grin2b mediates susceptibility to iq and cognitive impairments in developmental dyslexia

Upload: state-of-mind

Post on 02-Jun-2018

217 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/9/2019 GRIN2B mediates susceptibility to IQ and cognitive impairments in Developmental Dyslexia

    1/24

    !

    GRIN2Bmediates susceptibility to IQ and cognitive impairments in Developmental Dyslexia

    Sara Mascheretti1, Andrea Facoetti

    1,2, Roberto Giorda

    3, Silvana Beri

    3, Valentina Riva

    1, Vittoria Trezzi

    1,

    Maria R. Cellino4, Cecilia Marino

    1,5,6

    1Department of Child Psychiatry, Scientific Institute Eugenio Medea, Bosisio Parini (Lecco), Italy

    2 Developmental and Cognitive Neuroscience Lab, Department of General Psychology, University of

    Padua, Padua, Italy

    3Molecular Biology Laboratory, Scientific Institute Eugenio Medea, Bosisio Parini (Lecco), Italy

    4Centro Regionale di Riferimento per i Disturbi dellApprendimento CRRDA, ULSS 20, Verona, Italy

    5Centre de recherche de lInstitut universitaire en sant mentale de Qubec, Qubec (Qubec), Canada

    6 Dpartement de Psychiatrie et Neurosciences, Facult de Mdecine, Universit Laval, Qubec (Qubec),

    Canada

    Running Title:GRIN2Band susceptibility to Dyslexia

    !Sara Mascheretti, Ph.D.

    Child Psychopathology Unit, Scientific Institute Eugenio Medea

    via don Luigi Monza, 20

    23842 Bosisio Parini (Lecco), Italy

    Phone: +39.031.877.813

    Fax: +39.031.877.499

    e-mail address: [email protected]

    Conflicts of Interest and Source of Funding:Nothing to declare

  • 8/9/2019 GRIN2B mediates susceptibility to IQ and cognitive impairments in Developmental Dyslexia

    2/24

    #

    Abstract

    Objective(s) Developmental dyslexia (DD) is a complex heritable condition associated with impairments

    in multiple neurocognitive domains. Substantial heritability has been reported for DD and related

    phenotypes, and candidate genes have been identified. Recently, a candidate gene for human cognitive

    processes, i.e., GRIN2B, has been found significantly associated with working memory in a German DD

    sample. In this study, we explored the contribution of six GRIN2Bmarkers to DD and key DD-related

    phenotypes by association analyses in a sample of Italian nuclear families. Moreover, we assessed

    potential gene-by-environment interactions on DD-related phenotypes.

    Methods We performed a family-based association study to determine whether the GRIN2B gene

    influence both DD as a categorical trait and its related cognitive traits, in a large cohort of 466 Italian

    nuclear families ascertained through a proband affected by DD. Moreover, we tested the role of the

    selected GRIN2Bmarkers and a set of commonly-described environmental moderators, by applying a test

    for GxE interaction in sib pair-based association analysis of quantitative traits in 178 Italian nuclear

    families.

    Results Evidence for significant association were found with the categorical diagnosis of DD,

    performance IQ, phonemic elision and auditory short-term memory. No significant gene-by-environment

    effects were found.

    ConclusionsOur results add further evidence in support of GRIN2Bcontributing to DD and deficits in

    DD. More specifically, our data support the view that GRIN2Binfluences DD as a categorical trait and its

    related quantitative phenotypes, thus shedding further light into the etiologic basis and the phenotypic

    complexity of this disorder.

    Key Words: Developmental Dyslexia; Developmental Dyslexia-related neuropsychological traits;

    GRIN2B; N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors; association study; gene-by-environment interaction

  • 8/9/2019 GRIN2B mediates susceptibility to IQ and cognitive impairments in Developmental Dyslexia

    3/24

    $

    Riassunto

    Obiettivo: La Dislessia Evolutiva (DD) una condizione complessa ed ereditabile associata a deficit in

    differenti domini neurocognitivi. E stata riscontrata una significativa ereditabilit sia nella Dislessia

    Evolutiva che nei fenotipi ad essa associati. Recentemente, un gene implicato nelle fasi del

    neurosviluppo, e.i., GRIN2B, stato osservato essere significativamente associato alla memoria di lavoro

    in un campione di dislessici tedeschi. In questo studio abbiamo esplorato il contributo di sei marcatori del

    gene GRIN2Bsulla DD e dei principali fenotipi associati alla Dislessia Evolutiva per mezzo di analisi di

    associazione genetica in un campione di famiglie nucleari italiane.

    Metodi: Abbiamo messo a punto uno studio di associazione family-based per determinare se il gene

    GRIN2B influenza sia la DD come tratto categoriale sia i tratti cognitivi ad essa correlati, in un ampio

    campione di 466 famiglie nucleari italiane accertate per mezzo di un probando affetto da DD. Inoltre

    abbiamo testato il ruolo di specifici marcatori del gene GRIN2B e una serie di moderatori ambientali

    comunemente descritti, applicando test per linterazione GxE utilizzando analisi di associazione con

    coppie di fratelli in 178 famiglie nucleari italiane.

    RisultatiSono state trovate evidenze di associazioni significative tra la diagnosi categoria di DD, QI di

    Performance, elisione fonemica e memoria a breve termine uditiva. Non son stati riscontrati effetti di

    interazione gene-ambiente.

    ConclusioniI nostri risultati aggiungono una forte evidenza in supporto al contributo del gene GRIN2B

    sulla DD e sui deficit associate alla DD. Pi specificatamente, i nostri dati supportano la teoria per cui il

    gene GRIN2B influenza la DD come tratto categoriale e i fenotipi ad essa associati, tali risultati ci

    permettono di gettar luce sulle basi eziologiche e sulla complessit fenotipica di questo disturbo.

    Parole chiave: Dislessia Evolutiva; Dislessia Evolutiva tratti neuropsicologici correlati; GRIN2B;

    recettori N-methyl-D-aspartate; studio di associazione; interazione gene-ambiente.

  • 8/9/2019 GRIN2B mediates susceptibility to IQ and cognitive impairments in Developmental Dyslexia

    4/24

    %

    Introduction

    Developmental dyslexia (DD) is a complex heritable condition typically diagnosed in the first school

    years characterized by an impaired reading acquisition in spite of normal intelligence and adequate

    educational opportunities (American Psychiatric Association, 1994), and associated with impairments in

    multiple neurocognitive domains (Gabrieli, 2009). Several data show that intelligence quotient (IQ) is

    strongly associated with reading development and impairment (Stanovich, 1986; Shaywitz et al., 1995;

    Berninger et al., 2001; Ferrer and McArdle, 2004; Ferrer et al., 2007, 2010), although the exact nature of

    this relationship still remains unclear (Newman et al., 1991; Rispens, 1991; Jimnez Glez and Rodrigo

    Lpez, 1994; Stuebing et al., 2002).

