focus issue: number sense || number sense and nonsense

4
Number Sense and Nonsense Author(s): Zvia Markovits, Rina Hershkowitz, Maxim Bruckheimer, Judith Sowder and Larry Sowder Source: The Arithmetic Teacher, Vol. 36, No. 6, FOCUS ISSUE: NUMBER SENSE (February 1989), pp. 53-55 Published by: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/41194464 . Accessed: 16/06/2014 06:13 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp . JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. . National Council of Teachers of Mathematics is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The Arithmetic Teacher. http://www.jstor.org This content downloaded from 195.34.78.61 on Mon, 16 Jun 2014 06:13:36 AM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Upload: judith-sowder-and-larry-sowder

Post on 19-Jan-2017

232 views

Category:

Documents


10 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: FOCUS ISSUE: NUMBER SENSE || Number Sense and Nonsense

Number Sense and NonsenseAuthor(s): Zvia Markovits, Rina Hershkowitz, Maxim Bruckheimer, Judith Sowder and LarrySowderSource: The Arithmetic Teacher, Vol. 36, No. 6, FOCUS ISSUE: NUMBER SENSE (February1989), pp. 53-55Published by: National Council of Teachers of MathematicsStable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/41194464 .

Accessed: 16/06/2014 06:13

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range ofcontent in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new formsof scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

.

National Council of Teachers of Mathematics is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extendaccess to The Arithmetic Teacher.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 195.34.78.61 on Mon, 16 Jun 2014 06:13:36 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 2: FOCUS ISSUE: NUMBER SENSE || Number Sense and Nonsense

Research into Practice

Number Sense and Nonsense

Common sense has many aspects and is developed by a variety of expe- riences in and out of school. Number sense is one aspect of common sense that we rightly expect schooling to improve. But does it? Given a prob- lem, do students pay any attention to the meaning of the numbers in the data or in the solution they obtain? In a study devoted to estimation and reasonableness of results, we found that sixth- and seventh-grade students either do not have a reasonably devel- oped number sense or, if they have it, do not apply it to simple tasks in a mathematical context.

This ς 'number nonsense" occurred in purely numerical tasks as well as in tasks with a contextual setting. The former kinds of tasks are abstract and confined to the classroom, but the latter tasks refer to real situations that should naturally encourage the appli- cation of number sense. Consider this classic problem:

The height of a 10-year-old boy is 1.5 meters. What do you think his height will be when he is twenty?

Edited by Judith Sowder and Larry Sowder

San Diego State University San Diego, CA 92182 Prepared by Zvia Markovits,

Rina Hershkowitz, and Maxim Bruckheimer

Weizmann Institute of Science Rehovot, Israel

February 1989

About a third of the students in our study answered 3 meters! These stu- dents apparently invoked a standard algorithm without paying any atten- tion to its relevance or to the meaning of the number obtained. One suspects that their mathematical experience has been confined almost exclusively to applying one standard algorithm at any one time and obtaining one an- swer. Neither the choice of algorithm nor the meaning of the answer was explicitly questioned. In other words, number sense has not been explicitly and meaningfully developed.

We investigated the performance of sixth and seventh graders on a variety of tasks involving estimation and rea- sonableness of results, both of which invoke number sense. We also exam- ined the effect of a short unit of activ- ities on these two topics. Although the unit was written specifically for these grades, most of the tasks could be given to students in lower grades with some adjustment where necessary.

Number Sense in Purely Numerical Tasks Students were asked to locate the decimal point in the answers given in these two exercises:

3.5 5.5 x 4.5 x 3.2 1575 176

Students with number sense would find these exercises easy, even if they did not know the standard algorithm. But we found that students counted the digits after the decimal point, ob- taining 1.76 as the second answer. It never crossed their minds to consider

whether the answer made sense; ap- parently they think that algorithms never lie.

Experiences with whole numbers often carry over into work with frac- tions and decimals and dominate stu- dents' thinking to the extent that num- ber sense never appears to play a role in obtaining an answer. For example, consider another exercise we used:

The result of 426.5 - 0.469 is ...

a) less than 426.5 b) equal to 426.5

c) greater than 426.5

Explain your answer.

The correct answer, (c), was given by 40 percent of the 328 students. The explanation for most wrong answers was that "when divided, a number becomes smaller." Slightly better, but similar, results were obtained for mul- tiplication.

