exploring the relationship of meaning making structure ... · exploring the relationship of...

299
Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial -Leadership Effectiveness. A thesis presented For the degree of Doctor of Philosophy The University of Western Australia Stacie Fae Chappell, B. Comm, University of British Columbia April 2011

Upload: others

Post on 11-Jun-2020

11 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure,

Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership

Effectiveness. A thesis presented

For the degree of Doctor of Philosophy The University of Western Australia

UWA Business School

Stacie Fae Chappell,

B. Comm, University of British Columbia

April 2011

estephan
Typewritten Text
Page 2: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness
Page 3: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

1

ABSTRACT

This thesis explored the relationship between meaning-making structure (MMS),

emotional intelligence (EI), traditional intelligence (IQ) and managerial-

leadership effectiveness (MLE). Globalisation, technology, higher levels of

education and a growing crisis of meaning require managerial-leaders with

emotional, social and meaning-making abilities beyond traditional intelligence

(Pfeiffer 2001; Price 2003; Quatro, Waldman & Galvin 2007; Reams 2005;

Zohar & Marshall 2000). These meaning-making abilities include, but

transcend, those associated with emotional intelligence. For example,

developmental theory suggests differences in MLE are largely explained by an

individual’s internal MMS (Rooke & Torbert 2005).

Despite increased interest in the meaning-making abilities of managerial-

leaders, there is a gap in the literature exploring the relationship between MMS

and EI and their relative impact on MLE. The aim of this study was to explore

the relationship between MMS and EI and IQ in predicting MLE. The sample

consisted of 169 managerial-leaders who had completed a multi-source

feedback process as part of a leadership development program. Ability

measures were used to operationalise the three independent variables including

the Wonderlic Personal Test to measure IQ, the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso

Emotional Intelligence Test v2 to measure EI, and the Washington University

Sentence Completion Test to measure MMS. The dependent variable of MLE

was operationalized as other-average ratings on the multi-source feedback

instrument known as the Integral Leadership and Management Development

Profile (ILMDP).

Page 4: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

2

Based on the findings of this research, it is suggested that the independent

variables of MMS, EI and IQ do not uniformly predict ratings of MLE. Testing for

relationships at the full sample level revealed that MMS, EI and IQ are not

significantly related to MLE. Revisiting the trait and leadership literature

suggested one possible explanation of these results was the issue of

unobserved heterogeneity. Consequently, sample was tested for the existence

of subgroups which did in fact exist. Results of re-testing the hypotheses at the

subgroup level revealed differences in the existence and direction of the

relationships between IQ, EI and MMS with each other and in predicting MLE.

Testing for the mean differences in background characteristics and the

independent variables eliminated both as possible sources of variation between

the subgroups. However, significant differences existed between the groups in

terms of mean ratings of M LE. Implications for practice, theory and research

are discussed.

Page 5: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

3

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

Without a doubt, the first acknowledgement I wish to make on this thesis is to

the amazing Jeannette Chappell, who has at varying points along the way been

my research assistant, copy editor, sounding board, support system and best

friend. She also happens to be my mom. Thank you for being the beautiful way

that you are and for doing all that you did. This journey would not have been

possible without you.

Next of course are my supervisors Winthrop Professor Dr. David Plowman and

Winthrop Professor Dr. Geoffrey Soutar. Thank you for giving so generously of

your time, wisdom and patience. I have learned much from you both and it has

been an honour to work with you.

Thank you to Dr. Ron Cacioppe, former Managing Director of the AIM-UWA

Integral Leadership Center, for providing a rich field to learn about leadership

development, introducing me to Integral Theory, and providing access to the

UWA Integral Leadership and Management Development Profile clients and

data base.

Thank you to Donald Clarke who courageously took on the task of scoring the

MMS data with me. Your hours of work were a huge support and our

conversations were a powerful learning experience for me. Thank you to MHS

for providing access to research pricing for the MSCEITv2. Thank you to the

participants in the study for your interest in leadership development and support

of my research. Thank you to Mandie Colledge for help with data entry of

course, but equally if not more important for including me as part of your family.

Thank you to Roslyn Richards for support on technical matters of all sorts and

top ups of required wisdom.

I am grateful for the unconditional love and support I have received from my

family and circle of friends. Thank you to my sister Dr. Nicola Chappell for

nursing our father so lovingly and enabling me to feel safe to continue my

journey so far from home. Thank you to my dear friends Dr. Fiona Broadbent-

Bong and Dee Roche for the many conversations that helped me to stay the

Page 6: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

4

course. Thank you to Dr. George Trippe and Brendan McKeague for your

beautiful presence in our community of practice which fed my soul and reignited

my motivation to finish. Thank you to Melanie Pescud, Catherine Jordan, Dr.

Michele Roberts and Dr. Joanne Sneddon for sharing the journey and

encouraging me towards becoming an academic.

Last, but definitely not least, I want to acknowledge my gratitude to the late

Professor Peter Frost for providing an entry point into academia, introducing me

to leadership development and demonstrating the fundamental relationship

between meaning-making and leadership.

It might have appeared to go unnoticed,

but I’ve got it all here in my heart…. you are the wind beneath my wings.

Larry Henley and Jeff Sillbar, Wind Beneath My Wings

Page 7: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

5

STATEMENT OF CANDIDATE CONTRIBUTION

This thesis is my own composition, all sources have been acknowledged and

my contribution is clearly identified in the thesis. At the date of submission, no

material within this thesis has been published elsewhere.

Stacie F. Chappell

Geoffrey Soutar David Plowman

Page 8: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

6

LIST OF TABLES

Table 2.1 Loevinger’s Stages of Ego Development 61

Table 2.2 Implicit Values Within the CVF 75

Table 3.1 Relationship Between Total, Area, Function and Role ILMDP Scores 87

Table 3.2 Form 81 of the Washington University Sentence Completion Test 89

Table 3.3 Indicative Items for Faces and Pictures Tasks from MSCEITv2 Branch 1: Perceive and Appraise Emotion

96

Table 3.4 Indicative Items for Facilitation and Sensations Tasks from MSCEITv2 Branch 2: Facilitate Thought

97

Table 3.5 Indicative Items for Changes and Blends Tasks from MSCEITv2 Branch 3: Understanding Emotion

98

Table 3.6 Indicative Items for Emotion Management and Emotional Relations Tasks from MSCEITv2 Branch 4: Managing Emotions

99

Table 4.1 Mean IRR and IRA Statistics for the 32 ILMDP Items 129

Table 4.2 Central Tendency and Dispersion of Total # of ILMDP Items Meeting IRA Interpretive Standards

131

Table 4.3 Mean IRA and IRR Statistics for the ILMDP Scales 133

Table 4.4 Descriptive Statistics for The ILMDP Items 134

Table 4.5 CFA Summary Statistics for the Eight ILMDP Subscales 147

Table 4.6 AVE and Squared Correlations for the ILMDP Subscales 148

Table 4.7 Rotated Factor Loadings for the ILMDP Items 150

Table 4.8 Mean IRR Statistic for tems in Revised LE Scale 153

Table 4.9 Central Tendency and Dispersion of Total # of Revised LE Scale Items Meeting IRR Interpretive Standards

154

Table 4.10 Mean IRR Statistic for Items in Revised ME Scale 157

Table 4.11 Central Tendency and Dispersion of Total # of Revised ME Scale Items Meeting IRR Interpretive Standards

158

Table 4.12 MSCEITv2 Task Level Correlations 161

Table 4.13 MSCEITv2 Branch Level Correlations 161

Table 4.14 Comparison of the Reliabilities for the MSCEITv2 Total EI, Branch and Task Subscales across Three Studies

162

Table 5.1 Normality Statistics for Main Research Sample 168

Table 5.2 Background Characteristics of the Sample 170

Table 5.3 Industries Represented in the Sample 171

Page 9: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

7

Table 5.4 Frequency of Job Titles 172

Table 5.5 Descriptive Statistics for IQ, ED and MLE Variables 173

Table 5.6 Frequency of ED Level 174

Table 5.7 Descriptives for MSCEIT v2 Scales and Comparison to Norming Sample 175

Table 5.8 Correlations of the Independent Variables 176

Table 5.9 Latent Class Regression Fit Statistics: Leadership Effectiveness 182

Table 5.10 Latent Class Regression Fit Statistics: Management Effectiveness 183

Table 5.11 Size of the Latent Classes 183

Table 5.12 Descriptive Statistics on Variables for Leadership Effectiveness Groups 184

Table 5.13 Descriptive Statistics on Variables for Management Effectiveness Groups 185

Table 5.14 Correlations for Leadership Effectiveness Group One 186

Table 5.15 Regression for Leadership Effectiveness Group One 187

Table 5.16 Correlations for Leadership Effectiveness Group Two 188

Table 5.17 Regression for Leadership Effectiveness Group Two 188

Table 5.18 Correlations for Leadership Effectiveness Group Three 189

Table 5.19 Regression for Leadership Effectiveness Group Three 190

Table 5.20 Correlations for Management Effectiveness Group One 191

Table 5.21 Regression for Management Effectiveness Group One 191

Table 5.22 Correlations for Management Effectiveness Group Two 192

Table 5.23 Regression for Management Effectiveness Group Two 193

Table 5.24 Correlations for Management Effectiveness Group Three 194

Table 5.25 Regression for Management Effectiveness Group Three 194

Table 5.26 Nominally Scaled Background Variables 196

Table 5.27 Frequency (or Mean) of Background Variables for the LE Groups 197

Table 5.28 Structural Correlations After Varimax Rotation 198

Table 5.29 Group Centroid Values 199

Table 5.30 Frequency (or Mean) of Background Variables for the ME Groups 200

Table 6.1 Summary of Correlations for the Leadership Effectiveness Subgroups 209

Table 6.2 Summary of Correlations for the Management Effectiveness Subgroups 210

Table 6.3 Summary of Standardised Regression Coefficients for All Subgroups 212

Page 10: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

8

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure 1.1 MMS is Positively Correlated With IQ and EI (H1) 16

Figure 1.2 MMS has Discriminant Validity Beyond IQ and EI in Predicting MLE (H2) 16

Figure 2.1 The Competing Values Framework 74

Figure 3.1 ILMDP Domain 2: Management and Leadership Functions 81

Figure 3.2 Internal Versus External Dimension of ILMDP Model 83

Figure 3.3 ILMDP’s 10-Point Scale 86

Figure 3.4 Structure of MSCEITv2: Total EI, Area, Branch and Task Levels 95

Figure 4.1 ILMDP Brokering Subcale Item Loadings and Fit Statistics 136

Figure 4.2 ILMDP Directing Subscale Item Loadings and Fit Statistics 138

Figure 4.3 ILMDP Monitoring Subscale Item Loadings and Fit Statistics 139

Figure 4.4 ILMDP Achieving Subscale Item Loadings and Fit Statistics 141

Figure 4.5 ILMDP Stewarding Subscale Item Loadings and Fit Statistics 142

Figure 4.6 ILMDP Coaching Subscale Item Loadings and Fit Statistics 144

Figure 4.7 ILMDP Facilitating Subscale Item Loadings and Fit Statistics 145

Figure 4.8 ILMDP Visioning Subscale Item Loadings and Fit Statistics 146

Figure 4.9 Scree Diagram of ILDMP Items 149

Figure 4.10 Revised Leadership Effectiveness Scale Item Loadings and Fit Statistics 155

Figure 4.11 Revised Management Effectiveness Scale Item Loadings and Fit Statistics 159

Figure 5.1 A Representation of Latent Class Regression 179

Page 11: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

9

TABLE OF CONTENTS  

ABSTRACT   1    

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS   3    

STATEMENT  OF  CANDIDATE  CONTRIBUTION   5    

LIST  OF  TABLES   6    

LIST  OF  FIGURES   7      

TABLE  OF  CONTENTS   8    

  13  CHAPTER  1:  INTRODUCTION1.1  CHAPTER  OVERVIEW   13  1.2  BACKGROUND  TO  THE  RESEARCH   13  1.3  THE  RESEARCH  PROBLEM  AND  HYPOTHESES   15  1.4  JUSTIFICATION  FOR  THE  RESEARCH   17  1.5  AN  OVERVIEW  OF  THE  STUDY’S  METHODOLOGY   19  1.6  THE  STRUCTURE  OF  THE  THESIS   19  1.7  DEFINITION  OF  ACRONYMS   21  1.8  CHAPTER  SUMMARY   21  

  23  CHAPTER  2:  LITERATURE  REVIEW2.1  CHAPTER  OVERVIEW   23  2.2  BACKGROUND   24  2.3  THE  EVOLUTION  OF  MLE:  A  RETURN  TO  TRAIT-­‐BASED  RESEARCH   25  2.4  INTELLIGENCE  (IQ)   31  2.4.1  IQ  AND  MLE   34  2.5  EMOTIONAL  INTELLIGENCE  (EI)   36  2.5.1  BACKGROUND  OF  THE  EI  CONSTRUCT   37  2.5.2  ALTERNATE  MODELS  OF  EI   39     2.5.2.1  The  Expanded  View:  Mixed  Models  of  EI   39     2.5.2.2  The  Narrow  View:  Ability  Models  of  EI   42  2.5.3  THEORETICAL  CONTROVERSIES   46  2.5.4  ABILITY  EI  AND  MLE   49  2.6  MEANING-­‐MAKING  STRUCTURE  (MMS)   56  2.6.1  CONSTRUCTIVE-­‐DEVELOPMENTAL  THEORY   57  2.6.2  LOEVINGER’S  EGO-­‐DEVELOPMENT  THEORY   59  2.6.3  MMS  AND  MLE   66  2.6.4  MMS  AND  EI   70  2.7  MANAGERIAL-­‐LEADERSHIP  EFFECTIVENESS  (MLE)   72  2.8  CHAPTER  SUMMARY  AND  HYPOTHESES   77  

  79  CHAPTER  3:  MEASURES  AND  METHODS3.1  CHAPTER  OVERVIEW   79  

Page 12: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

10

3.2  DEFINITIONS  AND  MEASURES   79  3.2.1  THE  INTEGRAL  LEADERSHIP  AND  MANAGEMENT  DEVELOPMENT  PROFILE   80     3.2.1.1  ILMDP  Management  Roles   83     3.2.1.2  ILMDP  Leadership  Roles   84  3.2.2  THE  WASHINGTON  STATE  SENTENCE  COMPLETION  TEST   88  3.2.3  THE  MAYER,  SALOVEY  AND  CARUSO  EI  TEST   94     3.2.3.1  Branch  1:  Perceive  and  Appraise  Emotion   95     3.2.3.2  Branch  2:  Use  Emotion  to  Facilitate  Thought   97     3.2.3.3  Branch  3:  Understand  Emotion   97     3.2.3.4  Branch  4:  Manage  Emotion   98     3.2.3.5  MSCEITv2  Scoring  and  Measurement  Properties   100  3.2.1  THE  WONDERLIC  PERSONALITY  TEST   105  3.3  METHODS   106  3.3.1  SAMPLE  SOURCES   106     3.3.1.1  The  Separate  ILMDP  Sample   106     3.3.1.2  The  Main  Sample   106  3.3.2  DATA  COLLECTION  FOR  THE  MAIN  SAMPLE   107  3.3.3  DATA  SCORING  AND  ENTRY  OF  THE  MAIN  SAMPLE   109  3.3.4  DATA  ANALYSIS   111     3.3.4.1  Data  Preparation  and  Assumption  Checking   112     3.3.4.2  Inter-­‐Rater  Similarity   113     3.3.4.3  Measurement  Properties   119     3.3.4.4  Regression  Analysis  (H2)   122  3.4  CHAPTER  SUMMARY   122  

  125  CHAPTER  4:  PRELMINARY  DATA  ANALYSIS4.1  CHAPTER  OVERVIEW   125  4.2  THE  MEASUREMENT  PROPERTIES  OF  THE  ILMDP   126  4.2.1  THE  SAMPLE  AND  DATA  PREPARATION   127  4.2.2  INTER-­‐RATER  AGREEMENT  AND  RELIABILITY   128     4.2.2.1  IRA  and  IRR  for  the  ILMDP  Items   128     4.2.2.2  IRA  and  IRR  for  the  ILMDP  Scales   131  4.2.3  TESTING  FOR  OUTLIERS  AND  NORMALITY   133  4.2.4  MEASUREMENT  PROPERTIES  OF  THE  ILMDP  SUBSCALES   135     4.2.4.1  The  Brokering  Subscale   135     4.2.4.2  The  Directing  Subscale   136     4.2.4.3  The  Monitoring  Subscale   138     4.2.4.4  The  Achieving  Subscale   139     4.2.4.5  The  Stewarding  Subscale   141     4.2.4.6  The  Coaching  Subscale   142     4.2.4.7  The  Facilitating  Subscale   144     4.2.4.8  The  Visioning  Subscale   145     4.2.4.9  Summary  Statistics  and  Correlation  Analysis  with  Original  Scales   146  4.2.5  DISCRIMINANT  VALIDITY  OF  THE  ILMDP  SUBSCALES   147  4.2.6  EXPLORING  THE  ILMDP’S  STRUCTURE  IN  THE  PRESENT  STUDY   148  4.2.7  CONFIRMING  THE  REVISED  MLE  SCALES  IN  THE  PRESENT  STUDY   151     4.2.7.1  The  Revised  LE  Scale   152     4.2.7.2  Revised  ME  Scale   155  4.3  THE  RELIABILITY  OF  THE  MSCEITV2   160  4.4  THE  INTER-­‐RATER  SIMILARITY  OF  THE  WUSCT  SCORES   163  4.5  CHAPTER  SUMMARY   164  