    Following earlier descriptions (Hallgren, 1950) of high familial aggregation of the disorder, substantial

    heritability has been reported for DD (Fisher and De Fries, 2002). Twin studies show wide variation of

    heritability estimates (range: 0.18 - 0.72; Plomin and Kovas, 2005) across DD and DD-related

    phenotypes, while shared environmental influences appear to play a less important role in explaining the

    familial aggregation of the disorder (Pennington, 1995). The aetiology of DD involves multiple

    interacting risk factors, which can be either genetic or environmental, and underlies a continuously

    distributed liability. Since the early 1980s at least nine DD risk loci have been mapped to chromosomes 1,

    2, 3, 6, 15, 18 and X (Scerri and Schulte-Krne, 2010) and DYX1C1, KIAA0319, DCDC2and ROBO1

    have been suggested as DD susceptibility genes (Taipale et al., 2003; Francks et al., 2004; Meng et al.,

    2005; Hannula-Jouppi et al., 2005), although negative findings have also been reported.

    Recently, a candidate gene for human cognitive processes, i.e., GRIN2B, located on chromosome

    12p13.1, has been found significantly associated with working memory in a German DD sample (Ludwig

    et al., 2009). In this study, four markers within intron 3 were associated with short-term memory in DD,

    by suggesting that variation within this gene may contribute to the genetic background of specific DD-

    related neuropsychological phenotypes (Ludwig et al., 2009). More specifically, GRIN2Bgene codes for

    a specific subunit composition of N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors - a class of ionotropic

    glutamate receptors (i.e., GluN2B subunit). Over the past years, the involvement of N-methyl-D-aspartate

    receptors (NMDARs) in learning and memory formation has been emphasized (Cull-Candy et al., 2001;

    Lau and Zukin, 2007; Kalia et al., 2008). NMDAR are ionotropic, glutamatergic receptors involved in

    excitatory synaptic transmission in the central nervous system (Cull-Candy et al., 2001). In particular, the

  • 8/9/2019 GRIN2B mediates susceptibility to IQ and cognitive impairments in Developmental Dyslexia

    5/24

    &

    NMDAR-GluN2B subunit plays a critical role in experience-dependent, synaptic plasticity associated

    with learning and memory (Kutsuwada et al., 1996; Ito et al., 1997; Tang et al., 1999; Kim et al., 2005;

    Akashi et al., 2009; Fetterolf and Foster, 2011). Animal studies show that the Glun2b subunit is required

    for neuronal pattern formation in general, and for channel function and formation of dendritic spines in

    hippocampal pyramidal cells in particular (Ito et al., 1997; Cull-Candy et al., 2001; Kim et al., 2005;

    Akashi et al., 2009). Transgenic over-expression of Grin2bin the forebrain of mice, and in the cortex and

    hippocampus of rats results in an increased activation of the NMDARs, with mice and rats displaying a

    superior performance in various tests of learning and memory (Tang et al., 1999; Wang et al., 2009).

    These findings point towards GRIN2Bas a susceptibility gene for complex traits/disorders in humans.

    Nevertheless, while genetic contributions are clearly relevant to the aetiology of these phenotypic

    expressions, it is widely recognized that additional factors whose nature cannot be immediately

    identified as genetic also act as powerful predictors for human cognition and neurodevelopmental

    disorders, such as DD. A large range of factors, i.e. pre/perinatal (Bowen et al., 2002; Samuelsson et al.,

    2006; van Baar et al., 2009; Fried et al., 1997; Batstra et al., 2003; Gilger et al., 1992; Michaelsen et al.,

    2009; Hoque et al., 2012), socio-demographic and familial (Fergusson and Lynskey, 1993; Fergusson and

    Woodward, 1999; Melekian, 2001; Hoff and Tian, 2005), seems to impinge on human cognition and DD

    (Davis et al., 2001; Gayn and Olson, 2001, 2003; Byrne et al., 2002; Petrill et al., 2006; Harlaar et al.,

    2007), and may contribute to heighten the likelihood of developing this disorder. Given the above

    evidence, it seems then appropriate to complement genomic investigation of such complex traits by taking

    into account gene-by-environment interaction (GxE), which is a specific form of interplay, whereby

    genetic susceptibility conferred by a specified allele is modulated by a measured environmental factor

    (Rutter et al., 2006).

    Here, we sought to resolve which aspects of the phenotypic profile of DD were unambiguously

    attributable to GRIN2Beffects by exploring the contribution of four SNPs which have been previously

    shown to contribute to the genetic background of DD (i.e., rs1012586G/C; rs2268119A/T; rs2216128T/C;

    rs2192973C/T; Ludwig et al., 2009) and two markers (i.e., rs5796555-/A and rs11609779C/T) close

    enough to markers rs1012586G/C and rs2216128T/C, respectively, to be amplified and sequenced with

    them, to DD, defined as a categorical trait, and key DD-related phenotypes by association analyses in a

    sizeable sample of Italian nuclear families. Finally, we tested the role of the selected GRIN2Bmarkers

  • 8/9/2019 GRIN2B mediates susceptibility to IQ and cognitive impairments in Developmental Dyslexia

    6/24

    '

    and a set of commonly-described environmental moderators, by applying a test for GxE interaction in sib

    pair-based association analysis of quantitative traits (van der Sluis et al., 2008; Mascheretti et al., 2013a).

    Methods

    The protocol was approved by the Scientific Institute Eugenio Medea Ethics and Scientific Board.

    Sample

    This study is part of an ongoing project on the genetics of reading disabilities at the Department of Child

    Psychiatry and Rehabilitation Centre at the Scientific Institute Eugenio Medea, Bosisio Parini, Italy, and

    at the Centro Regionale di Riferimento per i Disturbi dellApprendimento CRRDA (Regional

    Reference Center for the Specific Learning Disability), ULSS 20, Verona, Italy (Mascheretti et al.,

    2013a,b, 2014). To date, 466 unrelated nuclear families of probands with DD (738 offspring) have been

    recruited; except for 47 families that had only one parent available, all parents were represented, yielding

    a total sample of 1623 individuals, all of Italian ancestry.

    The ascertainment scheme has been reported in detail elsewhere (Marino et al., 2003). Briefly, nuclear

    families were recruited if probands met the criteria for DD according to the DSM-IV (American

    Psychiatric Association, 1994). After parental informed consent, offspring underwent an extensive

    medical assessment and a battery of tests, which evaluate text, word, non-word reading (Cornoldi and

    Colpo, 1995, 1998; Sartori et al., 1995), writing-under-dictation of word, non-word and sentences-

    containing-homophones (Sartori et al., 1995), forward/backward digit spans (Reynolds and Bigler, 1994),

    phonemic elision and blending (Cossu et al., 1988), mathematics abilities (Cornoldi et al., 2003; Cornoldi

    and Lucangeli, 2004), and the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Revised WISC-R (Wechsler,

    1981) or the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, third edition WISC-III (Wechsler, 2006). For all

    tests standardized scores on the Italian population are provided. Sibs were administered only two subtests

    of the intelligence scale, i.e. vocabulary and block design, that show a high correlation (r) with,

    respectively, verbal IQ (r= 0.82; Wechsler, 1981, 2006) and performance IQ (r= 0.73; Wechsler, 1981,

    2006). The criteria used to define the probands affection status were: A) a performance on a timed text-

    reading test at least 2 standard deviations below the expected grade level mean on either accuracy or

    speed; or: B) an absolute score at least 2 standard deviations below the expected grade level mean on

  • 8/9/2019 GRIN2B mediates susceptibility to IQ and cognitive impairments in Developmental Dyslexia

    7/24

    (

    accuracy or speed in a reading list of unrelated words or non-words; and: C) IQ > 84. Siblings were

    included if they were fully-biological, older than 6 and younger than 18 years, if they had no history of

    neurological and sensorial disorders, and if the mean score of vocabulary and block design subtests was

    above 7, regardless of their reading performance. Blood or mouthwash samples were obtained from all

    offspring and their biological parents.