To improve number sense, students throughout their schooling should be given many varied tasks that require little, if any, computation. Here are some examples of tasks that increase number sense:

1 . Which is greater -

13 + 11 or 9 + 8? 46 - 19 or 46 - 17?

i + | or Ü? 0.0358 or 0.0016 + 0.393?

Many students will do exact calcu- lations before answering, but the teacher can use various strategies to wean them from this practice, for instance, by giving the ques- tions orally and imposing a time limit.

53

4UMBER SENSE

This content downloaded from 195.34.78.61 on Mon, 16 Jun 2014 06:13:36 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 3: FOCUS ISSUE: NUMBER SENSE || Number Sense and Nonsense

2. The result of 46 x 91 will be in the-

a) hundreds b) thousands c) ten thousands. Is 46 x 91 more or less than 5000? More or less than 3600?

3. If you could round only one of the numbers in the problem 32 x 83, rounding which factor would pro- duce a product closest to the exact answer?

4. a) 127 x D ends in 3. What can you say about the missing whole number? If you also know that the prod- uct is less than 4000, what can you then say about it? If the product is also greater than 3000, what is the missing number?

b) The sum of five two-digit num- bers is less than 100. For each of the following statements de- cide whether it is necessarily true, necessarily false, or possi- bly true. Explain your answers. (1) Each number is less than

20. (2) One number is greater than

60. (3) Four numbers are greater

than 20, and one is less than 20.

(4) If two numbers are less than 20, at least one is greater than 20.

(5) If all five numbers are dif- ferent, then their sum is greater than or equal to 60.

5. Without computing, explain why each of the following is incorrect:

a) 310 b) 119 520 46 630 137 150 940

+ 470 + 300 2081 602

c) 27 x 3 = 621 d) 36 - 0.5 = 18 e) 0.46

1.93 2.46 0.99

+ 0.87 672

Number Sense in Context Another question we used in the study was the following:

A bath has two outlets. The first alone empties the bath in 10 min- utes, and the second alone in 4 minutes. Both were opened at the same time. In how many minutes will the bath be empty? Choose the most appropriate an- swer from among the following and explain your answer.

a) 40 minutes b) 14 minutes c) less than 14 d) 7 minutes

but more than 1 f) A minutes e) 6 minutes h) about Vi g) about 3 minute

minutes

The students had not been taught to solve such problems using algebra, but number sense is all that is neces- sary: if one outlet empties the bath in four minutes, then the two outlets will empty the bath in less than four min- utes but not in as little as half a minute.

Once again we found that students tended to apply an algorithm and op- erate on the numbers in the question without thinking. They added, multi- plied, computed the mean, and so on. Even if we also allow one-half minute as a reasonable option, since it is less than four minutes, only 36 percent of the students who had not received the special instruction gave a reasonable answer. After receiving the brief spe- cial instruction 85 percent of the stu- dents gave a reasonable answer to a question in a similar context.

In this task, the algorithm - even if known - is irrelevant, but there is nothing inherently ridiculous in the answers obtained by incorrect algo- rithms. To conclude that an answer is incorrect, the student must relate it to the data in the question. This ability demands a more sophisticated num- ber sense than that required by the height problem at the beginning of this article - three meters is ridiculous for a man's height.

The general problem of learning to judge the reasonableness of a result in a given context involves number

sense. Although students may be en- couraged to check their answers, they do so far too often only in purely mathematical situations, where a standard algorithm has been applied, and they check only whether that al- gorithm has been performed correct- ly. The numerical work itself has no real-life context. This situation leads students to divorce the mathematical answer completely from its implica- tion in the context; the answer, they think, is mathematically correct even though it makes no sense in the con- text. This phenomenon is again dem- onstrated by the following task from our study:

A bookstore delivered 188 books to 6 libraries. How many books do you think each library received?

This question cannot be answered algorithmically ; nevertheless , many students did use the division algo- rithm. Fractional answers (31 Уз books, 31.3 books, etc.) were given by 34 percent of the students before doing our instructional activities, but only 3 percent gave such answers af- ter the instruction. When we asked whether a man can be three meters high or whether a library can receive thirty-one and one-third books, the answer was definitely "no!" But if these answers are given by the algo- rithms, then students assume that they are correct!

