., Siegal, H. A., Wang, J., Carlson, R. G., & Draus, P. J. (2005). Nonmedical drug use

among stimulant-using adults in small towns in rural Ohio. Journal of substance abuse

treatment, 28(4), 341-349.

53

Garnier-Dykstra, L. M., Caldeira, K. M., Vincent, K. B., O’Grady, K. E., & Arria, A. M. (2012).

Nonmedical use of prescription stimulants during college: Four-year trends in exposure

opportunity, use, motives, and sources. J Am Coll Health., 60(3), 226-234.

Geiser, C. (2013). Data Analysis with MPlus. The Guilford Press, New York, NY.

Hamilton, G. J. (2009). Prescription drug abuse. Psychol Sch., 46(9), 892-898.

Inciardi, J. A., Surratt, H. L., Cicero, T. J., & Beard, R. A. (2009). Prescription opioid abuse and

diversion in an urban community: the results of an ultrarapid assessment. Pain

Med., 10(3), 537-548.

Katz, N., Panas, L., Kim, M., et al. (2010) Usefulness of prescription monitoring programs for

surveillance—analysis of Schedule II opioid prescription data in Massachusetts, 1996–

2006. Pharmacoepidemiol and Drug Saf., 19(2), 115-123.

Kelly, B. C., Rendina, H. J., Vuolo, M., Wells, B. E., & Parsons, J. T. (2015) A typology of

prescription drug misuse: A latent class approach to differences and harms. Drug Alcohol

Rev., 34(2), 211-220.

Lazarsfeld, P., Henry, N. (1968). Latent Structure Analysis. Houghton Mifflin, New York.

Magidson, J., & Vermunt, J. (2002). Latent class models for clustering: A comparison with K-

means. Canadian Journal of Marketing Research, 20(1), 36-43.

Moore, D. R., Burgard, D. A., Larson, R. G., & Ferm, M. (2014). Psychostimulant use among

college students during periods of high and low stress: an interdisciplinary approach

utilizing both self-report and unobtrusive chemical sample data. Addict Behav., 39(5),

987-993.

54

Morasco, B. J., & Dobscha, S. K. (2008). Prescription medication misuse and substance use

disorder in VA primary care patients with chronic pain. Gen Hosp Psychiatry., 30(2), 93-

99.

Muthen, L.K., Muthen, B.O., 1998. Mplus User’s Guide, Seventh Edition. Muthen & Muthen,

Los Angeles, CA.

National Center for Health Statistics. (2014). Health, United States, 2014: With Special Feature

on Prescription Drugs. Hyattsville, MD. 2014. Accessed on: July 9th 2015 at:

http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/hus/hus14.pdf.

NIDA (2014). Stimulant ADHD Medications: Methylphenidate and Amphetamines. Accessed

on: December 3, 2016 at: https://www.drugabuse.gov/publications/drugfacts/stimulant-

adhd-medications-methylphenidate-amphetamines.

Patra, J., Fischer, B., Maksimowska, S., & Rehm, J. (2009). Profiling poly-substance use

typologies in a multi-site cohort of illicit opioid and other drug users in Canada–a latent

class analysis. Addiction Research & Theory, 17(2), 168-185.

Sansone, R. A., & Sansone, L. A. (2012). Doctor shopping: A phenomenon of many

themes. Innov Clin Neurosci., 9(11-12), 42.

Sherman, S. G., Sutcliffe, C. G., German, D., Sirirojn, B., Aramrattana, A., & Celentano, D. D.

(2009). Patterns of risky behaviors associated with methamphetamine use among young

Thai adults: a latent class analysis. J of Adolesc Health., 44(2), 169-175.

55

Smith, M. E., & Farah, M. J. (2011). Are prescription stimulants “smart pills”? The

epidemiology and cognitive neuroscience of prescription stimulant use by normal healthy

individuals. Psychol Bull, 137(5), 717.

Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. (2013). Results from the 2012

National Survey on Drug Use and Health: Detailed tables. Accessed on: July 9th 2015 at:

http://www.samhsa.gov/data/NSDUH/2012SummNatFindDetTables/DetTabs/NSDUH-

DetTabsTOC2012.htm.

Van den Bree, M. B., Johnson, E. O., Neale, M. C., & Pickens, R. W. (1998). Genetic and

environmental influences on drug use and abuse/dependence in male and female

twins. Drug Alcohol Depend., 52(3), 231-241.

White, B. P., Becker-Blease, K. A., & Grace-Bishop, K. (2006). Stimulant medication use,

misuse, and abuse in an undergraduate and graduate student sample. J Am Coll

Health., 54(5), 261-268.

Worley, J. (2012). Prescription drug monitoring programs, a response to doctor shopping:

purpose, effectiveness, and directions for future research. Issues Ment Health

Nurs., 33(5), 319-328.

56

CHAPTER 3: PAPER 2

DEVELOPMENT OF THE SUBTLE ADHD MALINGERING SCREENER (SAMS) FOR

IDENTIFICATION OF INDIVIDUALS MALINGERING ATTENTION DEFICIT

HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER

Formatted to the requirements of the Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology

57

Title: Development of the Subtle ADHD Malingering Screener (SAMS) For Identification of

Individuals Malingering Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

Sujith Ramachandran, Department of Pharmacy Administration, University of Mississippi,

University, MS, USA – 38655. Email: [email protected]; Phone: 6623803006; Fax:

6629155102 (Corresponding author)

Dr. John P. Bentley, Department of Pharmacy Administration, University of Mississippi,

University, MS, USA – 38655.

Dr. Erin R. Holmes, Department of Pharmacy Administration, University of Mississippi,

University, MS, USA – 38655.

Dr. Meagen Rosenthal, Department of Pharmacy Administration, University of Mississippi,

University, MS, USA – 38655.

Dr. Benjamin F. Banahan III, Department of Pharmacy Administration, University of

Mississippi, University, MS, USA – 38655.

Dr. John Young, Department of Psychology, University of Mississippi, University, MS, USA –

38655.

Running title: (60 characters) Development Of the Subtle ADHD Malingering Screener

Supplement data: Supplement 1: Malingerer instruction set

Word count – Abstract: 210/250; Main text: 3,287

58

ABSTRACT

Objective Development of a behavioral, self-reported, subtle scale, called the Subtle ADHD

Malingering Screener (SAMS), for use in the primary care setting to identify malingering among

individuals reporting symptoms of ADHD.

Method This study employs a cross-sectional experimental design using a self-administered

computer-based survey distributed to conveniently sampled groups of college students with and

without ADHD. Respondents were classified into three groups: the ADHD group, comprised of

individuals with ADHD; the Control group, comprised of individuals without ADHD, and the

Malingerer group, comprised of individuals without ADHD who were instructed to feign ADHD.

Factor analysis and psychometric testing were conducted to develop a final scale that can

distinguish the Malingerer group from the other groups.

Results A ten-item, two-factor solution was obtained with satisfactory model fit, reliability, and

construct validity. The psychological factor contained six items, and the academic factor

contained four items. Means for individual items and the sum subscale scores were all

significantly different among the three study groups.

Conclusion The SAMS presents an innovative approach to identify malingering of ADHD

symptoms, reduce overdiagnosis of ADHD, and address abuse of prescription stimulants. It is

short, easy to administer, and presents significant potential for direct use in the clinical setting.

59

INTRODUCTION

1. Introduction

The abuse of prescription stimulants, typically prescribed for ADHD, on college

campuses has been increasing in the past few years (Bollinger et al., 2005; Chen et al., 2016).

The Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration identifies individuals between

the ages of 18 and 25 years as a high risk group for abuse of prescription drugs (SAMHSA,

2010). College-aged students use more stimulants than their counterparts not enrolled in college

(Johnston et al., 2011; Johnston et al., 2016). Garnier-Dysktra et al. (2012) estimate that the

prevalence of prescription stimulant abuse in college populations can range up to 31%. Between

2006 and 2011, the nonmedical use of prescription stimulants among adults increased by 67%,

and stimulant-related emergency room visits have more than doubled (Chen et al., 2016).

1.1 Malingering of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)

Most adults misusing prescription stimulants obtain the drugs from friends/family

(Bavarian et al., 2013; Maxwell, 2011; Lessenger & Feinberg, 2008; McCabe et al., 2014).

Students who misuse stimulants might also be interested in seeking out a diagnosis of ADHD

(Zgierska, Miller and Ribago, 2012; Garnier Dysktra et al., 2012). Obtaining an ADHD

diagnosis offers students means to acquire prescription stimulants legally and additional

privileges such as extra time on tests, and isolated test environments, that most colleges and

universities offer to accommodate students with learning diabilities (Young & Gross, 2011;

Jasinksi & Ranseen, 2011). These benefits create external incentives that encourage malingering

60

behavior (Slick, Sherman & Iverson, 1999; Binder, 1992). Researchers estimate that 20% to 50%

of college students might be exaggerating or malingering about their ADHD symptoms during an

evaluation (Suhr et al., 2008; Sullivan, May & Galbally, 2007; Harrison, 2006).

1.2 Detection of Malingering

Malingering of ADHD symptoms in the physician’s office can be fairly easy (Quinn,

2003; Fisher & Watkins, 2008; Harp et al., 2011; Jachimowicz & Geiselman, 2004). This is

because it is difficult to accurately diagnose ADHD, and because physicians are often not

adequately equipped to identify cases of malingering (Harrison, 2006; Bollinger et al., 2005).

Tools commonly used for diagnosing ADHD, such as self-report inventories, observer symptom

ratings, cognitive measures and test of executive functions, can all be faked easily (Quinn, 2003;

Fisher & Watkins, 2008; Harp et al., 2011; Jachimowicz & Geiselman, 2004). Symptom validity

tests often perform better at detection of malingering, but they are usually very expensive, time

consuming, and require an expert for administration and interpretation (Jasinksi & Ranseen,

2011).

In order to detect malingering of ADHD, there is a need for tools that are short,

economical, sensitive, and easy to administer, score, and evaluate. Subtle scales can offer all of

these features, but they have not received much attention in the context of ADHD (Burkhart,

Gynther & Christian, 1978). Popular examples of subtle scales include the Minnesota

Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI; Hathaway & McKinley, 1940) and the Substance

Abuse Subtle Screening Inventory (SASSI; Miller, 1985), which are used to detect malingering

of any mental illness, and for alcohol and substance abuse, respectively. Subtle subscales present

in the MMPI and the Personality Assessment Inventory (PAI) have been used to identify

61

malingering of ADHD, but these scales were not designed specifically for ADHD, and hence

they show poor sensitivity (Harp et al., 2011; Young & Gross, 2011; Rios & Morley, 2013).

There is a need to develop subtle scales, grounded in theory, and tailored to identify malingering,

especially in the context of ADHD (Musso & Gouvier, 2012).

1.3 Objective

This study aims to develop a behavioral self-reported Subtle ADHD Malingering

Screener (SAMS), for use in the primary care setting to identify malingering among individuals

reporting symptoms of ADHD. In order for this scale to hold the most value in the detection of

malingering of ADHD and to prevent misuse of stimulants, it needs to meet certain criteria: The

scale needs to be short, easy to administer, and score; it needs to be suitable for administration in

the primary care setting, and should not need additional training for interpretation; it needs to be

tailored toward patients reporting ADHD symptoms; and it should display good sensitivity to

malingering.

62

METHODS

2. Methods

2.1 Study Design

This study employs a cross-sectional experimental design using a self-administered

computer-based survey distributed to conveniently sampled groups of college-enrolled adults

with and without ADHD. Approval was obtained from the University of Mississippi Institutional

Review Board.

2.2 Item Development

Potential SAMS items were developed based on the Accuracy of Knowledge (AoK)

framework, which says that malingerers can be identified by “the assessment of a person’s level

of relevant knowledge of the target condition that the person is attempting to simulate.” (Lanyon,

1997). New items were generated through an extensive literature review, and a series of in-depth

interviews conducted with students with and without ADHD recruited from the University of

Mississippi. Items from preexisting subtle scales such as the PAI, the MMPI, the Connor Adult

ADHD Rating Scale (CAARS), and the Clinical Assessment of Attention Deficit – Adult (CAT-

A) which fit the AoK framework were also included in the initial item pool. Using a combination

of new items and items borrowed from preexisting scales, a total of 125 items were generated in

accordance with scale development guidelines developed by Jackson (1966), Buss (1959),

Loevinger (1957), and Clark & Watson (1995). A 7-point response format from “Strongly

63

Disagree” to “Strongly Agree” was used in order to allow for for variability in responses

(Comrey, 1988). Qualitative pretests were conducted using cognitive interviews to help refine

the item pool.

2.3 Experimental design

This study employed a between-subjects design with alternate instruction sets (in both the

quantitative pretest and the main data collection task). Similar techniques have been commonly

used to develop and test subtle scales in extant literature (Cofer, Chance & Judson, 1949;

Lanyon, 1970; Myerholtz & Rosenberg, 1997; Myerholtz & Rosenberg, 1998; Lees-Haley,

English & Glenn, 1991; Wooley et al, 2012). Respondents were first asked to self-report

diagnosis of ADHD. To validate self-reported ADHD, respondents were also asked time since

ADHD diagnosis, time spent during ADHD diagnostic visit, and the specialty of the diagnosing

practitioner. Respondents were then assigned to one of three groups as part of the experimental

design.

Respondents who self-reported ADHD were instructed to respond to the test item pool

honestly; this was called the ADHD group. Respondents who self-reported not having ADHD

were randomized to one of two groups where they were either instructed to fake ADHD on the

test item pool (Malingerer group) (Quinn, 2003; Supplement 1), or instructed to respond honestly

(Control group). Participants in the Malingerer group were not given information about

symptoms of ADHD or other coaching to help them feign because research shows that

knowledge of ADHD or coaching did not significantly improve success rate of ADHD feigning

(Booksh et al., 2010; Rios & Morey, 2013; Tucha et al., 2009). Manipulation checks were

64

included to ensure that the instructions were followed. In addition to the item pool, the survey

also measured the respondent’s demographics and several other variables such as insurance

status, housing status, expected GPA, stimulant use behavior, and frequency of use of healthcare

services.

2.4 Pretest

A quantitative pretest was conducted to narrow the item pool and to filter the questions

that did not differentiate well between the study groups. A total of 278 students from the

University of Mississippi were recruited using the SONA Systems website, a psychology

department research platform. Participants were offered research credits as incentive for

participation in the study. There were a total of 53 completed responses in the ADHD group, 111

completed responses in the Malingerer group and 112 completed responses in the Control group.

Using the Student’s t-test, 38 items were identified from the initial test pool that showed

significant discrimination between the Malingerer group and the ADHD group. These items were

entered into a Principal Components Analysis (PCA) to estimate the factor structure of the scale

(Gerbing & Anderson, 1988; DeVellis, 2011). Only responses in the ADHD group and the

Control group were used in the PCA, because literature suggests that malingering might cause

distortions in the factor structure (Myerholtz & Rosenberg, 1997; Myerholtz & Rosenberg,

1998). Items with loadings less than or equal to 0.4 or items loading heavily on more than one

factor were deleted (Hinkin, 1995). Using a combination of the latent root criterion (or the

eigenvalue greater than 1 rule) (Nunnally, 1978), the scree plot (Cattell, 1966) and parallel

analysis (Franklin et al., 1995; O’Connor, 2000), two factors were obtained: a 6-item

psychological factor, and a 4-item academic factor, named because of the content of the items

65

that loaded highly on each factor. Both factors demonstrated satisfactory reliability with a

Cronbach’s alpha of 0.895 and 0.870, respectively.

2.5 Participants and study recruitment

For the main data collection task, a sample of the undergraduate population at the

University of Mississippi was used. Subject recruitment was conducted through a series of

announcements conducted in classes held across campus, in order to obtain a representative

sample. All participants were offered extra course credit as an incentive for participation. In

order to prevent coercion, students who did not wish to participate in the study were offered an

opportunity to earn the same extra credit through comparable alternative assignments. Additional

incentives, in the form of a chance to win one of five $25 gift cards, were offered to individuals

assigned to the Malingerer group in order to match real world incentives for malingering.