    Phenotypes

    We considered 8 phenotypes in the single-marker TDT analyses:

    1. DD as a discrete trait;

    2.

    READING, as measured by averaging speed (seconds) and accuracy (expressed in number of

    errors) grade-standardized scores in text, word and non-word reading tasks (Cornoldi and Colpo, 1995,

    1998; Sartori et al., 1995), since mean bivariate correlation among the above-mentioned tasks was

    substantial (r= 0.450);

    3. SPELLING, as measured by averaging the accuracy (expressed in number of errors), grade-

    standardized scores in writing-under-dictation word, non-word and sentences-containing-homophones

    (bivariate correlation among the above-mentioned tests r= 0.563; Sartori et al., 1995);

    4. Performance IQ, as measured by the WISC-R or the WISC-III. For siblings, the block design

    subtest was used to provide a prorated, performance IQ score (Wechsler, 1981, 2006);

    5. Auditory STM, as measured by averaging age-normed scores in the forward and backward digit

    spans (bivariate correlation among the above-mentioned spans r= 0.701; Reynolds and Bigler, 1994);

    6. Phonemic elision (ELISION; Cossu et al., 1988);

    7. Phonemic blending (BLENDING; Cossu et al., 1988);

    8.

    Mathematical abilities (Cornoldi et al., 2003; Cornoldi and Lucangeli, 2004), as measured by a

    principal-component-analysis factor which accounted for 53% of total test variance among mental and

    written calculation, number dictation and numerical facts (mean factor loadings= 0.64 0.08;

    MATHEMATICS).

    Of the total sample (n= 466; see Sample paragraph): i) 324 families (538 offspring) had complete

    measures of READING, SPELLING, performance IQ, auditory STM (group 1), and ii) 174 families (223

  • 8/9/2019 GRIN2B mediates susceptibility to IQ and cognitive impairments in Developmental Dyslexia

    8/24

    )

    offspring) had complete measures of ELISION, BLENDING and MATHEMATICS (group 2). Group 1

    and group 2 overlapped with respect to the phenotypic data available.

    Environmental data collection

    Selection of the environmental factors has been extensively described elsewhere (Mascheretti et al.,

    2013a,b). Briefly, by employing a large case-control sample, we removed all variables with at-risk

    category frequency under 5% and collapsed those variables that were highly correlated to obtain more

    concise and comprehensive variables (Mascheretti et al., 2013a,b). Environmental factors included in

    further analyses were:

    1.

    maternal smoking hereafter smoke;

    2.

    risk of miscarriage during pregnancy hereafter miscarriage;

    3. birth weight;

    4. breast feeding;

    5. parental age, as an average of father and mothers age at childs birth;

    6. socio-economic status SES, defined by parental employment;

    7. parental education, as an average of father and mothers educational qualifications.

    Dichotomous responses (yes/no) were coded with 0 when answers were no, and 1 when answers

    were yes, except for breastfeeding, which was coded as 0 if child had been breastfed. The employment

    response was coded according to the Hollingshead 9-point scale (Hollingshead, 1975). A score (from 10

    to 90) was assigned to each job; the higher of two scores was used when both parents were employed.

    The educational qualification response was scored according to a 9-point ordinal scale based on the

    Italian school system (range between 10, corresponding to fifth-grade elementary school, and 90,

    equivalent to a post-doctoral degree). One-hundred and seventy-eight families (144 pairs, 31 triplets, 2

    with four siblings and 1 with five siblings, 394 offspring) had at least one sibling and complete

    environmental data (group 3); in this latter group, complete phenotypes were available for READING,

    SPELLING, performance IQ, auditory STM, while only 60 families had complete data for ELISION,

    BLENDING and MATHEMATICS, which were therefore excluded from GxE analyses, due to limited

    power. As for group 2, Group 1 and group 3 overlapped with respect to the phenotypic data available. All

  • 8/9/2019 GRIN2B mediates susceptibility to IQ and cognitive impairments in Developmental Dyslexia

    9/24

    *

    environmental factors, except SMOKE, MISCARRIAGE, breast feeding, and parental marital status were

    z-transformed based on the sample norms to avoid a scaling effect in further analyses.

    Genotyping

    Amplification and sequencing of four portions of the human GRIN2B gene allowed typing of the

    following SNPs: rs5796555-/A; rs1012586G/C; rs2268119A/T; rs2216128T/C; rs11609779C/T;

    rs2192973C/T. In particular, we chose to genotype the four SNPs that in a previous study were found

    significantly associated with short-term memory in a German DD sample (i.e., rs1012586, rs2268119,

    rs2216128, and rs2192973; Ludwig et al., 2009), suggesting that variations in GRIN2Bmay contribute to

    the genetic background of specific cognitive processes which are correlated to DD. Markers rs5796555-

    /A and rs11609779C/T are close enough to markers rs1012586G/C and rs2216128T/C, respectively, to be

    amplified and sequenced with them. Amplifications were performed in 10-microliter reactions using

    JumpStart Red ACCUTaq LA DNA polymerase (Sigma) and the following protocol: 30 s at 96C, 35

    cycles of 15 s at 94C/20 s at 58C/30 sec at 68C, 5 min final elongation time. Sequencing reactions

    were performed with a Big Dye Terminator Cycle Sequencing kit (Applied Biosystems) and run on an

    ABI Prism 3130xl Genetic Analyzer. (Primers are available on request from the authors).

    Genotype error checking was completed in PEDSTATS (Wigginton and Abecasis, 2005) and inconsistent

    genotypes were zeroed-out and were not considered for further analysis. Families were excluded in the

    case of multiple inconsistent genotypes. Allelic frequencies and Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium for the

    markers under consideration were calculated in parents. No significant deviations from allele frequencies

    reported in Ludwig et al. (2009) were found. For all the 6 genotyped SNPs, p-values for deviation from

    the HardyWeinberg equilibrium were not lower than 0.008 (= 0.05/6), and thus no SNPs were excluded.

    The linkage disequilibrium structure of GRIN2Bwas analyzed using the parental genotypes only; linkage

    disequilibrium was extracted and plotted in Haploview 4.0). Even if markers rs5796555-/A and

    rs11609779C/T were typed as a by-product of rs1012586G/C and rs2216128T/C, respectively, they were

    not excluded from further analysis since they did not show correlation with any of other genotyped SNPs.

    Statistical analysis

    Genetic association analysis

  • 8/9/2019 GRIN2B mediates susceptibility to IQ and cognitive impairments in Developmental Dyslexia

    10/24

    !+

    Genetic association was investigated by using a family-based association test, i.e., the quantitative

    transmission disequilibrium test (QTDT, version 2.5.1) as modeled by Abecasis et al. (2000), which

    allows to control for population stratification bias and for the analyses of quantitative traits. Single-

    marker TDT analyses were carried out for DD as a discrete trait using the -ad option which allows to

    test using only affected individuals. Quantitative traits were analyzed using the -wega option which

    allows to adopt commonly used variance components, i.e., the environmental variance (e), polygenic

    variance (g) and additive major locus (a). Genetic findings are not presented for the whole set of families

    because of missing data and because transmission disequilibrium tests require individuals to have

    heterozygous ancestors in the pedigree so offspring of homozygous parents are uninformative (Abecasis

    et al., 2000). We calculated the genetic association for the total sample. Moreover, since following-up

    association signals over different severity groups may confirm a possible true contribution of the selected

    SNPs to DD and DD-related performance (Ludwig et al., 2009), we tested the genetic association for a

    subsample selected by severity, i.e., by selecting only the nuclear families in which at least one offspring

    scored !2.50 SD below the general population mean on either accuracy or speed in either text-, or word-,

    or non-word reading tasks (n= 292). Only empirical p-values are reported which are computed from

    10.000 Monte-Carlo permutations by entering all the selected markers (see Methods section,

    Genotyping paragraph) and the above-described phenotypes (see Methods section, Phenotypes

    paragraph) at the same time. Bonferroni correction for multiple testing was not applied because it would

    have been too conservative (Deffenbacher et al., 2004; Francks et al., 2004; Cope et al., 2005; Meng et

    al., 2005; Schumacher et al., 2006; Brkanac et al., 2007; Marino et al., 2012). Thus far, we decided to

    adjust the significance levels by the false discovery rate (FDR) method (Storey, 2002) applied to the 7

    neuropsychological traits analyzed for each marker, separately for each SNP.

    Gene-by-environment interaction analysis

    In order to explore the conjoint role of candidate genes markers and measured environmental factors upon

    performance IQ and DD-related neuropsychological skills (i.e., READING, SPELLING, and Auditory

    STM), we analyzed GxE effects through a general test for GxE interaction in sib pair-based association

    analysis of quantitative traits (van der Sluis et al., 2008).

    Briefly, this statistical model, is an extension of the Fulker et al.(1999) maximum likelihood variance

    components analysis of quantitative traits in sib-pairs data that incorporates environmental main effects

  • 8/9/2019 GRIN2B mediates susceptibility to IQ and cognitive impairments in Developmental Dyslexia

    11/24

    !!

    plus GxE effects (van der Sluis et al., 2008). The association effect is orthogonally decomposed here into

    between-family (b) and within-family (w) effects, that is the expectation of each sib genotype conditional

    on family genotype data and the deviation from this expectation for each offspring, respectively. To

    model the interaction effect, we used the sibling-specific value for each environmental risk variable; the

    phenotypic score (i.e., the observed phenotypic score y for subject j from family i with genotype g) is then

    modeled as:

    yijg="i + abAbi + awAwij + eEi+ ibgEijAbi + iwgEijAwij + #ij

    where"i is the family-specific intercept, aband aware the estimated between- and within-family additive

    genetic effects of the marker, according to the orthogonal decomposition, e represents the effects of the

    environmental risk factor, ibg and iwg represent the between- and within-family effects of the interaction of

    genotype g and the environmental risk factor, and #ij is the residual term (van der Sluis et al., 2008). As

    such, iwgbecomes an estimate of the GxE effect inasmuch as it indicates the change in the allelic variant

    marker association with the phenotype across the different ecological niches, and it represents the

    variation of SD units of the z-score on the neuropsychological composite. For instance, a positive value

    represents the increase of the effect of each additional transmission of the minor allele on the

    neuropsychological composite z-score when environmental variable increases. A negative value of iwg

    represents a decrease of the same effect (for more information, see Mascheretti et al., 2013a).

    The permutation procedure was repeated 1.000 times for each analysis. We decided to adjust the

    significance levels by the false discovery rate (FDR) method (Storey, 2002) applied to the 28 tests

    performed for each marker (7 environmental variable x 4 phenotypes), separately for each marker

    (Mascheretti et al., 2013a). Gender was taken into account in the extended equation because probands

    sex ratio (males:females) in our sample was nearly 3:1, and it may imply differences in mean scores

    between males and females.

    Moreover, since simulations studies revealed specific situations in which dichotomized variables

    performed as well as or better than the original quantitative factors, we subsequently decided to

    dichotomize raw scores of quantitative environmental variables (i.e., birth weight, parental age, SES, and

  • 8/9/2019 GRIN2B mediates susceptibility to IQ and cognitive impairments in Developmental Dyslexia

    12/24

    !#

    parental education; see Methods, Environmental data collection section) according to well-defined

    cut-off points.

    Results

    Genetic association analyses with DD and DD-related quantitative traits

    Significant associations were found between DD as a discrete trait and the minor alleles A and G of

    the markers rs5796555-/A and rs1012586G/C, respectively, in the total sample (informative families=

    331, T= -2.04, standard deviation= 8.63, nominal p-value= 0.043, and informative families= 347, T= -

    1.99, standard deviation= 8.65, nominal p-value= 0.047, respectively). In the selected-by-severity

    subsample DD as a discrete trait was significantly associated with the minor allele A of the marker

    rs5796555-/A (informative families= 227, T= -1.99, standard deviation= 6.65, nominal p-value= 0.048),

    while only a trend towards significance was observed for rs1012586G/C marker (informative families=

    234, T= -1.79, standard deviation= 6.84, nominal p-value= 0.076). Table 1 and 2 shows both the

    empirical p-values obtained after implementing 10.000 Monte-Carlo permutations and the q-values after

    FDR correction.Market-trait associations for READING and SPELLING were non-significant in both

    group 1 and in the selected-by-severity subsample (Table 1). Performance IQ was significantly associated

    to the minor alleles T, C and T of markers rs2268119A/T ( $2= 6.44;nominal p-value= 0.011;

    empirical p-value= 0.009; q-value= 0.004; 224 informative families; genetic effect= -4.714; Table 1),

    rs2216128T/C ($2= 12.38;nominal p-value

  • 8/9/2019 GRIN2B mediates susceptibility to IQ and cognitive impairments in Developmental Dyslexia

    13/24

    !$

    empirical p-value= 0.037; q-value= 0.041; 118 informative families; genetic effect= -0.427; Table 2).

    BLENDING and MATHEMATICS did not yield any significant association with any markers (Table 2)

    in both group 2 and the selected-by-severity subsample.