We also found that students know little of various everyday measure- ments that can serve as reference data for number sense in contextual prob- lems, for example, the speed of a car, the height of an eight-story building, or the number of liters of water in a bucket. When we asked for the speed of a plane flying from London to New York, a reasonable answer (we al- lowed anything from 500 to 1500 km/h) was given by 29 percent of the students who had no instruction. We also found that if we asked for the measures of various objects of the same kind (e.g., the width of a car, a truck, a main road, etc.), then often the answers were internally inconsis- tent (e.g., the width of car would be given as greater than that of a road). Therefore, we recommend that stu- dents first be asked to order the given

54 Arithmetic Teacher

This content downloaded from 195.34.78.61 on Mon, 16 Jun 2014 06:13:36 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 4: FOCUS ISSUE: NUMBER SENSE || Number Sense and Nonsense

objects qualitatively and only then be asked to assign individual measures to them. For example, order the follow- ing from the heaviest to the lightest and estimate the weight of each: car, bicycle, truck, dog, chair, elephant, kitten. Correct responses approxi- mately doubled after our instruction, indicating that similar instruction needs to be extended or improved in the classroom. We feel that this sort of number sense is not easy to acquire and probably needs to be developed gradually over a number of years of schooling.

Conclusion Number sense is not a single curricu- lar topic but has many aspects. The development of number sense is not something that occurs naturally for most students in the regular curricu- lum, as we have seen in this study. Evidence clearly suggests, however, that number sense can be developed with appropriate activities. The short instruction we gave to sixth and sev- enth graders improved their number sense, in some aspects very signifi- cantly. They also learned to look at all the answers they obtained in a dif- ferent way: the numbers in the answer had a meaning that needed to be ex- amined critically.

We suggest that activities of the types described here should be inte- grated into the curriculum, even be- fore sixth and seventh grades. We should begin in the first grade with appropriate tasks that call on number sense and give the children a less mechanical view of mathematics, w

Mathematics teachers from across the tors. Access MIX by a local call or an country and around the world are linking 800 number 24 hours a day, 365 days a their classrooms and sharing experienc- year. NCTM members receive $80 off es. Teachers, mathematics specialists, MIX'S annual subscription fee and have administrators, and nationally known ed- complete access to MIX services, ucation experts come together on MATHLINK, the electronic communi- , . cation network developed especially for q g mathematics teachers by the National η m Council of Teachers of Mathematics and l^||p*1 MceraW·1"

JOINMATHUNK! MATHLINK offers real-world activi- The On4Jne Solution for ties, electronic mail, and computer con- MattiemecsTeachers ferencing that focus on mathematics is- sues. You can exchange teaching tips, For more information, NCTM's low-cost lesson plans, and information about the pricing schedule, and a free copy of the latest mathematics education materials 8-page "Telecommunications Planning and involve students in tetecommunfca- Guide for Education," contact: tion projects CTB/McGraw-Hill

2500 Garden Road MATHLINK is available through MIX, Monterey, CA 93940 the McGraw-Hill Information Exchange (800) 538-9547 network dedicated exclusively to educa- (408) 649-7972

The Editorial Panel of the Arithmetic Teacher

would like to review manuscripts that integrate

mathematics-related software into ongoing classroom instruction. See the masthead* page for details regarding

submission.

! N^^^^ PLEASE HELP US EVALUATE THIS ISSUE ̂'/*^g I Your answers to the following questions and your comments will help us I in our planning: I What did you like best about this issue?

I What did you like least about this issue?

I Overall rating (please circle one): 1<+2^3<^4«->5«-»6**7<->8<-*9<->10

[ poor excellent

I What topics would you like to see addressed in future issues?

I Other comments:

I Indicate your professional position (teacher, grade level; teacher educa-

j tor; curriculum specialist; etc.):

| (Please include your name and address if you would like to receive I information on becoming a referee for the Arithmetic Teacher.)

[ Mail this completed questionnaire to NCTM ^^~-^^ 1906 Association Drive

^ΖΖΖΓ^Τ Reston, VA 22091 ^^^^^^ Ji^^ A/2/89

February 1989 55

This content downloaded from 195.34.78.61 on Mon, 16 Jun 2014 06:13:36 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions