feeley thesis

Upload: christian-darylle-bigay

Post on 02-Jun-2018

219 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    1/130

    IDENTIFYING STUDENT CONCEPTS OF GRAVITY

    By

    Roger Eastman Feeley

    B.S. University of Maine, 1989

    A THESIS

    Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the

    Requirements for the Degree of

    Master of Science in Teaching

    The Graduate School

    The University of Maine

    May 2007

    Advisory Committee:

    John R. Thompson, Assistant Professor of Physics, Cooperating Assistant Professor

    of Education, Advisor

    Michael C. Wittmann, Assistant Professor of Physics: Cooperating Assistant

    Professor of Education

    Herman Weller, Associate Professor of Science Education

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    2/130

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    3/130

    LIBRARY RIGHTS STATEMENT

    In presenting this thesis in partial fulfillment of the requirements for an advanced

    degree at The University of Maine, I agree that the Library shall make it freely

    available for inspection. I further agree that permission for fair use copying of this

    thesis for scholarly purposes may be granted by the Librarian. It is understood that any

    copying or publication of this thesis for financial gain shall not be allowed without my

    written permission.

    Signature:

    Date:

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    4/130

    IDENTIFYING STUDENT CONCEPTS OF GRAVITY

    By Roger E Feeley

    Thesis Advisor: Dr. John R. Thompson

    An Abstract of the Thesis Presented

    in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for theDegree of Master of Science in Teaching

    May, 2007

    This paper discusses a survey developed to investigate student concepts of "gravity"

    among AST 109 astronomy students and pre-service K-12 teachers. Survey questions

    were developed or modified from those in the literature [Berg 1991, Dostal 2005].

    Students were questioned on their reasoning about the behavior of objects on the

    surface of a planetary body (e.g., the Earth or the moon) and the causes of this

    behavior. Results of the survey successfully elicited student alternate conceptions with

    various aspects of gravity. These misconceptions include the tendency to attribute

    gravity to the presence of an atmosphere, and the belief that a threshold amount of

    gravity, mass, or weight is necessary for free-fall to occur.

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    5/130

    iii

    ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

    Had I been told a decade ago that I would be taking this path, I would have thought

    them crazy and asked them what they were smoking . . .

    First of all, I would like to thank John Thompson, my advisor, for his role in all this.

    He helped keep me on track, despite my bouts of cluelessness and my own best efforts

    to impede my progress. Id also like to thank Michael Wittman, for his encouragement

    throughout this process. I also feel indebted to the physics department, for being

    supportive of students as a whole.

    I am blessed to have the best mom and dad on the planet. They are the epitome of

    supportive parents. As for my siblings, thank you Amey, for cracking the whip, as well

    as your mastery of the English language. Thank you Arthur and Libby for giving me a

    place to put my thoughts into words. And thank you Martha and Tom for giving me

    encouragement to get through this. Oh yeah, and thank you all for being guinea pigs

    HA HA.

    Finally, I would like to thank Art Bell and George Noory of Coast to Coast AM,

    who, with their quest for truth, guarantee me job security.

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    6/130

    iv

    TABLE OF CONTENTS

    ACKNOWLEDGMENTS .......................................................................................... iii

    LIST OF TABLES.................................................................................................... vii

    LIST OF FIGURES..................................................................................................... x

    1 INTRODUCTION.............................................................................................. 1

    1.1 The Gravity of the Situation......................................................................... 1

    1.2 Scope of Thesis............................................................................................ 2

    2 THE SEARCH FOR TRUTH ............................................................................. 3

    2.1 Literature Review ........................................................................................ 3

    2.2 Interviews .................................................................................................... 8

    3 THE GRAVITY SURVEY............................................................................... 11

    3.1 Question 1 The Mother of all Questions .................................................. 11

    3.2 Question 2 The Mother Question Follow-Up........................................... 13

    3.3 Moon Base Alpha Question 10................................................................ 15

    3.4 Multiple-Choice Questions 3 5................................................................ 17

    3.4.1 Question 3 Gravity Up and Away from the Earth.............................. 17

    3.4.2 Question 4 Galilean Gravity ............................................................. 20

    3.4.3 Question 5 A Balloon on the Moon................................................... 22

    3.5 Venus as a Context for Comparison ........................................................... 24

    3.5.1 Questions 6 & 7 Your Weight and Mass on Venus ........................... 27

    3.5.2 Question 8 Venus Gravitational Force ............................................. 28

    3.5.3 Question 9 Free-fall on Venus .......................................................... 29

    3.6 The Likert-scale Questions......................................................................... 31

    3.6.1 Parameters Affecting Gravity .............................................................. 33

    3.6.2 The Existence of Gravity on the Moon ................................................ 34

    3.6.3 Gravitys Effect on Objects ................................................................. 35

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    7/130

    v

    4 RESULTS AND DISCUSSION........................................................................ 37

    4.1 Initial Categorization ................................................................................. 37

    4.2 The Mother of All Questions and initial groupings..................................... 38

    4.3 Free-Responses and Student Reasoning ..................................................... 40

    4.3.1 Student Reasoning for Question 1 Why a Pen Floats......................... 40

    4.3.2 Question 1 Free-Response ................................................................... 42

    4.3.3 Student Reasoning for Question 2 Why an Astronaut Falls ............... 46

    4.3.4 Question 2 Student Free-Response ...................................................... 48

    4.3.5 Question 1-2 The Pen/Astronaut Consistency Check ........................ 52

    4.3.6 Question 10 Free-Responses Why the Pen Falls in the Dome............ 54

    4.4 Multiple-choice results............................................................................... 58

    4.4.1 Question 3 Up and Away from Earth................................................ 58

    4.4.2 Question 4 Galilean Gravity ............................................................. 60

    4.4.3 Question 5 A Balloon on the Moon................................................... 63

    4.4.4 Question 6 Your Weight on Venus ................................................... 67

    4.4.5 Question 7 Your Mass on Venus....................................................... 68

    4.4.6 Question 8 The Gravitational Force of Venus ................................... 69

    4.4.7 Question 9 A Pens Free-fall on Venus ............................................. 70

    4.5 Likert-scale Questions................................................................................ 70

    4.5.1 Questions 11 15: Parameters Affecting Gravity ............................... 71

    4.5.1.1 Question 11 A Planets Atmosphere ............................................. 71

    4.5.1.2 Question 12 A Planets Rotation................................................... 71

    4.5.1.3 Question 13 A Planets Size ......................................................... 72

    4.5.1.4 Question 14 A Planets Mass........................................................ 72

    4.5.1.5 Question 15 A Planets Distance from the Sun ............................. 73

    4.5.2 Questions 17 19: Gravity on the Moon and Outer Space.................. 74

    4.5.2.1 Question 17 Orbital Zero Gravity ................................................. 74

    4.5.2.2 Question 18 Gravity in Outer Space.............................................. 75

    4.5.2.3 Question 19 Gravity on the Moon................................................. 76

    4.5.3 Questions 16 & 20-25: Gravitys Effect on Objects ............................ 77

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    8/130

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    9/130

    vii

    LIST OF TABLES

    Table 2.1-1: Astronaut Question: Berg & Brouwer (1991) with Dostal

    (2005).........................................................................................................8

    Table 4.1-1: Mean "score" of class groups....................................................................37

    Table 4.1-2: Independent sample t-test for equality of means (p) ...................................38

    Table 4.2-1: Mother of All Questions (Question 1) ........................................................39

    Table 4.2-2: Independent sample t-test for equality of means (p) ..................................40

    Table 4.3-1: Question 1 multiple-choice responses and student reasoning

    (Float = A+B+C)...................................................................................41

    Table 4.3-2: Mean "score" of student response groups.................................................. 41

    Table 4.3-3: Independent sample t-test for equality of means (p) ..................................42

    Table 4.3-4: Mother Question multiple-choice response distribution with

    student reasoning included........................................................................45

    Table 4.3-5: Question 1 student reasoning to Question 20 (Q20 correct

    response is False)......................................................................................46

    Table 4.3-6: Question 1 multiple-choice responses to Question 2 student

    reasoning (Floaters = A+B+C) ..............................................................47

    Table 4.3-7: Mean "score" of Question 2 student reasoning groups............................... 48

    Table 4.3-8: Independent sample t-test for equality of means (p) for

    different reasoning groups in Question 2................................................... 48

    Table 4.3-9: Question 2 student reasoning to Question 21 (Q21 correct

    response is False)......................................................................................51

    Table 4.3-10: Question 1 student reasoning to Question 2 student reasoning................. 52

    Table 4.4-1: Question 3 ................................................................................................59

    Table 4.4-2: Question 4 ................................................................................................61

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    10/130

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    11/130

    ix

    Table 4.5-13: Question 22 ............................................................................................79

    Table 4.5-14: Question 24 ............................................................................................80

    Table 4.5-15: Question 25 ............................................................................................81

    Table 5.1-1: Truth Table Twenty-two plus Two............................................................ 83

    Table 5.1-2: Truth Table Twenty-two plus Three.......................................................... 85

    Table 5.1-3: Truth Table Thirty-two plus Three (False is correct)................................. 86

    Table 5.1-4: Truth Table Thirty-three plus Three (False is correct) ...............................87

    Table 6.2-1: Gravity Survey and All-in-one Survey ranking comparison ...................... 90

    Table 7.1-1: Question 4 AST classes to multiple-choice responses ...............................93

    Table 7.1-2: Question 4 Pearson Chi-squared analysis of AST classes .......................... 94

    Table 7.1-3: Question 1 AST classes to multiple-choice responses ...............................95

    Table 7.1-4: Question 1 Pearson Chi-squared multiple-choice responses ...................... 95

    Table 7.1-5: AST classes to Question 1 student reasoning ............................................96

    Table 7.1-6: Question 1 constructed student responses Pearson Chi-squared

    analysis.....................................................................................................96

    Table 7.1-7: Question 5 AST classes to multiple-choice responses ...............................97

    Table 7.1-8: Pearson Chi-squared analysis of Question 5 multiple-choice

    responses..................................................................................................98

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    12/130

    x

    LIST OF FIGURES

    Figure 1: Gravity is arbitrary ........................................................................................99

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    13/130

    1

    1 INTRODUCTION

    Chapter 1

    INTRODUCTION

    1.1 The Gravity of the Situation

    This is an age of unparalleled intellectual and scientific advancement. Despite

    the richness and ready availability of this information, much of the general public is not

    engaged in or utilizing this knowledge. Existing within a physical world, a disconnect

    exists between the physical laws and peoples perceptions of those laws knowing

    how they work, or even that they exist.

    Being creative creatures, students come to the table of scientific discussion with

    preconceived ideas, based on their own experience what they have seen, heard, lived

    a constructivist attitude that does not allow them to easily embrace new knowledge

    imparted to them by the teacher of scientific law. Students are much more willing to

    entertain the impossible and claim it as truth. An outcome of this attitude is the

    popularity of Coast to Coast AM. Here listeners are exposed to alternate science

    theories from anti-gravityto zero point energy. Not to worry though, Newtonian

    physics and thermodynamics can be granted equal time.

    If that isnt enough to provoke a response, a motivational story may help. A

    student detailed a story in which a philosophy teaching assistant tried to explain that,

    while a pen always falls when you drop it on Earth, it would just float away if you let

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    14/130

    2

    go of it on the Moon. Outside of class, the student took a random survey of other

    college students. The respondents who answered that the pen would not fall on the

    moon, were also asked why the astronauts didnt float off the moon. A significant

    number of students responded that the astronauts wore heavy boots. (Rapaport, 1995)

    1.2 Scope of Thesis

    This research is to identify the prevalent models held by students. Given the

    length of the survey, the magnitude of data, and that this is a masters thesis, much of

    the surveys potential will not be realized in this paper. This thesis will look at the data

    and try to identify general concepts. An exhaustive analysis of consistency will not

    take place, but a general scheme of student reasoning will be noted. The chapter

    breakdown is as follows: Chapter 1 will introduce the situation. Chapter 2 includes

    the pre-survey research, such as the literature search, and the interviews. The design of

    the gravity survey is in Chapter 3. The results are given in Chapter 4, along with

    general comments concerning each question. Further analysis on selected portions of

    the survey are included in Chapter 5. Chapter 6 talks about the inclusion of another

    survey to help determine student reasoning. Chapter 7 discusses the effect of a

    curriculum change on student responses. Chapter 8 contains the conclusions.

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    15/130

    3

    2 THE SEARCH FOR TRUTH

    Chapter 2

    THE SEARCH FOR TRUTH

    2.1 Literature Review

    National Science Education Standards (National Research Council, 1996)

    suggests that the concept of gravity should be introduced in the fifth grade. By the

    eighth grade, students should understand gravitys role in tides and planetary motion,

    as well as holding people to the Earths surface (Adams, 2000). The science and

    technology standards of the State of Maine Learning Results (Maine Department of

    Education, 1997) requires that Maine high school students understand current theories

    of gravitational force.

    There is relatively little published research on student concepts of gravity.

    Most appear to be written by educators, rather than astronomers or physicists. Of the

    nearly two dozen papers published, over half were printed prior to 1990, and over three

    fourths before 2000. Since 2000, all of the papers addressing student concepts of

    gravity have dealt with college students. However, nearly all of the older studies have

    usually focused on elementary, middle and secondary school students rather than

    college students. These older studies tended to identify various student concepts but

    did not investigate how prevalent each concept was. A number of the papers were

    continuations of previous research.

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    16/130

    4

    Since most of the papers were focused on young children, it was not

    immediately known if the concepts of gravity held by children would be similar to

    those held by older students. Stepans, Beiswenger, and Dyche, (1970) indicate that

    misconceptions held by older students are just sophisticated versions of the earlier

    alternate conceptions. A review of the Noce, Torosantucci, and Vicentini (1988) paper

    indicated that childrens concepts of gravity did not appear to be significantly different

    from those of adults. In this paper 264 middle and secondary school students, 64 first

    year university Biology students, and 74 adults (including 53 elementary school

    teachers) were asked to predict what would happen if an astronaut on the moon lost a

    spanner he held in his hand. Of the 264 students, 223 (84%) held alternate

    conceptions, compared to 42 (66%) of the Biology students and 57 (77%) of the adults.

    Childrens ideas of gravity are rooted in how they perceive the world. There

    have been a number of studies investigating what they think about the Earth.

    Nussbaum and Novak (1976) interviewed second graders (n = 26) to develop a model

    of the childs version of Earth. The result was a scheme of five notions, starting with

    the most egocentric view: a flat Earth and no concept of space outside the atmosphere.

    The notions gradually progress toward a more conceptual view and become more

    sophisticated. Notion two is that the Earth is a ball composed of two hemispheres in

    space, the lower part solid and the upper part the sky, and we live on the flat part inside

    the ball. The third notion is that the Earth is a ball in space, and we live on the top of

    the ball. Notion four: the Earth is a ball where people live all around the ball, and

    objects either fall to the surface of the Earth or toward the bottom of the ball. The last

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    17/130

    5

    notion, five, is the notion of a round Earth where objects fall toward the center of the

    Earth. This work was followed up by Sneider and Pulos (1983), and by Nussbaum and

    Sharoni-Dagan (1983) with students at more advanced grade levels. All researchers

    found evidence supporting a progression of conceptual development towards the more

    scientific view as age and grade level advanced. The development process started with

    a large majority of the students using the most egocentric model (notion one) and

    gradually changed to the scientifically compatible notion five.

    Gravity is not a readily and well understood concept. Smith and Treagust

    (1988) interviewed 24 tenth grade students and tested 113 other students with paper

    and pencil. The 4 misunderstandings that arose were: a planets gravity is related to its

    distance from the Sun, the Suns gravity influences the gravity of the planets that orbit

    it, a planets rotation affects its gravity zero or slow rotating planets have less gravity

    than fast rotating planets, and the rotation of a planet is dependent on its position with

    respect to the Sun or to its size.

    In addition to the possible solar and rotational influences, many students

    believe that air affects gravity. (Bar, Zinn, & Goldmuntz, 1994; Minstrell, 1982; Noce

    et al (1988); Philips, 1991; Ruggiero, Cartelli, Dupr, & Vicentini-Missoni, 1985)

    Some students believe the force of gravity needs air to act as a conducting medium

    (Bar, Zinn, & Rubin, 1997). Because of this many students believe that there is no

    gravity in space or on the moon (Ameh, 1987; Berg & Brouwer, 1991; Dostal, 2005;

    Watts & Zylbersztajn, 1981).

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    18/130

    6

    Bar et al (1994) found through interviews with 400 children between ages 4 and

    13 that many do not consider weight and the force of gravity as being the same thing.

    This was also seen in other literature (Ameh, 1987; Bar et al, 1997; Noce et al, 1988;

    Watts & Zylbersztajn, 1981). Gravity is associated with free fall. Some students

    believe that an objects weight increases with height (Ameh, 1987; Bar et al, 1994,

    1997), while others believe that an objects weight decreases with height (Chandler,

    1991). Gunstone and White (1981) asked 458 university students to predict the

    movement of a spring scale needle when the scale, holding a bucket of sand, is moved

    from the classroom to the top of Mt. Everest. Although 136 (29%) students marked a

    correct response, only 56 (12%) gave a correct reason. Wrong student reasoning

    included, Gravitation attraction is constant everywhere, and Weight = mg and is

    independent of height.

    Over all age levels and education the majority of people do not use Newtons

    law of gravitation. (Baxter, 1989; Noce et al, 1988; Watts & Zylbersztajn, 1981) This

    use of common sense thinking put students at a disadvantage. Roger Osborne (1984)

    classifies childrens models of motion bygut dynamics, lay dynamicsandphysicists

    dynamics. Gut dynamics is intuitive, spontaneous, non-verbal and allows children to

    cope with common occurrences around them. Examples include heavy things fall

    faster and things need a push to get them going. (This idea is similar to diSessas

    phenomenological primitives (diSessa, 1993).) Lay dynamics is based on form and

    content of the language the child speaks and the images conveyed by those they are in

    contact with and the media and the books they read. Examples of lay dynamics:

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    19/130

    7

    astronauts are weightless in the space shuttle and if there is no force there is no

    motion. Physicists dynamics is the counterintuitive world of physics texts,

    experiments and problems students solve in class. When learning new concepts, many

    students retaingutand layphysics. As Osborne express it,

    Gut dynamicsenables one to play hockey, lay dynamicsone totalk about Star Wars, whilephysicists dynamicsenables one

    to do physics assignments.

    The creators of the Force Concept Inventory (Hestenes, Wells, & Swackhamer,

    (1992) add to this by noting that 1) Common sense beliefs about motion and force are

    generally incompatible with Newtonian concepts, 2) Conventional physics instruction

    produces little change in these beliefs, and 3) the result is instructor independent of the

    instruction as well as the mode of instruction.

    Six papers in the reviewed literature contained a question involving an

    astronaut dropping something on the moon. Four publications involved dropping a

    spanner or wrench (Berg & Brouwer, 1991; Noce et al, 1988; Ruggiero et al, 1985;

    Watts & Zylbersztajn, 1981), and 2 involved dropping a pen (Dostal & Meltzer, 2000;

    Dostal, 2005). It was never explained why a wrench and pen were chosen for each. A

    comparison of the Berg and Brouwer (1991) with the Dostal (2005) results is shown in

    Table 2.1-1. The Berg and Brouwer A group is ninth grade students planning to take

    physics, the B group is all ninth grade students. These ninth graders were from

    Edmonton, Alberta. The Dostal A groups are algebra-based physics classes and the C

    groups are calculus-based physics classes, both at Iowa State University. The results of

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    20/130

    8

    the other papers were not able to be tabulated for comparison. As expected, the

    calculus-based physics students appear to have a better understanding of gravity. What

    may not be expected is the similarity of the ninth graders with the algebra-based

    physics students. This could indicate that both groups of students may not have a solid

    understanding of gravity, and that the college students are still struggling with

    preconceived notions of gravity they acquired prior to high school.

    Student

    Response

    Berg and

    Brouwer

    A

    Berg and

    Brouwer

    B

    Dostal

    A-1

    Dostal

    A-2

    Dostal

    A-3

    Dostal

    C-1

    Dostal

    C-2

    Dostal

    C-3

    Dostal

    C-4

    Toward

    moons

    surface

    37% 29% 40% 42% 38% 73% 66% 68% 75%

    Away from

    astronaut13% 13%

    Away from

    the moon18% 22% 29% 22% 19% 15% 12% 11% 11%

    Floats (no

    force)30% 30% 31% 34% 38% 10% 19% 14% 12%

    Toward the

    astronaut2% 1%

    Other

    responses3% 5% 0% 2% 5% 2% 3% 7% 1%

    Totalresponses

    (n = 183) (n = 315) (n = 48) (n = 303) (n = 21) (n = 40) (n = 534) (n = 302) (n = 414)

    Table 2.1-1: Astronaut Question: Berg & Brouwer (1991) with Dostal (2005)

    2.2 Interviews

    One of the more invaluable tools in the design process of the gravity survey

    was the gathering of information by interviews (Redish 1999). It was here where the

    so-called air-gravity model and threshold models were first formulated. The

    interviews for this survey were informal events with no formal protocols or recording

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    21/130

    9

    devices. The interviewees consisted of nearly 45 adult parents, grandparents, and

    children. Ages were between 6 and 81. Interviews were granted by verbal permission,

    and took place inside privately owned homes.

    All interviews began with the question, Suppose you were standing on the

    moon holding this pen. What would happen if you were to let go of the pen? After

    receiving a sufficient answer from the interviewee, the follow-up question would be

    posed, Do you know about when the astronauts walked on the moon? Why didnt the

    astronauts float off the surface?

    Gleaning information from some of the children was at times difficult. It was

    sometimes hard to get definite answers, even after repeating 4 or 5 times. The adults

    were a bit easier to interview, but as a rule did not appear to possess a greater

    knowledge level.

    At the interviews end, of some of the interviewees were asked if they knew

    what caused the tides. It was hoped to get them to consider that the moon must have

    had some kind of gravitational effect. Most knew that the moon had something to do

    with the tides, but really didnt know what it was. At least two of the younger

    interviewees believed that the wind caused the tides.

    At the end of the interview process, two standard gravitational models were

    noted. The first is what was called thegravity-air or air-gravitymodel. The pen

    would float because the moon has no air. For some unknown reason air causes gravity,

    and since the moon has no air, there is no gravity. This model was common

    throughout the literature.

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    22/130

    10

    The second, initially labeled the gravity-weightmodel, went on to become the

    thresholdmodel. Perhaps the best example of this model came from a middle-school

    boy. He initially claimed that the pen would float because of no gravity on the moon.

    He was then asked what gravity was and his response was that it was the force that kept

    us on the earth. When asked why there wasnt any gravity on the moon, he considered

    for a moment and then recanted. He admitted that there was sort of gravity on the

    moon, but not as much as the earth. He really didnt know what gravity was, but knew

    it depended on the planet. Besides, that wouldnt change his answer anyway. The pen

    weighs so little on the earth that it would weigh next to nothing on the moon.

    Consequently, it would not be heavy enough to fall, and it would float upward.

    Adults and children both held similar views. At least one parent spoke of her

    knowledge that she knew that the astronauts put lead in their boots before they

    ventured out onto the lunar surface. At least two other adults spoke of a link of gravity

    to the speed of planetary rotation. The moon had less gravity because it didnt spin as

    fast.

    All-in-all, the interview process proved extremely useful in discovering and

    verifying the basic models students utilize in their dealings with gravity. A number of

    survey questions were the direct result of these interviews.

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    23/130

    11

    3 THE GRAVITY SURVEY

    Chapter 3

    THE GRAVITY SURVEY

    Since its genesis, the gravity survey has undergone a number of major revisions

    as well as minor cosmetic changes. Its initial form was developed through the

    utilization of interviews with the general populace as well as an extensive literature

    search of pertinent educational and scientific journals.

    3.1 Question 1 The Mother of all Questions

    The mother of all the questions (Question 1) that this survey was designed

    around concerned the prediction of how a small, light object would behave on the

    moon. It was multiple choice with an additional free-response portion, and was similar

    to a question found in the literature (Berg & Brouwer, 1991; Dostal, 2005; Noce et al,

    1988; Ruggiero et al, 1985; Watts & Zylbersztajn, 1981). The question read, Suppose

    you were standing on the moon holding a pen. If you were to let go of the pen, what

    direction will it move? The directional choices available to the student were, float

    upward,float around, staying about the same height, and levitate, but also move away

    horizontally, and fall toward the lunar surface. A fifth option was other/none of the

    above.

    The question was designed as a thought question, and not something that the

    students were expected to experience for themselves. The question was written in

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    24/130

    12

    second person narration to match the original interviews. It was felt that this

    perspective might aid in provoking a more descriptive and honest answer as to what the

    students believe, since they were being asked to imagine themselves experiencing the

    event. The semantics of this question was selected to avoid biasing the student toward

    any particular response. The phrase let go of was used instead of drop because it

    was felt that when something is dropped, it automatically infers that the object would

    move down since the direction of a dropped item on the earth is in the down direction.

    In the original version of the question, the pen was a Fisher Space Pen, rather

    than any generic pen. The pen was identified as a space pen to add realism to the

    question why would an astronaut notbring a space pen? After the initial test of the

    survey, there were indications that one or two students had taken issue with the fact

    that it was a space pen. For this reason the product endorsement was removed from

    this question.

    The given choices of motion were selected from the interviews and literature

    search. From the interviews, it was noted that the direction of motion of the pen was

    always important. Float upward, float around,and fall were the most common

    responses. Prior to the first version of the survey, the original directional choices for

    this question were to be, float up/away, float sideways, float in no specific direction,

    and fall. The 3 non-falling directional choices were modified to,float upward,float

    around, staying about chest height, and levitate, but also move away horizontally, after

    consultation with MST and PER students and faculty. After the first version of the

    survey was analyzed, the word chestwas replaced with the words the same. The

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    25/130

    13

    question never specified as to the original height of the pen. If the pen originated at a

    height other than chest height, there may be no compelling reason for the pen to move

    to chest height. It was thought that this change would make the selection more general

    and leave the students with three non-falling options float up, float around, and float

    up (levitate) and out.

    In addition to their selection of a particular response, students were asked to

    explain their response. It was expected that this portion of the question would provide

    student reasoning for student responses. The responses could then be additionally

    categorized by their explanation. This free response was designed to help determine

    what gravity model they were using, and to make sure that the distracters were

    interpreted appropriately by the students (i.e., that they chose a particular distracter for

    the same reason that we put that distracter in the question in the first place).

    3.2 Question 2 The Mother Question Follow-Up

    Question 2 of the survey was designed as a follow-up to the first question. As a

    free-response question it was also designed to help determine what gravity model they

    were using, but also to evoke cognitive conflict. Between July 1969 and December

    1972 there were six moon landings. Twelve astronauts spent a total of over 80 hours

    exploring the lunar surface. Why didnt the astronauts float off the lunar surface?

    Compare this answer to how you answered the question above. When asked in

    interviews, this question originally was phrased to ask the students how they

    remembered that the astronauts were kept on the surface. When designing this survey

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    26/130

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    27/130

    15

    3.3 Moon Base Alpha Question 10

    Another follow-up question to the pen question was another free-response

    question. This appeared in the survey as Question 10. Question 10 posed the same

    question as the mother question, but with the addition of an atmosphere. From the

    literature (Bar et al, 1994, 1997; Minstrell, 1982; Noce et al, 1988; Ruggiero et al,

    1985) as well as interviews, the existence of gravity was tied to the presence of an

    atmosphere or some component of air. The interviews indicated that some students

    make a connection between the oxygen contained in space suits and gravity. This

    question was designed to be exactly like the first question except for the addition of air.

    This question was chosen to identify evidence of a gravity-air model of reasoning and

    read:

    It is the year 2156 and people are living on the moon insidegiant geodesic domes. These domes are filled with air so that

    people can live inside the dome without having to wear spacesuits. Suppose someone is standing inside one of the domes,

    with a pen in hand. What will happen to the pen if they let goof it?

    The year 21561 was chosen to place the scenario in the future, and add to its

    credibility, since there are no lunar bases at this time. It was felt that if the scenario

    were presented as a thought question, taking place in present time, some students

    would take issue with the fact that there were no lunar bases in existence and the

    question would become moot. The domes were used in this question to minimize the

    1The year 2156 was selected as I will be 200 years old that year.

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    28/130

    16

    environmental differences between this question and the original pen question. Since

    space suits are totally localized, it was felt that an environment had to be created that

    would encompass the immediate surrounding area, thereby removing the suits but also

    reducing the sense of being enclosed. Air was used to fill the domes rather than

    oxygen to make the atmosphere less exotic and more familiar. By standing inside a

    large dome, the person with the pen would essentially be in the open, with a large

    volume of air surrounding them. The surroundings would be much more likely to be

    perceived as being in an enclosed, climate-controlled, sports stadium or arena. If they

    were within a small, oxygen filled, lunar enclosure, they might be more likely to treat

    the environment as a space suit, or identify the situation with that of the International

    Space Station (ISS) or space shuttle.

    Question 10 went through a number of revisions. The original wording of the

    reason why the domes were filled with air was to simulate conditions on Earth. It was

    originally felt that this text was sufficient in describing the conditions inside the dome.

    However, it was found that the lack of descriptive text caused some students to jump to

    conclusions and evoke other assumptions. This wording led a substantial number of

    students (nearly a third) to believe that simulating earth conditions meant the

    environment was the same as the earth in all respects. The pen would fall inside the

    dome exactly as it would on earth. The text, to simulate conditions on Earth, was

    replaced with, so that people can live inside the dome without having to wear space

    suits. This change was expected to address this issue.

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    29/130

    17

    3.4 Multiple-Choice Questions 3 5

    Additional questions were added to the survey to substantiate different student

    conceptions of gravity as well as check for consistency. As mentioned above, the

    interviews and literature (Ameh, 1987; Berg & Brouwer, 1991; Dostal, 2005; Noce et

    al, 1988; Ruggiero et al, 1985; Watts & Zylbersztajn, 1981) indicated that one student

    explanation for why the pen would not fall is because there is no gravity on the moon.

    There were 2 prevalent models as to why the moon has no gravity. One was that the

    moon has no atmosphere (or air freely interchanged with atmosphere), and since air

    causes gravity the moon has no gravity. In the second model there was no gravity in

    outer space (outer space being outside the earths atmosphere), and since the moon is

    in outer space, the moon by definition has no gravity. These two models appear to

    be related since outer space can be categorized as a vacuum, but students appeared to

    distinguish between the two. The other reason why the pen would not fall on the moon

    was the pens lack of heaviness. This lack of heaviness appeared to be a

    combination of the pens low mass and the moons low (or lack of) gravity (Berg &

    Brouwer, 1991; Dostal, 2005). In addition to these basic models, the initial

    information also indicated the existence of perceptions that atmosphere, mass, distance,

    and rotation could each affect gravity.

    3.4.1

    Question 3 Gravity Up and Away from the Earth

    To check for student understanding of the distance dependence of the

    gravitational force between two bodies, as well for the air-gravity connection, the first

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    30/130

    18

    additional question Question 3 read, As you move up and away from the Earths

    surface, what happens to the Earths gravitational force on you? Again, the question

    was written in the first person. Moving up and away was meant to indicate

    displacement, rather than motion. The question was thought to be less hypothetical

    than a question based on the moon or other location, and that their responses would

    come more from their common sense and gut feeling, rather than their understanding of

    a hypothetical situation. Although non-science students appear to be more comfortable

    using the colloquial phrase, force of gravity (the original phrasing choice), the

    terminology gravitational forcewas selected for use in the final survey to make the

    statement more scientifically accurate. The literature (Baxter, 1989; Noce et al, 1988;

    Watts & Zylbersztajn, 1981) suggested that most students do not hold a standard

    definition for gravity, or if they are even aware of Newtons law of gravitation, which

    states that the force between two masses m1and m2separated by distance r(measured

    from each center of mass), has the magnitude, F=Gm

    1m

    2

    r2

    , where G is a universal

    constant. Given its colloquial nature, gravity as a word encompasses a number of

    different meanings. Some students appeared to identify gravity as a physical force,

    while others seemed to use it to describe an environment or state. Most students would

    agree that gravitational force and the force of gravity are the two different ways to say

    the same thing, but to be technically accurate, gravitational force was used in the final

    version of the survey.

    The formulation of responses to this question were based on the simplified

    logic that there are only two possible avenues to answer the question, either the

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    31/130

    19

    gravitational force will not change (Gunstone & White, 1981), or it will. And, if the

    force changes, it either gets bigger (Ameh, 1987; Bar et al, 1994, 1997) or smaller

    (Chandler, 1991). These three options were found in the literature.; So, the initial

    responses were selected to be, the gravitational force on you... ...increases,

    ...decreases, and ...stays the same. To test for the air-gravity connection prompted 3

    additional responses. These responses were duplicates of the initial responses, with the

    added stipulation that the gravitational force goes to zero once you leave the Earths

    atmosphere. The creation of these 3 responses required clarification of the initial

    ...decreasesresponse phrase by adding, but never goes to zero. By adding this text, it

    was felt that it reduced the ambiguity that this statement might suggest that the

    gravitational force reaches zero in outer space, and that students who supported an air-

    gravity model would be less likely to choose this response. Chandler (1991) indicates

    that most students are unaware that gravity reaches to infinity. An additional other

    (please explain)response was included in the event the student did not agree with one

    of the other 6 choices.

    The original form of Question 3 was formulated to identify multiple parameters

    that were perceived to affect gravity, including, atmosphere, mass, distance, and

    rotation. The question was structured in two parts, the first part (a) asking what

    happens to the force of gravity, and the second part (b), asking the student to indicate

    why they chose their answer, as well as explaining their reasoning. Part (a) of the

    question was the original version of the Question 3. The selection of responses for the

    this part included, the force of gravity increases, the force of gravity decreases, the

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    32/130

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    33/130

    21

    Following other questions of the survey, the question was again written in the second

    person, and posed as a question. Let gowas used as an action verb rather than dropped

    to avoid biasing the question. A feather was chosen as one of the objects because it

    was assumed to be light. On the earth, feathers are generally felt to fall through the air

    slowly (more slowly than a pen) and have been known to move upward on air currents,

    thereby cheating earth gravity. Lead was chosen since it is commonly associated

    with being dense, and has the general perception of being heavier than most other

    things. Even a small piece would be perceived to be heavy.

    Answers were designed on the basis of how a student might consider the

    motion of two objects let go at the same time. With the objects in free-fall, being in a

    vacuum, on the lunar surface, the student using correct reasoning would be expected to

    answer, both will fall at the same rate. If the student reasoned that the lead is heavy

    enough to be attracted to the moon, but the feather was not, the lead will fall slowly,

    but the feather will float rather than fall. If the student believed that the moon has no

    gravity, or that neither the lead nor the feather were heavy enough to fall, both will

    float. If the student were hung up on pre-Galilean concepts, both will fall slowly, but

    the lead will fall a little faster than the feathermight be selected since it would be

    similar to the free-fall of the lead and feather on Earth.

    In the original design of this question, droppedwas used as the action verb

    rather than let go. It was felt that students would not be biased with the use of this

    word since the act of dropping an object on the earth just involves letting go of the

    object. A review of the responses to this question confirmed that the use of droppeddid

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    34/130

    22

    not infer an initial direction. In spite of this, droppedwas replaced by let goto avoid

    any perceived biasing and to be consistent with other questions. Another point that

    was considered in the design of the original version was that some students might

    believe that, at the instant being let go, an object does not immediately fall. On the

    earth, this would account for only a split second, but on the moon, with its weaker

    gravity, this time delay could account for a significant time delay. Motion photography

    of the astronauts on the moon show them bouncing around, getting more air time

    from a lunar hop than with a comparable earth hop. This could have been perceived as

    a type of time delay on the moon it takes longer for an object to feel the effects of the

    lower gravity.

    The original answers to the question were similar in nature to the final version.

    A problem arose in the wording of some of the responses. The original wording of the

    responses, both will start to fall slowly, but then the lead will start to fall faster, and

    both would start to fall slowly, but then the feather will float. Were selected to include

    the potentially perceived time delay. A number of students took this phrasing to mean

    that the lead was somehow accelerated more than normal, or that the rate of fall for

    each object was somehow changing.

    3.4.3

    Question 5 A Balloon on the Moon

    Question 5, designed to search for indications of the gravity-weight model, was

    not as much a gravity question as it was a buoyancy question. The question read,

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    35/130

    23

    Imagine you are on the moon, holding a balloon filled with Helium. What will

    happen to the balloon if you let go of it? The wording followed the wording of the

    original pen question, again written in the first person, and posed as a thought question.

    This survey was designed for students outside the science field, and it would be a safe

    assumption that many non-science students are unfamiliar with the concept of

    buoyancy. Therefore, there is the likelihood that students would attribute the buoyant

    properties of lighter than air objects such as blimps and balloons to a gravitational

    force rather than a buoyant force. The dilemma may not be that the lighter than air

    object located the moon will not fall, but how fast will it rise. Some students believe,

    that on the moon, the reason why the pen floats is because it is too light to be affected

    by the moons lower gravity. When weighed on the earth, a balloon filled with helium

    would be much lighter than the pen because helium is lighter than the air, and when on

    the moon the balloon would be even lighter than it would on the earth. The balloon

    could rise faster since it is much lighter than it is on the earth, or it may be so light it is

    unaffected by the moons minimal gravity. Other student reasoning that may arise is

    that since the moon has less or no gravity, things dont move as fast as they do on the

    earth. (Ameh, 1987) Therefore, an object that is set to rise will rise more slowly than

    it would on earth.

    Again, answers to the question were selected as to how a student might reason

    this problem, with both direction and relative speeds considered. Responses available

    to the student included, the balloon will float up, moving more quickly than it would on

    the Earth, the balloon will float up, moving more slowly than it would on the Earth , the

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    36/130

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    37/130

    25

    expected that students with a gravity-air model would key on the extreme atmospheric

    pressure of Venus and answer accordingly, and the slow rotation of Venus had the

    potential to activate a planetary rotation model (Smith & Treagust, 1988; Treagust &

    Smith, 1989).

    Venus is only slightly smaller than Earth, with a mass 80% Earths mass, and a

    diameter 95% Earths diameter. Venus size and mass alone dictate that the

    gravitational pull on the surface would be about 90% of Earths. Venus rotational

    period is 243 Earth sidereal days, slightly longer than its 225 day period of revolution

    about the Sun, making its day 8% longer than its year. The atmospheric pressure at

    the surface of Venus is approximately 90 atmospheres (~91 bars or ~1300 psi). This

    indicates that Venus atmosphere would exert a substantial buoyant force on any object

    at or near the planets surface. This buoyant force is directly related to the objects

    volume and the density of the surrounding atmosphere, and is equal in magnitude to the

    weight of the atmosphere that the object has displaced. It is felt up and away from the

    planets surface, and in the opposite direction of the gravitational force. The

    combination of these two forces gives the appearance of reducing the apparent

    magnitude of the gravitational force. Standing on the surface of Venus, the buoyant

    force exerted on a typical high school or college student (assuming they arent crushed

    by the pressure) could be estimated to reduce their apparent weight by about 10%.

    Consequently, this average-sized student on the surface of Venus would weigh on the

    order of 80% of their weight on Earth.

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    38/130

    26

    Although it could be argued that students possess equal levels of knowledge

    concerning the moon and Venus, and that students were expected to answer the

    questions situated on the moon with no additional information, students were given

    selected background information on Venus. The Venus set of questions were designed

    to elicit responses that would confirm specific models, and all students needed to have

    the same basis to work from. A preface to the Venus set of questions was written and

    read, Venus is sometimes called Earths sister planet. It is nearly the same size and

    mass, but Venus rotates once on its axis every 243 days, and has an atmospheric

    pressure 90 times that of the Earth.

    In the process of designing the Venus preface, the point arose that if the

    background information included an analogy that Venus atmospheric pressure was

    similar to the pressure of the ocean at the depth of about 3200 feet (~1 kilometer), the

    student response might be different. It is assumed that most students are aware that

    people float and consequently have less apparent weight in the water. It is possible that

    students might take this information and apply it to this question. However, it cannot

    be assumed that the student population that this survey was given to would understand

    the physics behind how submersibles dive and float. On the contrary, it appears that a

    general consensus could be that once things are below a threshold depth, the general

    properties of sinking and floating may not necessarily apply. The general public seems

    to understand that, if a submarine should go below crush depth(easily half a mile) the

    ambient oceanic pressure will compress the vessel, causing it to sink to the bottom.

    This can suggest that at lower depths, things are held down by the water pressure and

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    39/130

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    40/130

    28

    in their reasoning. Since a balance scale measures torque, rather than force, the correct

    reasoning becomes much more involved and too complex for this survey.

    The answers selected for both questions were, a lot more, a lot less, about the

    same, exactly the same, and there is not enough information to answer the question.

    The answers that were written to be responses to the weight question were purposely

    written to encompass a range (lotmore, lot less, aboutthe same) rather than being

    specific. This was to minimize quantitative reasoning and imply that the information

    provided was enough to answer the question. Since an average sized student would

    weigh about 80% of their Earth weight on Venus, the correct response to the weight

    question was about the same. The gravity-air model suggests that the atmosphere will

    push down on everything and one would weigh a lot moreon Venus. (The belief that

    gravity actually pushed up and the atmosphere pushed down was noted in interviews.)

    Since Venus revolves 243 times slower than the earth, the student who believes that

    planetary rotation is directly related to gravity might expect to weigh a lot lesson

    Venus. The exactly the sameoption was included in both sets of answers to avoid

    biasing the correct response to the mass question. The two-question pair was also used

    as a consistency check for mass and weight.

    3.5.2 Question 8 Venus Gravitational Force

    Question 8, the third Venus question, was written as a fill-in-the-blank

    statement. The question stated, The gravitational force of Venus is... ...the

    gravitational force of Earth. Other than that, Question 8 followed the same answer

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    41/130

    29

    format of Question 6 and Question 7. Responses included,much greater than,much

    less than,about the same as,exactly the same as, andthere is not enough information

    to answer the question. As with the mass and weight responses, the answers used

    language written in general terms to encompass a range (much greater, much less,

    about the same). Question 8 was included in the Venus section for a number of

    reasons. As a direct question, it was meant to determine if students connected

    gravitational force with size, mass, rotation or atmosphere. Additionally, Question 8

    was used as a consistency check with Question 6, comparing weight to gravitational

    force, and Question 9, comparing gravitational force to free-fall.

    3.5.3 Question 9 Free-fall on Venus

    Question 9, the final Venus question, asked how a pen would behave on Venus.

    Like the pen on the moon, this question was posed as a hypothetical thought question.

    Question 9 was written as a complete-the-sentence statement, and required the student

    to choose the most appropriate ending. To help clarify their response, most endings

    directly compared the movement of the pen on Venus with a similar pens movement

    on Earth. It read, Suppose you let go of a pen while standing on the surface of Venus.

    Compared to releasing an identical pen at the same height while standing on the surface

    of the Earth, the pen on Venus . . .

    Like the other Venus questions, it followed the same general answer format.

    Each answer choice that referred to the pen falling on Venus, included a time answer as

    well as a speed answer. The answers included, will hit the ground in much less time

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    42/130

    30

    (fall a lot faster) than the pen on Earth,will hit the ground in much greater time (fall a

    lot slower) than the pen on Earth, will hit the ground in about the same amount of time

    (fall about the same way) as the pen on Earth , andwill hit the ground in exactly the

    same amount of time (fall exactly the same way) as the pen on Earth. The options,will

    not fall, and There is not enough information to answer the question were also

    included. Question 9 was included as a contrast to the pen on the moon, and as a

    consistency check with Question 6, and Question 8.

    Questions 6, 8, and 9 were designed to work as a group. Students who give the

    same response for these three questions would be considered consistent in their

    reasoning connecting gravity and gravitational force with free-fall, and weight. Those

    who hold a pure gravity-air model would also tend to be consistent over the three

    questions. Students who do not believe in a gravity-air model but believe that Venus

    atmospheric pressure is great enough to affect the free-fall and weight of an object may

    be consistent over only two of the questions. Those who are of the opinion that Venus

    has an overly viscous atmosphere might only be consistent over Questions 6 and 8.

    Those who overestimate any additional outward or inward non-gravitational external

    forces due to the atmosphere might tend to be consistent over questions 6 and 9.

    Students who feel that the atmosphere pushes down and crushes objects, but does not

    affect an objects free-fall will probably be consistent over Questions 8 and 9.

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    43/130

    31

    3.6 The Likert-scale Questions

    Through the design process of the survey, it became evident that from an

    information-gathering standpoint, it was better to design questions that addressed a

    single issue rather than creating ones that required students to reason through multiple

    issues. Of the 10 questions that comprised the heart of the survey, all were

    location/environment specific, and most focused on how gravity (or the environment)

    affects the motion of, or the force on, an object. All of the multiple-choice questions

    asked what rather than why, leaving the free-response questions as the only ones

    that specifically asked for student reasoning. Since the multiple-choice questions did

    not directly ask students why they chose their answer, student reasoning was only as

    accurate as what was inferred by combining the responses of multiple questions. More

    questions were needed to check for the consistency of student responses and get a more

    accurate determination of student reasoning.

    These additional questions were meant to gather more specific information, and

    provide an opportunity to refine the survey data and help determine the reasoning

    behind the responses. These questions were meant to further explore student

    understanding by asking direct questions about gravity and the nature of gravity.

    Conceptions covered would include the existence of gravity in space and on the moon,

    what parameters affect gravity, and how gravity affects objects. In determining the

    style of question to add, a number of factors were involved. It was felt that additional

    free-response questions, although a more direct method of determining student

    reasoning, would make the survey too lengthy for students. Adding multiple-choice

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    44/130

    32

    questions of the same form and context as the core questions would not only make the

    survey too lengthy, but would have added complexity to the determination of student

    reasoning. After weighing these and other factors, the Likert psychometric response

    scale was determined to be the best format for the additional questions.

    In using the Likert scale, questions would be written as true-or-false statements.

    Students would specify their level of agreement to those statements. The student could

    answer that they were certain that the statement was true or false, or were not so sure

    that it was true or false. Their fifth option was that they did not know or were

    uncertain. This format had the advantage of allowing the student to pick the answer

    they were most comfortable with, and by giving students the option of being unsure, it

    was anticipated that students would be more likely to select answers that more

    accurately reflected what they actually believed. However, for the data analysis, the

    strength of conviction to a true or false answer was not needed, so student response

    confidence was disregarded.

    A total of 15 Likert-scale questions were added to the original 10 questions of

    the core survey. These additional questions fell into 3 general categories. One

    category included 5 questions that dealt with some parameters that, as some students

    believe, affect gravity. Another category was comprised of 3 questions that addressed

    the existence of gravity on the moon, in outer space, and in earth orbit. The third

    category included 7 questions regarding the nature of gravity.

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    45/130

    33

    3.6.1 Parameters Affecting Gravity

    As indicated above, the interviews and literature indicated that a substantial

    number of students believe in a gravity-air connection, and that some students link a

    planets rotation to its gravity. The literature also suggested that some students believe

    that a planets distance from the Sun also affects the planets gravity (Smith &

    Treagust, 1988; Treagust & Smith, 1989). Newtons law of gravitation, F=Gm

    1m

    2

    r2

    ,

    suggests that only 2 things can affect the gravitational force: mass and separation

    distance. To investigate how students weighed in on these concepts, the first 5

    additional questions (11-15) were included.

    All 5 questions had the same general format, A planets . . . affects its

    gravitational pull. Gravitational pull was used instead of gravity to make the

    statement more technically accurate. Atmosphere, rotation, mass, and distance

    from the Sun were inserted into the general question template to create Question 11,

    Question 12, Question 14, and Question 15. Size was inserted into Question 13

    instead of separation distance for reasons explained below. It was expected that most

    of these questions would be used with other survey questions to help identify student

    models as well as check for consistency. Exceptions to this were Question 15 (distance

    from the Sun) and Question 13 (size). A planets distance from the Sun was not

    brought up in any other survey questions, so there were no other questions with which

    to combine Question 15. Question 13 was not used for reasons explained below.

    Size was used in lieu of separation distance in Question 13 because it was

    expected that many students taking the survey would not be familiar with Newtons

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    46/130

    34

    law of gravitation, and might not realize that the separation distance of the masses is

    measured from each objects center of mass. The separation distance between a planet

    and an object resting on the planets surface is effectively the radius of the planet.

    Given this information, and that a planets radius is a measure of its size, it can be

    stated that a planets size affects its gravitational pull. However, although technically

    correct, the assumptions this statement was based on were not identified in the

    question. Without knowing the specific context of the question, students could not be

    expected to understand that the intent of the question was to look for separation

    distance. This may be a moot point, considering that another consequence of the

    questions word choice was that it could easily have been misinterpreted. Students

    might reasonably equate a planets size to its mass, and answer the two questions

    similarly. For this reason, it was expected that this question would not produce useful

    data.

    3.6.2

    The Existence of Gravity on the Moon

    The absence of gravity on the moon and in space are well-documented student

    concepts (Ameh, 1987; Berg & Brouwer, 1991; Dostal, 2005; Noce et al, 1988;

    Ruggiero et al, 1985; Sharma et al, 2004; Watts & Zylbersztajn, 1981). Numerous

    students believe that gravity ends with Earths atmosphere. It may be reasonable to

    conclude that most of these students hold a gravity-air model. Other students may

    consider that gravity is not necessarily influenced by the atmosphere, but reduces to

    zero at an appropriate outer radius from the Earth. Students who subscribe to any of

    the above ideas could reasonably assume that there is no gravity on the moon. The

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    47/130

    35

    moon has no atmosphere to speak of, which would evoke the gravity-air connection. If

    gravity ends with the Earths atmosphere, then the moon has no gravity because the

    moon is outside the Earths atmosphere. If there is no gravity in outer space, and the

    moon is defined to be in outer space, then by definition the moon has no gravity.

    There is no evidence that students have demarcated outer space. Outer space seems to

    be a general term for the space outside of the local vicinity of Earth or any other planet.

    Since the moon is a substantial distance away from the Earth and not planet-sized,

    outer space encompasses lunar space.

    Questions 17, 18, and 19 were designed to look for the existence or absence of

    gravity just outside Earths atmosphere, in outer space, and on the moon. The intent of

    this group of questions was to identify student models and check for consistency

    utilizing the perspectives of 3 different locations. Question 17 was the location closest

    to Earth and read, When in orbit, the astronauts are in zero gravity. Question 18

    read, There is no gravity in outer space, and Question 19 read, There is no gravity

    on the moon. These 3 questions would be used with other survey questions to help

    identify student models as well as check for consistency.

    3.6.3 Gravitys Effect on Objects

    The last group of questions added to the survey were added to focus on some of

    the conceptions students held about the nature of gravity. These 7 questions were

    meant to be used in concert with other survey questions. Two addressed free-fall

    speed, 3 were threshold-type questions and 2 related gravity to weight.

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    48/130

    36

    Question 16 and Question 23 dealt with free-fall as well as gravity terminology.

    They were included as a pair of questions. They both asked the same question using

    different terminology. Question 16 asked the students to respond to, A planets

    gravitational pull affects an objects falling speed, whereas Question 23 was written,

    Gravity affects how fast an object falls. The pairs purpose was two-fold. Asking

    the question twice would provide a consistency check on the concept, and asking it in

    two different ways would verify that students considered gravity and gravitational pull

    to be the same thing.

    Questions 20-22 dealt with the threshold model that arose from the interviews.

    Question 20 read, In low gravity, light objects may be too light to be affected by the

    gravitational force, and Question 21 rephrased the question without gravity, In an

    environment with no gravity, an object must be heavy enough in order to fall.

    Question 22 was added as more of a consistency check on Question 4, the feather-and-

    lead question. It read, With no atmosphere, heavy objects can fall faster than light

    ones.

    The last 2 questions, Question 24 and Question 25 were directed at gravitys

    effect on weight, and gravitys relationship with height. The literature had indicated

    that students associated gravity with free-fall and that an objects weight was related to

    its height above ground (Ameh, 1987; Bar et al, 1994, 1997; Noce et al, 1988; Watts &

    Zylbersztajn, 1981). Question 24 read, Gravity does not affect the weight of an

    object, and Question 25 stated, Heavy objects are hard to lift because Earths

    gravitational force increases as you lift.

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    49/130

    37

    4 RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

    Chapter 4

    RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

    4.1 Initial Categorization

    The initial analysis of the survey results involved checking the correct overall

    number of correct responses between classes. Statistical analysis of the data was done

    with SPSS. The correct responses were totaled for a score. The mean score and

    standard deviation for each class is shown in Table 4.1-1. An independent sample t-

    test for equality of means was performed to identify potential data groups. The results

    of this analysis suggest that there is no statistical difference in correct responses across

    AST 109 classes or across PHY 101, 102, and 105 classes. A comparison of all

    combined AST classes with all combined PHY classes is included in Table 4.1-2. A

    direct comparison of the PHY and AST combined groups indicated that there was a

    statistically significant difference in the mean score of the two groups.

    Class AST 109

    2004

    AST 109

    2005

    AST 109

    2006

    All AST PHY 105

    2005

    PHY 102

    2006

    PHY 101

    2006

    All PHY

    Mean score 10.52 10.92 10.44 10.63 9.19 8.50 7.63 8.66

    Standarddeviation

    4.10 4.74 4.20 4.35 4.36 3.65 2.94 3.91

    n 149 140 128 417 36 16 16 68

    Table 4.1-1: Mean "score" of class groups

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    50/130

    38

    Additional statistical analysis of the data yielded further differences. This

    difference was not unexpected given the subject nature of each class. Since the focus of

    this thesis is to investigate and identify individual student conceptions of gravity, and

    not curriculum development, for most analysis the AST and PHY student data was

    pooled together. For a small number of cases, particular groups of AST were separated

    from the pool of data and comparative analysis was run on the separate group and the

    data pool.

    ClassAST 109

    2005

    AST 109

    2006

    All PHY PHY 105

    2005

    PHY 102

    2006

    PHY 101

    2006

    AST 109

    2004.437 .874 .088 .061 .007

    AST 109

    2005 .379 .049 .050 .007

    AST 109

    2006.379 .122 .080 .010

    All AST .001

    PHY 105

    2005 .581 .196

    PHY 102

    2006 .461

    Table 4.1-2: Independent sample t-test for equality of means (p)

    4.2 The Mother of All Questions and initial groupings

    The responses from the mother of all questions indicated that although most

    students felt that a pen would fall on the moon, a substantial portion had other ideas.

    The results of all 485 students is shown in Table 4.2-1. (The correct response is D.)

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    51/130

    39

    An inspection of the multiple-choice responses indicate that 47% (n = 228) of the

    students responded that the pen will float or levitate. Correct responses were also

    grouped under the Mother Question responses. The overall number of correct

    responses between Mother Question responses were also scored. The mean score

    and standard deviation for each question is also included in Table 4.2-1.

    Suppose you were standing on the moon holding a pen.

    If you were to let go of the pen, what direction will it move?

    All

    (485)

    Mean

    score

    Standard

    deviation

    n

    A The pen will float upward from the lunar surface. 13% 7.81 2.872 64

    B The pen will float around, staying about the same height. 25% 7.73 3.047 119

    C The pen will levitate, but also move away horizontally. 9% 8.16 2.977 45

    A, B, and C combined (floaters) 47% 7.84 2.976 228

    D The pen will fall toward the lunar surface. (Correct) 51% 12.70 4.118 249

    E Other/None of the above. 2% 9.00 3.251 8

    Table 4.2-1: Mother of All Questions (Question 1)

    An independent sample t-test for equality of means was performed on the

    question groups. These results, shown in Table 4.2-2, suggest that there is no statistical

    difference in correct responses across questions A, B, and C. A comparison of

    question D to the other questions show a statistically significant difference in correct

    responses to all other groups. Student responses from A, B and C were combined into

    one group and identified as floaters; the students who gave the correct response are

    identified as fallers. The analysis of most questions involved the comparison of

    these two groups in order to check for the consistent use of specific models of gravity

    throughout the survey.

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    52/130

    40

    What direction will the pen move? B C D E

    A float up .861 .547 .000 .281

    B float around, about the same height. .424 .000 .258

    C levitate and move horizontally. .000 .469

    A, B, and C combined (floaters) .000

    D fall (fallers) .013

    E Other

    Table 4.2-2: Independent sample t-test for equality of means (p)

    4.3 Free-Responses and Student Reasoning

    4.3.1 Student Reasoning for Question 1 Why a Pen Floats

    The multiple-choice responses from the Mother Question indicated that

    although most students felt that a pen would fall on the moon, a substantial number of

    students had other ideas. The free-response portion of the question was then combined

    with the multiple-choice response and used to infer student models of thinking. Each

    response combination was reviewed and assigned to a student reasoning category. A

    comparison of the multiple-choice responses to the student reasoning categories for the

    Mother of All Questions is shown in table 4.3-1.

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    53/130

    41

    No

    Gravity

    Gravity

    Threshold

    Mass

    Threshold

    Stasis

    Threshold

    Other

    Threshold

    Other

    Reasoning

    Correct

    Reasoning

    Blank

    A (64) 27% 20% 12% 0% 6% 19% 0% 16%

    B (119) 24% 10% 6% 34% 2% 18% 0% 7%

    C (45) 22% 24% 2% 0% 20% 29% 0% 2%

    Float (228) 24% (55) 16% (36) 7% (16) 18% (40) 6% (15) 21% (47) 0% 7% (17)

    D (249)

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    54/130

    42

    Student

    reasoning

    Gravity

    Threshold

    Mass

    Threshold

    Stasis

    Threshold

    Other

    Threshold

    Other

    Reasoning

    Correct

    Reasoning

    No

    Gravity.119 .000 .001 .290 .003 .000

    Gravity

    Threshold .006 .171 .850 .227 .000

    MassThreshold

    .058 .059 .117 .012

    Stasis

    Threshold .439 .897 .000

    Other

    Threshold .562 .000

    Other

    Reasoning .000

    Table 4.3-3: Independent sample t-test for equality of means (p)

    4.3.2 Question 1 Free-Response

    Students who reasoned that the pen did not fall because there is no gravitywere

    included in the category of the same name. Of the 57 students (12% of all students)

    that made up this category, 96% (n = 55) werefloaters. Of the 228floaters, 24% (n =

    55) used this reasoning. Typical free responses were,

    There is no gravity on the moon.

    The pen will float around staying about the same height

    because there is no gravity in space.

    A substantial number of students (22% of all students, n = 107) reasoned that

    the pen wouldnt fall because there wasnt enough mass, gravity, or weight

    something to cause the pen to fall. These 107 students accounted for 47% of the

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    55/130

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    56/130

    44

    comprised 18% of thefloaters. Interestingly, all students of this group answered B, the

    pen will float around, staying about the same height. Typical reasoning included,

    There is less gravity on the moon so it will float, but

    enough to keep it from floating away.

    The gravity on the moon is weak, but there is enough that

    nothing will float away.

    The remaining students who held threshold reasoning, but also included

    references to rotation, outside forces and anything else, were given the Other

    Thresholdcategory. These students made up 6% (n = 15) of thefloaters.

    The remaining 21% offloaters(n = 47) held reasoning or responses that would

    not fit into any of the other categories. These included references to movies, rewriting

    the multiple-choice answer as their free response, and other answers that defied

    categorization. Examples of these include,

    The pen would levitate, but other factors, such as lunar

    wind would drive the pen horizontally as well as vertically.

    The other factor would probably be pressure, or possiblyinertia.

    I believe the pen would move upward due to the force of

    gravity on the moon + because objects tend to moveupward in movies.

    Of the responses from fallers, 83% (n = 207) included reasoning that was

    considered correct. Correct reasoning was one that did not include any obvious

    errors.

    The remaining free-responsefaller responses that were not considered correct

    included references to movies, rewriting the multiple-choice answer as their free

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    57/130

    45

    response, answers that could be interpreted as wrong, and a handful of fallers who

    answered with threshold reasoning. Examples of threshold reasoning responses

    included,

    The pen will slowly fall to the ground due to gravity butnot very fast because it doesn't have much mass.

    There is very little gravity on the moon, so the pen wouldslowly fall to the lunar surface, unless it was too light, in

    which case it may drift away or float, like a bubble wouldhere on earth.

    It was initially hoped that the identification and categorization of student

    thinking would aid in predicting student responses. However, this did not appear to be

    the case. A comparison of the multiple-choices of the Mother of All Questions to the

    constructed thresholdand no gravity reasoning is show in Table 4.3-4. It is apparent

    that, with the exception of the students with stasis threshold reasoning, student

    responses are nearly evenly distributed among the three choices of the Mother

    Question.

    A (64) B (119) C (45) D (249) E (8)

    No Gravity 30% 49% 18% 2% 2%

    Gravity Threshold 36% 33% 31% 0% 0%

    Mass Threshold 50% 44% 6% 0% 0%

    Other Threshold 27% 13% 60% 0% 0%

    G/M/O Combined 38% 31% 31% 0% 0%

    Stasis Threshold 0% 100% 0% 0% 0%

    Table 4.3-4: Mother Question multiple-choice response distribution with studentreasoning included

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    58/130

    46

    After the student responses to the Mother Question were categorized, the

    elicited interpretations of student reasoning were then compared to Question 20 of the

    Likert questions. Table 4.3-5 shows the comparison of all student reasoning to

    Question 20, In low gravity, light objects may be too light to be affected by the

    gravitational force. This question was written to be an example of how a student with

    thresholdreasoning might respond. Of all students with thresholdreasoning, 56% (n =

    60 of 107) indicated that they agreed with the Question 20 statement. Only 15% (n =

    16) of the thresholdstudents disagreed with Question 20 and provided a contradictory

    answer to their free-response reasoning. Nearly a third of the students who gave

    correct free-response answers agreed with the threshold statement. This could suggest

    that these 65 students were inconsistent in their response to Question 20 or that they

    possibly hold thresholdreasoning and that the pen is above that threshold.

    No Gravity Gravity

    Threshold

    Mass

    Threshold

    Stasis

    Threshold

    Other

    Threshold

    Other

    Reasoning

    Correct

    Reasoning

    True 37% 67% 56% 48% 53% 31% 31%

    False 25% 8% 13% 23% 13% 31% 47%

    Dont

    Know21% 22% 31% 20% 27% 34% 20%

    Blank 2% 3% 0% 0% 7% 3% 2%

    Total 100% (57) 100% (36) 100% (16) 100% (40) 100% (15) 100% (70) 100% (208)

    Table 4.3-5: Question 1 student reasoning to Question 20 (Q20 correct response isFalse)

    4.3.3 Student Reasoning for Question 2 Why an Astronaut Falls

    In addition to the free-response portion of the mother question, the astronaut

    follow-up question (Question 2) also gave indications of threshold thinking. The

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    59/130

    47

    complete data is shown in Table 4.3-6. Reasons that floaters gave for why the

    astronauts didnt float off the lunar surface included having enough gravity, mass, or

    weight (45%, n = 103) and wearing space suits (17%, n = 39). Thefallersresponded

    that the astronauts didnt float off the lunar surface because of gravity (39%, n = 98),

    lowor lightgravity (29%, n =73), or enoughgravity or mass (22%, n = 56). These

    three groups of responses made up 91% (n = 227) of thefallers. When considering the

    free-responses of all students to both mother and astronaut questions, 39% (190 of 485)

    of the students gave evidence of some type of thresholdreasoning.

    NoGravity

    EnoughWeight/Mass

    EnoughGravity

    Low/LightGravity

    SpaceSuits

    Gravity Other Blank

    A 5% 45% 3% 12% 22% 2% 5% 6%

    B 9% 30% 9% 15% 14% 3% 16% 4%

    C 2% 40% 16% 16% 18% 0% 7% 2%

    Floaters 6% 36% 9% 14% 17% 2% 11% 4%

    Fallers(D) 1% 9% 13% 29% 1% 39% 4% 3%

    Other 0% 12% 12% 50% 0% 0% 25% 0%

    All

    Students3% 22% 11% 23% 8% 21% 8% 4%

    Table 4.3-6: Question 1 multiple-choice responses to Question 2 student reasoning

    (Floaters = A+B+C)

    Student reasoning groups were checked and compared for the overall number of

    correct responses between student reasoning groups. The mean score and standard

    deviation for each group is shown in Table 4.3-7. An independent sample t-test for

    equality of means was also performed on these groups and is displayed in Table 4.3-8.

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    60/130

    48

    The results of this analysis shows a statistical difference in the correct response group

    to all other response groups.

    No

    Gravity

    Enough

    Weight/Mass

    Enough

    Gravity

    Low/Light

    Gravity

    Space

    Suits

    Gravity Other

    Mean

    score6.311 8.57 11.35 11.44 7.71 13.31 8.92

    Standard

    deviation2.442 3.678 3.977 3.595 3.344 4.481 3.498

    n 16 106 54 110 42 102 38

    Table 4.3-7: Mean "score" of Question 2 student reasoning groups

    Enough

    Weight/Mass

    Enough

    Gravity

    Low/Light

    Gravity

    Space

    Suits

    Gravity Other

    No Gravity .019 .000 .000 .133 .000 .009

    Enough

    Weight/Mass .000 .000 .195 .000 .606

    Enough

    Gravity .892 .000 .008 .003

    Low/Light

    Gravity .000 .001 .000

    Space Suits .000 .119

    Gravity .000

    Table 4.3-8: Independent sample t-test for equality of means (p) for differentreasoning groups in Question 2

    4.3.4 Question 2 Student Free-Response

    As in the mother question, the categories chosen were based on the

    interpretation of student free responses. If a students reasoning included components

    of more than one category, a determination was made as to which one category the

  • 8/11/2019 Feeley Thesis

    61/130

    49

    response would best fit. There were a handful (6% of floaters(n = 14) and 1% of

    fallers(n = 2)) of students who included the words no gravityin their reasoning as to

    why the astronauts did not float off the moon. Typically, the response added a

    reference to the lack of an outside force, but it seemed that none of the no gravity

    reasons utilized much scientific reasoning. Two typical examples of student reasoning

    were,

    They didnt [sic] float off into the lunar surfaces because

    they are at zero gravity.

    Since there is no gravity they can't be forced upward.

    Thresholdthinking was also evident when justifying the behavior of astronauts,

    usually in the context that the thresholdwas met. These students reasoned that the

    astronauts did not float off the moon because there was enoughgravity, they had

    enoughmass or weight, or were heavy enough. Given the grouping experience with

    the free response part of the mother question, the responses with mass, weight and

    heaviness were grouped together, and the gr