    Gene-by-environment interaction analysis

    We analyzed GxE interaction effects between 6 markers and 7 environmental factors through the general

    test for GxE interaction upon READING, SPELLING, performance IQ, and auditory STM composites in

    group 3. Although we found G-E combinations whose empirical p-values of the iwg term was significant,

    none survived after FDR correction. With regard to the investigation of the gender effect, no empirical p-

    values were significant.

    Finally, we analyzed GxE interaction effects between 6 markers and the 4 dichotomized quantitative

    environmental factors (see Methods, Statistical Analysis section, Gene-by-environment interaction

    analysis paragraph) through the general test for GxE interaction upon the same neurosychological

    phenotypes (i.e., READING, SPELLING, performance IQ, and auditory STM) in group 3. We found G-E

    combinations whose empirical p-values of the iwgterm was significant and which showed 33% overlap

    with results obtained by implementing quantitative variables, but none survived after FDR correction. No

    significant gender effect was found.

    Discussion

    The ability to read relies on a complex, highly integrated, large-scale network of different cognitive

    processes. Recently, research applied quantitative cognitive measures to dissect the heterogeneous DD

    phenotype with respect to the genetic contribution (Marlow et al., 2003; Paracchini et al., 2007; Schulte-

    Krne et al., 2007).

    This study was primarily designed to replicate previous findings of the involvement of the GRIN2Bgene

    in DD and its related neuropsychological phenotypes, in a sizable sample of Italian nuclear families

    ascertainedfor DD. We therefore investigated both linear and GxE interplay effects upon DD and DD-

    related quantitative phenotypes. QTDT analyses yielded significant associations between markers

    spanning within GRIN2B and DD as a categorical trait and its related quantitative phenotypes, i.e.,

    performance IQ, auditory STM and phonemic elision. These findings suggest that GRIN2Bcan account

    for not only part of the disabilities proper of DD, defined as a categorical trait, but also for part of the

  • 8/9/2019 GRIN2B mediates susceptibility to IQ and cognitive impairments in Developmental Dyslexia

    14/24

    !%

    reduced skills in key DD-related cognitive domains that are observable in these children. Indeed, DD is a

    heterogeneous disorder in which various phenotypic dimensions are involved, including phonological

    processing, and memory processes (Gabrieli, 2009). As such, our data support the view that GRIN2Bmay

    influence inter-individual variation in multiple DD-related phenotypes thus shedding further light into the

    etiologic basis and the phenotypic complexity of DD.

    The association between GRIN2Band forward/backward digit spans, which measure memory formation,

    storage and retrieval with an additional component of manipulation of items for the backward recall, can

    be considered a replication of a previous study in which this task had been found associated with GRIN2B

    variation in a sample ascertained for DD (Ludwig et al., 2009). This finding, together with the reported

    association with phonemic elision task, is in harmony with several studies showing that individuals with

    DD perform poorly in memory-related task (Swanson et al., 2009), and with genetic studies reporting that

    variation in reading performance is explained by specific genes and by a set of genes in common with

    STM and working memory. These evidence suggest a common genetic factor that could explain the

    storage and manipulation of phonological relevant information, which are important aspects of STM and

    working memory and fundamental steps in the reading acquisition process (van Leeuwen et al., 2009).

    Turning to the association with performance IQ, it points towards GRIN2B as a susceptibility gene

    explaining the observed phenotypic relationship between IQ and reading. Several studies show that IQ is

    one factor typically associated with differences in reading development and reading disabilities

    (Stanovich, 1986; Shaywitz et al., 1995; Berninger et al., 2001; Ferrer and McArdle, 2004; Ferrer et al.,

    2007, 2010), suggesting an underlying common genetic etiology between IQ and DD. Actually, twin

    studies show that shared genetic influences accounted for more than half of the phenotypic covariance

    between the reading tests and IQ in both reading-disabled (Light et al., 1998) and control twin samples

    (Wadsworth et al., 1995; Light et al., 1998; Luo etal., 2003; Wainwright et al., 2004). In particular, there

    was evidence that genetic influences contribute a larger proportion of the covariance between

    Performance IQ tests and reading measures than for Verbal IQ tests and reading measures (Wainwright et

    al., 2004). This is due to the common environment factor having a somewhat stronger influence on the

    correlations among Verbal IQ tests and reading measures, possibly through formal education or the

    intellectual environment within the home, while having less effect on the correlations among Performance

    tests (Wainwright et al., 2004). As such, these findings likely represent a first evidence in favor of the

  • 8/9/2019 GRIN2B mediates susceptibility to IQ and cognitive impairments in Developmental Dyslexia

    15/24

    !&

    GRIN2Bgene as one of the multiple genes implicated in the reported genetic covariance between reading

    (dis)ability and IQ.

    Finally, although our results were not significant, the investigation of GxE interplay effects provides

    preliminary evidence that GRIN2Bcan exert detrimental effects upon DD-related cognitive phenotypes in

    response to environmental disadvantage, contributing to the intense debate about unraveling the intimate

    mechanisms of response of this gene to environment. To date, only three studies investigated the

    interactions effects between GRIN2B and environment in humans, both showing that environment

    moderates the association between this gene and human behavioral and cognitive variation (Demontis et

    al., 2011; Sokolowski et al., 2012; Riva et al., under review). In the light of these data, a larger sample is

    needed in order to replicate previous studies and provide further evidence about the moderating effects

    exerted by the GRIN2B gene upon the development of complex traits in interaction with putative

    environmental risk variables.

    In summary, our data add further evidence indicating shared biological or cognitive processes that

    underlie DD and DD-related cognitive phenotypes. Since the N-methyl D-aspartate 2B receptor is found

    at high concentrations in the hippocampus (Charton et al., 1999), contributes to experience-dependent

    synaptic plasticity at structural and functional levels (Cull-Candy et al., 2001), and is implicated in both

    basic neuronal functions and central nervous system processes and in behavioural tasks (Lau and Zukin,

    2007; Kalia et al., 2008), it is plausible that GRIN2B might influence development of etiologically

    heterogeneous disorders in which learning and memory functions represent core features, such as DD.

    Nevertheless, these results should be viewed with some limitations in mind. First, the results of these

    univariate association analyses should be considered exploratory, and in the future, the relationship

    between potential risk loci and the various aspects of these traits may be better explored using newly

    developed methods for multivariate association analyses of multiple related traits. Second, as with all

    analyses of complex traits, the significance of the current association findings should be interpreted

    cautiously until they can be replicated in independent samples of DD.

    Acknowledgments

  • 8/9/2019 GRIN2B mediates susceptibility to IQ and cognitive impairments in Developmental Dyslexia

    16/24

    !'

    We thank all the parents and children who took part in this study. We express our gratitude to Ermanno

    Quadrelli, Lara Francesca Emilia Lanzoni, Lisa Meneghello and Elisabetta Furioni for helping in data

    collection. Finally, we thank Giorgia Menozzi for helping in statistical analyses.