Page 13: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

11

  167  CHAPTER  5:  RESULTS5.1  INTRODUCTION   167  5.2  THE  INITIAL  ANALYSIS  OF  THE  MAIN  SAMPLE   167  5.3  SAMPLE  BACKGROUND  CHARACTERISTICS   169  5.4  DESCRIPTIVE  STATISTICS   172  5.5  TESTING  HYPOTHESIS  ONE   175  5.6  TESTING  HYPOTHESIS  TWO   176  5.7  TESTING  FOR  SUBGROUPS   177  5.7.1  LATENT  CLASS  REGRESSION:  LE   182  5.7.2  LATENT  CLASS  REGRESSION:  ME   182  5.7.3  MEAN  DIFFERENCES  WITHIN  THE  LE  SUBGROUPS   184  5.7.4  MEAN  DIFFERENCES  WITHIN  THE  ME  SUBGROUPS   184  5.8  TESTING  THE  HYPOTHESES  FOR  THE  SUBGROUPS   185  5.8.1  H1  AND  H2  FOR  LEADERSHIP  EFFECTIVENESS  GROUP  ONE   186  5.8.2  H1  AND  H2  FOR  LEADERSHIP  EFFECTIVENESS  GROUP  TWO   187  5.8.3  H1  AND  H2  FOR  LEADERSHIP  EFFECTIVENESS  GROUP  THREE   189  5.8.4  H1  AND  H2  FOR  MANAGEMENT  EFFECTIVENESS  GROUP  ONE   190  5.8.5  H1  AND  H2  FOR  MANAGEMENT  EFFECTIVENESS  GROUP  TWO   192  5.8.6  H1  AND  H2  FOR  MANAGEMENT  EFFECTIVENESS  GROUP  THREE   193  5.9  SUBGROUP  BACKGROUND  DIFFERENCES   195  5.9.1  BACKGROUND  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  LE  GROUPS   196  5.9.2  BACKGROUND  DIFFERENCES  IN  THE  ME  GROUPS   199  5.10  SUMMARY   200  

  203  Chapter  66.1  INTRODUCTION   203  6.2  A  SUMMARY  OF  THE  THESIS   203  6.3  DISCUSSION   208  6.3.1  THE  RELATIONSHIPS  BETWEEN  IQ,  EI  AND  MMS  (H1)   208  6.3.2  INCREMENTAL  PREDICTIVE  VALIDITY  OF  MMS  (H2)   211  6.3.3  THE  THREE  LEADERSHIP  EFFECTIVENESS  GROUPS   216     6.3.3.1  Manage  the  Emotional  Landscape  Leaders   217     6.3.3.2  Read  the  Emotional  Landscape  Leaders   219     6.3.3.3  Intelligent  Ego  Leaders   219  6.3.4  THE  THREE  MANAGEMENT  EFFECTIVENESS  GROUPS   220     6.3.4.1  Strategise  the  Emotional  Environment  Managers   221     6.3.4.2  Intelligent  Perception  Managers   221     6.3.4.3  I  See  It  But  I  Don’t  Understand  It  Managers   222  6.4  IMPLICATIONS  FOR  PRACTICE   223  6.5  IMPLICATIONS  FOR  THEORY  AND  FUTURE  RESEARCH   225  6.6  LIMITATIONS  TO  THE  PRESENT  STUDY   229  6.7  CONCLUSIONS   231  REFERENCES   232  APPENDICES   259  

Page 14: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

12

Page 15: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

13

Chapter 1

INTRODUCTION

1.1 CHAPTER OVERVIEW

The purpose of this study was to contribute to our understanding of the

antecedents to Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness (MLE) by investigating the

relationships between this construct and meaning-making structure (MMS),

emotional intelligence (EI) and general mental ability (IQ). This chapter provides

an overview of the thesis, including the background to the study, the research

problem and related hypotheses, and the justification for the research. The

chapter also summarises the methodology used in the study, the thesis

structure and a list of the acronyms used throughout the thesis.

1.2 BACKGROUND TO THE RESEARCH

Organisational life is increasingly complex and the “hunger for compelling and

creative leadership” has never been greater (Burns 1978, pg 1). Corporate

scandals and the recent global financial crisis have illuminated the need for

managerial-leaders who have a broader set of skills and a view beyond the

bottom line. Globalisation, demands for corporate social responsibility, higher

levels of education and a growing crisis of meaning in society are some of the

reasons that traditional leadership abilities will not suffice (Hesselbein,

Goldsmith & Beckhard 1996; Tacey 2003; Wheatley 2006; Wheatley 2009).

Page 16: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

14

These and other challenges require managerial-leaders with emotional, social

and meaning-making abilities beyond traditional intelligence (IQ) (Pfeiffer 2001;

Price 2003; Reams 2005; Quatro, Waldman & Galvin 2007; Zohar & Marshall

2000).

Interacting with multiple stakeholders, the complexity of people management

and the stresses of organisational life are some of the reasons managerial-

leadership is an emotionally-laden process. Modern organisational life requires

managerial-leaders able to work within an emotional landscape, including

working with both their own emotions and the emotions of others. It is within

this context that the concept of Emotional Intelligence (EI) was introduced and

emerged as the sine qua non for effective leadership (Goleman 1998a).

The concept of EI struck a collective cord as an antidote to the perceived

monopoly of IQ and was quickly embraced by the corporate world. The EI

construct endorsed the importance of emotions and meaning-making to

effective managerial-leadership and had the added benefit of being an ability

that a person could develop. The result was the emergence of an industry

devoted to measuring and developing the EI of managerial-leaders. Alternate

models of EI have emerged, with varying degrees of support for their constructs

and their predictive and discriminant validity. However, there is general

agreement within the academic community that ability-based models of EI are

better than self-report and/or trait-based measures. The use of EI in applied

settings has also exceeded the boundaries of our research based

understanding of the construct (Mayer, Salovey & Caruso 2008). Despite

extravagant claims by some practitioners, it is unlikely EI is a panacea for

managerial-leadership effectiveness (Antonakis 2003; Antonakis 2004).

Page 17: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

15

Further, EI does not guarantee a moral high-ground. The often used ‘Hitler

example’ reminds us that people who are skilled at working with the emotional

landscape can operate within a fundamentally limited worldview (e.g. our tribe

versus their tribe). A more defensible hypothesis is that EI works in

combination with other intra-individual abilities to contribute to behaviour and,

ultimately, to managerial-leadership effectiveness.

Organisational life consists of multiple worldviews that require managerial-

leaders to understand and incorporate different viewpoints, to transcend the

perspective of the system within which they operate and to identify

commonalities beyond differences. These meaning-making abilities include, but

go beyond EI abilities (Tischler, Biberman & McKeage 2002) . Developmental

psychologists argue differences in MLE are largely explained by an individual’s

internal meaning-making structure (MMS), also described as an individual’s

collection of action-logics (Rooke & Torbert 2005). Increasing levels of MMS

would provide managerial-leaders with increased complexity in their thinking

and meaning-making capacities. Despite significant research exploring the

relationship between EI and other individual differences (J. Ciarrochi, A.Y. Chan

& P. Caputi 2000; Day & Carroll 2004; Saklofske, Austin & Minski 2003; Schutte

et al. 1998), to date no empirical research has explored the relationship

between MMS and EI.

1.3 THE RESEARCH PROBLEM AND HYPOTHESES

The purpose of this study was to broaden our understanding of MLE through an

investigation of the relationships between MMS, EI and IQ and MLE. Despite

extensive research for each of the constructs individually, there is a clear gap in

Page 18: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

16

our knowledge of the relationships between them. Based on a review of the

literature, which is discussed in Chapter 2, three hypotheses were suggested.

MSS is a valid individual difference that is thought to contribute to individual

effectiveness. As such, the first hypothesis posited that MMS was positively

correlated with IQ and EI, as can be seen in Figure 1.1.

Figure 1.1 MMS is Positively Correlated with IQ and EI (H1)

The second hypothesis posited that MMS was a distinct construct from both IQ

and EI and as such would have validity in predicting MLE as can be seen in

Figure 1.2.

Figure 1.2 MMS IQ and EI Predict MLE (H2)

Page 19: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

17

1.4 JUSTIFICATION FOR THE RESEARCH

Understanding the antecedents of MLE is important for research and practice.

Significant amounts of time and money are invested in leadership development

programs that are focussed on developing the EI capacities of managerial-

leaders. However, the relationship between EI and MLE continues to be poorly

understood. As such, this study contributed to a better understanding of this

relationship.

More specifically, this study addressed an area that previous researchers have

neglected; namely the relationship between MMS and MLE. First and foremost,

this was an empirical study of the construct of MMS in the leadership arena.

While MMS has been explored extensively in psychology, there has been

limited application of the theory to increase our understanding of MLE in a

business context. Despite the existence of valid and reliable measures of MMS

(Cook-Greuter 2005; Loevinger 1998), there is limited empirical research to

support the theoretical claim that this construct is significantly related to MLE.

The potential application of this relationship is considerable as it lends credibility

to the life-long developmental journey required if people are to become and

remain effective managerial-leaders.

Finally, the study made a significant contribution through the specific methods

and measures used to examine the hypotheses. In his critique of the field,

Antonakis (2009) challenged EI researchers to design studies that:

1. Avoid self-reported measures of MLE.

Page 20: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

18

2. Obtain MLE measures and individual difference scores from different

sources.

3. Use measures specifically designed to tap into EI rather than personality.

4. Use practising managerial-leaders in real-world contexts.

5. Have acceptable sample sizes.

6. Control for hierarchical nestings if this is pertinent (i.e. levels of analysis)

(Antonakis, Ashkanasy & Dasborough 2009).

This study addressed many of these issues. The dependent variable was

operationalized as multi-source feedback ratings from others. The individual

difference scores were drawn from ability measures for MMS, EI and IQ.

Further, the sample, although moderate in size, was drawn from practising

managerial-leaders operating in real-world contexts. In summary, this study

explored an area of both theoretical and practical importance. The research

made at least four contributions to our understanding of MLE, in that it has:

1. Extended the application and understanding of the relationship between

EI and MLE,

2. Expanded the application and understanding of the relationship between

MMS and MLE,

3. Explored the relationship between EI and MMS, and

4. Used a methodologically strong approach within the study.

Page 21: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

19

1.5 AN OVERVIEW OF THE STUDY’S METHODOLOGY

This study used a hypothetical-deductive approach and a survey methodology.

The research population included people participating in customised corporate

leadership development programs delivered by the AIM-UWA Integral

Leadership Centre1, and/or a leadership effectiveness unit within the part-time

MBA program at The University of Western Australia Business School.

Respondents in the study included practising managers who had completed a

competency-based 360o feedback process as part of their individual

development program. The results of the 360o feedback process were used as

the dependent measure of MLE within the study. Managers were invited to

participate in the study after receiving the results of their 360o feedback. The

final sample included 169 respondents who completed a battery of tests,

including an ability measure of EI and MMS. Analysis of the data collected was

undertaken in two stages, starting with the preliminary data analysis, after which

the three hypotheses were examined.

1.6 THE STRUCTURE OF THE THESIS

The thesis has six chapters. As has already been described, Chapter 1

introduced the research and provided an overview of the thesis, while in

Chapter 2 the relevant literature on MLE, IQ, EI and MMS and their

relationships are reviewed. The development of our understanding of MLE is

discussed through a brief historical review that details the resurgence of interest

in individual differences as antecedents to MLE. The importance of traditional

IQ and personality, and the growing interest in identifying additional constructs,

1 The AIM-UWA Integral Leadership Center is a joint venture between the Australian Institute of Management (AIM) and the University of Western Australia.

Page 22: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

20

are also examined. Of particular interest is the concept of EI, which has

received varying levels of attention for almost a century, but has had a

resurgence of interest in recent decades.

In the discussion, EI is delineated as an ability, rather than an aspect of

personality, and the important relationship between EI and MLE is outlined.

Building on this foundation, the discussion turns to the need for meaning-

making abilities beyond EI. The concept of MMS is introduced and the

relationships between MMS, EI and MLE are discussed. The chapter

concludes with a summary of the hypotheses posited.

In Chapter 3 the constructs within the research hypotheses are outlined and the

methods that were used in the study’s research design are described.

Operational definitions and measures for MLE, IQ, EI and MMS are provided,

including a discussion of the psychometric properties of each of the measures.

This discussion is followed by a description of the research methods that were

used, including data collection, entry, coding and analysis.

Chapter 4 presents the preliminary data analysis, including an analysis of the

measurement properties for the dependent variable measure, the reliability of

the EI measure, and inter-rater similarity and agreement for the relevant

measures. Building on this foundation, Chapter 5 presents results from the data

analysis including testing of the hypotheses. Chapter 6 provides a summary of

the study including a discussion of the results. Finally, the study’s conclusions,

limitations and implications for practice, theory and research are discussed.

Page 23: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

21

1.7 DEFINITION OF ACRONYMS

The following is a list of acronyms that are used throughout the thesis:

ED Ego-development

EI Emotional intelligence

GMA General mental ability, also referred to as traditional

intelligence within this study

IQ The empirical score on a test of traditional intelligence

ILMDP Integral Leadership and Management Development Profile

(measure employed in the study for behaviourally complex

managerial-leadership effectiveness)

LE Leadership effectiveness

ME Management-effectiveness

MLE Managerial-leadership effectiveness

MMS Meaning-making structure

MSCEITv2 Mayer, Salovey, Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test

(measure of EI employed in the study)

MSF Multi-source feedback

WPT Wonderlic Personnel Test (measure of IQ employed in the

study)

WUSCT Washington University Sentence Completion Test (measure

of MMS employed in the study)

1.8 CHAPTER SUMMARY

The present chapter presented an introduction and overview of the study. The

background to the study, including the re-emergence of the EI construct and the

importance of MMS to the phenomenon of MLE, was discussed with specific

reference to the lack of empirical research that has explored the relationship

Page 24: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

22

between EI and MMS. This led to the specific research problem and research

hypotheses intended to explore the relationships between MMS, EI, IQ and

MLE. A brief justification for the research was provided followed by an overview

of the methodology of the study. The structure of the thesis and a list of key

terms used in the study were also presented. The next chapter provides a

review of past research that led to the present study.

Page 25: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

23

Chapter 2

LITERATURE REVIEW

2.1 CHAPTER OVERVIEW

In the previous chapter the research question was introduced, which asked:

What are the relationships between meaning-making structure (MMS), emotional intelligence (EI), traditional intelligence (IQ), and managerial-leadership effectiveness (MLE)?

The purpose of this chapter is to establish the theoretical foundation for this

research question and the related hypotheses. To begin, a broad level

contextualisation of the research is provided in Section (Section 2.2) followed

by a review of the evolution in our understanding of MLE, specifically outlining a

return to the study of trait-based explanations (section 2.3). This section

concludes with an indication of three key trait-based antecedents to MLE that

are particularly salient given the current context of managerial-leadership: IQ, EI

and meaning-making structure (MMS).

Section 2.4 reviews the concept of IQ, its relationship with MLE and the

rationale for exploring other abilities as antecedents of MLE. Section 2.5

reviews the EI construct as it provides:

1. The background to the EI construct (section 2.5.1).

2. A discussion of alternate EI models: trait versus ability (section 2.5.2).

Page 26: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

24

3. A summary of the key controversies in the field of study (section 2.5.3).

4. A summary of the research that has examined the relationship between

the ability model of EI and MLE (section 2.5.4).

Next, in section 2.6, the concept of meaning-making structure (MMS) is

reviewed. This section begins by articulating the difference between meaning,

meaning-making and MMS. Next, an overview of constructive developmental

theory, the theoretical domain within which MMS is placed (section 2.6.1), an

overview of Loevinger’s constructive developmental theory of ego-development

(ED) as a seminal operationalisation of MMS (section 2.6.2), a summary of the

current understanding of the relationship between MMS and MLE (section

2.6.3) and with EI (section 2.6.4).

Finally, the construct of MLE, the dependent variable in the research, is

delineated through the lens of behavioural complexity (section 2.7). The chapter

concludes with a summary of the chapter and the research hypotheses.

2.2 BACKGROUND

Globalisation and technology have fundamentally changed the dynamics of

organisational life, challenging organisations and managers to operate in an

increasingly dynamic and complex arena. Successful organisations have

realised that retaining a competitive edge in the new economy requires

harnessing their full potential through exceptional leadership. Organisational

innovation and creativity and/or learning are heralded as the critical ‘x’ factor

that will sustain organisations in the new information age, but management

gurus warn that traditional organisational structures and managerial

Page 27: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

25

competencies and a unitary focus on profit and maximising shareholder wealth

alone will not suffice (Conger 1994; Duchon & Plowman 2005; Fairholm 2004;

Gehrke & Gehrke 2008). This is articulated succinctly by Pfeiffer (2001, p. 138):

Most agree that solutions to society’s most vexing problems will require citizens to possess not only well-developed intellectual abilities, but also equally impressive social and emotional skills.