Interested students set up appointments with the research team, and completed the survey in

person on a computer assigned to them. The survey was designed using Qualtrics, and all data

were collected anonymously.

66

ANALYSIS

3. Analysis

Analysis was conducted using IBM SPSS AMOS 22.0 (Chicago, Illinois) and MPlus 7.4

(Muthen & Muthen). Descriptive statistics were calculated and compared across the three study

groups. A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted, in MPlus, using the factor

structure obtained from the PCA in the pretest. Because the items were measured on an ordinal

scale with limited response options, CFA was conducted using robust weighted least squares

estimation (i.e., the WLSMV estimator in MPlus). This approach is recommended in cases of

categorical outcome variables which violate the assumption of normal distribution (Newsom,

2015; Rhemtulla, Brosseau-Liard, & Savalei, 2012). Similar to the pretest, only responses in the

ADHD group and the Control group were used in the CFA, because literature suggests that

Malingering might cause distortions in the factor structure (Myerholtz & Rosenberg, 1997;

Myerholtz & Rosenberg, 1998).

The loading estimates, standardized residuals and modification indices were used to

identify items that can be considered for deletion. Model fit was estimated using the Chi Square

statistic, the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), and the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation

(RMSEA; Hair et al., 2006). Convergent validity and discriminant validity were estimated using

the Gerbing & Anderson (1988) approach. The Fornell & Larcker (1981) approach to convergent

and discriminant validity using Average Variance Extracted (AVE) was not applicable in this

case because the CFA model was run using WLSMV estimators. Reliability for each factor was

67

assessed through Cronbach’s alpha and composite reliability (Hair et al, 2006; Fornell &

Larcker, 1981)

.

68

RESULTS

4. Results

4.1 Sample Characteristics

A total of 637 respondents completed the survey. The respondent sample had a mean age

of 20.5 years, was comprised of 63.6% females, 74.1% Caucasians, and 46.9% freshmen. 16.6%

(106) of the sample self-reported ADHD, and 12.7% (81) self-reported other mental illnesses.

Approximately 35% of sample self-reported misusing prescription stimulants. Eight respondents

self-reported attempting to feign ADHD in a physician’s office. These 8 responses were

excluded from all subsequent analyses because they do not belong in any one of the three

predefined study groups. The breakdown of each of the demographic characteristics across the

three study groups, and the significance of the difference between the three groups is provided in

Table B1.

Table B1: Demographics of the sample

Characteristic

ADHD

N = 102

Malingere

-rs

N = 264

Controls

N = 259

N (%) N (%) N (%) χ2 df p

Age (Mean [SD]; F) 20.9[2.12

]

20.5 [1.68] 20.5

[1.73]

2.38 2, 625 0.093

Female 51 (49.5) 178 (68.2) 173 (65.5) 11.6

5

2 0.003

Ethnicity

28.2

0

12 0.002

Caucasian 88 (85.4) 175 (67) 201 (75.8)

African American 6 (5.8) 51 (19.5) 36 (13.6)

Hispanic or Latino/a 2 (1.9) 5 (1.9) 0 (0)

Asian or Pacific Islander 2 (1.9) 22 (8.4) 19 (7.2)

69

American Indian 0 (0) 1 (0.4) 0 (0)

Biracial or Multiracial 3 (2.9) 6 (2.3) 8 (3.0)

Other 2 (1.9) 0 (0.4) 1 (0.4)

Greek membership 62 (60.2) 116 (44.6) 122 (46.0) 7.73 2 0.021

School year

7.27 6 0.297

Freshman 42 (40.8) 119 (45.6) 135 (50.9)

Sophomore 28 (27.2) 57 (21.8) 62 (23.4)

Junior 20 (19.4) 60 (23.0) 41 (15.5)

Senior (and above) 13 (12.6) 25 (9.6) 27 (10.2)

Residence

21.0

2

10 0.021

On-campus residence hall 44 (42.7) 123 (47.1) 142 (53.6)

Fraternity/Sorority house 9 (8.7) 14 (5.4) 10 (3.8)

Other on-campus housing 1 (1.0) 12 (4.6) 1 (0.4)

Parent/Guardian home 1 (1.0) 7 (2.7) 3 (1.1)

Off-campus housing 48 (46.6) 103 (39.5) 107 (40.4)

Other 0 (0) 2 (0.8) 2 (0.8)

Expected GPA 31.9

7

6 <0.00

1 3.5 to 4 31 (30.1) 122 (46.9) 124 (47.0)

3 to 3.49 37 (35.9) 107 (41.2) 103 (39)

2.5 to 2.99 27 (26.2) 28 (10.8) 30 (11.4)

2 to 2.49 8 (7.8) 3 (1.2) 7 (2.7)

Below 2 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0)

Insurance status 7.15 12 0.848

Private health insurance

from parents/family

80 (77.7) 181 (69.6) 194 (73.2)

Independent private health

insurance plan

7 (6.8) 14 (5.4) 15 (5.7)

Private health insurance

from Employer

4 (3.9) 15 (5.8) 14 (5.3)

No health insurance 5 (4.9) 14 (5.4) 13 (4.9)

State Medicaid plan 2 (1.9) 18 (6.9) 11 (4.2)

Medicare plan 1 (1.0) 8 (3.1) 7 (2.6)

Other 4 (3.9) 10 (3.8) 11 (4.2)

Self-reported ADHD 103 (100) 0 (0) 0 (0) 629.

0

2 <0.00

1 Other mental illnesses 31 (30.4) 23 (8.8) 27 (10.2) 33.2

7

2 <0.00

1 Time since ADHD Diagnosis - - -

70

Less than 1 year ago 14 (13.9) - -

1 to 2 years ago 13 (12.9) - -

3 to 5 years ago 27 (26.7) - -

More than 5 years ago 47 (46.5) - -

Type of physician who

diagnosed ADHD

- - -

Primary Care Provider 11 (10.8) - -

Specialist 83 (81.4) - -

Not sure 8 (7.8) - -

ADHD Diagnosis visit time - - -

Less than 30 minutes 20 (19.4) - -

30 to 60 minutes 36 (35.0) - -

60 to 120 minutes 29 (28.2) - -

Greater than 120 minutes 10 (9.7) - -

Emergency room visits in the

past 6 months

12.6

8

6 0.048

Never 67 (66.3) 150 (57.7) 171 (65.3)

1 to 3 times 29 (28.7) 106 (40.8) 86 (32.8)

4 to 6 times 4 (4.0) 4 (1.5) 5 (1.9)

More than 6 times 1 (1) 0 (0) 0 (0)

Hospital Visits in the past 6

months

1.99 2 0.369

Never 100 (98) 254 (98.1) 253 (96.2)

1 to 3 times 2 (2.0) 5 (1.9) 10 (3.8)

4 to 6 times 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0)

More than 6 times 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0)

Office Visits in the past 6

months

22.7

1

6 <0.00

1 Never 10 (9.8) 49 (18.9) 55 (20.8)

1 to 3 times 63 (61.8) 182 (70.3) 173 (65.5)

4 to 6 times 23 (22.5) 24 (9.3) 31 (11.7)

More than 6 times 6 (5.9) 4 (1.5) 5 (1.9)

Use of stimulants with a valid

Rx

96 (94.1) 9 (3.5) 4 (1.5) 503.

0

4 <0.00

1 Use of stimulants without a

valid Rx

41 (40.2) 81 (31.2) 97 (36.6) 4.50 4 0.342

Frequency of stimulants misuse 2.07 4 0.723

1 to 2 occasions 11 (27.5) 30 (37.0) 37 (38.9)

3 to 5 occasions 12 (30.0) 19 (23.5) 20 (21.1)

71

6 or more occasions 17 (42.5) 32 (39.5) 38 (40.0)

History of illegal use 28.1

6

6 <0.00

1 Less than 1 year ago 8 (20.0) 40 (49.4) 40 (42.1)

1 to 2 years ago 15 (37.5) 27 (33.3) 42 (44.2)

3 to 5 years ago 8 (20.0) 11 (13.6) 11 (11.6)

More than 5 years ago 9 (22.5) 3 (3.7) 2 (2.1)

Reasons for illegal use - - -

Experimental 7 (17.5) 21 (25.9) 22 (23.2)

Academic 33 (82.5) 72 (88.9) 84 (88.4)

Entertainment 2 (0.05) 14 (17.3) 12 (12.6)

Other 7 (17.5) 4 (4.9) 7 (7.4)

Source of stimulants 15.1

8

6 0.019

Borrowed from

family/friends

27 (67.5) 67 (82.7) 76 (0.8)

Purchased from

stranger/friend

10 (25.0) 27 (33.3) 32 (33.7)

Pharmacy 9 (22.5) 1 (1.2) 1 (1.1)

Other 0 0 0

ADHD: Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder; SD: Standard Deviation; df: degrees of

freedom; GPA: Grade Point Average

4.2 Manipulation Checks

The goal of the manipulation check was to ensure that the Malingerer group was

following instructions to feign ADHD correctly. Items included in the manipulation check were

those that were considered to be easy to feign; so the responses of the Malingerer group should

resemble those of the ADHD group very closely. Table B2 shows the means, standard

deviations, and p values of the differences between each of the three groups. Overall, it was

found that the Malingerer group was not significantly different from the ADHD group on all but

one of the items in the manipulation check, indicating that the instructions were clearly

communicated and followed.

72

Table B2: Differences between the three groups among the items used in the Manipulation

check

Item ADHD

Mean

(SD)

Malinge

-rer

Mean

(SD)

Control

Mean

(SD)

ADHD

vs

Maling

-erer p

ADHD

vs

Control

p

Maling-

erer vs

Control

p

I have ADD/ADHD 6.5 (0.9) 5.2 (1.5) 1.5 (1.1) <0.01 <0.01 <0.01

I have difficulty keeping my

focus while reading

6.2 (0.9) 5.9 (1.1) 3.8 (1.8) 0.10 <0.01 <0.01

I have trouble sitting still 5.8 (1.5) 5.6 (1.3) 3.3 (1.8) 0.61 <0.01 <0.01

I tend to act impulsively 5.2 (1.8) 5.1 (1.3) 2.9 (1.7) 0.63 <0.01 <0.01

I have trouble paying attention

in class

6.1 (0.9) 6.0 (1.0) 3.5 (1.8) 0.65 <0.01 <0.01

Note: Each item was scored on a scale of 1 through 7.

4.3 SAMS Items

A total of 10 items were identified from the pretest using a combination of individual

item discriminatory power and the principal components analysis (PCA). Model structure

obtained from the PCA contained 2 factors, the psychological factor and the academic factor,

with 6 items and 4 items, respectively. Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) using the WLSMV

estimator was conducted using the model structured obtained from the PCA. In this measurement

model, each item was allowed to load on only one of the two factors; non-target loadings were

fixed to zero. The chi square statistic for the model fit was found to be 108.153 (df = 34; p <

0.0005), with a Root Mean Square Error of Approximation of 0.078 (90% CI=0.062-0.094),

Weighted Root Mean Square Residual value of 0.752 and a comparative fit index of 0.986,

indicating satisfactory model fit (Hair et al., 2006). Table B3 shows the mean sum subscale score

for the psychological factor and the academic factor in each of the study groups. Both factors

significantly differentiated all three study groups in post-hoc comparisons. The Malingerer group

73

scored significantly higher than the two other groups on both factors, indicating the ability of the

SAMS to identify malingering behavior.

Table B3: Mean sum scale scores on each factor of the Subtle ADHD Malingering

Screener (SAMS) in each study group

Group Psychological factor

Mean (SD)

Academic factor

Mean (SD)

ADHD group 19.7 (7.0) 10.6 (5.6)

Malingerer group 26.4 (7.1) 15.8 (5.7)

Control group 11.0 (5.8) 5.5 (3.0)

Total 18.9 (9.6) 10.7 (6.7)

Note: Both the psychological factor and the academic factor significantly differentiated all

three study groups in post-hoc comparisons (p < 0.05).

Note: The minimum and maximum possible score for each subscale are: Psychological factor

= 6 – 42; Academic factor = 4 – 28.

4.4 Psychometric properties

Cronbach’s alpha for the psychological subscale was found to be 0.909, and that for the

academic subscale was found to be 0.916, indicating satisfactory reliability scores. Convergent

validity was demonstrated by the consistently high factor loadings for each item in the scale, as

seen in Table B4. The correlation between the two subscales was found to be 0.793. The

MODEL TEST option in MPlus was used to test if the correlation between the two latent

variables was different from 1. With a p-value less than 0.0005, the correlation was shown to be

significantly different from 1 indicating significant discriminant validity.

74

Table B4: Means, standard deviations, and effect sizes for each of the three study groups on

each item in the Subtle ADHD Malingering Scale (SAMS)

Item Subscale

Standar-

dized

loadings

ADHD

Mean

(SD)

Malinger

-er Mean

(SD)

Control

Mean

(SD)

ADHD vs

Malingere

-r d

ADHD vs

Control

d

Malingere

-r vs

Control d

Item 1 PSYCH 0.772 3.9 (1.9) 5.2 (1.4) 2.1 (1.6) -0.81 1.11 4.53

Item 2 PSYCH 0.652 4.2 (1.9) 5.1 (1.6) 2.6 (1.9) -0.51 0.83 4.39

Item 3 PSYCH 0.842 3.0 (1.7) 4.4 (1.7) 1.6 (1.1) -0.84 1.14 3.37

Item 4 PSYCH 0.721 2.2 (1.6) 3.4 (1.7) 1.5 (1.2) -0.67 0.53 2.57

Item 5 PSYCH 0.759 2.8 (1.6) 4.1 (1.6) 1.6 (1.1) -0.83 0.90 3.28

Item 6 PSYCH 0.875 3.5 (1.6) 4.3 (1.6) 1.7 (1.3) -0.44 1.33 3.59

Item 7 ACAD 0.763 2.8 (2.1) 4.2 (1.7) 1.5 (1.0) -0.73 0.97 3.18

Item 8 ACAD 0.899 2.5 (1.6) 3.8 (1.7) 1.3 (0.8) -0.75 1.08 2.83

Item 9 ACAD 0.883 2.8 (1.8) 4.1 (1.7) 1.3 (0.9) -0.79 1.15 3.10

Item 10 ACAD 0.925 2.5 (1.6) 3.8 (1.7) 1.4 (0.9) -0.76 0.95 2.88

Note: All items significantly differentiated all three groups in post-hoc analyses; Factor scores are

the standardized factor estimates obtained from the Confirmatory Factor Analysis. ‘d’ indicates

effect size.

Note: Items have been blinded to protect the distribution of the scale. Please contact the authors

for the full scale.

.

75

DISCUSSION

5. Discussion

This study is the first of its kind to develop a scale tailored toward identification of

malingering in ADHD. As such, it offers great potential for direct use in the clinical setting. A

ten-item, two-factor solution was obtained for the SAMS in this study. The CFA showed

satisfactory model fit. With an RMSEA of 0.078 and a CFI value of 0.986, the SAMS scale

meets the Hair et al. (2006) criteria of an RMSEA value less than 0.1 and a CFI greater than 0.9

necessary for a satisfactory model fit (Hair et al., 2006). Each of the ten items in the final factor

solution showed high factor loadings (>0.6), and significant differentiation among the three study

groups. The scale showed satisfactory reliability (Cronbach’s alpha > 0.9), and construct validity

(AVE > 0.5) validity (Hair et al., 2006). Each individual SAMS item was also found to

significantly differentiate all three groups in the study, and not just the ADHD group and the

Malingerer group.

The sum scale score was used to calculate the total score for the SAMS in order to

preserve ease of scoring, and reduce time of administration in the physician’s office. The mean

scores on both the psychological and academic factor follow patterns predicted by Lanyon

(1997) in the Accuracy of Knowledge framework. The Malingerer group consistently endorsed

items more than the ADHD group or the Control group, and this response behavior can help

identify the Malingerer group.

76

The respondent sample obtained in this study is fairly representative of the demographics

of the University of Mississippi’s student population (University of Mississippi Office of

Institutional Research, Effectiveness, and Planning, 2016). Several of the demographic

characteristics were found to be significantly different among the three study groups. While

some of these, such as a higher incidence of comorbid mental illnesses in the ADHD group

versus other groups, were expected, some others, such as higher Greek membership in the

ADHD group, were unexpected. More than 35 of the sample self-report misuse of stimulants

which was in line with findings from Garnier-Dykstra et al. (2012). Over 80% of the individuals

in the ADHD group were diagnosed by a specialist, and more than 80% also spent greater than

30 minutes during their ADHD diagnosis visit. About 46% of the ADHD group were also

diagnosed over 5 years ago. These findings support the validity of the self-reported ADHD

measure against the presence of possible malingerers within the ADHD group.

The success of the experimental design was crucial to the development of the SAMS. The

five items used in the manipulation check clearly indicate that the instructions used in the

Malingerer group were simple, clear, and easy to follow. However, the Malingerer group and the

ADHD group significantly differed (p < 0.01; Table 3) on one manipulation check item: “I have

ADD/ADHD.” This finding was not unexpected, because the instructions provided to the

malingerers asked them to imagine visiting the physician in order to obtain an ADHD diagnosis,

implying that they had not been diagnosed yet (See supplement-1 for the Malingerer instruction

set).

77

5.1 Limitations

This scale was developed using an experimental design, rather than a real-world

malingerer population. This design was chosen to facilitate ease of identification of the

malingerers and to help provide the required sample size in the Malingerer group. Despite the

use of additional incentives, the Malingerer group in the study may not have been representative

of the true malingerer population in the real world. The experimental design also prioritizes

internal validity over external validity, which makes it difficult to generalize the psychometrics

of the SAMS, calculated from one University population, to the real world setting. Finally, the

ability of the SAMS to detect malingering of ADHD reduces potential access points for abusers,

but does not directly reduce the problem of prescription stimulant abuse. Further testing and

interventions need to be developed in order to achieve that goal.

5.2 Clinical Implications and Future Research

The SAMS presents an innovative approach to identify malingering of ADHD symptoms,

reduce overdiagnosis of ADHD, and for early identification of prescription stimulant abuse

directly in the clinical setting. It is designed to be short, inexpensive, and does not need

additional training for administration, scoring or evaluation. These features make it a valuable

resource for primary care providers. While further research is needed to develop a cutoff score,

and calculate the sensitivity and specificity of the SAMS, this study shows the potential this scale

holds for future clinical use. Future research in this area needs to test the ability of SAMS to

resist faking in the real world and compare it with existing subtle scales that may be used for

similar purposes. There is also a need to focus on the needs of malingerers identified by this

scale, and to develop interventions that can help malingerers cope with their stressors and

78

address potential addiction problems. Finally, this study successfully demonstrates the

methodology of developing a subtle scale with the application of a theoretical framework for

item development. This technique can be applied in several other areas in healthcare such as the

early identification of opioid abusers, injection drug users, and others to implement early

targeted interventions

.

79

BIBLIOGRAPHY

80

Bavarian, N., Flay, B. R., Ketcham, P. L., & Smith, E. (2013). Illicit use of prescription

stimulants in a college student sample: A theory-guided analysis. Drug and Alcohol

Dependence, 132(3), 665-673.

Binder, L. M. (1992). Deception and malingering. In A. E. Puente & R. J. McCaffrey (Eds.),

Handbook of Neuropsychological Assessment: A Biopsychosocial Perspective (Critical

Issues in Neuropsychology) (pp. 353–374). New York: Plenum.

Bollinger, L. C., Bush, C., Califano, J. A., et al. (2005). Under the counter: The diversion and

abuse of controlled prescription drugs in the U.S. The National Center on Addiction and

Substance Abuse at Columbia University (CASA). Retrieved from:

http://www.casacolumbia.org/addiction-research/reports/under-the-counter-diversion-

abuse-controlled-perscription-drugs. Accessed on: July 9th 2015.

Booksh, R. L., Pella, R. D., Singh, A. N., & Gouvier, W. D. (2009). Ability of college students to

simulate ADHD on objective measures of attention. Journal of Attention Disorders.

31(4), 325-338.

Burkhart, B. R., Gynther, M. D., & Christian, W. L. (1978). Psychological mindedness,

intelligence, and item subtlety endorsement patterns on the MMPI. Journal of Clinical

Psychology, 34(1), 76-79.

Buss, A. H. (1959). The effect of item style on social desirability and frequency of endorsement.

Journal of Consulting Psychology, 23(6), 510.

81

Cattell, R. B. (1966). The scree test for the number of factors. Multivariate Behavioral

Research, 1(2), 245-276.

Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1995). Constructing validity: Basic issues in objective scale

development. Psychological Assessment, 7(3), 309.

Chen, L. Y., Crum, R. M., Strain, E. C., Alexander, G. C., Kaufmann, C., & Mojtabai, R. (2016).

Prescriptions, nonmedical use, and emergency department visits involving prescription

stimulants. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 77(3), 297-304.

Cofer, C. N., Chance, J., & Judson, A. J. (1949). A study of malingering on the Minnesota

Multiphasic Personality Inventory. The Journal of Psychology, 27(2), 491-499.

Comrey, A. L. (1988). Factor-analytic methods of scale development in personality and clinical

psychology. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56(5), 754.

DeVellis, R. F. (2011). Scale Development: Theory and Applications (Vol. 26). Sage

Publications.

Fisher, A. B., & Watkins, M. W. (2008). ADHD rating scales' susceptibility to faking in a

college student sample. Journal of Postsecondary Education and Disability, 20(2), 81-92.

Fornell, C., & Larcker, D. F. (1981). Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable

variables and measurement error. Journal of Marketing Research, 39-50.

Franklin, S. B., Gibson, D. J., Robertson, P. A., Pohlmann, J. T., & Fralish, J. S. (1995). Parallel

analysis: a method for determining significant principal components. Journal of

Vegetation Science, 6(1), 99-106.

82

Garnier-Dykstra, L. M., Caldeira, K. M., Vincent, K. B., O’Grady, K. E., & Arria, A. M. (2012).

Nonmedical use of prescription stimulants during college: Four-year trends in exposure

opportunity, use, motives, and sources. Journal of American College Health, 60(3), 226-

234.

Gerbing, D. W., & Anderson, J. C. (1988). An updated paradigm for scale development

incorporating unidimensionality and its assessment. Journal of Marketing Research, 186-

192.

Hair, J. F., Black, W.C., Babin, B. J., Anderson, R. E., & Tatham, R. L. (2006). Multivariate

Data Analysis. Pearson Education.

Harrison, A. G. (2006). Adults faking ADHD: You must be kidding! The ADHD Report, 14(4),

1-7.

Harp, J. P., Jasinski, L. J., Shandera-Ochsner, A. L., Mason, L. H., & Berry, D. T. (2011).

Detection of malingered ADHD using the MMPI-2-RF. Psychological Injury and

Law, 4(1), 32-43.

Hathaway, S. R., & McKinley, J. C. (1940). A multiphasic personality schedule (Minnesota): I.

Construction of the schedule. Journal of Psychology, 10, 249-254.

Hinkin, T. R. (1995). A review of scale development practices in the study of organizations.

Journal of Management, 21(5), 967-988.

Jachimowicz, G., & Geiselman, R. E. (2004). Comparison of ease of falsification of attention

deficit hyperactivity disorder diagnosis using standard behavioral rating scales. Cognitive

Science Online, 2(1), 6-20.

83

Jackson, D. N. (1966). A modern strategy for personality assessment: The Personality Research

Form. Research Bulletin, (30).

Jasinski, L. J., & Ranseen, J. D. (2011). Malingered ADHD evaluations: A further complication

for accommodations reviews. The Bar Examiner, 79, 6-16.

Johnston, L. D., O’Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., & Schulenberg, J. E. (2011). Monitoring the

Future national survey results on drug use, 1975–2010: Volume II, College students and

adults ages 19– 50. Ann Arbor: Institute for Social Research, The University of

Michigan.

Johnston, L. D., O’Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., Schulenberg, J. E. & Miech, R. A. (2016).

Monitoring the Future national survey results on drug use, 1975– 2015: Volume 2,

College students and adults ages 19–55. Ann Arbor: Institute for Social Research, The

University of Michigan.

Lanyon, R. I. (1970). Development and validation of a psychological screening inventory.

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 35(1p2), 1-24.

Lanyon, R. I. (1997). Detecting deception: Current models and directions. Clinical Psychology:

Science and Practice, 4(4), 377-387.

Lees-Haley, P. R., English, L. T., & Glenn, W. J. (1991). A fake bad scale on the MMPI-2 for

personal injury claimants. Psychological Reports, 68(1), 203-210.

Lessenger, J. E., & Feinberg, S. D. (2008). Abuse of prescription and over-the-counter

medications. Journal of the American Board of Family Medicine, 21(1), 45 – 54.

84

Loevinger, J. (1957). Objective tests as instruments of psychological theory. Psychological

Reports, 3(3), 635-694.

Maxwell, J. C. (2011). The prescription drug epidemic in the United States: a perfect

storm. Drug and Alcohol Review, 30(3), 264-270.

McCabe, S. E., West, B. T., Teter, C. J., & Boyd, C. J. (2014). Trends in medical use, diversion,

and nonmedical use of prescription medications among college students from 2003 to

2013: Connecting the dots. Addictive Behaviors, 39(7), 1176-1182.

Miller, G. A. (1985). The substance abuse subtle screening inventory (SASSI) manual.

Springville, IN: SASSI Institute.

Musso, M. W., & Gouvier, W. D. (2012). Why is this so hard?: A review of detection of

malingered ADHD in college students. Journal of Attention Disorders,

1087054712441970.

Myerholtz, L. E., & Rosenberg, H. (1997). Screening DUI offenders for alcohol problems:

Psychometric assessment of the substance abuse subtle screening inventory. Psychology

of Addictive Behaviors, 11(3), 155.

Myerholtz, L., & Rosenberg, H. (1998). Screening college students for alcohol problems:

Psychometric assessment of the SASSI-2. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs,

59(4), 439.

Newsom, J. (2015). Practical Approaches to Dealing with Nonnormal and Categorical Variables.

[PDF document] Retrieved from:

http://web.pdx.edu/~newsomj/semclass/ho_estimate2.pdf

85

Nunnally, J. (1978). Psychometric Methods (2nd ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

O’Connor, B. P. (2000). Using parallel analysis and Velicer’s MAP Test. Behavior Research

Methods, Instruments & Computers, 32, 396-402.

Quinn, C. A. (2003). Detection of malingering in assessment of adult ADHD. Archives of

Clinical Neuropsychology, 18(4), 379-395.

Rhemtulla, M., Brosseau-Liard, P. E., & Savalei, V. (2012). When can categorical variables be

treated as continuous? A comparison of robust continuous and categorical SEM

estimation methods under suboptimal conditions. Psychological Methods, 17(3), 354.

Rios, J., & Morey, L. C. (2013). Detecting Feigned ADHD in Later Adolescence: An

Examination of Three PAI–A Negative Distortion Indicators. Journal of Personality

Assessment, 95(6), 594-599.

Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. (2010). Results from the 2009

National Survey on Drug Use and Health: Volume I. Summary of National Findings

(Office of Applied Studies, NSDUH Series H-38A, HHS Publication No. SMA 10-

4586Findings). Rockville, MD. Retrieved from:

http://www.gmhc.org/files/editor/file/a_pa_nat_drug_use_survey.pdf

Slick, D. J., Sherman, E. M., & Iverson, G. L. (1999). Diagnostic criteria for malingered

neurocognitive dysfunction: Proposed standards for clinical practice and research. The

Clinical Neuropsychologist, 13(4), 545-561.

Suhr, J., Hammers, D., Dobbins-Buckland, K., Zimak, E., & Hughes, C. (2008). The relationship

of malingering test failure to self-reported symptoms and neuropsychological findings in

86

adults referred for ADHD evaluations. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 23, 521–

530.

Sullivan, B. K., May, K., & Galbally, L. (2007). Symptom exaggeration by college adults in

attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and learning disorder assessments. Applied

Neuropsychology, 14, 189–207.

Tucha, L., Sontag, T. A., Walitza, S., & Lange, K. W. (2009). Detection of malingered attention

deficit hyperactivity disorder. ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 1(1),

47-53.

University of Mississippi Office of Institutional Research, Effectiveness, and Planning. (n.d.)

Fall 2016-2017 Enrollment. Retrieved from: http://irep.olemiss.edu/fall-2016-2017-

enrollment/

Visser, S. N., Bitsko, R. H., Danielson, M. L., Perou, R., & Blumberg, S. J. (2010). Increasing

prevalence of parent-reported attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder among children—

United States, 2003 and 2007. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 59(44), 1439-

1443.

Wooley, C. N., Rogers, R., Fiduccia, C. E., & Kelsey, K. (2012). The effectiveness of substance

use measures in the detection of full and partial denial of drug use. Assessment,

1073191112447098.

Young, J. C., & Gross, A. M. (2011). Detection of response bias and noncredible performance in

adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of Clinical

Neuropsychology, 26(3), 165-175.

87

Zgierska, A., Miller, M., & Rabago, D. (2012). Patient satisfaction, prescription drug abuse, and

potential unintended consequences. Journal of the American Medical

Association, 307(13), 1377-1378.

88

APPENDIX

89

APPENDIX – A: Malingerer Group Instructions

“Imagine yourself having trouble in school. Things aren’t working out as you planned but your

counselor’s only advice is to buckle down. You want to get some help. You hear about adult

ADHD on a television show. When talking to a friend about it, your friend tells you that you

could get special accommodations from the university, like untimed tests and rescheduling of

exams if two are given on the same day. Your friend adds that the stimulant medications that are

generally prescribed have minimal side effects and that you can take the medicine only when you

need it, just for school. You decide to read a book on ADHD. You find out that some ADHD

adults even collect social security benefits. You conclude that you have enough of the symptoms.

You convince yourself that you have ADHD. You go to the doctor and you really want to get

help. In order to get these benefits, you need to convincingly act like a person who has ADHD.”

Source: Quinn, C. A. (2003). Detection of malingering in assessment of adult ADHD. Archives

of Clinical Neuropsychology, 18(4), 379-395.

90

CHAPTER 4: PAPER 3

PSYCHOMETRIC VALIDATION OF THE SUBTLE ADHD MALINGERING SCALE

(SAMS) AND COMPARISON OF DIAGNOSTIC ACCURACY WITH THE PERSONALITY

ASSESSMENT INVENTORY (PAI)

Formatted to the requirements of the Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology

91

Title: Psychometric Validation of the Subtle ADHD Malingering Scale (SAMS) and Comparison

of Diagnostic Accuracy with the Personality Assessment Inventory (PAI)

Sujith Ramachandran, Department of Pharmacy Administration, University of Mississippi,

University, MS, USA – 38655. Email: [email protected]; Phone: 6623803006; Fax:

6629155102 (Corresponding author)

Dr. John P. Bentley, Department of Pharmacy Administration, University of Mississippi,

University, MS, USA – 38655.

Dr. Erin R. Holmes, Department of Pharmacy Administration, University of Mississippi,

University, MS, USA – 38655.

Dr. Meagen Rosenthal, Department of Pharmacy Administration, University of Mississippi,

University, MS, USA – 38655.

Dr. Benjamin F. Banahan III, Department of Pharmacy Administration, University of

Mississippi, University, MS, USA – 38655.

Dr. John Young, Department of Psychology, University of Mississippi, University, MS, USA –

38655.

Running title: (60 characters) Psychometric Validation Of Subtle ADHD Malingering Screener

Supplement data: None

Word count: 2934 (Abstract: 241)

92

ABSTRACT

Objective Development of a cut-off score and psychometric testing of the Subtle ADHD

Malingering Screener (SAMS), and comparison of its diagnostic accuracy with the Personality

Inventory Assessment – Adult (PAI).

Method This study employs a cross-sectional experimental design using a self-administered

computer-based survey distributed to conveniently sampled groups of college students with and

without ADHD. Respondents were classified into three experimental groups: an ADHD group, a

Control group, and a Malingerer group, comprised of individuals who were instructed to feign

ADHD. ROC curve analysis and CART analysis were used to determine a cutoff score.

Diagnostic accuracy of the SAMS was compared to the Personality Assessment Inventory –

Adult.

Results Using a combination of ROC curve analysis and CART analysis, respondents were

classified as malingerers if their SAMS psychological factor score was greater than 15, and the

SAMS academic factor score was greater than 7. The sensitivity of the SAMS for detecting

malingering was found to be 90.4%, with a specificity of 80.1%. The SAMS showed superior

performance compared to the PAI, as it had significantly higher sensitivity (90% vs 51%; p <

0.0005).

Conclusion This is the first study to estimate the diagnostic accuracy of the SAMS. The SAMS

is recommended for use in the primary care setting setting to identify patients who need

additional testing to confirm an ADHD diagnosis. Further research is needed to test the

93

generalizability of these findings. The SAMS can help reduce overdiagnosis of ADHD and

potential misuse of stimulants by helping to identify malingering of ADHD symptoms.

94

INTRODUCTION

1. Introduction

Exaggeration of symptoms of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) or

malingering of ADHD in the physician’s office is prevalent in 20 to 50% of college students

(Suhr et al., 2008; Sullivan, May & Galbally, 2007; Harrison, 2006) and is on the rise due to the

increasing abuse of prescription stimulants and the presence of external incentives to encourage

malingering (Chen et al., 2016; Slick, Sherman & Iverson, 1999; Binder, 1992). Most attempts to

malinger ADHD are successful because research suggests that faking of ADHD symptoms is

fairly easy (Quinn, 2003; Fisher & Watkins, 2008; Harp et al., 2011; Jachimowicz & Geiselman,

2004).

1.1 Challenges in Detection of ADHD Malingering

Tools commonly used for confirming a diagnosis of ADHD include several scales such as

self-report inventories, observer symptom ratings, cognitive measures, test of executive

functions, and symptom validity tests (SVTs). Several researchers have shown that most of these

tools have poor sensitivity toward malingering. Self-report inventories such as the ADHD

Behavior Checklist have been shown to be ineffective at detection of malingering (Quinn, 2003).

Jachimowicz & Geiselman (2004) found that the falsification rate, or the percentage of students

who can successfully malinger on a given instrument, was 65% on the Wender Utah Rating

Scale (WURS), 75% on the ADHD Rating Scale IV (ARS), 90% on the Conners Adult ADHD

Rating Scale (CAARS), and 95% on the Brown Adult ADHD Scale (BAAS). Fisher & Watkins

95

(2008) found that 77% and 93% of students successfully faked on the ADHD behavior checklist

and the College ADHD Response Evaluation (CARE).

SVTs are the only tools that have been shown to offer moderately good sensitivities toward

malingering (Sollman, Ranseen & Berry, 2010). Quinn (2003) found that the Integrated Visual

and Auditory Continuous Performance Test (IVA CPT) had sensitivity and specificity above

90%. Rios & Morey (2013) found that the subtle scales such as Negative Impression scale (NIM)

and Rogers Discriminant Function (RDF) subscales of the Personal Assessment Inventory (PAI)

showed satisfactory sensitivity (64% and 84%, respectively). Marshall et al. (2010) found that

combining two or more SVTs, or one SVT and one cognitive test, can offer sensitivities and

specificities in the range of 95% and 98% respectively. Harrison, Edwards & Parker (2007)

found that malingerers obtained higher scores on the CAARS and lower scores on the Woodcock

Johnson Psychoeducational Battery – III (WJPB – III), than individuals who have ADHD, and

proposed that these two tests be used in combination to identify malingerers. However, Jasinski

et al. (2011) tested a wide range of behavioral rating scales, cognitive tests, and SVTs and found

that most students were able to fake their symptoms on the all the scales tested. Though there is a

lack of consensus in the literature, some SVTs, and some cognitive tests, may offer adequate

sensitivity to malingering. However, their potential is limited by their drawbacks. SVTs and

other cognitive performance tests are time consuming, complex, impose heavy respondent

burden and often need computerized testing and scoring and require training for interpretation of

scores.

1.2 Subtle Scales

Subtlety in scale development is the lack of face validity in the items of a given instrument. It

is defined as “degree to which the psychopathological meaning of an item can be determined in

96

an a priori fashion” (Burkhart, Gynther & Christian, 1978). The disadvantages of SVTS can be

overcome by subtle scales. Subtle scales hold an advantage over traditional SVTs in detecting

malingering because their lack of face validity makes faking un-intuitive. Most subtle scales

suffer from poor sensitivity at detecting malingering of ADHD because they were not

specifically developed to detect malingering in the context of ADHD (Musso & Gouvier, 2012).

The newly developed subtle scale, the Subtle ADHD Malingering Scale (SAMS), grounded in

the Accuracy of Knowledge framework (Lanyon, 1997), was developed for to be a short, simple,

and easy tool for administration in the clinical setting to detect malingering of ADHD. This scale

was also shown to have acceptable factor validity, reliability, and construct validity

(Ramachandran et al., in preparation).

1.3 Objective

This study aims to calculate the sensitivity and specificity of the SAMS to malingering of

ADHD and to test its resistance to malingering compared to the PAI.

97

METHODS

2. Methods

2.1 Study Design

This study employed a cross-sectional, experimental design using a self-administered,

computer-based survey distributed to conveniently sampled groups of college-enrolled adults

with and without ADHD. The study design and sample were previously explained in detail in

Ramachandran et al. (in preparation). Approval was obtained from the University of Mississippi

IRB.

2.2 Study Participants and Data Collection

This study sampled from the undergraduate population at the University of Mississippi.

Students with ADHD and students without ADHD were both sampled in order to identify the

scale’s ability to differentiate individuals who actually have ADHD from those who are

malingering symptoms. Respondents were assigned to one of three groups as part of the

experimental design. Respondents who self-reported ADHD were instructed to respond to the

test item pool honestly; this was called the ADHD group. Respondents who self-reported not

having ADHD were randomized to one of two groups where they were either instructed to fake

ADHD on the test item pool, called the Malingerer group, or instructed to respond honestly,

called the Control group. Self-reported ADHD was validated by asking respondents the time

98

since their ADHD diagnosis, the time spent during ADHD diagnostic visit, and the specialty of

the diagnosing practitioner.

Study recruitment was conducted through a series of announcements conducted in classes

held across the campus of University of Mississippi, in order to obtain a representative sample.

All participants were offered extra course credit as an incentive for participation. Additional

incentives, in the form of a chance to win one out of five $25 gift cards, were offered to

individuals assigned to the Malingerer group in order to match real world incentives for

malingering. Interested students set up appointments with the research team, and completed the

survey in person on a computer assigned to them. The survey was designed using Qualtrics, and

all data were collected anonymously.

2.3 Survey Design

Respondents were asked to indicate their age, gender, ADHD diagnosis, and other

demographic information before responding to the SAMS. To provide a comparator to the

SAMS, the Personality Assessment Inventory – Adult (PAI - A) was administered at the end of

the survey. The PAI was chosen because it contains multiple validity scales such as the Negative

Impression scale (NIM), the Positive Impression Scale (PIM), the Malingering Index (MAL) and

the Rogers Discriminant Function (RDF). These subscales showed satisfactory sensitivity and

specificity toward malingering of ADHD (Rios & Morey, 2013).

2.4 Data Analysis

All data were analyzed using IBM SPSS (Chicago, IL). Demographic characteristics

were compared across all three study groups using t-test and chi square, as appropriate.

99

2.4.1 Selecting a Cut-off Point

In order to calculate an optimal cut-off point, a combination of ROC curve analysis and

CART analysis was used. ROC curve analysis is the most popular technique for estimating an

efficient cut-off score for a diagnostic test (Fletcher, Fletcher & Fletcher, 2014). However, the

items in the SAMS are expected to have different distributions because they were grounded in

different constructs of the Accuracy of Knowledge framework (Lanyon, 1997; Ramachandran et

al., in preparation). Using a total scale score of such a combination of items to calculate a cut-off

point might cause a loss in the detail of information provided by each type of question. In order

to effectively utilize all the subscales of the SAMS to provide a simple, but effective scoring

algorithm, Classification and Regression Trees (CART) analysis was also used in the calculation

of a cut-off score (Moisen, 2008).

CART analysis is an advanced technique of calculating cut-off points for scales that

attempt to distinguish one group of individuals from another (Breiman et al., 1984). It provides

results which can be simple, yet powerful and easily visualized, especially in cases where more

than two groups might be present in the data (Franck, 2013). Weigel, Meston & Rosen (2005)

recommend that a combination of ROC analysis followed by CART analysis be used to estimate

cut-off points. Following the estimation of a cut-off score, psychometric indices such as

sensitivity, specificity, false positive rate, and false negative rate were calculated. The ability of

the scale to resist malingering can be estimated from the falsification rate or the false negative

rate.

100

2.4.2 Comparison with Pre-existing Scales

The comparison of the performance of the SAMS and the PAI was conducted using a

concordance & discordance analysis, as used in Myerholtz & Rosenberg (1997). To compare the

degree of correspondence between the two screening instruments, a phi coefficient (Cheetam &

Hazel, 1969; Kuhn, 1973) and a Cohen’s kappa (Cohen, 1960) were calculated. The McNemar

test was also used to compare the sensitivity & specificity of the two scales (Cheetam & Hazel,

1969).

101

RESULTS

3. Results

3.1 Demographic characteristics

The data collection effort resulted in a total of 637 completed responses. The

demographic characteristics of the respondents are presented in Table 1. The respondent sample

had a mean age of 20.5 years, was comprised of 63.6% females, 74.1% Caucasians, and 46.9%

freshmen. 16.6% (106) of the sample self-reported ADHD, and 12.7% (81) self-reported other

mental illnesses. 35.3% of the total sample self-reported misusing prescription stimulants. Eight

respondents self-reported attempting to feign ADHD in a physician’s office. These 8 responses

were excluded from all subsequent analyses because they did not fit into any one of the three

predefined study groups. The breakdown of each of the demographic characteristics across the

three study groups is provided in Table C1.

102

Table C1: Demographics of the sample

Characteristic

ADHD

N = 102

Malingere

rs

N = 264

Controls

N = 259

N (%) N (%) N (%) χ2 df p

Age (Mean [SD]; F) 20.9[2.12

]

20.5 [1.68] 20.5

[1.73]

2.38 2, 625 0.093

Female 51 (49.5) 178 (68.2) 173 (65.5) 11.6

5

2 0.003

Ethnicity

28.2

0

12 0.002

Caucasian 88 (85.4) 175 (67) 201 (75.8)

African American 6 (5.8) 51 (19.5) 36 (13.6)

Hispanic or Latino/a 2 (1.9) 5 (1.9) 0 (0)

Asian or Pacific Islander 2 (1.9) 22 (8.4) 19 (7.2)

American Indian 0 (0) 1 (0.4) 0 (0)

Biracial or Multiracial 3 (2.9) 6 (2.3) 8 (3.0)

Other 2 (1.9) 0 (0.4) 1 (0.4)

Greek membership 62 (60.2) 116 (44.6) 122 (46.0) 7.73 2 0.021

School year

7.27 6 0.297

Freshman 42 (40.8) 119 (45.6) 135 (50.9)

Sophomore 28 (27.2) 57 (21.8) 62 (23.4)

Junior 20 (19.4) 60 (23.0) 41 (15.5)

Senior (and above) 13 (12.6) 25 (9.6) 27 (10.2)

Residence

21.0

2

10 0.021

On-campus residence hall 44 (42.7) 123 (47.1) 142 (53.6)

Fraternity/Sorority house 9 (8.7) 14 (5.4) 10 (3.8)

Other on-campus housing 1 (1.0) 12 (4.6) 1 (0.4)

Parent/Guardian home 1 (1.0) 7 (2.7) 3 (1.1)

Off-campus housing 48 (46.6) 103 (39.5) 107 (40.4)

Other 0 (0) 2 (0.8) 2 (0.8)

Expected GPA 31.9

7

6 <0.00

1 3.5 to 4 31 (30.1) 122 (46.9) 124 (47.0)

3 to 3.49 37 (35.9) 107 (41.2) 103 (39)

2.5 to 2.99 27 (26.2) 28 (10.8) 30 (11.4)

2 to 2.49 8 (7.8) 3 (1.2) 7 (2.7)

Below 2 0 (0) 0 (0) 0 (0)

Insurance status 7.15 12 0.848

103

Private health insurance

from parents/family

80 (77.7) 181 (69.6) 194 (73.2)

Independent private health

insurance plan

7 (6.8) 14 (5.4) 15 (5.7)

Private health insurance

from Employer

4 (3.9) 15 (5.8) 14 (5.3)

No health insurance 5 (4.9) 14 (5.4) 13 (4.9)

State Medicaid plan 2 (1.9) 18 (6.9) 11 (4.2)

Medicare plan 1 (1.0) 8 (3.1) 7 (2.6)

Other 4 (3.9) 10 (3.8) 11 (4.2)

Self-reported ADHD 103 (100) 0 (0) 0 (0) 629.

0

2 <0.00

1 ADHD: Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder; SD: Standard Deviation; df: degrees of

freedom; GPA: Grade Point Average

3.2 Estimation of a Cut-off Score & Psychometric Indices

As seen in Figure 1, the ROC Curve analysis using the total sum scale score of the

SAMS was found to provide significant discrimination between the malingerers and the honest

respondents, with an area under the curve of 0.901 (Standard error: 0.012; p: < 0.0001). The

selected cut-off from the ROC curve analysis was a total SAMS score greater than 27;

respondents greater than this score were classified as malingerers. This score offered a sensitivity

of 89.2% and a specificity of 77.8%.

104

NOTE: AUC = 0.901; Standard error = 0.0.012; p < 0.0001

The risk estimate for the CART analysis, as seen in figure 2, was 0.239, with a standard

error of 0.017. The cutoff score obtained from CART analysis utilized each individual factor

score. Respondents were classified as malingerers if their psychological scale score was greater

than 15 and the academic scale score was greater than 7. This classification provided a sensitivity

of 90.3% and a specificity of 80.1%, both superior to the results of the ROC curve analysis. For

the remainder of this study, the classification algorithm from the CART analysis was used for the

SAMS scores. Given these values of sensitivity & specificity, the classification algorithm

105

provided by the CART analysis was found to have a false positive rate of 19.9% and a false

negative rate of 9.7%.

Figure 2: CART Analysis Tree diagram

NOTE “Psych”: Psychological subscale; “Acad”: Academic subscale

The results of the CART analysis can also be used to differentiate all three study groups.

The respondents who score less than or equal to 15 on the Psychological factor are comprised of

a majority of the Control group, and those scoring greater than 15 on the Psychological factor

and less than or equal to 7 on the Academic factor were mostly comprised of the ADHD group.

The classification accuracy of the ROC curve analysis and the CART analysis in each study

subgroup is provided in Table C2.

ADHD group: 102

Malingerer group: 264

Control group: 258

Study Subgroup (N)

ADHD group: 28

Malingerer group: 15

Control group: 212

Psych ≤ 15

ADHD group: 74

Malingerer group: 249

Control group: 46

Psych > 15

ADHD group: 25

Malingerer group: 10

Control group: 23

Acad ≤ 7

ADHD group: 49

Malingerer group: 239

Control group: 23

Acad > 7

106

Table C2: Classification accuracy of the SAMS and PAI in each study group

Scale/Group ADHD group

N (%)

Malingerer group

N (%)

Control group

N (%)

ROC curve classification

Malingerer 56 (56.6) 231 (89.2) 22 (8.7)

Honest respondent 43 (43.4) 28 (10.8) 231 (91.3)

Total 99 259 253

CART classification

Malingerer 48 (48.5) 234 (90.3) 22 (8.7)

Honest respondent 51 (51.5) 25 (9.7) 231 (91.3)

Total 99 259 253

PAI classification

Malingerer 14 (15.1) 129 (50.6) 23 (9.2)

Honest respondent 79 (84.9) 126 (49.4) 226 (90.8)

Total 93 255 249

NOTE: ROC curve classification rule: Total SAMS score greater than 27; CART

classification rule: SAMS psychological factor score greater than 15 and SAMS academic

factor score greater than 7; PAI classification rule: Negative Impression Scale score greater

than 92 or Malingering scale score greater than 3. The “total” provi

ded is the number of completed respondents for each scale, which is different for the PAI and

the SAMS.

3.3 Comparator Scale

This study used the PAI as a comparator to the SAMS. The classification rules for the

PAI were obtained from previous research identifying malingering of ADHD using the PAI

(Rios & Morley, 2013). Rios & Morley (2013) used three rules to classify a PAI respondent as a

malingerer: a Negative Impression Scale (NIM) score greater than 92, and a Malingering (MAL)

scale score greater than 3, and a Roger’s Discriminant Function (RDF) greater than 0. However,

107

because the RDF was based on a large number of items, each with possible missing values, there

were a large number of missing values in the RDF variable; and it had to be dropped from the

classification rule. In order to provide the PAI the best chance of identifying malingerers, a rule

of either an NIM score greater than 92 or a MAL score greater than 3 was used, essentially

stacking the odds against the SAMS. Using this classification rule, the PAI provided a sensitivity

of 51.0% and a specificity of 89.2%.

The total degree of agreement between the PAI and the SAMS was found to be (417 out

of 592) 70.4%. A phi coefficient of 0.455 and a Cohen’s kappa of 0.408 were found for the two

scales, indicating a moderate degree of agreement. The McNemar test was also used to compare

the sensitivity and the specificity of the two scales. The SAMS was shown to have significantly

higher sensitivity (p < 0.0005) and lower specificity (p < 0.0005) than the PAI.

108

DISCUSSION

4. Discussion

This study provides critical research for the further development of the Subtle ADHD

Malingering Scale (SAMS) and builds value for its use in the clinical setting. The study used

data collected in the Ramachandran et al. (in preparation) research study to develop a cut-off

score for SAMS and estimate its classification accuracy. A combination of ROC curve analysis

and CART analysis were used to estimate the cut-off score. The CART analysis classification

rule showed moderate improvement over that of the ROC curve analysis in terms of sensitivity

and specificity. But the CART analysis is truly superior to the ROC curve analysis because it

utilizes separate scoring rules for each of the factors in the SAMS, thereby making it harder to

fake. It also provides an opportunity to distinguish all three study groups, instead of merely the

malingerer group. This ability can provide advantages in the clinical setting to distinguish the

two honest study groups: the ADHD group and the Control group. Attempts to classify all three

groups might also provide further confirmation of the ADHD diagnosis in the physician’s office.

The multiple cut off scores provided by the CART analysis can also make it harder to ‘coach’ to

malinger on the SAMS, because successful malingering will require that the scores fall within a

narrower range.

The CART classification rule comes with its own drawbacks. The risk estimate from the

CART analysis was found to be 0.239, indicating that 23.9% of the respondent pool was

misclassified by this technique. The misclassification rate is highest in the ADHD group at

109

48.5%. This high false positive rate among ADHD respondents indicates that the results of the

SAMS should be interpreted with caution in the clinical setting. Being classified as a

‘malingerer’ by the SAMS at a physician’s office should signal the need for additional testing, or

a referral to a specialist, in the case of primary care providers. This recommendation for

additional testing can maximize efficiency of resource use by only recommending additional

testing or referral in some of the patients reporting ADHD symptoms, instead of all of them.

The comparator scale used in this study, the PAI, contains 348 items and includes a

complex scoring algorithm. As such its utility in the physician’s office is limited. Rios & Morley

(2013) found that the NIM scale provided a sensitivity and specificity of 64% and 73%,

respectively, while the MAL scale provided a sensitivity and specificity of 38% and 81%. This

study used a combination of the NIM and the MAL scales to identify malingering and found a

sensitivity and specificity of 51% and 89%, respectively. Despite using fewer items, the SAMS

showed superiority over the PAI at identifying malingering of ADHD, demonstrating the value

of a scale that is tailored toward malingering of ADHD symptoms. The PAI had a significantly

higher specificity than the SAMS, but this is caused by the fact that the PAI classifies so few

respondents as malingerers (as seen in the high false negative rate), that most of the non-

malingerer population was correctly classified.

4.1 Limitations

While demonstrating the potential of the SAMS to address malingering of ADHD, this

study carries certain limitations. The data collection for this study was conducted along with the

development of the SAMS itself (Ramachandran et al., in preparation). Further research needs to

be conducted in diverse populations to test the applicability and generalizability of these results.

110

The comparability of the malingerer group used in this study to the real world malingerer

population is unknown. Psychometric indices such as positive predictive value could not be

calculated from this study because of the lack of an accurate estimate of the prevalence of

malingering.

4.2 Clinical Implications & Future Research

The SAMS presents immense potential for use in the clinical setting to help identify

malingering of ADHD. It is tailored toward malingering of ADHD symptoms, and has

demonstrated satisfactory sensitivity, specificity, and high resistance to malingering. Physicians

evaluating patients for ADHD are recommended to administer the SAMS, pending further

research, to help validate the veracity of the patient’s claims. Patients flagged as ‘malingerers’ by

the SAMS are recommended for additional testing or referral to a specialist to identify if they

truly have ADHD. There is a need for further research testing the SAMS directly in the clinical

setting and in diverse populations. There is also a need to develop interventions for patients

identified as ‘malingerers’ by the SAMS. These individuals may need help for drug dependence

and counseling to help them cope with the stressors that have encouraged drug-seeking behavior.

111

BIBLIOGRAPHY

112

Binder, L. M. (1992). Deception and malingering. In A. E. Puente & R. J. McCaffrey (Eds.),

Handbook of Neuropsychological Assessment: A Biopsychosocial Perspective (Critical

Issues in Neuropsychology) (pp. 353–374). New York: Plenum.

Breiman, L., Friedman, J. H., Olshen, R. A., Stone, C. J. (1984). Classification and Regression

Trees. Wadsworth, Pacific Grove, CA.

Burkhart, B. R., Gynther, M. D., & Christian, W. L. (1978). Psychological mindedness,

intelligence, and item subtlety endorsement patterns on the MMPI. Journal of Clinical

Psychology, 34(1), 76-79.

Cheetham, A. H., & Hazel, J. E. (1969). Binary (presence-absence) similarity coefficients.

Journal of Paleontology, 1130-1136.

Chen, L. Y., Crum, R. M., Strain, E. C., Alexander, G. C., Kaufmann, C., & Mojtabai, R. (2016).

Prescriptions, nonmedical use, and emergency department visits involving prescription

stimulants. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 77(3), 297-304.

Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educational and Psychological

Measurement, 20(1), 37-46.

Fisher, A. B., & Watkins, M. W. (2008). ADHD rating scales' susceptibility to faking in a

college student sample. Journal of Postsecondary Education and Disability, 20(2), 81-92.

Fletcher, R. H., Fletcher, S. W., & Fletcher, G. S. (2012). Clinical Epidemiology: The Essentials.

Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.

113

Franck, C. (2013). CART: Classification and Regression Trees. [PowerPoint slides] Retrieved

from: www.lisa.stat.vt.edu/sites/default/files/CART.pptx Accessed on: July 9, 2015.

Hanley, J. A., & McNeil, B. J. (1983). A method of comparing the areas under receiver operating

characteristic curves derived from the same cases. Radiology, 148(3), 839-843.

Harrison, A. G. (2006). Adults faking ADHD: You must be kidding! The ADHD report, 14(4),

1-7.

Harrison, A. G., Edwards, M. J., & Parker, K. C. (2007). Identifying students faking ADHD:

Preliminary findings and strategies for detection. Archives of Clinical

Neuropsychology, 22(5), 577-588.

Harp, J. P., Jasinski, L. J., Shandera-Ochsner, A. L., Mason, L. H., & Berry, D. T. (2011).

Detection of malingered ADHD using the MMPI-2-RF. Psychological Injury and

Law, 4(1), 32-43.

Jachimowicz, G., & Geiselman, R. E. (2004). Comparison of ease of falsification of attention

deficit hyperactivity disorder diagnosis using standard behavioral rating scales. Cognitive

Science Online, 2(1), 6-20.

Jasinski, L. J., Harp, J. P., Berry, D. T., Shandera-Ochsner, A. L., Mason, L. H., & Ranseen, J.

D. (2011). Using symptom validity tests to detect malingered ADHD in college

students. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 25(8), 1415-1428.

Kuhn, G. M. (1973). The phi coefficient as an index of ear differences in dichotic

listening. Cortex, 9(4), 450-457.

114

Lanyon, R. I. (1997). Detecting deception: Current models and directions. Clinical Psychology:

Science and Practice, 4(4), 377-387.

Marshall, P., Schroeder, R., O’Brien, J., et al. (2010). Effectiveness of symptom validity

measures in identifying cognitive and behavioral symptom exaggeration in adult attention

deficit hyperactivity disorder. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 24(7), 1204-1237.

Moisen, G. G. (2008). Classification and regression trees. Ecological Informatics, 1, 582-588.

Musso, M. W., & Gouvier, W. D. (2012). “Why Is This So Hard?”: A Review of Detection of

Malingered ADHD in College Students. Journal of Attention Disorders,

1087054712441970.

Myerholtz, L. E., & Rosenberg, H. (1997). Screening DUI offenders for alcohol problems:

Psychometric assessment of the Substance Abuse Subtle Screening Inventory.

Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 11(3), 155.

Quinn, C. A. (2003). Detection of malingering in assessment of adult ADHD. Archives of

Clinical Neuropsychology, 18(4), 379-395.

Rios, J., & Morey, L. C. (2013). Detecting feigned ADHD in later adolescence: An examination

of three PAI–A negative distortion indicators. Journal of Personality Assessment, 95(6),

594-599.

Slick, D. J., Sherman, E. M., & Iverson, G. L. (1999). Diagnostic criteria for malingered

neurocognitive dysfunction: Proposed standards for clinical practice and research. The

Clinical Neuropsychologist, 13(4), 545-561.

115

Sollman, M. J., Ranseen, J. D., & Berry, D. T. (2010). Detection of feigned ADHD in college

students. Psychological Assessment, 22(2), 325.

Suhr, J., Hammers, D., Dobbins-Buckland, K., Zimak, E., & Hughes, C. (2008). The relationship

of malingering test failure to self-reported symptoms and neuropsychological findings in

adults referred for ADHD evaluations. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 23, 521–

530.

Sullivan, B. K., May, K., & Galbally, L. (2007). Symptom exaggeration by college adults in

attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and learning disorder assessments. Applied

Neuropsychology, 14, 189–207.

Wiegel, M., Meston, C., & Rosen, R. (2005). The female sexual function index (FSFI): Cross-

validation and development of clinical cutoff scores. Journal of Sex & Marital

Therapy, 31(1), 1-20.

116

CHAPTER 5

SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, AND FUTURE RESEARCH

117

CONCLUSIONS

This dissertation explores a growing problem in society with regard to prescription drugs.

The study was unique in that it chose to focus on prescription stimulants, rather than the opioids

which has been the focus of most recent research in this field of study. This dissertation is the

first study of its kind to attempt to address the issue of prescription stimulant abuse by

developing techniques to identify individuals attempting to inappropriately access stimulants.

Two different techniques were explored in this study: the first used administrative claims data to

classify individual stimulant users using a latent class analysis; and the second attempted to

develop a short subtle scale called the Subtle ADHD Malingering Screener (SAMS) for

administration in the primary care provider’s office to identify individuals malingering

symptoms of ADHD with the intentions of obtaining prescriptions of stimulants for misuse or

diversion.

The specific goal of this dissertation was to help develop strategies that can be used in the

prevention of abuse of prescription psychostimulants. The combination of techniques

developed/tested in this dissertation provide a health policy analyst with tools, such as the

SAMS, and the prescription claims based latent classes, required to develop these strategies.

These tools still need several rounds of further research before they can be used in strategies

tailored for drug abuse prevention, but this dissertation will hopefully serve as the platform for

the development and refinement of these tools. The conclusions from each of the three papers are

118

discussed below, along with directions for future research and the clinical implications of this

dissertation.

Concluding Comments on Paper One

This paper developed a deeper understanding of the differences in stimulant use

behaviors between abusers and non-abusers, using a latent class analysis to first classify

individuals according to their behaviors. It is one of the first studies to attempt to exclusively

characterize stimulant use behaviors, and validate the developed latent classes using stimulant

abuse diagnoses identified from administrative claims data. The use of administrative claims data

to identify misusers of prescription stimulants revealed critical insights that can help future

research in this area. This study was also the first to use a combination of Medicaid

administrative claims data and PMP data to comprehensively capture all use of stimulants while

estimating the risk behaviors.

This study developed a 2-class model with 4.1% of the sample in the one class, and

95.9% of the sample in the other class. The 2-class model, however, was not predictive of a

diagnosis of dependent or nondependent stimulant abuse. It is possible that many individuals

classified in Class 1 were in fact abusing prescription stimulants, but had not been diagnosed;

Unfortunately, there was no way to know their true misuse status from the data. Hence the

findings of the prediction of stimulant misuse using latent classes need to be treated with caution.

It is possible that alternative validation mechanisms with more reliable identification of misusers

might have discovered a correlation with the latent classes obtained in this study. It is possible

that any combination of these reasons was responsible for the non-significant finding obtained in

this study. The latent classes obtained in this study might still be indicative of stimulant misuse,

119

but further research into the stimulant use behavior, perhaps using alternative validation

techniques, is needed. This study demonstrated the value of latent class techniques to help

identify variations in patterns of stimulant use, while also displaying the limitations of

administrative claims databases in capturing true misuse status.

Concluding comments on Paper Two

This study is the first of its kind to attempt to develop a scale to identify malingering in

the context of ADHD. A ten-item, two-factor solution was obtained for the SAMS in this study.

The sum scale score was used to calculate the total score for the SAMS in order to preserve ease

of scoring, and reduce time of administration in the physician’s office. Each individual SAMS

item was also found to significantly differentiate all three groups in the study, and not just the

ADHD group and the Malingerer group. The mean scores on both the psychological and

academic factor follow patterns predicted in the Accuracy of Knowledge framework.

The SAMS presents an innovative approach to identify malingering of ADHD symptoms,

reduce overdiagnosis of ADHD, and for early identification of prescription stimulant abuse

directly in the clinical setting. It is designed to be short, inexpensive, and does not need

additional training for administration, scoring, or evaluation, which makes it a valuable resource

for primary care providers. There is a need to focus on the needs of malingerers identified by this

scale, and to develop interventions that can help malingerers cope with their stressors and

address potential addiction problems. Finally, this study successfully demonstrates the

methodology of developing a subtle scale with the application of a theoretical framework for

item development. This technique can be applied in other areas in such as the early identification

120

of opioid abusers, injection drug users, to implement early targeted interventions. Further

research required on the SAMS was conducted as part of paper three, as presented below.

Concluding comments on Paper Three

This study provides critical research for the further development of the Subtle ADHD

Malingering Scale (SAMS) and builds value for its use in the clinical setting. The study used

data collected in paper two to develop a cut-off score for SAMS and estimate its classification

accuracy. Using CART analysis, a cut off score was developed for the SAMS. Respondents were

classified as malingerers if their psychological subscale score was greater than 15 and the

academic subscale score was greater than 7. This classification provided a sensitivity of 90.3%

and a specificity of 80.1%. The SAMS also showed significantly higher sensitivity than the PAI.

Being classified as a ‘malingerer’ by the SAMS at a physician’s office should signal the need for

additional testing, or a referral to a specialist, in the case of primary care providers.

The SAMS presents immense potential for use in the clinical setting to help identify

malingering of ADHD. It is tailored toward malingering of ADHD symptoms, and has

demonstrated satisfactory sensitivity, specificity, and high resistance to malingering. Physicians

evaluating patients for ADHD are recommended to administer the SAMS, pending further

research, to help validate the veracity of the patient’s claims. Patients flagged as ‘malingerers’ by

the SAMS are recommended for additional testing or referral to a specialist to identify if they

truly have ADHD. There is a need for further research testing the SAMS directly in the clinical

setting and in diverse populations. There is also a need to develop interventions for patients

identified as ‘malingerers’ by the SAMS. These individuals may need help for drug dependence

and counseling to help them cope with the stressors that have encouraged drug-seeking behavior.

121

IMPLICATIONS

Implications for Research & Practice

This dissertation was carried out with the goal of making a change in the healthcare

system that contributes to millions of cases of prescription stimulant abuse every year. The latent

classes developed in paper one, with further refinement, can provide a tool to payers such as

commercial insurance companies or Medicaid to help identify individuals who are in need of

intervention for drug abuse, and to help curtail others who may be diverting their prescriptions.

Once the identification of these individuals can be accomplished with reasonable accuracy,

tailored interventions need to be developed that involve a combination of lock-in programs to

prevent further drug diversion, and medical intervention to help treat possible drug abuse

problems. Further research is needed to develop validation tools that can estimate if latent classes

using behavioral risk factors can perform reliably at identifying individuals who are misusing or

diverting stimulant prescriptions.

Papers two and three serve a different purpose in the prevention of drug abuse. They used

a college student population, to help develop a tool for use in the physician’s office to help

identify malingerers of ADHD symptoms. This can help address overdiagnosis of ADHD, and

over-prescribing of stimulants. The SAMS displayed potential for clinical use with high levels of

sensitivity and specificity.. A true estimate of prevalence of malingering in various settings needs

to be estimated to calculate a positive and negative predictive value that can help direct

recommendations for the individuals identified by SAMS. Further testing in other populations

122

including children under the age of 18 years, and adults not enrolled in colleges is required

before the SAMS can be recommended in the clinical setting.

123

BIBLIOGRAPHY

124

Agrawal, A., Lynskey, M. T., Madden, P. A., Bucholz, K. K., & Heath, A. C. (2006). A latent

class analysis of illicit drug abuse/dependence: results from the National Epidemiological

Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Addiction, 102(1), 94-104.

Angold, A, Erkanli, A., Egger, H. L., Costello, J. (2000). Stimulant treatment for children: a

community perspective. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent

Psychiatry, 39, 975–984.

Arria, A. M., & DuPont, R. L. (2010). Nonmedical prescription stimulant use among college

students: Why we need to do something and what we need to do. Journal of Addictive

Diseases, 29, 417–426.

Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. (2014). Auxiliary variables in mixture modeling: Three-step

approaches using M plus. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary

Journal, 21(3), 329-341.

Babcock, Q., Byrne, T. (2000). Student perceptions of methylphenidate abuse at a public liberal

arts college. Journal of American College Health, 49, 143–148.

Barkley, R. A., Fischer, M., Smallish, L., & Fletcher, K. (2003). Does the treatment of attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder with stimulants contribute to drug use/abuse? A 13-year

prospective study. Pediatrics, 111(1), 97-109.

125

Bavarian, N., Flay, B. R., Ketcham, P. L., & Smith, E. (2013). Illicit use of prescription

stimulants in a college student sample: A theory-guided analysis. Drug and alcohol

dependence, 132(3), 665-673.

Biederman, J., Wilens, T., Mick, E., Spencer, T. J., Faraone, S. V. (1999). Pharmacotherapy of

attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder reduces risk for substance use disorder. Pediatrics,

104:e20.

Binder, L. M. (1992). Deception and malingering. In A. E. Puente & R. J. McCaffrey (Eds.),

Handbook of Neuropsychological Assessment: A Biopsychosocial Perspective (Critical

Issues in Neuropsychology) (pp. 353–374). New York: Plenum.

Birnbaum, H. G., White, A. G., Schiller, M., Waldman, T., Cleveland, J. M., & Roland, C. L.

(2011). Societal costs of prescription opioid abuse, dependence, and misuse in the United

States. Pain Medicine, 12(4), 657-667.

Bollinger, L. C., Bush, C., Califano, J. A., et al. (2005). Under the counter: The diversion and

abuse of controlled prescription drugs in the U.S. The National Center on Addiction and

Substance Abuse at Columbia University (CASA). Available at:

http://www.casacolumbia.org/addiction-research/reports/under-the-counter-diversion-

abuse-controlled-perscription-drugs. Accessed on: July 9th 2015.

Booksh, R. L., Pella, R. D., Singh, A. N., & Gouvier, W. D. (2010). Ability of college students to

simulate ADHD on objective measures of attention. Journal of Attention Disorders.

13(4), 325-338.

Boyd, C. J., & McCabe, S. E. (2008). Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and

Policy. Substance abuse treatment, prevention, and policy, 3, 22.

126

Breiman, L., Friedman, J. H., Olshen, R. A., Stone, C. J. (1984). Classification and Regression

Trees. Wadsworth, Pacific Grove, CA.

Burkhart, B. R., Gynther, M. D., & Christian, W. L. (1978). Psychological mindedness,

intelligence, and item subtlety endorsement patterns on the MMPI. Journal of Clinical

Psychology, 34(1), 76-79.

Buss, A. H. (1959). The effect of item style on social desirability and frequency of endorsement.

Journal of Consulting Psychology, 23(6), 510.

Caisley, H., & Müller, U. (2012). Adherence to medication in adults with attention deficit

hyperactivity disorder and pro re nata dosing of psychostimulants: a systematic review.

Eur. Psychiatry, 27(5), 343-349.

Carlson, R. G., Wang, J., Falck, R. S., & Siegal, H. A. (2005). Drug use practices among

MDMA/ecstasy users in Ohio: a latent class analysis. Drug Alcohol Depend., 79(2), 167-

179.

Cattell, R. B. (1966). The scree test for the number of factors. Multivariate Behavioral

Research, 1(2), 245-276.

Charach, A., Figueroa, M., Chen, S., Ickowicz, A., & Schachar, R. (2006). Stimulant treatment

over 5 years: effects on growth. Journal of the American Academy of Child &

Adolescent Psychiatry, 45(4), 415-421.

Cheetham, A. H., & Hazel, J. E. (1969). Binary (presence-absence) similarity coefficients.

Journal of Paleontology, 1130-1136.

127

Chen, L. Y., Crum, R. M., Strain, E. C., Alexander, G. C., Kaufmann, C., & Mojtabai, R. (2016).

Prescriptions, nonmedical use, and emergency department visits involving prescription

stimulants. J Clin Psychiatry., 77(3), 297-304.

Chiauzzi, E., DasMahapatra, P., & Black, R. A. (2013). Risk behaviors and drug use: A latent

class analysis of heavy episodic drinking in first-year college students. Psychol Addict

Behav., 27(4), 974.

Clark, S.L. & Muthen, B. (2009). Relating Latent Class Analysis Results to Variables not

Included in the Analysis. Mplus Web Notes. Available at:

http://www.statmodel.com/download/relatinglca.pdf Accessed on: December 20th, 2016.”

Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1995). Constructing validity: Basic issues in objective scale

development. Psychological Assessment, 7(3), 309.

Chen, L. Y., Crum, R. M., Strain, E. C., Alexander, G. C., Kaufmann, C., & Mojtabai, R. (2016).

Prescriptions, nonmedical use, and emergency department visits involving prescription

stimulants. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 77(3), 297-304.

Cofer, C. N., Chance, J., & Judson, A. J. (1949). A study of malingering on the Minnesota

Multiphasic Personality Inventory. The Journal of Psychology, 27(2), 491-499.

Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educational and Psychological

Measurement, 20(1), 37-46.

Cohen, M. J. (2013). Off-Label: Combating the Dangerous Overprescription of Amphetamines

to Children. The George Washington Law Review, 82, 174.

128

Compton, W. M., & Volkow, N. D. (2006). Abuse of prescription drugs and the risk of

addiction. Drug and alcohol dependence, 83, S4-S7.

Comrey, A. L. (1988). Factor-analytic methods of scale development in personality and clinical

psychology. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56(5), 754.

DeVellis, R. F. (2011). Scale Development: Theory and Applications (Vol. 26). Sage

Publications.

Dietz, P., Striegel, H., Franke, A.G., Lieb, K., Simon, P., & Ulrich, R. (2013). Randomized

response estimates for the 12‐month prevalence of cognitive‐enhancing drug use in

university students. Pharmacotherapy: The Journal of Human Pharmacology and Drug

Therapy, 33(1), 44-50.

Fisher, A. B., & Watkins, M. W. (2008). ADHD rating scales' susceptibility to faking in a

college student sample. Journal of Postsecondary Education and Disability, 20(2), 81-92.

Falck, R. S., Siegal, H. A., Wang, J., Carlson, R. G., & Draus, P. J. (2005). Nonmedical drug use

among stimulant-using adults in small towns in rural Ohio. Journal of substance abuse

treatment, 28(4), 341-349.

Fletcher, R. H., Fletcher, S. W., & Fletcher, G. S. (2012). Clinical Epidemiology: The Essentials.

Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.

Fornell, C., & Larcker, D. F. (1981). Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable

variables and measurement error. Journal of Marketing Research, 39-50.

Fortuna, R. J., Robbins, B. W., Caiola, E., Joynt, M., & Halterman, J. S. (2010). Prescribing of

controlled medications to adolescents and young adults in the United States. Pediatrics,

129

126(6), 1108–1116, Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2010-0791. Accessed on

July 9th 2015.

Frankenberger, W., Lozar, B., Dallas, P. (1990). The use of stimulant medication to treat

attention deficit hyperactive disorder (ADHD) in elementary school children. Develop

Disabilities Bulletin, 18, 1–13.

Franck, C. (2013). CART: Classification and Regression Trees. [PowerPoint slides] Retrieved

from: www.lisa.stat.vt.edu/sites/default/files/CART.pptx Accessed on: July 9, 2015.

Franklin, S. B., Gibson, D. J., Robertson, P. A., Pohlmann, J. T., & Fralish, J. S. (1995). Parallel

analysis: a method for determining significant principal components. Journal of

Vegetation Science, 6(1), 99-106.

Garnier-Dykstra, L. M., Caldeira, K. M., Vincent, K. B., O’Grady, K. E., & Arria, A. M. (2012).

Nonmedical use of prescription stimulants during college: Four-year trends in exposure

opportunity, use, motives, and sources. Journal of American College Health, 60(3), 226-

234.

Gerbing, D. W., & Anderson, J. C. (1988). An updated paradigm for scale development

incorporating unidimensionality and its assessment. Journal of Marketing Research, 186-

192.

Hair, J. F., Black, W.C., Babin, B. J., Anderson, R. E., & Tatham, R. L. (2006). Multivariate

Data Analysis. Pearson Education.

130

Hall, K. M., Irwin, M. M., Bowman, K. A., Frankenberger, W., & Jewett, D. C. (2005). Illicit use

of prescribed stimulant medication among college students. Journal of American College

Health, 53(4), 167-174.

Hamilton, G. J. (2009). Prescription drug abuse. Psychology in the Schools, 46(9), 892-898.

Hanley, J. A., & McNeil, B. J. (1983). A method of comparing the areas under receiver operating

characteristic curves derived from the same cases. Radiology, 148(3), 839-843.

Hansen, R. N., Oster, G., Edelsberg, J., Woody, G. E., & Sullivan, S. D. (2011). Economic costs

of nonmedical use of prescription opioids. The Clinical journal of pain, 27(3), 194-202.

Harrison, A. G. (2006). Adults faking ADHD: You must be kidding! The ADHD Report, 14(4),

1-7.

Harrison, A. G., & Edwards, M. J. (2010). Symptom exaggeration in post-secondary students:

Preliminary base rates in a Canadian sample. Applied Neuropsychology, 17(2), 135-143.

Harrison, A. G., Edwards, M. J., & Parker, K. C. H. (2007). Identifying students faking ADHD:

Preliminary strategies for detection. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22, 577-588.

Harp, J. P., Jasinski, L. J., Shandera-Ochsner, A. L., Mason, L. H., & Berry, D. T. (2011).

Detection of malingered ADHD using the MMPI-2-RF. Psychological Injury and

Law, 4(1), 32-43.

Hathaway, S. R., & McKinley, J. C. (1940). A multiphasic personality schedule (Minnesota): I.

Construction of the schedule. Journal of Psychology, 10, 249-254.

Hinkin, T. R. (1995). A review of scale development practices in the study of organizations.

Journal of Management, 21(5), 967-988.

131

Inciardi, J. A., Surratt, H. L., Cicero, T. J., & Beard, R. A. (2009). Prescription opioid abuse and

diversion in an urban community: the results of an ultrarapid assessment. Pain

Med., 10(3), 537-548.

Jachimowicz, G., & Geiselman, R. E. (2004). Comparison of ease of falsification of attention

deficit hyperactivity disorder diagnosis using standard behavioral rating scales. Cognitive

Science Online, 2(1), 6-20.

Jackson, D. N. (1966). A modern strategy for personality assessment: The Personality Research

Form. Research Bulletin, (30).

Jasinski, L. J., & Ranseen, J. D. (2011). Malingered ADHD evaluations: A further complication

for accommodations reviews. The Bar Examiner, 79, 6-16.

Jasinski, L. J., Harp, J. P., Berry, D. T., Shandera-Ochsner, A. L., Mason, L. H., & Ranseen, J.

D. (2011). Using symptom validity tests to detect malingered ADHD in college

students. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 25(8), 1415-1428.

Johnston, L. D., O’Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., Schulenberg, J. E. (2005). Monitoring the

Future National Survey Results on Drug Use, 1975–2004, vol. I: Secondary School

Students (NIH Publication No. 05-5727). National Institute on Drug Abuse, Bethesda,

MA, 680 pp.

Johnston, L. D., O’Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., & Schulenberg, J. E. (2011). Monitoring the

Future National Survey Results on Drug Use, 1975-2012: Volume 2, College Students

and Adults Ages 19-50. Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research, The University of

Michigan.

132

Johnston, L. D., O'Malley, P. M., Miech, R. A., Bachman, J. G., & Schulenberg, J. E. (2014).

Monitoring the future national results on adolescents drug use: 1975–2013 overview, key

findings on adolescent drug use. Ann Arbor: Institute for Social Research, The University

of Michigan.

Johnston, L. D., O’Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., Schulenberg, J. E. & Miech, R. A. (2016).

Monitoring the Future national survey results on drug use, 1975– 2015: Volume 2,

College students and adults ages 19–55. Ann Arbor: Institute for Social Research, The

University of Michigan.

Kaloyanides, K. B., McCabe, S. E., Cranford, J. A., & Teter, C. J. (2007). Prevalence of illicit

use and abuse of prescription stimulants, alcohol, and other drugs among college

students: relationship with age at initiation of prescription stimulants. Pharmacotherapy:

The Journal of Human Pharmacology and Drug Therapy, 27(5), 666-674.