    References

    Abecasis, GR, Cookson, WO, Cardon, LR. (2000). Pedigree tests of transmission disequilibrium.Eur J

    Hum Genet 8:545-551.

    Akashi, K, Kakizaki, T, Kamiya, H, Fukaya, M, Yamasaki, M, Abe, M, et al.(2009). NMDA receptor

    GluN2B (GluR epsilon 2/NR2B) subunit is crucial for channel function, postsynaptic macromolecular

    organization, and actin cytoskeleton at hippocampal CA3 synapses.J Neurosci 29:10869-10882.

    American Psychiatric Association. (1994).DSM-IV Manuale diagnostico e statistico dei disturbi

    mentali. Edizioni Masson Milano.

    Batstra, L, Hadders-Algra, M, Neeleman, J. (2003). Effect of antenatal exposure to maternal smoking on

    behavioural problems and academic achievement in childhood: prospective evidence from a Dutch birth

    cohort.Early Hum Dev 75:21-33.

    Berninger, VW, Abbott, RD, Thomson, JB, Raskind, WH. (2001). Language phenotype for reading and

    writing disability: A family approach. Scientific Studies of Reading 5:59-106.

    Byrne, B, Delaland, C, Fielding-Barnsley, R, Quain, P, Samuelsson, S, Hoien, T, Corley, R, DeFries, JC,

    Wadsworth, S, Willcutt, E. Olson, RK. (2002). Longitudinal twin study of early reading development in

    three countries: preliminary results.Ann Dyslexia52:4974.

    Bowen, JR, Gibson, FL, Hand, PJ. (2002). Educational outcome at 8 years for children who were born

    extremely prematurely: a controlled study.J Paediatr Child Health 38:438-444.

    Brkanac, Z, Chapman, NH, Matsushita, MM, Chun, L, Nielsen, K, Cochrane, E, et al.(2007). Evaluation

    of candidate genes for DYX1 and DYX2 in families with dyslexia.Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr

    Genet 144B:556-560.

    Charton, JP, Herkert, M, Becker, CM, Schroder, H. (1999). Cellular and subcellular localization of the

    2B-subunit of the NMDA receptor in the adult rat telencephalon.Brain Res 816:609-617.

  • 8/9/2019 GRIN2B mediates susceptibility to IQ and cognitive impairments in Developmental Dyslexia

    17/24

    !(

    Cope, N, Harold, D, Hill, G, Moskvina, V, Stevenson, J, Holmans, P, et al.(2005). Strong evidence that

    KIAA0319 on chromosome 6p is a susceptibility gene for developmental dyslexia. Am J Hum Genet

    76:581-591.

    Cornoldi C, CG. (1995). Prove di lettura MT, nuove prove di lettura MT per la scuola media inferiore.

    Organizzazioni Speciali Firenze.

    Cornoldi, C, Colpo, G. (e gruppo MT) (1998).Prove di Lettura MT per la Scuola Elementare

    2. Organizzazioni Speciali Firenze.

    Cornoldi, C, Lucangeli, D, Bellina, M. (2003).AC-MT, Test di Valutazione delle Abilit di Calcolo -

    Gruppo MT. Edizioni Erickson Trento.

    Cornoldi, C, Lucangeli, D. (2004). Arithmetic education and learning disabilities in Italy. J Learn

    Disabil 37:42-49.

    Cossu, G, Shankweiler, D, Liberman, IY, Katz, L, Tola, G. (1988). Awareness of phonological segments

    and reading ability in Italian children.Applied Psycholinguistics 9:1-16.

    Cull-Candy, S, Brickley, S, Farrant, M. (2001). NMDA receptor subunits: diversity, development and

    disease. Curr Opin Neurobiol 11:327-335.

    Davis, CJ, Gayan, J, Knopik, VS, Smith, SD, Cardon, LR, Pennington, BF, Olson, RK, DeFries, JC.

    (2001) Etiology of reading difficulties and rapid naming: the Colorado Twin Study of Reading Disability.

    Behav Genet31:625635.

    Deffenbacher, KE, Kenyon, JB, Hoover, DM, Olson, RK, Pennington, BF, DeFries, JC, et al. (2004).

    Refinement of the 6p21.3 quantitative trait locus influencing dyslexia: linkage and association

    analyses.Hum Genet 115:128-138.

    Demontis, D, Nyegaard, M, Buttenschon, HN, Hedemand, A, Pedersen, CB, Grove, J, et al. (2011).

    Association of GRIN1 and GRIN2A-D with schizophrenia and genetic interaction with maternal herpes

    simplex virus-2 infection affecting disease risk.Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet156B:913-922.

    Fergusson, DM, Lynskey, MT. (1993). Maternal age and cognitive and behavioural outcomes in middle

    childhood.Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol 7:77-91.

    Fergusson, DM, Woodward, LJ. (1999). Maternal age and educational and psychosocial outcomes in

    early adulthood.J Child Psychol Psychiatry 40:479-489.

  • 8/9/2019 GRIN2B mediates susceptibility to IQ and cognitive impairments in Developmental Dyslexia

    18/24

    !)

    Ferrer, E, McArdle, JJ. (2004). An experimental analysis of dynamic hypotheses about cognitive abilities

    and achievement from childhood to early adulthood.Dev Psychol 40:935-952.

    Ferrer, E, McArdle, JJ, Shaywitz, BA, Holahan, JM, Marchione, K, Shaywitz, SE. (2007). Longitudinal

    models of developmental dynamics between reading and cognition from childhood to adolescence.Dev

    Psychol 43:1460-1473.

    Ferrer, E, Shaywitz, BA, Holahan, JM, Marchione, K, Shaywitz, SE. (2010). Uncoupling of reading and

    IQ over time: empirical evidence for a definition of dyslexia.Psychol Sci 21:93-101.

    Fetterolf, F, Foster, KA. (2011). Regulation of long-term plasticity induction by the channel and C-

    terminal domains of GluN2 subunits.Mol Neurobiol 44:71-82.

    Fisher, SE, De Fries, JC. (2002). Developmental dyslexia: genetic dissection of a complex cognitive trait.

    Nat Rev Neurosci3:767-780.

    Francks, C, Paracchini, S, Smith, SD, Richardson, AJ, Scerri, TS, Cardon, LR, et al. (2004). A 77-

    kilobase region of chromosome 6p22.2 is associated with dyslexia in families from the United Kingdom

    and from the United States.Am J Hum Genet 75:1046-1058.

    Fried, PA, Watkinson, B, Siegel, LS. (1997). Reading and language in 9- to 12-year olds prenatally

    exposed to cigarettes and marijuana.Neurotoxicol Teratol 19:171-183.

    Fulker, DW, Cherny, SS, Sham, PC, Hewitt, JK. (1999). Combined linkage and association sib-pair

    analysis for quantitative traits.Am J Hum Genet 64:259-267.

    Gabrieli, JD. (2009). Dyslexia: a new synergy between education and cognitive

    neuroscience. Science 325:280-283.

    Gayn, J, Olson, RK. (2001). Genetic and environmental influences on orthographic and phonological

    skills in children with reading disabilities.Dev Neuropsychol20:483507.

    Gayn, J, Olson, RK. (2003). Genetic and environmental influences on individual differences in printed

    word recognition.J Exp Child Psychol84:97123.

    Gilger, JW, Pennington, BF, Green, P, Smith, SM, Smith, SD. (1992). Reading disability, immune

    disorders and non-right-handedness: twin and family studies of their relations.Neuropsychologia30:209-

    227.

    Hallgren, B. (1950). Specific dyslexia (congenital word-blindnes): a clinical and genetic study. Acta

    Psychiatr Neurol Suppl65:1-287.

  • 8/9/2019 GRIN2B mediates susceptibility to IQ and cognitive impairments in Developmental Dyslexia

    19/24

    !*

    Hannula-Jouppi, K, Kaminen-Ahola, N, Taipale, M, Eklund, R, Nopola-Hemmi, J, Krinen, H, et al.

    (2005). The axon guidance receptor gene ROBO1 is a candidate gene for developmental dyslexia. PloS

    Genet1:e50.

    Harlaar, N, Dale, PS, Plomin, R. (2007). Reading exposure: a (largely) environmental risk factor with

    environmentally-mediated effects on reading performance in the primary school years. J Child Psychol

    Psychiatry48:11921199.

    Hoff, E, Tian, C. (2005). Socioeconomic status and cultural influences on language.J Commun

    Disord 38:271-278.

    Hollingshead, AB. (1975). Four factor index of social status. New Haven, CT: Yale University.

    Hoque, MM, Ahmed, NU, Khan, FH, Jahan, R, Yasmeen, HN, Chowdhury, MA.

    (2012). Breastfeeding and cognitive development of children: assessment at one year of age.Mymensingh

    Med J. 21:316-321.

    Ito, J, Araki, A, Tanaka, H, Tasaki, T, Cho, K. (1997). Intellectual status of children with cerebral palsy

    after elementary education.Pediatr Rehabil 1:199-206.

    Jimnez Glez, JE, Rodrigo Lpez, M. (1994). Is it true that the differences in reading performance

    between students with and without LD cannot be explained by IQ?J Learn Disabil27:155-163.

    Kalia, LV, Kalia, SK, Salter, MW. (2008). NMDA receptors in clinical neurology: excitatory times

    ahead.Lancet Neurol 7:742-755.

    Kim, MJ, Dunah, AW, Wang, YT, Sheng, M. (2005). Differential roles of NR2A- and NR2B-containing

    NMDA receptors in Ras-ERK signaling and AMPA receptor trafficking.Neuron 46:745-760.

    Knopik, VS, DeFries, JC. (1999). Etiology of covariation between reading and mathematics performance:

    a twin study. Twin Res 2:226-234.

    Kovas, Y, Haworth, CM, Harlaar, N, Petrill, SA, Dale, PS, Plomin, R. (2007). Overlap and specificity of

    genetic and environmental influences on mathematics and reading disability in 10-year-old twins.J Child

    Psychol Psychiatry 48:914-922.

    Kutsuwada, T, Sakimura, K, Manabe, T, Takayama, C, Katakura, N, Kushiya, E, et al. (1996).

    Impairment of suckling response, trigeminal neuronal pattern formation, and hippocampal LTD in

    NMDA receptor epsilon 2 subunit mutant mice.Neuron 16:333-344.

  • 8/9/2019 GRIN2B mediates susceptibility to IQ and cognitive impairments in Developmental Dyslexia

    20/24

    #+

    Lau, CG, Zukin, RS. (2007). NMDA receptor trafficking in synaptic plasticity and neuropsychiatric

    disorders.Nat Rev Neurosci 8:413-426.

    Light, JG, DeFries, JC. (1995). Comorbidity of reading and mathematics disabilities:genetic and

    environmental etiologies.J Learning Disabilities 28:96-106.

    Light, JG, DeFries, JC, Olson, RK. (1998). Multivariate behavioral genetic analysis of achievement and

    cognitive measuress in reading-disabled and control twin pairs.Hum Biol70:215-237.

    Ludwig, KU, Roeske, D, Herms, S, Schumacher, J, Warnke, A, Plume, E. (2009). Variation in GRIN2B

    contributes to weak performance in verbal short-term memory in children with dyslexia.Am J Med Genet

    B Neuropsychiatr Genet 153:503-511.

    Luo, D, Thompson, LA, Detterman, DK. (2003). Phenotypic and behavioral genetic covariation between

    elemental cognitive components and scholastic measures. Behav Genet 33:221-246.

    Marino, C, Giorda, R, Vanzin, L, Molteni, M, Lorusso, ML, Nobile, M, et al.(2003). No evidence for

    association and linkage disequilibrium between dyslexia and markers of four dopamine-related

    genes.Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 12:198-202.

    Marino, C, Meng, H, Mascheretti, S, Rusconi, M, Cope, N, Giorda R, et al. (2012). DCDC2 genetic

    variants and susceptibility to developmental dyslexia.Psychiatr Genet 22(1):25- 30.

    Markowitz, EM, Willemsen, G, Trumbetta, SL, van Beijsterveldt, TC, Boomsma, DI. (2005). The

    etiology of mathematical and reading (dis)ability covariation in a sample of Dutch twins. Twin Res Hum

    Genet 8:585-593.

    Marlow, AJ, Fisher, SE, Francks, C, MacPhie, IL, Cherny, SS, Richardson, AJ, et al. (2003). Use of

    multivariate linkage analysis for dissection of a complex cognitive trait.Am J Hum Genet 72:561-570.

    Mascheretti, S, Bureau, A, Battaglia, M, Simone, D, Quadrelli, E, Croteau, J, et al. (2013a). An

    assessment of gene-by-environment interactions in developmental dyslexia-related phenotypes. Genes

    Brain Behav 12:47-55.

    Mascheretti, S, Marino, C, Simone, D, Quadrelli, E, Riva, V, Cellino, MR, et al.(2013b) Putative risk

    factors in developmental dyslexia: A case-control study of italian children. J Learning Disabilities PMID:

    23757350.

  • 8/9/2019 GRIN2B mediates susceptibility to IQ and cognitive impairments in Developmental Dyslexia

    21/24

    #!

    Mascheretti, S, Riva, V, Giorda, R, Beri, S, Lanzoni, LF, Cellino, MR, Marino, C. (2014). KIAA0319

    and ROBO1: evidence on association with reading and pleiotropic effects on language and mathematics

    abilities in developmental dyslexia.J Hum Genet59:189-197.

    Melekian BA. (2001). Family characteristics of children with dyslexia.Journal of Learning

    Disabilities 23:386-391.

    Meng, H, Hager, K, Held, M, Page, GP, Olson, RK, Pennington, BF, et al. (2005). TDT-association

    analysis of EKN1 and dyslexia in a Colorado twin cohort.Hum Genet118:87-90.

    Michaelsen, KF, Lauritzen, L, Mortensen, EL. (2009). Effects of breast-feeding on cognitive

    function.Adv Exp Med Biol 639:199-215.

    Newman, S, Wright, S, Fields, H. (1991). Identification of a group of children with dyslexia by means of

    IQ-achievement discrepancies.Br J Educ Psychol61:139-154.

    Paracchini, S, Scerri, T, Monaco, AP. (2007). The genetic lexicon of dyslexia.Annu Rev Genomics Hum

    Genet 8:57-79.

    Pennington, BF. (1995). Genetics of learning disabilities.J Child Neurol1:S69-77.

    Petrill, SA, Deater-Deckard, K, Thompson, LA, Dethorne, LS, Schatschneider, C. (2006). Reading skills

    in early readers: genetic and shared environmental influences.J Learn Disabil39:4855.

    Plomin, R, Kovas, Y. (2005). Generalist genes and learning disabilities.Psychol Bull 131:592-617.

    Reynolds, CR, Bigler, ED. (1994). Test of memory and learning. Edizioni Erickson Trento.

    Rispens, J, van Yperen, TA, van Duijn, GA. (1991). The irrelevance of IQ to the definition of learning

    disabilities: some empirical evidence.J Learn Disabil24:434-438.

    Rutter, M, Moffitt, TE, Caspi, A. (2006). Gene-environment interplay and psychopathology: multiple

    varieties but real effects.J Child Psychol Psychiatry 47:226261.

    Samuelsson, S, Finnstrom, O, Flodmark, O, Gaddlin, PO, Leijon, I, Wadsby, M. (2006). A longitudinal

    study of reading skills among very-low-birthweight children: is there a catch-up? J Pediatr

    Psychol31:967-977.

    Sartori, G, Job, R, Tressoldi, PE. (1995).Batteria per la valutazione della dislessia e della disortografia

    evolutiva. Organizzazioni Speciali Firenze.

    Scerri, TS, Schulte-Korne, G. (2010). Genetics of developmental dyslexia.Eur Child Adolesc

    Psychiatry 19:179-197.

  • 8/9/2019 GRIN2B mediates susceptibility to IQ and cognitive impairments in Developmental Dyslexia

    22/24

    ##

    Schulte-Korne, G, Ziegler, A, Deimel, W, Schumacher, J, Plume, E, Bachmann, C, et al. (2007).

    Interrelationship and familiality of dyslexia related quantitative measures.Ann Hum Genet 71:160-175.

    Schumacher, J, Anthoni, H, Dahdouh, F, Konig, IR, Hillmer, AM, Kluck, N, et al.(2006). Strong genetic

    evidence of DCDC2 as a susceptibility gene for dyslexia.Am J Hum Genet 78:52-62.

    Shaywitz, BA, Fletcher, JM, Shaywitz, SE. (1995). Defining and classifying learning disabilities and

    attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.J Child Neurol 10 Suppl 1:S50-7.

    Snowling, MJ (2013). Early identification and interventions for dyslexia: a contemporary view. JORSEN

    13:7-14.

    Sokolowski M, Ben-Efraim YJ, Wasserman J, Wasserman D. (2012). Glutamatergic GRIN2B and

    polyaminergic ODC1 genes in suicide attempts: Associations and gene-environment interactions with

    childhood/adolescent physical assault.Mol Psychiatry doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.112; 10.1038/mp.2012.112.

    Stanovich, KE. (1986). New beginnings, old problems. S.Ceci, Handbook of Cognitive, Social, and

    Neuropsychological Aspects of Learning Disabilities 1:3-54. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbraum

    Associates.

    Storey, J. (2002). A direct approach to false discovery rates. J Royal Statistical Society, Series 64:479-

    498.

    Stuebing, KK, Fletcher, JM, LeDoux, JM, Lyon, GR, Shaywitz, SE, Shaywitz, BA. (2002). Validity of

    IQ-discrepancy classifications of reading disabilities: A metaanalysis. American Educational Research

    Journal39:469-518.

    Swanson HL, Xinhua Z, Jerman O. (2009). Working memory, short memory, and reading disabilities: a

    selective meta-analysis of the literature.J Learn Disabil42:260287.

    Taipale, M, Kaminen, N, Nopola-Hemmi, J, Haltia, T, Myllyluoma, B, Lyytinen, H, et al. (2003). A

    candidate gene fir developmental dyslexia encodes a nuclear tetratricopeptide repeat domain protein

    dynamically regulated in brain.Proc Natl Acad Sci USA100:11553-11558.

    Tang, Q, Gandhoke, R, Burritt, A, Hruby, VJ, Porreca, F, Lai, J. (1999). High-affinity interaction of (des-

    Tyrosyl)dynorphin A(2-17) with NMDA receptors.J Pharmacol Exp Ther 291:760-765.

    Thompson, LA, Fagan, JF, Fulker, DW. (1991). Longitudinal prediction of specific cognitive abilities

    from infant novelty preference. Child Dev 62:530-538.

  • 8/9/2019 GRIN2B mediates susceptibility to IQ and cognitive impairments in Developmental Dyslexia

    23/24

    #$

    van Baar AL, Vermaas J, Knots E, de Kleine MJ, Soons P. (2009). Functioning at school age of

    moderately preterm children born at 32 to 36 weeks' gestational age.Pediatrics :251-257.

    van der Sluis, S, Dolan, CV, Neale, MC, Posthuma, D. (2008). A general test for gene-environment

    interaction in sib pair-based association analysis of quantitative traits. Behav Genet 38:372-389.

    van Leeuwen, M, van den Berg, SM, Peper, JS, Hulshoff Pol, HE, Boomsma, DI. (2009). Genetic

    covariance structure of reading, intelligence and memory in children.Behav Genet 39:245-254.

    Wadsworth, SJ, DeFries, JC, Fulker, DW, Plomin, R. (1995). Cognitive ability and academic

    achievement in the Colorado Adoption Project: A multivariate genetic analysis of parent-offspring and

    sibling data. Bahv Genet 25:1-15.

    Wainwright, M, Wright, MJ, Geffen, GM, Geffen, LB, Luciano, M, Matin, NG. (2004). Genetic and

    environmental sources of covariance between reading tests used in neuropsychological assessment and IQ

    subtests. Behav Genet 34:365-376.

    Wang D, Cui Z, Zeng Q, Kuang H, Wang LP, Tsien JZ, Cao X. (2009). Genetic enhancement of memory

    and long-term potentiation but not CA1 long-term depression in NR2B transgenic rats. PLoS One

    4:e7486.

    Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Revised. Organizzazioni Speciali Firenze.

    Wechsler, D. (2006). Wechsler intelligence scale for children - third edition. Organizzazioni Speciali

    Firenze.

    Wigginton, JE, Abecasis, GR. (2005). PEDSTATS: descriptive statistics, graphics and quality assessment

    for gene mapping data.Bioinformatics 21:3445-3447.

  • 8/9/2019 GRIN2B mediates susceptibility to IQ and cognitive impairments in Developmental Dyslexia

    24/24