Today, it is generally accepted that superior managerial leadership requires a

combination of abilities, although that has not always been the case (Quinn,

1996). In terms of how this applies to MLE definitions, the theory of behavioural

complexity is very useful in terms of capturing this perspective as detailed in

Section 2.7. In terms of antecedents to MLE, our understanding of relevant

abilities has similarly expanded. Until recently, our view of intelligence has

been narrowly defined and many theorists suggest significant variance in

managerial leadership effectiveness can be attributed to abilities beyond the

traditional IQ (Caruso & Salovey 2004; Rooke & Torbert 2005; Sternberg et al.

1995; Zohar & Marshall 2000). However, having argued the importance of

multiple abilities for MLE, much of the work has focussed on measuring and

developing EI, rather than understanding how it relates to other fundamental

abilities, particularly as antecedents to MLE.

The next section describes the origins of trait-based research of MLE, including

the journey away from and ultimate return to this line of enquiry.

2.3 THE EVOLUTION OF MLE: A RETURN TO TRAIT-BASED RESEARCH

Managerial-leadership is a broad and multi-faceted phenomenon that can be

conceptualised at an individual, dyadic, group or organizational level (Lussier &

Page 28: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

26

Achua 2009). Theories vary based on their focus (leader or follower centred),

their intention (descriptive or prescriptive), and/or their applicability (universal or

contingent) (Yukl 2006). Despite more than a century of study, managerial-

leadership researchers have failed to reach an agreement as to a unitary theory

or definition of the construct (Bass 1990). Instead, a spectrum of theories and

models of managerial-leadership have emerged and exhaustive reviews

continue to be written (see Bass 1990; Daft & Pirola-Merlo 2009; Yukl 2006).

During the Nineteenth Century and into the Twentieth Century, MLE was

thought to be explained solely by individual differences. Great man theories

and trait theories suggested superior managerial-leaders were born with key

personality characteristics (i.e. energy, intelligence, self-confidence, honesty

etc.). This path of inquiry, which was spearheaded by Carlyle (1840/1993) and

pursued for more than 100 years, produced mixed results in establishing a short

list of personality characteristics that would consistently predict superior MLE.

Stogdill (1948) identified some traits that appeared to differentiate leaders from

non-leaders (intelligence, alertness to needs of others, task awareness,

initiative, persistence, and self-confidence) but, otherwise, concluded trait

research was not consistent in predicting MLE (Kirkpatrick & Locke 1991).

The emphasis then shifted to identifying an appropriate balance of task versus

relationship focused behaviours. Examples of these distinctions include

initiating structure versus consideration, as described in the so-called Ohio

State University studies (Kerr et al. 1974), and the employee oriented versus

production oriented behaviour, as described in the so-called University of

Michigan studies (Kahn et al. 1960). These initial models did not incorporate

situational factors and had varied success in identifying behaviours that could

Page 29: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

27

be applied across a range of circumstances. The work of managerial-leaders

was increasingly seen as dynamic and unpredictable, rather than linear, logical

and analytical (Mintzberg 1973). As such, behaviourist models, known as

contingency theories, attempted to isolate the critical situational factors that

shaped leadership and management effectiveness. Effectiveness was thought

to result from the appropriate interaction of a managerial-leader style with the

context in which the leader operated and the followers who were involved.

Examples include Fiedler’s (1967) Contingency Model, Hersey-Blanchard’s

(1969) Life Cycle Theory of Leadership (later termed Situational Leadership

Theory), and House’s (1971) Path-Goal Theory.

The next evolution of thinking in managerial-leadership theory involved a step

change from focusing on the managerial-leader’s personality, behaviours and

contingency skills to include the interactions between the leader and their

followers (Yukl 2006). Relationship theories that emphasised the processes

and structures of influence in the relationship between the managerial-leader

and followers became important in understanding MLE. Examples include

servant (Greenleaf 2007), transformational (Bass 1998) and charismatic

(Conger & Kanungo 1988) leadership theories. Such theories focused on

followers’ values, motivations and the interpersonal dynamics between leaders

and followers. This was a critical shift in the study of leadership, which was now

seen to be a shared meaning-making process and an emotionally laden

phenomenon (Hunt 1999).

In recent decades, trait theories have re-emerged through an increasing interest

in intra-personal capacities (Day & Zaccaro 2007 ; Lord, De Vader & Alliger

1986; Zaccaro 2007). Such capacities are thought to differentiate between

Page 30: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

28

authentic and pseudo transformational leaders (Bass & Steidlmeier 1999; Price

2003; Zaccaro 2007). The emotional, ethical and spiritual capacities of

managerial-leaders are increasingly considered to be central to MLE (Crosby &

Kiedrowski 2008). However, there is an important distinction in the thinking now

when compared with that of a century ago. Managerial-leadership, and some of

the associated attributes, are no longer considered to be something with which

you are born but, rather, as capacities that can be developed and learned

(Bennis 2003; Thomas 2008; Zaccaro 2007). A further distinction can be seen

in the increasing theoretical foundation for current trait-based research in

contrast to research that was undertaken in the early Twentieth Century, which

was largely atheoretical (House & Aditya 1997).

In a review of trait-based perspectives of leadership, Zaccaro (2007, p. 6)

argued for the inclusion of multiple leader attributes that were organised “in a

coherent and meaningful conceptual construction” and that the interaction

effects of attributes should be examined. For example, the model of leader

attributes and leader performance includes distal traits, such as cognitive

abilities, proximal attributes, such as social appraisal skills, and the managerial-

leader’s environment (Zaccaro, Kemp & Bader 2004). This is just one of many

taxonomies suggested for organising individual differences related to MLE

(Bass 1990; Locke et al. 1991; Mumford et al. 2000; Yukl 2006). The current

challenge for trait-based research is to explore multivariate predictions (Lord,

De Vader & Alliger 1986; Zaccaro 2007) and to test “not only on multiple

personal attributes but also on how these attributes work together to influence

performance” (Zaccaro, Kemp & Bader 2004, p. 8). Towards this end, Tischler,

Biberman and McKeage (2002) argue the case for exploring the relationship

Page 31: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

29

between performance, EI and complimentary constructs that might stand as

proxies for spiritual intelligence.

The approach to understanding MLE has returned to the beginning with the re-

emergence of interest in individual differences. The obvious question then, is

what are the critical trait-based antecedents? The prevalence of traditional

intelligence (IQ) as an important antecedent to MLE has withstood the test of

time, as detailed in Section 2.4 below. Leaders require IQ in order to analyse

and to solve problems using logic. IQ is critical to the process of strategic and

operational planning. Setting objectives/goals and working through strategies

towards their achievement requires processing and integrating information from

a variety of sources. In fact, higher levels of IQ would benefit the analysis

phase of any type of managerial problem (i.e. staffing, production, development

or marketing etc.). However, the literature suggests that traditional intelligence

(IQ) has a positive but small relationship with leadership (Stogdill, 1969). IQ

appears to be threshold skill; a minimum level is required for effective

leadership after which, other competencies, such as emotional intelligence,

contribute more to predicting effective leadership (George 2000; Leban & Zulauf

2004; Rosete & Ciarrochi 2005).

Emotional intelligence (EI) is the ability to recognise, understand, use and

manage emotions in oneself and others (Mayer, Caruso & Salovey 1999b). EI

underpins a number of leadership competencies: accurate self-assessment,

emotional self-control, empathy, the ability to read, attune, empathise with and

influence other’s emotional states. Leaders with accurate assessment of their

own abilities are able to contribute from their strengths and to leverage others’

strengths to compensate for their weaknesses. Leaders without emotional self-

Page 32: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

30

control are at the mercy of their emotional reactions and run the risk of seriously

damaging their credibility and even their career by simply being in the wrong

place at the wrong time. Finally, the ability to empathise and influence others is

central to creating effective relationships, managing constructive conflict and

achieving through other people. The case for EI as an important antecedent to

MLE is made in Section 2.5 below.

When leaders possess both IQ and EI abilities, they bring a broader range of

skills to their role, but to what end? Logic, reason and emotional savvy are no

longer enough. Drath and Palus (1994, p. 13) ‘refer to leadership as a social

meaning-making process’. The premise is effective managers will require very

specific meaning-making abilities in addition to intellectual and emotional

abilities (King & Nicol 1999; Tischler, Biberman & McKeage 2002). Drawing on

the work of Ken Wilber, Young (2002a, p. 35), describes meaning-making

capacity as different levels of consciousness:

..as individuals progress or move to higher levels of consciousness, their perspectives enlarge, their identities broaden and both become more comprehensive… they engage in Wilber’s definition of transformative spirituality ….each stage (has) new dimensions of existence, modes of learning, desires, fears apperceptions, motivations, moral sensibilities (Wilber 2000).

A higher, or more complex, level of consciousness leads to the capacity for

more complex meaning-making and is critical to the way that individuals

perceive the world (Loevinger 1976; Cook-Greuter 2004). It follows that a more

complex meaning-making structure would enable a leader to recognise,

facilitate meaning and be of service at increasing levels of complexity (Rooke &

Torbert 2005). As leaders move to increasing levels of consciousness, they

gain a higher awareness of their mind/body connections; conduct more

Page 33: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

31

complete environmental and stakeholders analysis; understand the

perspectives of all stakeholders better; utilise a more encompassing and

therefore enhanced morality; leverage enhanced states of intuition and

synchronicity; and ultimately effect transformation in others (Young 2002a). As

Rooke and Torbert (2005, p. 67) noted:

Most developmental psychologists agree that what differentiates leaders is not so much their philosophy of leadership, their personality, or their style of management. Rather, it’s their internal ‘action logic’ – how they interpret their surroundings and react when their power or safety is challenged.

Consequently, Section 2.6 below presents the argument that MMS is an

individual difference that has predictive validity over and above IQ and EI.

In summary, the above discussion has presented a brief review of the history of

MLE starting with and returning to an interest in trait-based constructs. The

current challenge for researchers of trait-based antecedents to MLE is to

explore multivariate attributes and also interaction effects. Towards this end the

case was made for the study of a multivariate antecedent to MLE that includes

an intellectual, emotional and meaning-making dimension. What follows next is

a detailed discussion of each of the aspects of the proposed multivariate

antecedent to MLE: IQ, EI and the capacity for meaning-making.

2.4 INTELLIGENCE (IQ)

There have been different explanations for the basis of human intelligence

since the beginning of recorded history, including its location in the heart, liver

and head (Gardner 1983). A single definition for the construct is elusive, as is

reflected in a statement by a group of prominent psychologists:

Page 34: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

32

Individuals differ from one another in their ability to understand complex ideas, to adapt effectively to the environment, to learn from experience, to engage in various forms of reasoning, to overcome obstacles by taking thought…Concepts of ‘intelligence’ are attempts to clarify and organize this complex set of phenomena. Although considerable clarity has been achieved in some areas, no such conceptualization has yet answered all the important questions, and none commands universal assent. Indeed, when two dozen prominent theorists were recently asked to define intelligence, they gave two dozen, somewhat different, definitions (Neisser et al. 1996, p. 77).

Modern intelligence theories are varied, but they generally fall within two

categories that suggest intelligence is a unitary quality of mind, (which is often

referred to as ‘g’) or that intelligence is a set of independent factors (Sternberg

1990). Charles Spearman (1904) pioneered the former argument and

suggested intelligence consists of two factors, which he termed general

intelligence and specific intelligence. The first factor reflected a person’s

general pool of mental energy, which is consistent with the concept of ‘g’, while

the second factor, often referred to as ‘s’, represents a person’s level of mental

energy that is specific to the particular domain of knowledge being tested

(Spearman 1927). Today ‘g’ is commonly referred to as cognitive ability,

general mental ability (GMA) or the g-factor (Viswesvaran & Ones 2002). It has

been defined as “the aggregate or global capacity of the individual to act

purposefully, to think rationally, and to deal effectively with his environment”

(Wechsler 1944, p. 3).

Measures of general mental ability are commonly referred to as a person’s

intelligence quotient (IQ). The first measure of IQ, the Metrical Scale of

Intelligence (Binet & Simon 1905) was devised to identify children who were at

risk of failing within the standard school system. In the past century alternative

tests have been developed to measure IQ, more specific cognitive abilities and

Page 35: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

33

also related constructs, such as scholastic aptitude (Neisser et al. 1996).

However, measures of IQ are thought to be better predictors of performance

than are measures of specific abilities, such as mathematical reasoning, verbal

aptitude, memory or perceptual speed (Ceci & Williams 2007). Despite

differences in content, various IQ tests correlate positively with each other,

which supports the ‘g’ concept of intelligence (Neisser et al. 1996).

Despite differences of opinion regarding the structure and processes involved in

cognitive ability, experts in the field tend to agree on the fundamentals (Eysenck

2000). For example, Cattell’s (1963; 1987) distinction between fluid and

crystallised intelligence is widely accepted. Fluid intelligence represents the

power of reasoning and ability to process information. Fluid intelligence peaks

within a person’s first 20 years of life and remains relatively constant after that,

with the exception of a decline in old age. Crystallised intelligence is consistent

with the concept of ‘s’ that was proposed by Spearman and represents

specialised knowledge that is context specific (i.e. an ability to write an

academic paper). This form of intelligence can continue to expand with use

throughout life. However there are at least three controversies that continue to

plague IQ researchers, namely race differences (Gottfredson 2005; Hunt &

Carlson 2007; Rushton & Jensen 2005; Rushton & Jensen 2006; Sternberg

2005), gender differences (Irwing & Lynn 2006; Jackson & Rushton 2006; Lynn

& Irwing 2004) and a suggestion of an increasing level of intelligence in the

general population, often dubbed the Flynn effect (Flynn 1984; Sundet, Barlaug

& Torjussen 2004 ). These issues represent a continued debate about the

relative importance of heritability and the environment in predicting peoples’ IQ

scores.

Page 36: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

34

IQ tests were the subject of much criticism in the 1960s and 1970s, with claims

of bias and limited validity (McClelland 1973). Advances in meta-analytic

procedures facilitated the improvement of counter arguments and provided

statistical support for the predictive relationship between IQ and work outcomes

(Bertua, Anderson & Salgado 2005; Hülsheger, Maier & Stumpp 2007; Hunter,

Schmidt & Judiesch 1990; Hunter 1986; Hunter & Hunter 1984; Schmidt &

Hunter 1998; Schmidt & Hunter 2004). Currently, measures of general

cognitive ability are thought to predict job performance across a spectrum of

occupations and to be one of best predictors of success in applied settings

(Ones, Viswesvaran & Dilchert 2005; Salgado et al. 2003). Further, the

strength of the relationship between IQ and performance seems to increase

with job complexity (Schmidt & Hunter 1998).

2.4.1 IQ and MLE

There is strong support for a relationship between IQ and managerial-

leadership (Bass 1990; Fiedler & Garcia 1987; Lord, De Vader & Alliger 1986;

Mann 1959; Stogdill 1948). The need to deal with complexity is one of the

theoretical underpinnings for the relationship between IQ and MLE (Fiedler &

Garcia 1987; Locke et al. 1991). The role of creativity in effective problem

solving and working with and through others also supports this relationship (Kim

2008; Simonton 2008). At a strategic level, managerial-leaders have to

engineer sustainable competitive advantage, align purposes and values and

harness the human potential of their organisation. Operationally, managers

must engage a broader, more educated and diverse set of stakeholders

including employees, customers, shareholders and various related interest

groups. In their re-examination of Mann’s (1959) meta-analysis using modern

Page 37: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

35

statistical methods, Lord et al. (1986) found IQ was correlated (r= .50) with

perceptions of managerial-leadership effectiveness. Further, cognitive ability

seemed to predict managerial-leadership success 20 years down the track

(Howard & Bray 1988).

It is important to remember the phenomenon of intelligence is not the same as

IQ (Nettelbeck & Wilson 2005). A ubiquitous belief in the primacy of intelligence

underpins an implicit leadership theory within followers which has been found to

contribute to the relationship between IQ and MLE beyond the tangible

advantages that have already been discussed (Judge, Colbert & Ilies 2004;

Rubin, Bartels & Bommer 2002). Rubin et al. (2002) found that the perceived-

intelligence of an managerial-leader was not equivalent to their IQ and that both

IQ and self-monitoring skills assisted a managerial-leader. Judge et al. (2004)

found a positive relationship between IQ, perceived-intelligence and

perceptions of MLE.

The terms ‘book-smart’ and ‘street-smart’ are familiar in lay vocabulary and

reflect the intuitive notion that IQ alone does not predict real world success.

Indeed, IQ alone may have a curvilinear relationship with performance unless

moderated by social and/or emotional abilities (Zaccaro 2007). Neisser (1976)

explored the difference between practical intelligence (PQ) and academic

intelligence (IQ) and suggested there were significant differences in the nature

of the tasks that were best addressed by each intelligence. However, IQ

continues to be one of the most significant individual differences predictors of

MLE (Van Rooy & Viswesvaran 2004). At the same time it is clear IQ is not

sufficient if we are to fully understand the phenomenon of MLE. A number of

theorists have suggested the need for a broader conceptualisation and see

Page 38: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

36

intelligence as a multi-faceted construct with abilities that are relatively

independent of one another.

An early proponent of multiple intelligence theory was Thorndike (1920) who will

be discussed in more detail in Section 2.4.1. Thurstone (1938) claimed that

intelligence was made up of seven primary mental abilities, which he termed

verbal comprehension, verbal fluency, numbers, spatial visualisation, memory,

reasoning, perceptual speed. Guilford (1967) suggested there were 120

different intelligences. His view was based on a three dimensional view of

basic mental abilities, which he termed operations (the act of thinking), contents

(the terms in which we think such as words or symbols) and products (the ideas

we come up with). In a much simpler model, Gardner (1983) suggested there

were seven types of intelligence, which he termed verbal, mathematical-logical,

spatial, kinaesthetic, musical, interpersonal and intrapersonal. Following this

line of thinking, many theorists have suggested expanded views of intelligence

(Das, Naglieri & Kirby 1994; Sternberg 1988).

In summary, IQ is an important individual difference construct that contributes to

individual job performance and to MLE. IQ has consistently predicted variations

in MLE and, consequently, studies that explore the relationship between

individual differences and MLE need to include a measure of IQ in order to test

the incremental predictive validity of any additional constructs.

2.5 EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE (EI)

Awareness of, and interest in, the relationship between emotional intelligence

(EI) and MLE has grown exponentially since the 1990s. However, the construct

Page 39: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

37

itself has a long and somewhat controversial history. The background to the EI

concept is presented in the next section, followed by a discussion of alternate

models of EI, a review of the key controversies in the field and a summary of

current understanding regarding the relationship between the ability model of EI

and MLE.

2.5.1 Background of the EI Construct

In order to understand the concept of emotional intelligence (EI) it is important

to appreciate its historical roots, starting with Thorndike’s (1920, p. 228) notion

of social intelligence, which he saw as “the ability to understand and manage

men and women… to act wisely in human relations”. Social intelligence was

seen as part of intelligence in its multi-faceted form (Thorndike 1920; Thurstone

1938). It has been argued that the psychological “ability to judge correctly the

feelings, moods, motivations of individuals” (Wedeck 1947, p. 133) is distinct

from personality. However, after some initial attempts to define and measure

social intelligence (Moss & Hunt 1927; Vernon 1933), the concept was declared

untenable (Cronbach 1960). An important shift in focus from ‘social’ to

‘emotional’ intelligence began in the 1960s when it was suggested that

emotions “do not at all deserve being put into opposition with

‘intelligence’…they are, it seems, themselves a high order of intelligence”

(Mowrer 1960, p. 307 ).

Research on social intelligence waned until theories suggesting a broader

conceptualization of intelligence, such as the structure of intellect theory

(Guilford 1967) and multiple intelligences (Gardner 1983), gained prominence.

At least two separate fields of study emerged, namely the specific study of

Page 40: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

38

interactions between emotions and cognition and the expansion of the concept

of intelligence (Jordan et al. 2002). Gardner (1983, p. 43) suggested two

distinct emotionally laden intelligences as part of a spectrum of multiple

intelligences, with interpersonal intelligence being defined as “a person’s

capacity to understand the intentions, motivations and desires of other people

and consequently to work effectively with others” and intrapersonal intelligence

being defined as “the capacity to understand oneself – including one’s own

desires, fears, and capacities – and to use such information effectively in

regulating one’s own life”.

The concept of emotional intelligence was formally introduced into the

academic literature when Salovey and Mayer (1990, p. 189) presented their

ability-based model of emotional intelligence (EI), defining it initially as:

A subset of social intelligence that involves the ability to monitor one’s own and other’s feelings and emotions, to discriminate among them, and to use this information to guide one’s thinking and action.

The new label of EI focused attention on “the relatedness of several aspects of

emotion processing that together contribute to social psychological functioning”

(Jordan et al. 2006, p. 200). Salovey and Mayer continued to develop the

ability model of EI that was explicitly placed at the intersection of the two fields

of emotion and intelligence (Mayer & Salovey 1993; Mayer & Geher 1996;

Mayer, Caruso & Salovey 1998; Mayer, Salovey & Caruso 2000; Salovey et al.

1995) and presented an evidenced-based argument that EI met the standards

of a form of intelligence (Mayer, Caruso & Salovey 1999a; Mayer et al. 2001).

However, many researchers were reluctant to embrace the construct.

Notwithstanding, and actually predating this reluctance, Goleman (1995)

Page 41: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

39

popularised the term ‘EI’ in his best-selling book that laid the foundation for

alternative EI models.

2.5.2 Alternate Models of EI

The existence of alternate models of EI is a central controversy in the literature:

…the construct of EI has become fractured in the struggle between the scientist trying to develop a valid psychological construct on the one hand, and marketers attempting to develop a commercially viable psychological framework on the other. (Jordan et al. 2006, pg 198)

While the field of psychology has been studying the intersection of affect and

cognition for more than 30 years (Forgas 2001), the exploration of EI is

relatively new, particularly when compared to other constructs, such as IQ, or

personality variables, such as the Big Five (Van Rooy & Viswesvaran 2004).

Some see the continuing search for a clear, universally accepted definition and

measure of EI as elusive (Matthews, Roberts & Zeidner 2004), while others

believe this search is consistent with the natural evolution of scientific inquiry

(Antonakis, Ashkanasy & Dasborough 2009; Ashkanasy & Daus 2005). At

present, the literature separates the EI field into two distinct paradigms. In one,

EI is seen to be a mental ability, as conceptualised by Mayer and Salovey

(1990) while, in the other, EI is seen as a mixture of traits, competencies and

skills (Bar-On 1997; Sala 2002; Schutte et al. 1998).

2.5.2.1 The Expanded View: Mixed Models of EI

Central to the EI literature over the past two decades has been the emergence

and commercial dominance of mixed models of EI, as described by Mayer

(2008, p. 504) who noted:

Page 42: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

40

Subsequent interpreters of our work, however were instrumental to (what we regard as) unmooring the concept from its key terms. These interpreters appear to have confused what we thought of as expressions of EI with the ability itself.

The mixed model paradigm conceptualises EI in a variety of ways, including as:

• A collection of non-cognitive capabilities, competencies and skills (Bar-

On 1997); traits and competencies (Cooper & Sawaf 1997; Goleman

1998b).

• Personal characteristics and behaviours (Dulewicz & Higgs 1998;

Dulewicz & Higgs 2000; Dulewicz, Higgs & Slaski 2003).

• Behavioural tendencies (Pérez, Petrides & Furnham 2005; Petrides &

Furnham 2001).

With such broad conceptualisations, mixed models failed to attain content

validity beyond established measures of personality, such as IQ or the Big Five

(Brackett & Mayer 2003; Dawda & Hart 2000; Mayer, Roberts & Barsade 2008;

Petrides & Furnham 2001; Rosete & Ciarrochi 2005). The use of mixed models

in commercial settings, along with extravagant and unsubstantiated claims of

EI’s predictive validity (Goleman 1995; Goleman 1998a) has led to reduced

credibility for the EI construct (Landy 2005).

A key criticism of mixed models is the use of self-judgement scores to assess

EI. The use of the term ‘intelligence’ implies that what is being measured is a

mental ability. However, self-judgement and ability measures are conceptually

and empirically different (Brackett & Mayer 2003; Brackett et al. 2006; Mayer &

Page 43: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

41

Salovey 1997; Mayer, Caruso & Salovey 1999a; Mayer, Caruso & Salovey

2000; Mayer, Salovey & Caruso 2000).

Low correlations between self-report and ability measure of EI suggested they

are different constructs (Ashkanasy & Dasborough 2003; Bastian, Burns &

Nettelbeck 2005; Brackett & Mayer 2003; Brackett et al. 2006; Brannick et al.

2009; Karim & Weisz 2010; Lopes, Salovey & Straus 2003; Matthews, Zeidner

& Roberts 2002; Rosete & Ciarrochi 2005; Van Rooy & Viswesvaran 2004;

Warwick & Nettelbeck 2004). Another concern is the issue of common method

variance when researchers use self-judgement measures for both antecedent

and dependent variables (Barbuto Jr & Burbach 2006; Gardner & Stough 2002;

Newcombe & Ashkanasy 2002; Schutte et al. 2001; Sy, Tram & O’Hara 2006;

Wong & Law 2002). Both of these criticisms extend to ‘Stream 2’ models and

measures of EI (Daus & Ashkanasy 2005) that, although based on the Mayer

and Salovey (1997) ability model, use self and/or peer judgment methodologies.

Examples include the Trait-Meta-Mood-Scale (Salovey et al. 1995), the

Workgroup Emotional Intelligence Profile (Jordan et al. 2002), Wong & Law’s

self-report measures of EI (Wong & Law 2002) or the Assessing Emotions

Scale (AES) (Schutte et al. 1998).

The growing academic view is that mixed models do not qualify as measures of

intelligence (Mayer, Roberts & Barsade 2008; Van Rooy & Viswesvaran 2004;

Zeidner, Matthews & Roberts 2004); that they lack discriminant and incremental

validity from existing valid and reliable constructs such as the Big Five

(Cartwright, Pappas & West 2008; Davies, Stankov & Roberts 1998; MacCann

et al. 2003; Matthews, Zeidner & Roberts 2002; Warwick & Nettelbeck 2004);

and that they are more susceptible to faking on the part of respondents (J.

Page 44: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

42

Ciarrochi, A.Y. Chan & P. Caputi 2000; Day & Carroll 2008; J.V. Ciarrochi, A.Y.

Chan & P. Caputi 2000). Increasingly there have been calls for delimiting the

label of EI exclusively for ability models (Ashkanasy & Daus 2005; Brackett &

Mayer 2003; Mayer, Roberts & Barsade 2008).

2.5.2.2 The Narrow View: Ability Models of EI

The ability paradigm assumes specific mental abilities exist that are associated

with processing and integrating emotional information that can be measured

(Mayer, Roberts & Barsade 2008). Ability-based measures use performance

based tasks that can be assessed as being completed correctly or not (Mayer &

Salovey 1997; Mayer, Caruso & Salovey 1999a; Mayer, Caruso & Salovey

2000; Mayer, Salovey & Caruso 2000). This seems straight forward in theory

but, in practice, “there is considerable difficulty in determining objectively correct

responses to stimuli involving emotional content and in applying truly veridical

criteria in scoring tasks of emotional ability” (Roberts, Zeidner & Matthews 2001,

p. 201-202). Mayer et al (2000) argued that there are evolutionary and cultural

foundations for establishing correct responses to tests of emotional ability.

Three common methods are target criteria (asking the target individual

displaying the emotional stimuli what they are feeling), expert criteria (using a

panel of experts in emotion), and consensus criteria (the most common

response of a large sample of people).

Emotional abilities can be studied individually or as an integrated whole (Mayer,

Roberts & Barsade 2008). Measures of individual emotional abilities include

the Diagnostic Analysis of Non-Verbal Accuracy (DANVA-2), which measures

people’s ability to discriminate emotional cues (Nowicki & Duke 1994); the

Page 45: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

43

Profile of Non-Verbal Sensitivity (PONS), which measures people’s ability to

accurately decode non-verbal emotional cues (Rosenthal 1979); the Japanese

and Caucasian Brief Affect Recognition Test (JACBART), which measures

people’s ability to discriminate emotional cues (Matsumoto et al. 2000); and the

Levels of Emotional Awareness Scale, which measures people’s ability to

accurately detect and discriminate emotional signals in oneself and others

(Lane et al. 1990)

In contrast to these scales, there are two integrated EI models (Mayer, Roberts

& Barsade 2008). The first model is based on a theory of Emotional Adaptation

(EA), which is related to “the functioning of the emotion systems and their

distinct motivational properties”, as opposed to an intelligence (Izard et al. 2001,

p. 252). EA is operationalised in this case as Emotional Knowledge (EK) and

measured though the Emotional Knowledge Test (EKT). This provides a

performance score on two abilities, namely, emotional perception and labelling

(Izard et al. 2001).

The second model expands the focus beyond the knowledge of emotions.

Salovey and Mayer’s (1990) model is now known as the four-branch ability

model that suggests EI is made up of an ability to:

1. Accurately perceive emotions in self and others.

2. Use emotions to facilitate thought.

3. Understand emotion.

Page 46: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

44

4. Regulate and manage emotions in self and others (Mayer & Salovey

1997).

The Salovey and Mayer (1990) EI model was originally measured using the

Multifactor Emotional Intelligence Scale (MEIS), which included 402-items

across four subscales (Mayer, Caruso & Salovey 1999a). The MEIS was

criticised because of differences between the expert and consensus scoring

methods and because of the low reliability of some of the subscales (Conte

2005; Roberts, Zeidner & Matthews 2001; Wong & Law 2002).

In response to the latter concern, the shorter and more reliable Mayer-Salovey-

Caruso-Emotional-Intelligence-Test version 2 (Mayer, Caruso & Salovey 2000)

was introduced to researchers in 2000 and published for more general use in

2002 as the MSCEITv2 (Mayer, Salovey & Caruso 2002). This version of ability

EI seemed to have acceptable discriminant validity from traditional measures of

intelligence (J. Ciarrochi, A.Y. Chan & P. Caputi 2000; Farrelly & Austin 2007;

Lopes, Salovey & Straus 2003; Mayer, Salovey & Caruso 2004; O'Connor &

Little 2003; Schulte, Ree & Carretta 2004; Roberts et al. 2006; Rosete &

Ciarrochi 2005; J.V. Ciarrochi, A.Y. Chan & P. Caputi 2000) and personality

(Farrelly & Austin 2007; Lopes, Salovey & Straus 2003; Lopes et al. 2004;

Mayer, Salovey & Caruso 2004; Rode et al. 2008; Rosete & Ciarrochi 2005).

The lack of relationship between the MSCEITv2 and Raven’s Progressive

Matrices may be evidence that ability-EI is more like crystalised than fluid

intelligence (Farrelly & Austin 2007).

Ability EI has been found to be associated with better social relations for

children and adults, positive perceptions by others, better family and intimate

Page 47: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

45

relationships, academic achievement, better social relations at work and in

negotiations and better psychological well-being (Mayer, Roberts & Barsade

2008). In addition, evidence is amassing that ability EI has incremental

predictive validity. After controlling for cognitive ability and personality, EI, as

measured by the MSCEITv2, was significant in predicting lower alcohol use

(Brackett, Mayer & Warner 2004; Rossen & Kranzler 2009); a lower incidence

of deviant behaviour (Brackett & Mayer 2003); positive relationships with others

(Rossen & Kranzler 2009); lower anxiety (Bastian, Burns & Nettelbeck 2005)

and fewer psychiatric symptoms of distress (David 2005). EI, however, was not

a significant incremental predictor of academic grade-point-average (GPA) or

life satisfaction and has been found to be more correlated with social desirability

than were IQ or long term affect (Rode et al. 2008).

Nevertheless, ability EI is empirically distinct from measures of social

desirability (Rode et al. 2008) and appears to increase with age (Kafetsios &

Zampetakis 2008; Mayer, Caruso & Salovey 1999a; Palmer et al. 2005).

However, the “the underlying process remains somewhat vague” (Lindebaum

2009, p. 226). It appears there are gender differences with regard to ability EI

with some evidence that women score higher than men on some branch scores

(Bay & McKeage 2006; Day & Carroll 2004; Kafetsios & Zampetakis 2008;

Lyons & Schneider 2005). There are also significant positive relationships

emerging for men only on some outcome measures (Brackett, Mayer & Warner

2004; Brackett et al. 2006).

In summary, the Mayer and Salovey (1997) ability model has emerged as the

dominant paradigm for scientific inquiry into EI:

Page 48: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

46

Thus far, the measurement evidence tends to favour the ability-based EI approach described here over other research alternatives (such as dismissing EI or using mixed models). (Mayer, Roberts & Barsade 2008, p. 510)

The MSCEITV2 v2.0, appears to have greater convergent, discriminant and

incremental validity than any of the mixed models of EI (Brackett & Mayer 2003;

Daus & Ashkanasy 2005) and is the most accurate test currently available for

measuring the four-branch EI ability model (Rossen & Kranzler 2009).

However, as with any emerging area of scientific inquiry, there are clear

differences of opinion regarding the ability EI model and measure. These are

summarised in the next section.

2.5.3 Theoretical Controversies

The construct of EI is not without its critics (Antonakis 2004; Antonakis,

Ashkanasy & Dasborough 2009; Fiori & Antonakis 2011; Gignac 2005; Palmer

et al. 2005; Roberts et al. 2006; Rossen & Kranzler 2008). First, the very

existence of the construct is still being debated. Critics suggest there is a lack of

convincing evidence to support the claim that EI meets the criteria of an

intelligence and that emotion and cognition cannot be studied as separate

constructs (Antonakis, Ashkanasy & Dasborough 2009; Conte 2005; Landy

2005; Matthews, Roberts & Zeidner 2004). In response, supporters of ability EI

have suggested the case has been argued sufficiently in the research effort to

date (Antonakis, Ashkanasy & Dasborough 2009; Ashkanasy & Daus 2005;

Cote & Miners 2006; Gohm 2004; Mayer 2001; Mayer, Salovey & Caruso 2004;

Mayer, Roberts & Barsade 2008; Mayer, Salovey & Caruso 2008) and that a

failure to accept the available evidence is symptomatic of a tenacious belief in

Page 49: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

47

‘g’ and an unwillingness to admit the debate has moved onto establishing

reliable and valid EI measures.

An enduring and erroneous concern often raised is the supposed lack of a

singular definition and measure of EI (Conte 2005; Landy 2005; Locke 2005).

This is based on the failure to discriminate between the mixed and ability

models, as was discussed earlier in this chapter (Ashkanasy & Daus 2005;

Locke 2005).

A second key criticism involves operationalisation. Researchers who have

explored the EI construct agree existing measures can and should be improved.

A specific limitation of current ability measures is that they do not put

respondents in situations in which they experience real emotions (Ashkanasy &

Daus 2005; Ashton-James 2003). For example, there may be a method effect

related to the scales used to measure the fourth branch of the ability EI model

(i.e. manage emotions) because the test-taker provides their considered

response to a hypothetical situation rather than performing a specific task

(Rode et al. 2008). Additionally there is some confusion as to whether ability EI

is best modelled and measured as a global score or as distinct abilities (Lopes

et al. 2004; Maul 2011; Palmer et al. 2005). In addition, the measurement

design of the MSCEITv2 has drawn criticism with regard to its generalisability

(Follesdal & Hagtvet 2009). The measurement properties of the MSCEITv2 are

discussed in more detail in the measures section of Chapter 3.

The scoring methodology used by the MSCEITV2 is a limitation that is

recognised by both EI critics and advocates. Despite its inherent limitations,

proponents of the MSCEITv2 have argued that consensus/expert scoring can

Page 50: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

48

provide reliable indicators of individual differences (Ashkanasy & Daus 2005;

Mayer, Salovey & Caruso 2004). Critics have argued that consensus scoring is

“in direct contrast to traditional measures of intelligence where an objective

measure of truth is considered” (Matthews, Zeidner & Roberts 2002, p. 186).

Further, they have suggested that, because consensus scores are based on

modal responses, they may not provide accurate scores for individuals with

above average EI (Conte 2005; Matthews, Zeidner & Roberts 2002; Matthews,

Roberts & Zeidner 2004). Finally, critics have suggested that this type of

measurement produces what is known as difference scores, which can “suffer

from unreliability, ambiguous interpretation, confounded effects and untested

constraints, thus resulting in potentially flawed findings” (Antonakis, Ashkanasy

& Dasborough 2009, p. 250). Antonakis (2009) recommends an analysis

method to see whether the difference between a respondent’s score and that of

the consensus/expert score is predicted by their IQ. However, this approach is

not commonly employed as yet.

There are a number of suggestions within the literature for moving forward.

First, the need to use more representative samples by moving beyond

populations of children, adolescents or college students is well recognised

(Landy 2005). It is also important for researchers to include control variables of

IQ and/or personality in order to facilitate the assessment of incremental

predictive validity beyond these antecedents (Landy 2005). Similarly, there is

interest in undertaking research that explores the relationship between EI and

actual performance, rather than self-reported behaviour (Rossen & Kranzler

2009). Finally, many studies have used cross-sectional designs that cannot

unequivocally demonstrate EI leads to dependent variables (Landy 2005). To

Page 51: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

49

do so would require randomised control trials and, given the claim that a

person’s EI can be developed, longitudinal data.

Daus and Ashkanasy (2005, p. 457) reviewed the stages involved in developing

and validating a new construct and have concluded that:

Mayer, Caruso and Salovey are to be applauded in that it has only been a decade and a half since the construct/term was first introduced and they have developed a solid and comprehensive measure, in addition to amassing considerable evidence/data regarding the psychometric and predictive properties.

Such differences of opinion are examples of a healthy debate on a robust

construct, with the general opinion that ability measures “exhibit test validity as

a group… [and that]...EI is a predictor of significant outcomes across diverse

samples in a number of real-world domains” (Mayer, Roberts & Barsade 2008,

p. 527). Researchers on both sides of the EI debate agree it is important to

explore the relationship between this construct and important outcome

variables, such as MLE (Antonakis, Ashkanasy & Dasborough 2009). As such,

the next section summarises our current understanding of this relationship.

2.5.4 Ability EI and MLE

Leadership is intrinsically an emotional process, whereby leaders recognize followers emotional states, attempt to evoke emotions in followers, and then seek to manage followers’ emotional states accordingly. (Kerr et al. 2006, pg 268)

Over recent decades a clearly articulated argument has emerged suggesting

managerial-leadership is an emotion-laden process and that EI is a key factor in

MLE (Ashkanasy & Tse 2000; Boal & Hooijberg 2000; Dasborough &

Ashkanasy 2002; George 2000; House & Aditya 1997; Humphrey 2002;

Megerian & Sosik 1996). At a foundational level, EI underpins individual

Page 52: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

50

effectiveness (George 2000; Mayer & Salovey 1993; Mayer & Salovey 1997;

Salovey & Mayer 1990; Salovey et al. 1999; Salovey, Mayer & Caruso 2002).

For example, EI contributes to an individual’s ability to perceive and use

emotional information which can have a positive impact on cognitive decision-

making (Day & Carroll 2004) and individuals with greater EI are better equipped

to cope with emotionally challenging situations (Salovey et al. 1999).

A recent meta-analysis provides empirical support for the relationship between

EI and job performance (O'Boyle Jr et al. 2010). In a study of clerical and

administrative staff, evidence suggests that after controlling for age, education,

verbal ability and the Big Five personality traits, EI is associated with greater

stress tolerance, merit increases, higher company rank and better peer and/or

supervisor ratings of interpersonal facilitation (Lopes et al. 2006). Ability EI also

explains a significant amount of the variance in public speaking effectiveness, a

task that requires the ability to calm one’s nerves in a stressful situations and an

ability to include appropriate levels of emotion in order to influence listeners

(Rode et al. 2007). EI also has social utility in terms of the negotiation process

in that the ability to understand emotions “positively predicts one’s counterpart’s

outcome satisfaction” (Mueller & Curhan 2006, p. 122) and emotion recognition

has predictive validity in negotiation performance outcomes (Elfenbein et al.

2007).

EI abilities also seem to be integral to developing relationships (Ashkanasy &

Tse 2000; Pescosolido 2002). Studies exploring the ripple effect of emotion

and its influence on group behaviour have provided evidence that positive

emotional contagion at a group level exists, and can lead to “improved

cooperation, decreased conflict, and increased perception of task performance”

Page 53: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

51

(Barsade 2002, p. 3). Emotional regulation is also positively associated with the

quality of social interactions (Lopes et al. 2005; Rossen & Kranzler 2009).

Beyond indices of individual effectiveness, EI is also thought to contribute to

MLE. Transformational and charismatic styles of leadership, which engage at

an emotional level, can create strong affiliations from followers (Conger 1990;

Megerian & Sosik 1996). George (2000) identified five areas in which ability EI

enhances MLE, namely:

1. Developing a collective sense of goals and how to go about achieving

them.

2. Instilling knowledge and appreciation of the importance of work activities

and behaviours in others.

3. Generating and maintaining excitement, enthusiasm, confidence and

optimism in an organization as well as trust and cooperation.

4. Encouraging flexibility in decision making and change.

5. Establishing and maintaining a meaningful identify for an organization.

Prati et al. (2003, p. 27) suggested EI enables managerial-leaders to “induce

collective motivation in team members”, which can impact on the performance

of their team. The extent to which a leader experiences positive moods has

also been found to be correlated with pro-social behaviour and better quality

customer service (George & Bettenhausen 1990; George 1995). Similarly,

Jordan et al. (2006, p. 8) found “teams with members who were able to regulate

Page 54: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

52

their experience and expression of emotions achieved a higher performance

than those teams where members were not able to control their emotions”.

While the theoretical relationship between EI and MLE has been clearly

articulated, empirical evidence to support these claims is still mixed (Walter,

Cole & Humphrey 2011). Much of the exploration of the relationship between

EI and MLE has used trait-based or self/other-reported measures of EI which,

as was discussed earlier in this chapter, are not sufficiently dissimilar to

established personality measures (see Barbuto Jr & Burbach 2006; Barling,

Slater & Kelloway 2000; Dulewicz & Higgs 1999; Gardner & Stough 2002;

Mandell & Pherwani 2003; Palmer et al. 2001; Sears & Holmvall 2009; Skinner

& Spurgeon 2005; Sosik & Megerian 1999; Wong & Law 2002). Other valid

concerns include the lack of samples that “avoid problems associated with

common-source/methods variance…[and]…use practising leaders in real world

contexts” (Antonakis, Ashkanasy & Dasborough 2009, p. 249). For example, in

a student sample, EI predicted leader emergence and was related to

transformational leadership (Daus & Harris 2003).

However, studies examining the relationship between ability EI and measures

of MLE with practising managerial-leaders are limited and have produced mixed

results including those that support the hypothesised relationship (Byron 2007;

Cote et al. 2010; Clarke 2010; Kerr et al. 2006; Leban & Zulauf 2004; Rosete &

Ciarrochi 2005; Rubin, Munz & Bommer 2005), and those that do not (Byron

2007; Collins 2001; Weinberger 2009). A summary table of these studies can

be found in Appendix A.

Page 55: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

53

Collins (2001) did not find support for the hypothesised relationship between

ability EI and three measures of leadership success (multi-source feedback,

position and salary). Similarly, using a sample of managers from a multinational

organisation, Weinberger (2009) tested the relationship between ability EI and

transformational, transactional and laissez-fair leadership styles but did not find

any significant relationships.

In contrast, Leban and Zulauf (Leban & Zulauf 2004) found significant

relationships between facets of ability EI and subscale scores of

transformational leadership. Similarly, Rubin (2005) found the ability to

recognise emotions positively predicted subordinates’ ratings of

transformational leadership behaviours and that this main effect was moderated

by the personality trait of extraversion. Other researchers have found ability EI

significantly predicted supervisors’ ratings of employees’ job performance

(Janovics & Christiansen 2001). Rosete and Ciarrochi (2005) also explored

the relationship between ability EI and performance ratings. Significant

correlations were found between the direct manager’s ratings and Total EI, the

Strategic and Experiential EI areas and the Understand Emotion branch of EI.

Kerr et al. (2006) found evidence of significant relationships between MLE

ratings and Total EI, Experiential EI, the Perceive Emotion branch of EI and the

Facilitate Thought branch of EI. Most of this variation was accounted for by the

combined effects of two branch scores (Perceiving Emotion and Facilitate

Thought), which was labelled as Experiential EI. Surprisingly, the managing

emotions branch was not correlated with average other-ratings on 360o

feedback (Kerr et al. 2006). Most recently, aspects of ability-EI were associated

Page 56: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

54

with leadership emergence (Cote et al. 2010) and multi-source feedback

measures of MLE (Clarke 2010) after controlling for IQ and personality.

Variation across the studies may suggest the existence of interaction effects

with regard to ability EI (Van Rooy & Viswesvaran 2004). While the evidence

supports a direct relationship between EI and MLE, it also suggests an indirect

relationship. Individuals low in one EI ability may compensate for that deficit by

being better at other aspects (Carroll 1993). Indeed, Prati et al. (2003, p. 30)

argued EI is likely to “moderate the effect of specific personality traits on leader

and team member interactions”. Cote & Miners (2006, p. 2) presented the

foundation for interactive models and suggested:

Compensatory effects may explain why emotional intelligence predicted job performance in some past studies but not in others. If compensatory effects exist, emotional intelligence should predict job performance only some of the time, depending on the other abilities that individuals possess.

Thus, the relationship between EI in predicting MLE may be moderated by other

variables (Cote & Miners 2006). Evidence of this effect was found when the

association between EI and task performance and organisational citizenship

behaviour (directed at the organisation, but not at individuals) became more

important as cognitive intelligence decreased (Cote & Miners 2006). Similarly,

EI was found to enhance the relationship between the personality construct of

agreeableness and both task and contextual performance (Shaffer 2005).

Interaction effects between ability EI and extraversion significantly and

positively predicted ratings of transformational leadership behaviours (Rubin,

Munz & Bommer 2005). However, high extraversion combined with low EI did

not lead to the same results. Further, Rode et al. (2007) found

conscientiousness moderated the relationship between EI and both

Page 57: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

55

interpersonal effectiveness and public speaking effectiveness. There is

mounting evidence of an interaction effect between EI and gender (Brackett et

al. 2006; Byron 2007; Day & Carroll 2004; Joseph & Newman 2010; Lyons &

Schneider 2005) and the relevance of subgroups (Joseph & Newman 2010;

Karim & Weisz 2010). For example, Joseph and Newman (2010) found

evidence that the relationship between emotional regulation and job

performance was stronger for high emotional labour jobs than for low emotional

labour jobs.

Finally, another area of interest that has received limited attention in the study

of EI’s relationship with MLE relates to MSF self-other agreement (SOA). There

is support for a relationship between SOA and MLE (Sosik & Megerian 1999).

Managers who are highly self-aware can incorporate information from their

context about themselves and more accurately gauge their skill level (Atwater &

Yammarino 1992; Atwater & Yammarino 1997; Yammarino & Atwater 1997).

Self-awareness has been conceptualised as the degree of SOA and has been

shown to correspond to measures of managerial skill and unit effectiveness

(Shipper et al. 2003). Self-awareness, as indicated by SOA, has been found to

be correlated with personality measures of EI and performance ratings (Atwater

& Yammarino 1997; Megerian & Sosik 1996; Sosik & Megerian 1999).

However, results have been mixed and are not easily comparable due to

differences in the ways in which SOA has been conceptualised. Most

significantly, no studies have explored the relationship between ability EI and

SOA.

In summary, the construct of EI is an important individual difference that

contributes to individual effectiveness and to MLE. Further, ability based

Page 58: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

56

measures of EI have better construct validity. The next section explores the

concept of meaning-making structure (MMS) that may provide complementary

but distinct information about individual differences as they relate to MLE.

2.6 MEANING-MAKING STRUCTURE (MMS)

The concept of meaning-making, originally grounded in the field of philosophy

and then explored extensively within psychology, has gained increasing

prominence as an important aspect of understanding MLE. Although related, it

is important to understand the differences between meaning, meaning-making

and meaning-making structure (MMS).

Meaning can be defined as ‘mental representation of possible relationships

among things, events, and relationships’ (Baumeister 1991, p. 15). Consistent

with this definition, much of the initial research focused on the content and

sources of what gives people meaning (Debats 1999; O'Connor & Chamberlain

1996; Schnell 2009) and also the intensity and depth of this sense of meaning

(Ebersole 1998; Reker 2005).

Meaning-making is the process of constructing these mental representations

and can be understood differently depending on the epistemology chosen (Park

2010). One view is meaning-making as a process or outcome of dealing with

trauma (Park & Folkman 1997; Taylor 1983; Thompson & Janigian 1988).

Competing explanations within the cognitive sciences define meaning-making

as the result of brain activity (Barsalou 2008). A third perspective is that

meaning-making is a process of constructing our reality and happens at both

the individual and collective levels (Kegan 1980; Loevinger 1976; Piaget 1954).

Page 59: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

57

It is through the latter perspective, referred to as constructive developmental

theory, that the concept of meaning-making structure (MMS) emerges. MMS’s

are the ‘set of assumptions in your head that allows you to interpret sensory

information, anticipate future events, and plan accordingly’ (Drath 1994, p. 3).

The remainder of this section addresses the concept of internal action logics, or

the complexity of people’s MMS. First, the theoretical domain of constructive

developmental theory is introduced which is followed by an explanation of one

particularly influential theory of MMS: Loevinger’s (1976) theory of ED. Finally,

a summary of the application of MMS to the study of MLE is presented.

2.6.1 Constructive-Developmental Theory

Being is a process, and human being is the process of organizing meaning, making meaning. (Drath 1990, pg 485)

Constructive-developmental (CD) theory focuses on the evolution of meaning-

making processes throughout the human experience. Originating from Piaget’s

(1954) seminal work on child development, CD (or neo-Piagetian) theory

suggests there are stages of psychosocial growth that extend into adulthood

and for the duration of a person’s life (Kegan 1980). The emphasis is on the

inherent structure within these stages and the antecedents and processes

involved in their development over time (McCauley et al. 2006).

Complimentary ‘stage’ models within the CD theoretical domain include:

1. Loevinger’s (1976) theory of ego-development, which was subsequently

developed by Cook-Greuter (2004), and Torbert (2009).

2. Kohlberg’s (1969) theory of moral development.

Page 60: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

58

3. Kegan’s (1980) orders of consciousness theory.

While differences exist among the theorists, there are important commonalities

that include the idea of articulated stages of meaning-making that can be

placed along a continuum. At each successive stage, the structure for

meaning-making expands to become more complex. “CD theory posits that the

capacity to make meaning of oneself and one’s environment is an unfolding

process moving towards deeper understanding, wisdom and effectiveness in

the world” (Cook-Greuter 2004, p. 277).

Stages at one end of the continuum reflect guiding principles that are ego-

centric in contrast to stages at the other end of the continuum, which are world-

centric (Cook-Greuter 2004). The number of, and labels for, these stages vary

depending on the theory that is used. As such, the convention of dividing the

developmental spectrum into three tiers, or categories of stages, facilitates

comparison and discussion across the theories, despite differences in stage

labels (e.g. pre-conventional, conventional and post-conventional). Pre-

conventional stages are the most ego-centric and represent the simplest

meaning-making lens. These stages are developmentally normal in children,

but may be suboptimal for adults (Pfaffenberger 2005). Conventional stages

have more complexity and represent the dominant paradigm, or most

commonly held set of meaning-making structures in the adult population

(approximately 75% to 80% of adults) (Cook-Greuter 2004). Finally, post-

conventional stages reflect the most complex meaning-making structures and

are experienced by very few individuals, possibly 10% to 20 % of adults (Cook-

Greuter 2004).

Page 61: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

59

Loevinger’s (1966) theory of ego development (ED) is considered a seminal

contribution to CD theory, not only because it was the first to develop a rigorous

measurement methodology, but also because it attempted to describe and

measure personality as a whole. Consequently, the next section describes

Loevinger’s (1966) ED theory.

2.6.2 Loevinger’s Ego-Development Theory

Meaning-making structure can be conceptualised as the stage of an individual’s

ego-development (ED) (Loevinger 1976). Loevinger defines ego as a person’s

fundamental frame of reference to the world in which they operate, as a unitary

domain, or as ‘master trait’ in contrast to theorists who conceptualised it as a

multifaceted construct (Snarey, Kohlberg & Noam 1983). This frame of

reference acts as a lens through which the world is experienced and

interpreted. The capacity for this lens to change, to expand and to

accommodate increasing levels of complexity is described as ego-development

(ED) and represents the course of an individual’s character development. ED is

less about cognitive or reasoning growth than about “impulses and methods for

controlling impulses, personal preoccupations and ambitions, interpersonal

attitudes and social values – what psychologists normally call personality” (Blasi

1998, p. 15). Throughout the remainder of the thesis the terms MMS and ED

will be used interchangeably.

Originally conceptualised as a four stage theory and drawing on the concepts of

interpersonal integration (Sullivan, Grant & Grant 1957), Loevinger’s (1976)

theory and measure of ED evolved through an iterative process of testing and

revising scoring instructions with successive data sets (Loevinger 1993). The

Page 62: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

60

result was a stage model of ED with detailed qualitative differences in the four

domains of impulse control, interpersonal modes, conscious preoccupation and

cognitive style. Each of these domains progressively builds more complexity

into how people define themselves and operate in their world (Loevinger 1976;

Loevinger 1997).

Consistent with constructive developmental theory, Loevinger’s theory

articulates a continuum of ED stages, each with key guiding principles also

known as action-logics (Rooke & Torbert 2005). Ego development “represents

a structural stage change in a hierarchical, invariantly sequential manner, with

an inner logic to the stages and to their progression” (Loevinger 1997, p. 543).

Loevinger (1966) was loathe to provide a more specific definition of ED and

preferred instead to point to the broad manifestations of ED at different stages

as is detailed in Table 2.1.

There are at least nine stages of ED, which move from the single action logic of

survival through to increasing stages of complexity and ambiguity (Hy &

Loevinger 1996). The first three stages of E1: Presocial and Symbolic, E2:

Impulsive and Self-Protective, and E4: represent the pre-conventional stages of

ED. The next three stages, E4: Conformist, E5: Self-Aware, and E6:

Conscientious, represent the conventional stages of ED. Finally, the post-

conventional stages of ED are E7: Individualistic, E8: Autonomous, and E9:

Integrated. Loevinger’s ED theory describes the evolution of an individual’s

theories-in-use (Argyris & Schön 1974) or mental models (Senge 1990).

Page 63: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

61

Table 2.1 Loevinger’s Stages of Ego Development (Manners 2001, pg 544)

EI

Presocial and Symbiotic

Exclusive focus on gratification of immediate needs; strong attachment to mother, and differentiating her from the rest of the environment, but not her/himself from mother; preverbal, hence inaccessible to assessment via the sentence completion method.

E2

Impulsive and Self-Protective

Demanding; impulsive; conceptually confused; concerned with bodily feelings, especially sexual and aggressive; no sense of psychological causation; dependent; good and bad seen in terms of how it affects the self; dichotomous good/bad, nice/mean.

E3

Self-Protective

Wary; complaining; exploitive; hedonistic; preoccupied with staying out of trouble, not getting caught; learning about rules and self-control; externalizing blame.

E4

Conformist

Conventional; moralistic; sentimental; rule-bound; stereotyped; need for belonging; superficial niceness; behaviour of self and others seen in terms of externals; feelings only understood at banal level; conceptually simple, ‘black and white’ thinking.

E5

Self-Aware

Increased, although still limited, self-awareness and appreciation of multiple possibilities in a situation; self-critical; emerging rudimentary awareness of inner feelings of self and others; banal level reflections on life issues: God, death, relationships, health.

E6

Conscientious

Self-evaluated standards; reflective; responsible; empathic; long term goals and ideals; true conceptual complexity displayed and perceived; can see the broader perspective and can discern patterns; principled morality; rich and differentiated inner life; mutuality in relationship; self-critical; values achievement.

E7

Individualistic

Heightened sense of individuality; concern about emotional dependence; tolerant of self and others; incipient awareness of inner conflicts and personal paradoxes, without a sense of resolution or integration; values relationship over achievement; vivid and unique way of expressing self.

E8

Autonomous

Capacity to face and cope with inner conflicts; high tolerance for ambiguity and can see conflict as an expression of the multifaceted nature of people and life in general; respectful of the autonomy of the self and of others; relationship seen as interdependent rather than dependent/independent; concerned with self-actualization; recognizes the systemic nature of relationships; cherishes individuality and uniqueness; vivid expression of feelings.

E9

Integrated

Wise; broadly empathic; full sense of identity; able to reconcile inner conflicts and integrate paradoxes. Similar to Maslow’s description of the ‘self-actualised’ person who is growth motivated, seeking to actualize potential capacities, to understand her/his intrinsic nature, and to achieve integration and synergy within the self (Maslow, 1962).

Each ED stage is a unique structure that contributes to a person’s meaning-

making ability (Loevinger 1976). An individual’s developmental stage

“influences what they notice or can become aware of, and therefore, what they

Page 64: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

62

can describe, articulate, influence, and change” (Cook-Greuter 2004, p. 277).

An individual is not necessarily aware of or able to describe their current lens, or

developmental stage, particularly at the earlier stages, because it is the water in

which they are swimming. A person’s current meaning-making lens is the

‘subject’ of one’s reality. The existence of this meaning-making lens becomes

apparent only if it fails to adequately explain our experiences. At this point we

become aware of the lens and it is no longer the subject of our reality but rather

an observable, or objective, part of a larger unfolding reality. Simultaneously a

new lens emerges as our subjective truth. The ongoing process of subject

becoming object is known as developmental movement and occurs through a

process known as equilibration (Manners & Durkin 2000). When information

cannot be easily understood with the available meaning-making complexity, it is

experienced as disequilibrium or cognitive conflict (Murray 1983). In response,

an individual has the choice to assimilate or accommodate this incoherent

information, as Block (1982, p. 286) notes:

Assimilation is to be viewed as the invocation by the individual of existing adaptive structures, schemes or scripts to process experience, accommodation is to be viewed as the shift over by the individual to the formation of new (and the reformation of old) adaptive structures, schemes and scripts to process experience.

Each successive stage adds complexity to an individual’s capacity to identify,

understand and work with information from their environment. Whole new ways

of perceiving and interacting with the world are possible. This process has

specific characteristics as Cook-Greuter (2004, p. 277) has noted:

• Later stages are reached only by journeying though the earlier stages. Once a stage has been traversed, it remains a part of the individual’s response repertoire, even when more complex, later stages are adopted.

Page 65: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

63

• Each later stage includes and transcends the previous ones. That is, the earlier perspectives remain part of our current experience and knowledge (just as when a child learns to run, it doesn’t stop being able to walk).

• Each later stage in the sequence is more differentiated, integrated, flexible and capable of functioning optimally in a rapidly changing and complexifying world…

• …As development unfolds, autonomy, freedom, tolerance for difference and ambiguity, as well as flexibility, reflection and skill in interacting with the environment increase, while defenses decrease.

• A person who has reached a later stage can understand earlier world views, but a person at an earlier stage cannot understand later ones.

• Development occurs through the interplay between person and environment, not just by one or the other. It is a potential and can be encouraged and facilitated by appropriate support and challenge.

The inherent paradox is that the increasing complexity facilitates solving

problems that were impenetrable while, at the same time, enabling a

recognition of problems that was not possible before this ED stage. In short,

every ED stage has inherent strengths and limitations (Lichtenstein 1995).

Theoretically ED continues throughout the course of a person’s life. In reality it

appears ED “tends to increase with age throughout childhood and then

stabilizes as a function of age in adolescence and adulthood” (Newman,

Tellegen & Bouchard 1998, p. 985). Studies have found that most adults do not

progress to post-conventional levels of ego-development (Cook-Greuter 1990;

Loevinger, Wessler & Redmore 1970) and, in fact, stabilise at the self-aware

stage (Loevinger et al. 1985; Redmore & Loevinger 1979; Redmore 1983).

Page 66: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

64

A review of the literature exploring processes involved in adult ego-

development suggested individual differences may account for the extent to

which a person engages in the type of experiences that can lead to ego-

development (Manners & Durkin 2000, p. 504), including “self-acceptance, the

desire for challenge, and openness to new experiences”. While evidence

supports the theoretical assumption that some individual differences are

associated with key ED levels (Rozsnafszky 1981), the non-monotonic, or non-

linear, nature of ED is a key characteristic that differentiates it from many other

psychometrics that assume an increasing or decreasing linear relationship

between variables (Loevinger 1993).

A pattern of curvilinear relationships exist between ED and other individual

differences, such as defence mechanisms (Cramer 1999), friendliness,

conformity and compliance (Westenberg & Block 1993) and adjustment (Helson

& Wink 1987). As such, it is reasonable to suggest there is a relationship

between EI and ED. Specifically, the two constructs may be correlated. There

have been specific calls for research exploring the relationship between ED and

the socio-emotional domain (Manners & Durkin 2001) and how cognitive and

affective processes relate to one another at different stages of ED (Hauser

1993). However, to date, no studies have explored the relationship between EI

and ED.

In contrast to the empirical support for Loevinger’s (1976) theory and measure

of ED, two aspects of the theory have been challenged, namely:

1. The idea of ego as a master trait has not been consistently supported by

empirical data.

Page 67: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

65

2. While there is strong support for the sequential nature of the various ego

stages, there is also an indication of a possible regression of ego stage

transitions (Manners & Durkin 2001).

An examination of the structural nature of ED that used structural equation

modelling procedures to test alternate models, found the model in which ED

was a master trait was not the best fit to the data (Novy et al. 1994). However,

Novey et al. (1994) did caution that the results could have been confounded

due to limitations in finding appropriate measures to represent the sub-

constructs of ED. With regard to the theorised invariant forward nature of ED,

Loevinger (1976, p. 386) suggested some “regression in the form of

disorganization is required to make reorganization possible”, which has been

observed in longitudinal studies (Adams & Fitch 1982; Loevinger et al. 1985;

Redmore 1983).

Higher levels of ED are associated with indicators of individual effectiveness,

such as “increased intrinsic motivation, broader perception of ethical dilemmas,

greater self-acceptance following moral failure, richer conceptualization of a

moral self, and progressive integration of personal and moral identity”

(Giesbrecht & Walker 2000, p. 163). There are also positive associations

between the level of ED and aspects of maturity, such as tolerance of

ambiguity, affect regulating repression (Helson & Wink 1987) and the three

components of ego-resiliency (psychological mindedness, intellectualism and

resiliency) (Westenberg & Block 1993).

One of the criticisms of Loevinger’s measure of ED, the Washington University

Sentence Completion Test (WUSCT), is that it is limited in measuring higher ED

Page 68: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

66

stages (Redmore & Waldman 1975; Sutton & Swensen 1983). The WUSCT is

discussed in more detail in Chapter 3. Cook-Greuter (2004) adapted the

measure to address these concerns and also changed the language to be more

specific to managerial-leadership settings. There are two commercially available

instruments that are based on Loevinger’s original measure, namely the Harthill

Leadership Development Profile (LDP) (Rooke & Torbet 1998; Torbert & Livne-

Tarandach 2009) and the SCTi Map, which was subsequently renamed as the

Leadership Maturity Framework (Cook-Greuter 2005). Loevinger’s theory and

measure has extensive application outside management. Yet despite its

widespread use within psychology, applications of CD and ED theories within

the field of MLE have been minimal. It has been argued by developmental

researchers that individuals at higher levels of ED will make qualitatively

different contributions in their fields of influence (Cook-Greuter 1994; Kegan &

Lahey 1984; Kegan 1995; Torbert 1987; Torbert & Fisher 1992; Torbert 1996).

This would logically extend to the roles played by managerial-leaders and the

profound influence that organisations have within the modern world. As such,

the next section reviews the role ED has played in our understanding of MLE.

2.6.3 MMS and MLE

The application of constructive developmental theory to managerial-leadership

was originally explored during the 1980s (Merron 1985; Merron, Fisher &

Torbert 1986; Merron, Fisher & Torbert 1987; Torbert 1985; Torbert 1987).

Since that time a number of writers have presented theories of MLE based on

constructive developmental theory, including Rooke and Torbert’s (2005)

internal action logic(s) model, Eigel and Kuhnert’s (2005, p. 259) Leadership

Development Level (LDL) model, which they defined as “the measurable

Page 69: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

67

capacity to understand ourselves, others, and our situations”, and Joiner and

Joseph’s (2007, p. 35) leadership agility model, which they defined as “the

ability to lead effectively under conditions of rapid change and high complexity”.

Empirical studies have found mixed support for a relationship between MMS

and a leader’s position within the organisational hierarchy (Gratch 1987; Smith

1980; Torbert 1994). In contrast, there appears to be a relationship between a

leader’s developmental level and the creation of high-involvement workplaces

and shared responsibility with subordinates (Joiner & Josephs 2007). In a

paper that applied Wilber’s (1993) spectrum of consciousness model, which is

based on CD theory, Young (2002b, p. 31) argued:

CEOs who operate at higher levels of consciousness will engage in more objective problem solving and will experience more affective social development, enabling them to make better or more effective business decisions than CEOs operating at lower levels of consciousness.

The results of a case study analysis of small business owner and operators

supported this hypothesis, as it was found that people at higher developmental

levels were able to notice more cues in their environment and to focus on

broader and more strategic issues (Hirsch 1988). Similarly, Rooke and Torbert

(1998) found CEOs with less complex MMS were not successful at leading

organisational transformation, in contrast to CEOs who were at higher levels of

MMS. They suggested a minimum MMS level is required if a person is to be

able to successfully effect organisational change.

Based on this research, it was logical to suggest developmental order is likely to

predict the use of transformational leadership behaviours (Kuhnert 1994;

Kuhnert & Lewis 1987). However, studies have found mixed support for this

Page 70: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

68

relationship (Mehltretter 1996; Spence 2005; Steeves 1997), although the

different sample sizes, measures and methods used in these studies have

limited their generalisability. Regardless, a review of the literature by McCauley

et al. (2006) concluded that there was support for a relationship between more

general measures of MLE and developmental order. In particular, they

suggested that post-conventional managerial-leaders:

have been found to be more likely to delegate, hold people accountable, influence through rewards and expertise (rather than coercive power), look for underlying causes of problems, act as change agents, and be more comfortable with conflict. (McCauley et al. 2006, p. 647)

It appears developmental order is related to more sophisticated and

collaborative means of enacting basic communication skills, problem solving,

handling conflict and managing performance (Fisher & Torbert 1991; Merron,

Fisher & Torbert 1987; Spillett 1995). Individuals with higher levels of MMS are

more likely to use altruism, anticipation, humour, consultation and to avoid the

use of coercive power (Smith 1980). MMS has also been found to be

significant in managerial-leaders’ decisions to request feedback and to explore

how behaviour change might improve their effectiveness (Merron & Torbert

1984 in Merron, 1987; Quinn & Torbert 1987; Torbert 1987). However, no

empirical studies have explored a potential relationship between developmental

order and competency based, behaviourally complex MLE.

Developmental order has also been found to be related to effectiveness as

perceived by colleagues (Harris 2005; Harris & Kuhnert 2008; Strang 2006).

West Point cadets with higher levels of MMS, for example, were rated by

superior officers as being more effective in carrying out their leadership

responsibilities (Lewis et al. 2005). Similarly, a comparison of high-performing

Page 71: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

69

group of principals and an average performing group found the former group

had significantly higher levels of MMS (Sweeney 1991). Empirical evidence

also supported a relationship between ED and consulting competence as

perceived by peers and expert raters (Bushe & Gibbs 1990). However, a study

of teachers acting in peer leadership positions, that used the MSF instrument of

Leader Behaviour Description Questionnaire (LBDQ) (Stogdill 1963) as the

measure of MLE, did not support such a relationship as the teachers’ MMS

explained only 1% of variance in two LBDQ subscales (initiating structure and

consideration) (Gammons 1993).

People with advanced MMSs are rare and such worldviews are thought to

require a considerable commitment to reflection and learning (Torbert & Fisher

1992). For example, in the studies that have been mentioned most had a modal

stage of E5 Self-Aware followed next by E6 Conscientious (Fisher & Torbert

1991; Gammons 1993; Gratch 1987; Hirsch 1988; Merron, Fisher & Torbert

1987; Quinn & Torbert 1987; Smith 1980; Torbert 1983 in Torbert, 1994). The

single exception is the study by Bushe & Gibbs (1990) where the sample was

drawn from Organisational Development consultants, not practising managerial-

leaders. In a five-year search for late stage managerial-leaders, Torbert (1996)

located a very small sample (n=6) at the magician stage (roughly equivalent to

Loevinger’s Autonomous stage) and described their behavioural complexity as

a manifestation of principles from chaos theory (Torbert 1996).

Every stage of developmental complexity makes a contribution, but also

generates potential problems in terms of meaning-making capacity. As such,

different levels of developmental complexity map to managerial-leadership

strengths and weaknesses (Drath 1990). For example, Drath (1990) provided a

Page 72: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

70

detailed discussion of the strengths and weaknesses that arose from two

specific contributions from the institutional stage (Kegan 1982) (roughly

equivalent to Loevinger’s Self-Aware stage). The first contribution, an ability to

perceive interpersonal relationships as objects, facilitates effective instrumental

relationships and the primacy of thinking over feeling in decision making, both

of which contribute to MLE in modern organisations. However, experiencing

relationships as objects also leads to difficulty with intimacy, addressing conflict,

and recognising and expressing emotion. The second contribution, a strong

internal sense of self, facilitates drive, ambition, willingness to be responsible

and accountable and being comfortable in working in hierarchies of authority. It

also leads to difficulty with accepting healthy criticism, relaxing and appreciating

and accepting others. The predominance of the institutional stage within

organisations and the competing demands of the related strengths and

weaknesses explain why there are so many “managers who would empower

others but cannot” (Drath 1990, p. 488).

2.6.4 MMS and EI

At a theoretical level, if one assumes MMS is a master trait, it is reasonable to

assert that it will have a relationship with other key constructs of individual

difference. At an empirical level, there is support for the relationship between

MMS and individual differences related to emotions.

Increased cognitive complexity is related to increased emotional range

(Sommers 1981). People at the post-conventional MMS levels have more

empathy than people at conformist or pre-conformist MMS levels (Carlozzi, Gaa

& Liberman 1983). MMS is significantly correlated with the Levels of Emotional

Page 73: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

71

Awareness Scale (LEAS) (r= .40), a cognitive-developmental model of

emotional experience (Lane et al. 1990). Based on the concept of levels of

emotional awareness, Lane and Schwartz (1987) suggested CD theory might

explain a range of normal and abnormal emotional states.

MMS is also significantly correlated with the Range and Differentiation of

Emotional Experience Scale (RDEES) (r= .39), a measure of emotional

complexity (Kang & Shaver 2004). Kang and Shaver (2004) suggested the

RDEES would be most related to the first of the four branches of the MSCEITv2

(Mayer, Salovey & Caruso 2002) (i.e. the ability to perceive emotions in oneself

and in others). However, they did not test this hypothesis empirically. Indeed,

no studies have explored the relationship between ability-based EI models

(Mayer, Salovey & Caruso 2002) and MMS.

The previous discussion reviewed the concept of MMS by:

1. Grounding MMS within the domain of constructive developmental theory.

2. Operationalising MMS through Loevinger’s ED theory.

3. Summarising the application of MMS to the study of MLE.

4. Presenting support for the suggested relationship between EI and MMS.

The above review of the literature has established that ED is a valid construct

taken from developmental psychology and that it is distinct from personality or

IQ. However, there has been no empirical testing of the suggested relationship

between MMS and EI.

Page 74: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

72

The next section discusses the specific conceptualisation of the dependent

variable in the research: MLE as behavioural complexity.

2.7 MANAGERIAL-LEADERSHIP EFFECTIVENESS (MLE)

The dependent variable in the present study was managerial-leadership

effectiveness (MLE). Given the breadth and depth of ways that have been

suggested to conceptualise MLE, it is important to delineate the phenomenon of

interest within the present study. First, recognising that leadership can be a

formal role and also a process exercised by anyone in the organization (Raelin

2003), this study was concerned with the type of leadership exercised by

people in formal management roles. Further, acknowledging the contrasting

views about the differences and/or similarities of management versus

leadership, the present study assumed the two were distinct, but

complimentary, processes and that both are required to be effective (Bass

1990; Cacioppe & Albrecht 2000; Kotter 1990; Quinn et al. 1996). For this

reason the thesis used the term managerial-leadership coined by Vaill (1998,

p.4) to describe a phenomenon that is a:

blend of thought and action, of individual and group behaviour, of abstract and concrete focus, of problem solving and problem finding, of creativity and routine, of economics and humanities, of societal contribution and self-advancement.

Managerial-leaders operate in unprecedented complex and paradoxical

contexts that require multiple perspectives and skills (Handy 1994; Lewis 2000).

Evidence from prior research supports the view that effective managerial-

leaders think and act in complex ways by drawing on multiple cognitive models

and performing multiple roles (Denison, Hoojiberg & Quinn 1995; Hart, Hart &

Page 75: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

73

Quinn 1993; Hooijberg & Quinn 1992). Based on the premise of competing and

sometimes contradictory demands, Quinn’s Competing Values Approach

(Quinn & McGrath 1982; Quinn & Rohrbaugh 1983) integrated a disparate

literature on managerial-leadership and organizational effectiveness. This

meta-theoretical model applied to understanding MLE was renamed the

Competing Values Framework (CVF) (Quinn 1984; Quinn 1988). Detailed

descriptions of the CVF model can be found elsewhere (see Denison, Hoojiberg

& Quinn 1995; Hart, Hart & Quinn 1993; Quinn 1988; Quinn et al. 1996) and so

only a brief description of the model is provided here.

At the heart of the CVF is the assumption that MLE requires an ability to work

with competing values and perspectives (i.e. an ability to deal with paradox).

The CVF has two dimensions that intersect. The vertical dimension represents

a range from flexibility to control, while the horizontal dimension represents a

shifting focus on external to internal aspects of the relevant system, as can be

seen in Figure 2.1. The resulting four competing and complementary models of

organisational theory represent different theoretical contributions to our

understanding MLE and have been termed the open systems model, the

rational goal model, the internal process model and the human relations model.

The bottom half of the CVF reflects the thinking at the beginning of the twentieth

Century, which is represented by the rational-goal model (with an emphasis on

productivity, profit and goal clarification), and internal process model (with an

emphasis on stability, control and documentation) (Quinn et al. 1996).

Page 76: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

74

Figure 2.1 The Competing Values Framework (Quinn 1996)

The upper half of the CVF reflects thinking about MLE in the latter part of the

Twentieth Century, which is represented in the human relations and the open

systems models. The human relations model emphasises commitment,

cohesion and morale (Quinn et al. 1996), while the open systems model

signalled a shift in organisational awareness of, responsiveness to and

symbiosis with, a dynamic external environment that emphasised adaptation,

creative problem solving, innovation and change management (Quinn et al.

1996).

Each of the four models in the CVF represents theoretical contributions drawn

from the evolution in our understanding of MLE. However, each on its own is

incomplete. Quinn (1996) labelled this context as the ‘both=and’ paradigm,

which requires leadership and management competencies from all four

quadrants of the CVF (Toor & Ofori 2008; Young & Dulewicz 2008; Yukl 1989).

Each model can be further delineated into two distinct managerial-leadership

roles (as is shown in Table 2.2), each of which represents a unique set of

values in its emphasis on specific means and ends. The implication is that

MLE comes from a range of competencies from each of these competing and

complimentary roles.

Page 77: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

75

Table 2.2 Implicit Values Within the CVF (Quinn 1996)

MODEL OF MLE

CRITERIA FOR EFFECTIVENESS

CVF ROLES IMPLICIT VALUES

RATIONAL GOAL

Productivity and Profit

Producer Productivity and Accomplishment

Director Direction and Goal Clarity

INTERNAL PROCESS

Stability and Continuity

Coordinator Stability and Control

Monitor Documentation and Info Management

HUMAN RELATIONS

Commitment, Cohesion, and

Morale

Facilitator Participation and Openness

Mentor Commitment and Morale

OPEN SYSTEMS

Adaptability, External Support

Innovator Innovation and Adaptation

Broker Growth and Resource Acquisition

The four quadrant structure of the CVF has been validated in many studies that

have explored both MLE (Buenger et al. 1996; Denison, Hoojiberg & Quinn

1995; Kalliath, Bluedorn & Strube 1999; Wyse & Vilkinas 2004) and

organisational culture (Howard 1998; Lamond 2003). However, some of these

studies have challenged the number (Hooijberg & Choi 2000) and the

placement (Denison, Hoojiberg & Quinn 1995) of some of the suggested roles.

The CVF inspired the introduction of a theory of behaviourally complex

managerial-leadership, premised on the idea that MLE requires an ability to

exercise paradoxical skills (Boal & Hooijberg 2000; Denison, Hoojiberg & Quinn

1995; Hooijberg & Quinn 1992; Hooijberg, Hunt & Dodge 1997). According to

this theory, MLE is “the ability to perform multiple roles and behaviours that

circumscribe the requisite variety implied by an organizational or environmental

context” (Denison, Hoojiberg & Quinn, p. 526). Behavioural complexity was

seen to consist of two facets, which were termed:

Page 78: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

76

1. A behavioural repertoire, which refers to the capacity or breadth of roles

a leader has an ability to enact.

2. Behavioural differentiation, which refers to managers’ ability to adjust

their leadership functions/ behaviours according to the context in which

they are operating (Denison, Hoojiberg & Quinn 1995; Hooijberg & Quinn

1992; Hoojiberg 1996).

A manager’s behavioural repertoire has been shown to correlate with measures

of individual MLE and with organisational effectiveness (Denison, Hoojiberg &

Quinn 1995; Hart, Hart & Quinn 1993; Hoojiberg 1996).

Much of the research that has examined behavioural complexity has used the

CVF model. Models of behavioural complexity that are based on the CVF

include the Integral Leadership and Management Model (ILMM) (Cacioppe &

Albrecht 2000; Cacioppe & Albrecht 2001) and the Integrated Competing

Values Framework (ICVF) (Vilkinas & Cartan 2006). Both models build on the

CVF and adapt the underlying structure in unique ways. For example, the ICVF

and the ILMM introduced a people/task dimension, which is important given the

behavioural theories found within the MLE literature. In addition, the ICVF

introduced an ‘integrator’ role as the behavioural control room for the other eight

operational roles (Vilkinas & Cartan 2001; Vilkinas & Cartan 2006). The

‘integrator role’ represents a managerial-leader’s capacity for critical

observation and reflective learning and added a further level of complexity to

the ICVF. The ILMM introduces a series of ‘Self-Qualities’ that were intended to

represent behavioural aspects of emotional intelligence. While both models

extended the theory of behavioural complexity in meaningful ways, further

Page 79: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

77

research is required to establish their validity as alternate conceptual

elaborations of this important construct.

2.8 CHAPTER SUMMARY AND HYPOTHESES

The present chapter presented the theoretical foundations for the key

constructs in the proposed research: Managerial-leadership effectiveness

(MLE), traditional intelligence (IQ), emotional intelligence (EI) and meaning-

making structure (MMS). The evolution in thinking about MLE was reviewed and

the re-emergence of interest in trait theories was discussed.

A review of the research suggested there was empirical support for a positive

relationship between IQ and ability-based EI, between IQ and MMS and

between MMS and various individual differences related to emotional range and

complexity. However, no studies were found that had explicitly tested the

relationship between these three constructs. As such, the hypothesis that the

individual difference constructs of MMS, EI and IQ are positively correlated and

have discriminant validity was suggested (H1). More specifically:

H1a: MMS, EI and IQ are positively correlated.

H1b: MMS, EI and IQ have discriminant validity from each other.

The literature review also found evidence to suggest IQ, EI and MMS are

predictors of MLE. However, no studies were found that have explored the

combined impact these constructs have on MLE. As such, the hypothesis that

the individual difference constructs of MMS, EI and IQ have incremental

predictive validity (H2) was suggested. More specifically:

Page 80: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

78

H2a: IQ is a significant predictor of MLE.

H2b: EI is a significant incremental predictor, beyond IQ, of MLE.

H2c: MMS is a significant incremental predictor, beyond IQ and EI, of MLE.

The measures and methods employed to examine these hypotheses are

discussed in detail in the next chapter.

Page 81: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

79

Chapter 3

MEASURES AND METHODS

3.1 CHAPTER OVERVIEW

In the previous chapter the relevant literature was reviewed and the suggested

hypotheses presented. The present chapter builds on this foundation and

presents the measures (section 3.2) and methods that were used in this study

(section 3.3). The chapter explains and justifies how the various constructs

were operationalised and how the data were collected, prepared and,

ultimately, analysed.

3.2 DEFINITIONS AND MEASURES

In the following sections the managerial-leadership effectiveness (section

3.2.1), ego-development (section 3.2.2), emotional intelligence (section 3.2.3)

and traditional intelligence (section 3.2.4) constructs are discussed in turn. Each

construct is operationally defined, the nature and number of items for each of

the scales that were used are outlined, the scales’ scoring method is described

and the psychometric properties of the measures are summarised.

Page 82: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

80

3.2.1 The Integral Leadership and Management Development Profile

The dependent variable in the research model was managerial-leadership

effectiveness (MLE). MLE can be defined as behavioural complexity or “the

ability to perform multiple roles and behaviours that circumscribe the requisite

variety implied by an organizational or environmental context” (Denison,

Hoojiberg & Quinn 1995, p. 526). MLE behavioural complexity was

operationalised in the present study as ratings from others on the Integral

Leadership and Management Development Profile (ILMDP) (Cacioppe &

Albrecht 2000; Cacioppe & Albrecht 2001). The ILMDP is based on a model of

behavioural complexity that integrates elements of Quinn’s (1996) Competing

Values Framework with integral theory (Wilber 1993; Wilber 1996). The ILMDP

has three nested domains of skills, which have been termed:

1. Self-Qualities.

2. Management and Leadership Functions and Roles.

3. Strategic Leadership Skills.

Domain 2, the Management and Leadership Functions and Roles, provided the

measure of behavioural complexity that was used in the study and which is

described in detail subsequently. A full description of the ILMDP’s other

domains can be found elsewhere (Cacioppe & Albrecht 2000; Cacioppe &

Albrecht 2001).

The ILMDP Management and Leadership Functions and Roles result from the

intersection of three dimensions of focus (relationship to task focus,

Page 83: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

81

organisation to individual focus, and internal versus external focus). The first

two dimensions intersect to create a four-quadrant model, which is shown in

Figure 3.1. The vertical axis is drawn from integral theory and represents a

range in focus from the unique or individual to the whole or organisation

(Cacioppe & Albrecht 2000; Rubin, Munz & Bommer 2005). MLE requires an

ability to link individual team members to the broader team and, ultimately, from

the team to the larger system (Cacioppe & Albrecht 2001). This dimension

represents a range in attention from a focus on the individual or the details

within the system, to a broad view of the whole system.

Figure 3.1 ILMDP Domain 2: Management and Leadership Functions

(adapted from Cacioppe, 2000)

The horizontal axis is drawn from MLE theory and represents a range in

intended action from focussing on relationship development, to focussing on

task or outcome completion. MLE requires both an ability to accomplish tasks

and to build relationships (Cacioppe & Albrecht 2000; Cacioppe & Albrecht

2001). This dimension represents the required balance of actions directed at

achieving results by building relationships with the people through whom these

Page 84: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

82

results will be accomplished. The resulting four quadrants represent two

functions of Leadership Effectiveness (i.e. Visionary Leadership and People

Leadership), and two functions of Management Effectiveness (i.e. Strategic

Goal Management and Performance Management). Visionary Leadership

requires the ability to develop an overall purpose and vision and the ability to

facilitate the changes to attain this vision. People Leadership requires the

ability to make other individuals feel valued and the ability to develop their

contribution to the organisation. Strategic Goal Management requires an ability

to define organisational goals and the ability to develop systems to attain these

goals. Finally, Performance Management requires the ability to initiate action

towards a specific goal and the ability to monitor progress.

The ILMDP employs a third dimension that represents a range in focus from the

external to the internal, as can be seen in Figure 3.2. This dimension identifies

an external and internal role for each function, resulting in eight MLE roles (i.e.

visioning, facilitating, brokering, directing, achieving, monitoring, stewarding,

and coaching) (Cacioppe & Albrecht 2000). The eight roles provide the ILMDP

version of behaviourally complex MLE. Each role can be categorised as

belonging to one of two areas as four of the roles contribute to management

effectiveness and four of the roles contribute to leadership effectiveness. These

various roles are described in turn in subsequent sections.

Page 85: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

83

Figure 3.2 Internal versus External Dimension of ILMDP Model

(Adapted from Cacioppe, 2000)

3.2.1.1 ILMDP Management Roles

In the ILMDP, management effectiveness consists of four roles, or skill sets (i.e.

achieving, brokering, monitoring and directing), which are discussed in turn.

The achieving role has an internal emphasis and asks one item for each of the

following four behaviours:

• I come up with good solutions when problems arise that might get in the

way of achieving our goals.

• I promote and support attempts to improve the standards of our products

and services.

• I provide people within our section with clear, accurate and timely

feedback on their performance.

• I create and develop new ideas and ways of approaching things.

The brokering role has an external emphasis and asks one item for each of

the following four behaviours:

Page 86: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

84

• I appropriately influence others to benefit the interests of our division,

section or workgroup.

• I effectively represent the interests and achievements of our work group

or section to higher levels of management.

• I negotiate effectively in order to obtain resources and outcomes which

help the overall success of our section.

• I am effective when speaking and presenting ideas at meetings or forums

held outside of our work group.

The directing role has an external focus and asks one item for each of the

following four behaviours:

• I keep a focus on important, high priority activities.

• I effectively delegate responsibility by giving people challenging jobs and

the freedom they need to do the job.

• I develop plans which clearly set out goals, tasks and timelines.

• I clearly communicate to people in the work group how their tasks and

activities fit into the broader organisational goals and objectives.