Katz, N., Panas, L., Kim, M., et al. (2010) Usefulness of prescription monitoring programs for

surveillance—analysis of Schedule II opioid prescription data in Massachusetts, 1996–

2006. Pharmacoepidemiol and Drug Saf., 19(2), 115-123.

Kelly, B. C., Rendina, H. J., Vuolo, M., Wells, B. E., & Parsons, J. T. (2015) A typology of

prescription drug misuse: A latent class approach to differences and harms. Drug Alcohol

Rev., 34(2), 211-220.

Kube, D. A., Peterson, M. C., Palmer, F. B. (2002). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder:

comorbidity and medication use. Clinical Pediatrics, 41, 461–470.

Kuhn, G. M. (1973). The phi coefficient as an index of ear differences in dichotic

listening. Cortex, 9(4), 450-457.

133

Lanyon, R. I. (1970). Development and validation of a psychological screening inventory.

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 35(1p2), 1-24.

Lanyon, R. I. (1997). Detecting deception: Current models and directions. Clinical Psychology:

Science and Practice, 4(4), 377-387.

Lazarsfeld, P., Henry, N. (1968). Latent Structure Analysis. Houghton Mifflin, New York.

Lees-Haley, P. R., English, L. T., & Glenn, W. J. (1991). A fake bad scale on the MMPI-2 for

personal injury claimants. Psychological Reports, 68(1), 203-210.

LeFever, G., Dawson, K. (1999). The extent of drug therapy for attention deficit hyperactivity

disorder among children in public schools. American Journal of Public Health, 89, 1359–

1364.

Lessenger, J. E., & Feinberg, S. D. (2008). Abuse of prescription and over-the-counter

medications. Journal of the American Board of Family Medicine, 21(1), 45 – 54.

Levine, S. B., Coupey, S. M. (2009). Nonmedical use of prescription medications: An emerging

risk behavior among rural adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 44, 407–9.

Loevinger, J. (1957). Objective tests as instruments of psychological theory. Psychological

Reports, 3(3), 635-694.

Magidson, J., & Vermunt, J. (2002). Latent class models for clustering: A comparison with K-

means. Canadian Journal of Marketing Research, 20(1), 36-43.

Manchikanti, L. (2006). Prescription drug abuse: what is being done to address this new drug

epidemic? Testimony before the Subcommittee on Criminal Justice, Drug Policy and

Human Resources. Pain Physician, 9(4), 287.

134

Marsh, L. D., Key, J. D., Payne, T. P. (2000). Methylphenidate misuse in substance abusing

adolescents. Journal of Child & Adolescent Substance Abuse, 9, 1-14.

Marshall, P., Schroeder, R., O’Brien, J., et al. (2010) Effectiveness of symptom validity

measures in identifying cognitive and behavioral symptom exaggeration in adult attention

deficit hyperactivity disorder. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 24(7), 1204-1237.

Maxwell, J. C. (2011). The prescription drug epidemic in the United States: a perfect

storm. Drug and alcohol review, 30(3), 264-270.

McCabe, S. E., Teter, C. J., & Boyd, C. J. (2004). The use, misuse and diversion of prescription

stimulants among middle and high school students. Substance Use & Misuse, 39(7), 1095

– 1116.

McCabe, S. E., Teter, C. J., & Boyd, C. J. (2006). Medical use, illicit use and diversion of

prescription stimulant medication. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 38(1), 43 – 56.

McCabe, S. E., & West, B. T. (2013). Medical and nonmedical use of prescription stimulants:

Results from a national multicohort study. Journal of the American Academy of Child

and Adolescent Psychiatry, 52, 1272–1280, Available at:

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2013.09.005. Accessed on: July 9th 2015.

McCabe, S. E., West, B. T., Teter, C. J., & Boyd, C. J. (2014). Trends in medical use, diversion,

and nonmedical use of prescription medications among college students from 2003 to

2013: Connecting the dots. Addictive behaviors, 39(7), 1176-1182.

McHugh, R. K., Nielsen, S., & Weiss, R, D. (2015) Prescription drug abuse: from epidemiology

to public policy. Journal of substance abuse treatment, 48(1), 1-7.

135

Miller, G. A. (1985). The substance abuse subtle screening inventory (SASSI) manual.

Springville, IN: SASSI Institute.

Miller, A. R., Brehaut, J. C., Raina, P., McGrail, K. M., & Armstrong, R. W. (2004). Use of

medical services by methylphenidate-treated children in the general population.

Ambulatory Pediatrics, 4(2), 174–180, Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1367/A03-

031R1.1. Accessed on: July 9th 2015.

Miller, T., Novak, S. P., Galvin, D. M., Spicer, R. S., Cluff, L., & Kasat, S. (2015). School and

Work Status, Drug-Free Workplace Protections, and Prescription Drug Misuse Among

Americans Ages 15–25. Journal of studies on alcohol and drugs, 76(2), 195–203.

Moisen, G. G. (2008). Classification and regression trees. Ecological Informatics, 1, 582-588.

Molina, B. S., Hinshaw, S. P., Arnold, L. E., et al. (2013) Adolescent substance use in the

multimodal treatment study of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (MTA) as

a function of childhood ADHD, random assignment to childhood treatments, and

subsequent medication. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent

Psychiatry, 52(3), 250-263.

Moline, S., Frankenberger, W. (2001) Use of stimulant medication for treatment of attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A survey of middle and high school students’ attitudes.

Psychology in the School, 38, 1–16.

Moore, D. R., Burgard, D. A., Larson, R. G., & Ferm, M. (2014). Psychostimulant use among

college students during periods of high and low stress: an interdisciplinary approach

utilizing both self-report and unobtrusive chemical sample data. Addict Behav., 39(5),

987-993.

136

Morasco, B. J., & Dobscha, S. K. (2008). Prescription medication misuse and substance use

disorder in VA primary care patients with chronic pain. Gen Hosp Psychiatry., 30(2), 93-

99.

Musso, M. W., & Gouvier, W. D. (2012). Why is this so hard?: A review of detection of

malingered ADHD in college students. Journal of Attention Disorders,

1087054712441970.

Muthen, L.K., Muthen, B.O., 1998. Mplus User’s Guide, Seventh Edition. Muthen & Muthen,

Los Angeles, CA.

Myerholtz, L. E., & Rosenberg, H. (1997). Screening DUI offenders for alcohol problems:

Psychometric assessment of the substance abuse subtle screening inventory. Psychology

of Addictive Behaviors, 11(3), 155.

Myerholtz, L., & Rosenberg, H. (1998). Screening college students for alcohol problems:

Psychometric assessment of the SASSI-2. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs,

59(4), 439.

National Center for Health Statistics. (2014). Health, United States, 2014: With Special Feature

on Prescription Drugs. Hyattsville, MD. 2014. Available at:

http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/hus/hus14.pdf. Accessed on: July 9th 2015.

National Institute on Drug Abuse. (2013). Abuse of prescription (Rx) drugs affects young adults

most. Available at: http://www.drugabuse.gov/related-topics/trends-

statistics/infographics/abuse-prescription-rx-drugs-affects-young-adults-most. Accessed

on: July 9th 2015.

137

Novak, S. P., Kroutil, L. A., Williams, R. L., & Van Brunt, D. L. (2007). The nonmedical use of

prescription ADHD medications: results from a national Internet panel. Substance abuse

treatment, prevention, and policy, 2(1), 32.

Newsom, J. (2015). Practical Approaches to Dealing with Nonnormal and Categorical Variables.

[PDF document] Retrieved from:

http://web.pdx.edu/~newsomj/semclass/ho_estimate2.pdf

Nunnally, J. (1978). Psychometric Methods (2nd ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

O’Connor, B. P. (2000). Using parallel analysis and Velicer’s MAP Test. Behavior Research

Methods, Instruments & Computers, 32, 396-402.

Office of National Drug Control Policy. (2004). The Economic Costs of Drug Abuse in the

United States, 1992- 2002, Pub. No. 207303, at vi (Dec. 2004).

Office of National Drug Control Policy. (2011). Epidemic: Responding to America’s

prescription drug abuse crisis. Washington, DC: Author.

Patra, J., Fischer, B., Maksimowska, S., & Rehm, J. (2009). Profiling poly-substance use

typologies in a multi-site cohort of illicit opioid and other drug users in Canada–a latent

class analysis. Addiction Research & Theory, 17(2), 168-185.

Paulozzi, L. J., Budnitz, D. S., Yongli, X. (2006). Increasing deaths from opioid analgesics in the

United States. Pharmacoepidemiology and Drug Safety, 15, 618-627.

Paulozzi, L. J., Mack, K. A., & Hockenberry, J. M. (2014). Vital signs: Variation among states in

prescribing opioid pain relievers and benzodiazepines. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly

Report, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 63, 563–568.

138

Pella, R. D., Hill, B. D., Shelton, J. T., Elliott, E., & Gouvier, W. D. (2011). Evaluation of

embedded malingering indices in a non-litigating clinical sample using control, clinical,

and derived groups. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 090.

Poulin, C. (2001). Medical and nonmedical stimulant use among adolescents: from sanctioned to

unsanctioned use. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 165(8), 1039-1044.

Quinn, C. A. (2003). Detection of malingering in assessment of adult ADHD. Archives of

Clinical Neuropsychology, 18(4), 379-395.

Rhemtulla, M., Brosseau-Liard, P. E., & Savalei, V. (2012). When can categorical variables be

treated as continuous? A comparison of robust continuous and categorical SEM

estimation methods under suboptimal conditions. Psychological Methods, 17(3), 354.

Rios, J., & Morey, L. C. (2013). Detecting Feigned ADHD in Later Adolescence: An

Examination of Three PAI–A Negative Distortion Indicators. Journal of Personality

Assessment, 95(6), 594-599.

Rowland, A. S., Umbach, D. M., Stallone, L., Naftel, A. J., Bohlig, E. M., Sandler, D. P. (2002).

Prevalence of medication treatment for attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder among

elementary school children in Johnston County, North Carolina. American Journal of

Public Health, 92, 231–234.

Sansone, R. A., & Sansone, L. A. (2012). Doctor shopping: A phenomenon of many

themes. Innov Clin Neurosci., 9(11-12), 42.

139

Sherman, S. G., Sutcliffe, C. G., German, D., Sirirojn, B., Aramrattana, A., & Celentano, D. D.

(2009). Patterns of risky behaviors associated with methamphetamine use among young

Thai adults: a latent class analysis. J of Adolesc Health., 44(2), 169-175.

Slick, D. J., Sherman, E. M., & Iverson, G. L. (1999). Diagnostic criteria for malingered

neurocognitive dysfunction: Proposed standards for clinical practice and research. The

Clinical Neuropsychologist, 13(4), 545-561.

Smith, M. E., & Farah, M. J. (2011). Are prescription stimulants “smart pills”? The

epidemiology and cognitive neuroscience of prescription stimulant use by normal healthy

individuals. Psychological bulletin, 137(5), 717.

Smith, S. M., Dart, R. C., Katz, N. P., et al. (2013). Classification and definition of misuse,

abuse, and related events in clinical trials: ACTTION systematic review and

recommendations. PAIN, 154(11), 2287-2296.

Sollman, M. J., Ranseen, J. D., & Berry, D. T. (2010). Detection of feigned ADHD in college

students. Psychological Assessment, 22(2), 325.

Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. (2007). Results from the 2006

National Survey on Drug Use and Health: National Findings. Department of Health and

Human Services: Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. Available

at: https://www.asipp.org/documents/2006NSDUH.pdf. Accessed on: December 20th

2016.

Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. (2010). Results from the 2009

National Survey on Drug Use and Health: Volume I. Summary of National Findings

(Office of Applied Studies, NSDUH Series H-38A, HHS Publication No. SMA 10-

140

4586Findings). Rockville, MD. Retrieved from:

http://www.gmhc.org/files/editor/file/a_pa_nat_drug_use_survey.pdf

Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA). (2011). Drug abuse

warning network, 2008: National estimates of drug-related emergency department visits.

HHS publication no. SMA 11-4618. Rockville, MD.

Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. (2013a). Results from the 2012

National Survey on Drug Use and Health: Detailed tables. Available at:

http://www.samhsa.gov/data/NSDUH/2012SummNatFindDetTables/DetTabs/NSDUH-

DetTabsTOC2012.htm. Accessed on: July 9th 2015.

Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. (2013b). Emergency department

visits involving attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder stimulant mediations. Available

at: http://www.samhsa.gov/data/2k13/DAWN073/sr073-ADD-ADHD-medications.htm.

Accessed on: July 9th 2015.

Suhr, J., Hammers, D., Dobbins-Buckland, K., Zimak, E., & Hughes, C. (2008). The

Relationship of Malingering Test Failure to Self-Reported Symptoms and

Neuropsychological Findings in Adults Referred for ADHD Evaluations, Archives of

Clinical Neuropsychology, 23, 521–530.

Sullivan, B. K., May, K., & Galbally, L. (2007). Symptom Exaggeration by College Adults in

Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and Learning Disorder Assessments. Applied

Neuropsychology, 14, 189–207.

141

Swanson, J., Greenhill, L., Wigal, T. I. M., et al. (2006). Stimulant-related reductions of growth

rates in the PATS. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent

Psychiatry, 45(11), 1304-1313.

Swanson, J. M., Elliott, G. R., Greenhill, L. L., et al. (2007). Effects of stimulant medication on

growth rates across 3 years in the MTA follow-up. Journal of the American Academy of

Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 46(8), 1015-1027.

Teter, C. J., McCabe, S. E., Cranford, J. A., Boyd, C. J., & Guthrie, S. K. (2005). Prevalence and

motives for illicit use of prescription stimulants in an undergraduate student

sample. Journal of American College Health, 53(6), 253-262.

Teter, C. J., Falone, A. E., Cranford, J. A., Boyd, C. J., & McCabe, S. E. (2010). Nonmedical use

of prescription stimulants and depressed mood among college students: frequency and

route of administration. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 38, 292-298.

Toomey, R., Lyons, M. J., Eisen, S. A., et al. (2003). A twin study of the neuropsychological

consequences of stimulant abuse. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(3), 303-310.

Tucha, L., Sontag, T. A., Walitza, S., & Lange, K. W. (2009). Detection of malingered attention

deficit hyperactivity disorder. ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 1(1),

47-53.

University of Mississippi Office of Institutional Research, Effectiveness, and Planning. (n.d.)

Fall 2016-2017 Enrollment. Retrieved from: http://irep.olemiss.edu/fall-2016-2017-

enrollment/

142

Van den Bree, M. B., Johnson, E. O., Neale, M. C., & Pickens, R. W. (1998). Genetic and

environmental influences on drug use and abuse/dependence in male and female

twins. Drug Alcohol Depend., 52(3), 231-241.

Visser, S. N., Bitsko, R. H., Danielson, M. L., Perou, R., & Blumberg, S. J. (2010). Increasing

prevalence of parent-reported attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder among children—

United States, 2003 and 2007. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 59(44), 1439-

1443.

Volkow, N. D., Swanson, J. M. (2003). Variables that affect the clinical use and abuse of

methylphenidate in the treatment of ADHD. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 1909–

1918.

Weiner, S. G., Griggs, C. A., Mitchell, P. M., et al. (2013). Clinician impression versus

prescription drug monitoring program criteria in the assessment of drug-seeking behavior

in the emergency department. Annals of emergency medicine, 62(4), 281-289.

White, A. G., Birnbaum, H. G., Mareva, M. N., et al. (2005). Direct costs of opioid abuse in an

insured population in the United States. Journal of Managed Care Pharmacy, 11(6), 469.

White, B. P., Becker-Blease, K. A., & Grace-Bishop, K. (2006). Stimulant medication use,

misuse, and abuse in an undergraduate and graduate student sample. Journal of American

College Health, 54(5), 261-268.

Whiteside, L. K., Cunningham, R. M., Bonar, E. E., Blow, F., Ehrlich, P., & Walton, M. A.

(2015). Nonmedical prescription stimulant use among youth in the emergency

department: Prevalence, severity and correlates. Journal of substance abuse

treatment, 48(1), 21-27.

143

Wiegel, M., Meston, C., & Rosen, R. (2005). The female sexual function index (FSFI): Cross-

validation and development of clinical cutoff scores. Journal of Sex & Marital

Therapy, 31(1), 1-20.

Wilens, T. E., Gignac, M., Swezey, A., Monuteaux, M. C., & Biederman, J. (2006).

Characteristics of adolescents and young adults with ADHD who divert or misuse their

prescribed medications. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent

Psychiatry, 45(4), 408-414.

Wilford, B. B., Finch, J., Czechowicz, D. J., & Warren, D. (1994). An overview of prescription

drug misuse and abuse: Defining the problem and seeking solutions. The Journal of Law,

Medicine & Ethics, 22(3), 197-203.

Wooley, C. N., Rogers, R., Fiduccia, C. E., & Kelsey, K. (2012). The effectiveness of substance

use measures in the detection of full and partial denial of drug use. Assessment,

1073191112447098.

Worley, J. (2012). Prescription drug monitoring programs, a response to doctor shopping:

purpose, effectiveness, and directions for future research. Issues Ment Health

Nurs., 33(5), 319-328.

Young, J. C., & Gross, A. M. (2011). Detection of response bias and noncredible performance in

adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of Clinical

Neuropsychology, 26(3), 165-175.

Young, A. M., Glover, N., & Havens, J. R. (2012). Nonmedical use of prescription medications

among adolescents in the United States: a systematic review. Journal of Adolescent

Health, 51(1), 6-17.

144

Zachor, D. A., Roberts, A. W., Hodgens, J. B., Isaacs, J. S., & Merrick, J. (2006). Effects of

long-term psychostimulant medication on growth of children with ADHD. Research in

developmental disabilities, 27(2), 162-174.

Zedillo, E., & Wheeler, H. (2012). Rethinking the “War on Drugs” Through the US-Mexico

Prism. Yale Center for the Study of Globalization, 33.

Zgierska, A., Miller, M., & Rabago, D. (2012). Patient satisfaction, prescription drug abuse, and

potential unintended consequences. Journal of the American Medical

Association, 307(13), 1377-1378.

Zullig, K. J., & Divin, A. L. (2012). The association between non-medical prescription drug use,

depressive symptoms, and suicidality among college students. Addictive

behaviors, 37(8), 890-899.

145

CURRICULUM VITAE

146

Sujith Ramachandran

Master of Science, Pharmacy Administration

128 Faser Hall Office: (662) 915-7262

Department of Pharmacy Administration Fax: (662) 915-5102

University of Mississippi School of Pharmacy Mobile: 662-380-3006

University, MS 38677-1848 USA Email: [email protected]

EDUCATIONAL QUALIFICATIONS:

▪ Currently pursuing a PhD in the Department of Pharmacy Administration at the University of

Mississippi, University, MS. (Expected completion: December, 2016)

o Dissertation:

▪ Identification Of Stimulant Misuse & Malingering Of ADHD

o Advisor: Dr. John P. Bentley

▪ Master of Science in Pharmacy Administration (2011 – 2014) at the University of Mississippi,

University, MS.

o Thesis Project: Determining The Physician And Patient Characteristics Influencing The

Use Of Atypical Antipsychotics In Children

o Advisor: Dr. Benjamin F. Banahan

▪ Bachelor of Pharmacy (2007 – 2011) at Osmania University, Hyderabad, India.

RESEARCH SKILLS:

▪ Survey design, data collection & management, analysis

▪ Survey development using Qualtrics™

▪ Qualitative research design and analysis

▪ Big data analysis

▪ Multivariate Statistical analysis

▪ Programming skills:

o SPSS®

o SAS®

o Visual Basic for Applications (VBA), for Excel

▪ Model development and analysis using TreeAge® Pro suite

▪ Microsoft® Office suite

147

RESEARCH EXPERIENCE:

• Conjoint study design, and patient simulation techniques

• Designing & conducting physician & patient surveys

• In-Depth Interviews with family practitioners, pharmacists, specialists, and patients

• Moderating focus groups of practitioners, patients, and pharmacists

• Model building such as: budget impact models, cost effectiveness models

• Statistical techniques such as: Multivariate data analysis, and longitudinal data modelling

• Design and analysis of transaction claims data

WORK EXPERIENCE:

▪ Research Analyst, Center for Pharmaceutical Marketing and Management, University of

Mississippi (January – May, 2013 and January, 2014 – current):

o Mississippi Medicaid, Drug Utilization Review team:

▪ Analysis of Mississippi Medicaid claims data

▪ Research design and analysis

▪ SAS® programming to answer questions relevant to policy decisions

▪ Marketing Intern, Bavarian Nordic Immunotherapeutics, Mountain View, CA (June – August,

2013)

o Physician participation in prostate cancer clinical trials: A primary research study

assessing motivators, barriers, contact strategies, product ratings and perceptions of

clinical trials in advanced prostate cancer

o Competitive landscape analysis and market overview

▪ Health economic and analogue analysis

ACADEMIC EXPERIENCE:

Publications:

▪ Ramachandran S, Banahan BF, Bentley JP, West DS, Patel A. Factors Influencing the Use of

Atypical Antipsychotics in Children with Psychosis. Journal of Managed Care Pharmacy. Accepted

for publication.

▪ Khanna R, Jariwala K, Holmes ER, & Ramachandran S. (2014). Autism Familiarity And Knowledge

Among Pharmacy Students. Currents in Pharmacy Teaching and Learning, 6(1), 150-157.

▪ Ramachandran S, Jackson TH, Holmes ER, McCaffrey DJ, Pace PF, Bentley JP, Joshi N, West-

Strum DS. Primary Medication Non-adherence: The effect of a Pharmacist Administered

Intervention in a Pharmacy Grocery Chain. Journal of American Pharmacists Association. Drafted

for submission.

▪ Ramachandran S, Shahpurwala Z, Banahan BF, Hardwick SP. Control Of Potential Opioid Abuse:

Prescription Drug Monitoring Programs. Mississippi Pharmacist. Drafted for submission.

148

▪ Ramachandran S, Shahpurwala Z, Banahan BF, Hardwick SP. Impact of Medicaid Managed Care

Expansion on Access to Providers in Mississippi. Journal of Managed Care Pharmacy. Drafted for

submission.

Posters:

▪ Ramachandran S, Bentley JP, Holmes ER, Rosenthal M, Banahan BF, Young J. Development of

the Subtle ADHD Malingering Screener (SAMS) to identify malingering of ADHD Symptoms. Best

Podium Award. Southern Pharmacy Administration Conference, Oxford, MS, June 2016.

▪ Ramachandran S, Banahan BF, Hardwick SP, Noble S. Quality Of Care And Health Care Utilization

Among Children And Young Adults Using Antipsychotics In Mississippi Medicaid. Southern

Pharmacy Administration Conference, Oxford, MS, June 2016.

▪ Jackson TH, Ramachandran S, McCaffrey DJ, Bentley JP, Pace PF, Holmes ER, Joshi N, Porter J,

West-Strum DS. Primary Medication Non-adherence: The Effect of a Pharmacist Administered

Intervention in a Pharmacy Grocery Chain. Best ESAS poster award. American Pharmacists

Association Annual Meeting & Exposition, Baltimore, MD, March 2016.

▪ Ramachandran S, Gangan N, Bhattacharya K, Shahpurwala Z, Banahan BF. GLP-1 Agonist Case

Study. Case Study first-place Winner. Pharmaceutical Marketing Research Group Annual

meeting, Philadelphia, PA, October 2015.

▪ Ramachandran S, Banahan BF, Hardwick SP, Noble S. Quality Of Care And Health Care Utilization

Among Children And Young Adults Using Antipsychotics In Mississippi Medicaid. International

Society for Pharmacoeconomic and Outcomes Research, Washington, DC, May 2016.

▪ Ramachandran S, Banahan BF, Hardwick SP, Noble S. Quality of Care and Health Care Utilization

Among Foster Children In Mississippi Medicaid. International Society for Pharmacoeconomic

and Outcomes Research, Washington, DC, May 2016.

▪ Shah R, Ramachandran S, Nunna S, Banahan BF, Hardwick SP, Noble S. An Assessment Of

Follow-Up Visit And Use Of Psychosocial Services Among Foster And Non-Foster Children

Starting Antipsychotic Therapy. International Society for Pharmacoeconomic and Outcomes

Research, Washington, DC, May 2016.

▪ Ramachandran S, Banahan BF, Bentley JP, West DS, Patel A. Factors Influencing the Use of

Atypical Antipsychotics in Children with Psychosis. Academy of Managed Care Pharmacy Nexus,

Orlando, FL, October 2015.

▪ Ramachandran S, Banahan BF, Hardwick SP, Clark JP. Impact of the Shift to Medicaid Managed

Care on Resource Utilization and Costs for Beneficiaries in Mississippi Medicaid. International

Society for Pharmacoeconomics and Outcomes Research, Philadelphia, PA, May 2015.

▪ Shahpurwala Z, Ramachandran S, Banahan BF, Hardwick SP, Clark JP. Potential Savings from a

15 day Initial Fill Policy for Selected Outpatient Medications in a State Medicaid Program.

International Society for Pharmacoeconomics and Outcomes Research, Philadelphia, PA, May

2015.

▪ Shahpurwala Z, Ramachandran S, Banahan BF, Hardwick SP, Clark JP. Impact of Medicaid

Managed Care Expansion on Access to Providers in Mississippi. International Society for

Pharmacoeconomics and Outcomes Research, Philadelphia, PA, May 2015.

149

▪ Ramachandran S, Banahan BF, Null K, Clark JP, Minor DS. Estimating The Risk Of Angioedema

Associated With Use Of Dipeptidyl-Peptidase Inhibitors. International Society of

Pharmacoeconomics and Outcomes Research, Montreal, Canada, May 2014.

▪ Ramachandran S, Mahabaleshwakar R, Shah RM, Null KD, Banahan BF, Hardwick SP, Clark JP.

Performance of the National Quality Forum Diabetes Adherence Measure in the Mississippi

Medicaid Population. International Society of Pharmacoeconomics and Outcomes Research,

Montreal, Canada, May 2014.

▪ Ramachandran S, Banahan BF, Bentley, JB, West DW, Patel A. Determining The Physician And

Product Factors Influencing The Use Of Atypical Antipsychotics In Children; A working paper.

Presented at Pharmaceutical Marketing Research Group, Jersey City, NJ, October 2013.

▪ Ramachandran S, Null KD. Identifying High Cost Patients In Managed Care: An Application Of

Fractal Mathematics. Academy of Managed Care Pharmacy, San Diego, CA, October 2013.

▪ Ramachandran S, Banahan BF, Clark J, Hardwick S, Null KD. Health Care Expenditure Associated

With Off-label Use Of Atypical Antipsychotics In Medicaid Children: A Retrospective Analysis Of

Mississippi Medicaid Data. International Society of Pharmacoeconomics and Outcomes

Research, New Orleans, LA, May 2013.

▪ Ramachandran S, Jackson T, Dikun JA, Shah RM, Wamble DE. An Analysis Of Stress In Pharmacy

School Faculty. The American Association of Colleges of Pharmacy, March 2013.

▪ Ramachandran S, Mahabaleshwarkar R, Yang Y. Cost Effectiveness Analysis Of Addition Of

Telaprevir Or Boceprevir To Standard Therapy Versus Standard Therapy Alone For The

Treatment Of Previously Untreated Chronic Hepatitis-C Virus Genotype 1 Infection. International

Society of Pharmacoeconomics and Outcomes Research, Washington DC, June 2012.

▪ Ramachandran S, Mahabaleshwarkar R, Yang Y. Cost Consequence Analysis Of Telaprevir Versus

Boceprevir For The Treatment Of Previously Untreated Chronic Hepatitis-C Virus Infection.

Meeting in the Middle, Austin, TX, June 2012.

COURSEWORK:

▪ Research techniques: Research Methods, Primary Data Techniques, Secondary Data Techniques,

Data Management and Statistical Software (SAS®)

▪ Health care landscape: Pharmacoeconomics, Pharmaceutical and Health Care Policy, Health

Economics, Pharmacoepidemiology, Advanced Pharmaceutical Marketing & Patient Behavior

▪ Marketing: Drug Development and Marketing, Marketing theory, Consumer behavior,

Quantitative Methods in Psychology, Marketing strategy, Customer Relationship Management

▪ Statistics: Quantitative Methods in Psychology, General Linear Models, Applied Multivariate

analysis, Mediation and Moderation

150

MANUSCRIPT REVIEW:

• Occasional Reviewer for Currents in Pharmacy Teaching and Learning

• Occasional Reviewer for Aging and Mental Health

AWARDS:

▪ Best podium presentation award at Southern Pharmacy Administration Conference, Oxford, MS,

June 2016.

▪ Best poster award at American Pharmacists Association (APhA) Annual Meeting & Exposition,

Baltimore, MD, March 2016.

▪ Graduate Achievement Award, 2015 – 16 and 2015 Honors day program nominee.

▪ UM Phi Kappa Phi Academic Honors society scholarship, 2015 – 16.

▪ Terrence E. Downer Marketing Scholarship, 2015 – 16.

▪ Winner of the inaugural National Case Study Contest and invitee to the Pharmaceutical

Marketing Research Group (PMRG) Annual meeting in Philadelphia, PA, October 2015.

▪ 3 Minute Thesis competition:

o Grand Prize Winner, and University of Mississippi represent at the Conference of

Southern Graduate Schools (CSGS) 2015.

o People’s Choice Award at CSGS, March 2015, New Orleans, LA.

▪ School of Pharmacy Teaching Assistant of the years 2012-2013, and 2013-2014.

▪ “Who’s Who Among Students in American Universities and Colleges” at the University of

Mississippi, 2013-14.

▪ Rho Chi Academic honors society, 2013 initiate.

▪ Phi Kappa Phi academic honors society, 2013 initiate.

▪ Best presentation for a poster titled ‘Oncolytic Viruses’ presented at a national conference,

Hyderabad, India.

▪ Academic scholarship:

o Osmania University College of Pharmacy (2007 – 2011).

o Narayana Junior College (2005 – 2007).

PROFESSIONAL CITIZENSHIP:

▪ International Society of Pharmacoeconomics and Outcomes Research (ISPOR; 2011 – Present)

o President of UM ISPOR Student Chapter (2015-16)

o Member of the ISPOR National Education subcommittee (2014-15)

o Vice-President UM ISPOR Student Chapter (2014-15)

o Secretary UM ISPOR Student Chapter (2012-13)

▪ Pharmaceutical Marketing Research Group (PMRG; 2011 – Present)

o Member of the National PMRG Institute Academia subcommittee (2014 – Present)

o Member of the National PMRG Institute Membership subcommittee (2015 – Present)

o President of UM PMRG Student Chapter (2014-15)

o Vice President of UM PMRG Student Chapter (2013-14)

151

▪ University of Mississippi Graduate Student Council (2011 – Present)

o Vice President (2013-14)

o Member of the Chancellor’s committee on Graduate Council (2013-14)

o Member of the Vice Chancellor’s committee on Student Affairs (2013-14)

o Senator for the department of Pharmacy Administration in the University of Mississippi

Graduate Student Council (2012-13).

o Chair of the Graduate Student Council Rules sub-committee (2012-13).

▪ Phi Kappa Phi Academic Honor society (2013-16).

▪ Who’s Who society at the University of Mississippi (2013-14).

▪ Rho Chi Academic Honor society (2013-16).

▪ University of Mississippi Indian Student Association (2011 – Present)

o Vice President (2011-12)