The monitoring role has an internal focus and asks one item for each of the

following four behaviours:

• I make sure that our decisions and actions comply with the organisation’s

policies, standards and rules.

• I make sure our section has good information about how we are

progressing toward our targets and goals.

• I monitor activities and procedures to ensure that our branch or section is

working as effectively and efficiently as possible.

• I follow up on decisions to make sure they are implemented.

3.2.1.2 ILMDP Leadership Roles

In the ILMDP, leadership effectiveness consists of four roles, or skill sets,

(visioning, facilitating, coaching and stewarding), which are discussed in turn.

Page 87: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

85

The visioning role has an external focus and asks one item for each of the

following four behaviours:

• I inspire others to believe in and have enthusiasm for the organisation’s

values and its vision.

• I communicate a long range vision for the section so people have a clear

sense of direction and purpose.

• I work at developing possibilities and new opportunities that contribute to

our vision or goals.

• I initiate and implement changes necessary to achieve our vision and

benefit the organisation as a whole.

The facilitating role has an internal focus and asks one item for each of the

following four behaviours:

• I manage meetings effectively (e.g. set realistic agendas, encourage

participation, keep to time, set action items).

• I use a participative style of management by involving others when

planning, goal setting, decision making etc.

• I effectively address and manage conflicts and disagreements as they

arise within our section.

• I facilitate group discussions effectively (e.g. clarify issues, help get

consensus, use brainstorming etc.).

The coaching role has an internal focus and asks one item for each of the

following four behaviours:

• I help and encourage people within the Division/Section develop their

skills and potential (e.g. share my skills, discuss training opportunities,

suggest ways to improve etc.).

• I demonstrate team leadership and build good team relations.

• I effectively manage people not performing to the required standard (e.g.

help them find solutions, set agreed outcomes, coaching etc.).

• I praise people for their positive contributions and achievements.

Page 88: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

86

The stewarding role has an external focus and asks one item for each of the

following four behaviours:

• I develop positive relations with people who use our services and/or

products (e.g. external clients, internal customers, etc.).

• I make sure our section gets feedback from our customers about how

well we are meeting their needs.

• I liaise and cooperate with other divisions, departments or sections so

the organisation, overall, provides good service.

• I demonstrate a strong commitment to customer satisfaction in my day-

to-day activities (e.g. respond to customer complaints and ideas on how

we can improve our service).

Each of the ILMDP items are scored on an bipolar 10-point Likert-type scale

that ranges from one (strongly disagree) to ten (agree totally), with a ‘not

applicable category on the side, as can be seen in Figure 3.3. Two anomalies

are noted in Cacioppe and Albrecht’s (2000) scale. Firstly, there is a positive

bias in the words used to anchor the numbers on the Likert scale. The most

neutral answer, ‘neither agree nor disagree’, is set at four rather than at a mid-

point in the scale. Secondly, there are more categories and as such different

adverbs used on the positive side of the scale (i.e. 6 = agree somewhat and 9 =

agree very strongly) than are used on the negative side. Justifications for the

use of an 10-point scale or the text anchors used are not detailed by the

developers of the scale (Cacioppe & Albrecht 2000; Cacioppe & Albrecht 2001).

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Not

Applicable Disagree Strongly

Disagree Moderately

Disagree Slightly

Neither Agree nor Disagree

Agree Slightly

Agree Somewhat

Agree Moderately

Agree Strongly

Agree Very

Strongly

Agree Totally

Figure 3.3 ILMDP’s 10-Point Scale

Responses to the ILMDP are used to calculate 15 scores that are based on the

32-items (i.e. eight role scores, four function scores, a leadership score, a

Page 89: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

87

management score and a total MLE score), as can be seen in Table 3.1. Each

of the eight roles is scored by averaging the four-items that were used to

measure the relevant subscale. Each function score is an average of the eight-

items from the two relevant role scales. Similarly, the Leadership area score is

an average of the 16-items from the four leadership role scales and the

Management area score is an average of the 16-items from the four

management role scales. Finally, the total MLE score is the average of all of

the 32-items.

Table 3.1 Relationship Between Total, Area, Function and Role ILMDP Scores

(Adapted from Cacioppe & Albrecht 2001)

ILMDP Area Functions Roles

Total MLE

Management

Strategic Goal Management

Achieving (4-items) Brokering (4-items)

Performance Management

Directing (4-items) Monitoring (4-items)

Leadership

Visionary Leadership

Visioning (4-items) Facilitating (4-items)

People Leadership

Coaching (4-items) Servicing (4-items)

There has been limited empirical investigation of the ILMDP’s underlying

structure, although Cacioppe and Albrecht (2001, p. 130) noted that the ILMDP

items were “adapted and refined from the literature (e.g. Quinn et al., 1996),

have been tested with statistical methods and reflect feedback received from

over 500 managers as to what behaviours effective leaders and managers need

to demonstrate”. Cacioppe and Albrecht (2001, p. 131) also referred to the

results of a confirmatory factor analysis of 304 target managers ratings for their

super-ordinates on the ILMDP, although there is only a limited discussion of the

analysis. They examined:

Page 90: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

88

A number of statistics to see if they met accepted benchmark levels (ratio of chi-square to degrees of freedom being less than 2, the comparative fit index being greater than .93, the non-normed fit index exceeding .9 and the RMSEA confidence intervals falling below .08). All of the indices reached benchmark levels…We further established that the 8-factor model provided a superior fit to alternative 1-factor, 2-factor and 4-factor models…. The 2-factor model consisted of the left and right halves, the 4-factor model consisted of the four quadrants…..The results clearly support the validity of the 8-factor model of leadership and management.

This is the only published indication of the ILMDP’s measurement properties.

Another paper by the same authors updated this discussion with minor changes

and a more succinct explanation of the model (Cacioppe & Albrecht 2000). A

key change noted was the move from using a five-item scale for each role to a

four-item scale. However, there was no discussion of the statistical support for

such a shift.

In summary, the 32-items of the ILMDP used in the study provided a measure

of MLE behavioural complexity. It consists of eight four-item subscales, each of

which was measured on a 10-point bipolar Likert-type scale. The ILMDP is

structured as a multi-source feedback measure, with ratings from others in

addition to self-assessment, and provides a convenient sample of MLE

behavioural complexity. However, before the ILMDP role, function and total

scores can be used the constructs’ psychometric properties need to be

examined.

3.2.2 The Washington State Sentence Completion Test

MMS was measured through the Washington University Sentence Completion

Test (WUSCT, form 81) (Hy & Loevinger 1996). The WUSCT is a structured

projective paper and pencil test as can be seen in Table 3.2.

Page 91: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

89

Table 3.2 Form 81 of the Washington University Sentence Completion Test

(Hy & Loevinger, 1996, pgs 28-31)

1. When a child will not join in group activities… 2. Raising a family… 3. When I am criticised… 4. A man's job… 5. Being with other people… 6. The thing I like about myself is… 7. My mother and I… 8. What gets me into trouble is… 9. Education… 10. When people are helpless… 11. Women are lucky because… 12. A good father … 13. A girl has a right to… 14. When they talked about sex, I… 15. A wife should… 16. I feel sorry… 17. A man feels good when… 18. Rules are… 19. Crime and delinquency could be halted if… 20. Men are lucky because… 21. I just can’t stand people who… 22. At times he (she) worried about… 23. I am… 24. A woman feels good when… 25. My main problem is… 26. A husband has a right to … 27. The worst thing about being a man (woman)… 28. A good mother… 29. When I am with a woman (man) … 30. Sometimes he (she) wished that… 31. My father… 32. If I can’t get what I want… 33. Usually he (she) felt that sex… 34. For a woman a career is… 35. My conscience bothers me if… 36. A man (woman) should always…

The WUSCT assesses an individual’s overall MMS and assigns an ED stage.

It has 36 sentence stems, such as ‘When a child will not join in group

activities….’ and ‘Being with other people…’ There are both male and female

versions of the WUSCT, which are essentially the same except for variations in

relevant pronouns (i.e. he versus she) and nouns (i.e. man versus woman) in

Page 92: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

90

items 22, 27, 29, 30, 33 and 36. The equivalent wording for the female version

is shown in parenthesis in Table 3.6. Items 1 to 18 and 19 to 36 can be used

as alternative matched forms, but reliability is maximised when the 36 sentence

stems are included in the calculation of a person’s ED stage (Hy & Loevinger

1996).

A key advantage of the WUSCT is the existence of a feedback loop between

the theoretical model and the measure (Hauser 1976; Hauser 1993). This

includes the development of a comprehensive manual containing detailed

scoring guides for each of the 36 sentence stem items and instructions for

scoring an overall ED level score (Hy & Loevinger 1996). The scoring guide for

each sentence stem contains a text introduction, expected response categories

and examples of actual responses.

Item responses from a sample are pooled (i.e. all responses to item #1 are

collated) to ensure the scoring of each response occurs “without regard to

other responses or other data concerning the subject who made the response”

(Loevinger 1993, p. 7). Each response is assigned to one of the nine ED levels

by matching it with examples from the scoring guide. The scores for the 36

sentence stems are then collated for each individual and the collated

responses are used to assess the person’s core ego functioning level.

Three processes are used to triangulate the overall ED score. First, item

responses are used to arrive at an ED total item sum (TIS) score. Second, a

standard ogive scoring algorithm is used to determine an ED level score.

Finally, the overall protocol is reviewed visually by the researcher to assess the

impression of ED level indicated by the whole protocol. The three scores are

compared and a final ED level is assigned.

Page 93: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

91

The scoring manual contains a self-training guide with graduated exercises that

enable researchers to train themselves as WUSCT raters. The self-training

module produces raters who have high agreement among themselves and with

raters who were personally trained by Loevinger (median correlation between

raters for total protocol scores r = .86 and for agreement within a half stage

(r = .94) (Loevinger 1979). A substantial number of studies have consistently

found high levels of inter-rater reliability in various populations (Manners &

Durkin 2001). Such high levels of inter-rater reliability are a testament to the

underlying construct validity (Loevinger 1979). For example, “median complete

agreement between raters per item was 77%. The median correlation between

raters for total protocol scores was .86” (Loevinger 1979’, p. 285).

The WUSCT has been found to have high levels of split-half reliability (Novy &

Francis 1992; Redmore & Waldman 1975) and test-retest reliability (Redmore

& Waldman 1975; Weiss, Zilberg & Genevro 1989). Loevinger (1979) has

argued the open-ended and semi-structured format of the WUSCT provides

one form of assessing content validity as it invites the test-taker to articulate

their frame of reference, the very concept the test claims to measure.

Traditional means of assessing external validity are not always useful with

structural-developmental theories because the relationship between such

underlying structures and overt behaviour is often complex and non-linear

(Broughton 1978; Loevinger 1976; Loevinger 1993; Manners & Durkin 2001).

For example, initial studies exploring the construct validity of ED theory and the

WUSCT produced mixed results (Hauser 1976). Comprehensive reviews of

the literature have concluded there is strong evidence to support the construct

validity of the WUSCT (Hauser 1976; Hauser 1993; Loevinger 1979; Loevinger

Page 94: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

92

1993; Manners & Durkin 2001). Construct validity was found in studies

assuming non-monotonic relationships, where the importance of certain

aspects peaked with specific stages of development. For example,

delinquency occurred more frequently at lower ED levels (specifically the

impulsive stage) (Frank & Quinlan 1976); intellectualising and projection

increased with ED stages (Haan, Stroud & Holstein 1973); behavioural modes

of responsibility corresponded with ED levels (Blasi 1971) and conformity

maximally occurred at the conformist ED range (Hoppe 1972). Evidence for the

unitary structure of the WUSCT was found in a principal component analysis

(Loevinger, Wessler & Redmore 1970) and in two studies that failed to identify

item subsets within the SCT (Blasi 1971; Lambert 1972 in Loevinger, 1979

#565). Finally, the WUSCT has been found to correlate with other measures of

ED (Helson & Wink 1987; Rozsnafszky 1981; Sutton & Swensen 1983;

Westenberg & Block 1993). In their comprehensive review, Manners and

Durkin (2001, p. 548) reviewed evidence for the central tenets of ED theory

and the relationship of the WUSCT to alternate measures and concluded there

was “substantial support for the construct validity of ego development”.

With regard to the discriminant validity of the WUSCT, numerous studies have

confirmed that ED is distinct from, although moderately related to, key

personality traits, such as openness to experience, emotional security and

intimacy, and psychological-mindedness, and aspects of interpersonal

relations, such as greater nurturance, trust, valuing of individuality, and

responsibility (Hauser 1993). Positive correlations between ED levels, as

identified by the WUSCT, and IQ have been reported that range from .10 to

.50, with higher correlations for samples that included gifted students (Hauser

Page 95: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

93

1976; Loevinger 1976; McCrae & Costa 1980; Westenberg & Block 1993). The

results of a meta-analysis support the assertion that ED is distinct from verbal

intelligence with a weighted average correlation between these two constructs

of .32, being much lower than correlations between alternative tests of

traditional intelligence (r= .88) (Cohn & Westenberg 2004). The few studies

exploring the relationship between ED and socio-economic status (SES)

suggest there is a relationship, particularly for adolescents (Browning 1987;

Redmore & Loevinger 1979). However, it appears this relationship may be

less relevant for adults (Hansell et al. 1984) and, in some contexts, may be

irrelevant (Snarey & Lydens 1990). Rather than SES alone, broadening

experiences, such as higher education and work complexity, have consistently

been associated with higher levels of ED (Hansell et al. 1984; Snarey & Lydens

1990). In summary, ED is a distinct construct from personality, IQ and SES

(Manners & Durkin 2001).

The theoretical assumption of non-monotonic relationships between ED and

behaviour, which was mentioned earlier, means evidence for the predictive

validity of the WUSCT has been difficult to obtain and, consequently, is largely

based on probabilistic relationships (Manners & Durkin 2001). In a review of 13

studies, behavioural and group membership correlates were found to exist

between ED, as measured by the WUSCT, and conformity, delinquency,

responsibility and occupational choice (Loevinger 1979). As was noted by

Manners and Durkin (2001, p. 558), “further research is required before the

predictive validity of ego development can be said to have substantive empirical

support”.

Page 96: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

94

In summary, for this study the concept of MMS has been operationalised as an

individual’s ED score as measured by the WUSCT. The WUSCT is an

established measure with substantial support for inter-rater reliability, construct

validity and discriminant validity from the other constructs that were of interest in

the study.

3.2.3 The Mayer, Salovey and Caruso EI Test

EI was measured using Mayer, Salovey and Caruso’s (2002) Emotional

Intelligence Test (MSCEITv2). Specifically the four branch scores of Perceiving

Emotion, Facilitating Thought, Understanding Emotion and Managing Emotion

were employed. As was described in Chapter 2, the MSCEITv2 is an ability

measure of EI that is based on Mayer and Salovey’s (1997) four-branch ability

EI model. The MSCEITv2 is available in hard copy (question booklet and

answer sheet) and has an online version. The test takes approximately 40

minutes to 60 minutes to complete.

The MSCEITv2 has 141-items that are spread disproportionately over eight

tasks referred to as the Faces task, the Pictures task, the Sensations task, the

Facilitation task, the Blends task, the Changes task, the Emotion Management

task and the Emotional Relations task. Each of these tasks relate to one of four

EI abilities (i.e. Perceiving and Appraising Emotion, Using Emotion to Facilitate

Thought, Understanding Emotions, and Managing Emotions), as can be seen in

Figure 3.4.

Branch 1 (referred to as Perceive Emotion throughout this thesis) is measured

through Task A (Faces) and Task E (Pictures). Branch 2 (referred to as

Page 97: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

95

Facilitate Thought) is measured through Task B (Facilitation) and Task F

(Sensations). Branch 3 (referred to as Understand Emotion) is measured

through Task C (Changes) and Task G (Blends). Branch 4 (referred to as

Manage Emotion) is measured through Task D (Emotion Management) and

Task H (Emotional Relations). The four abilities map to two EI areas labelled

Experiential EI and Strategic EI. These two areas are combined to calculate

Total EI. The four branches and eight tasks with sample items are described in

the following sections.

Figure 3.4 Structure of MSCEITv2: Total EI, Area, Branch and Task Levels

(Mayer, Salovey & Caruso, 2002, pg 71)

3.2.3.1 Branch 1: Perceive and Appraise Emotion

The first branch of the MSCEITv2 (Perceive Emotion), refers to a person’s

ability to recognise and express how they are feeling and how those around

them are feeling. Indicative examples of the task items are displayed in Table

3.3.

This ability involves “paying attention to and accurately decoding emotional

signals in facial expressions, tone of voice, and artistic expressions” (Mayer,

Page 98: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

96

Salovey & Caruso 2002, pg 19). The MSCEITv2 assesses this ability through

the Faces (Task A) and Pictures (Task E) tasks. Task A and E require

respondents to view a face or picture and assess how much each of five

emotions are being expressed.

Table 3.3 Indicative Items for Faces and Pictures Tasks from MSCEITv2

Branch 1 Perceive and Appraise Emotion

Task A:

Faces

Instructions: How much is each feeling below expressed by this face?

Task E:

Pictures

Instructions: How much is each feeling below expressed by this picture?

(NOTE - these sample items have been created as indicative examples of format only in keeping with the request of the test publishers to avoid invalidating actual test items).

Page 99: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

97

3.2.3.2 Branch 2: Use Emotion to Facilitate Thought

The second branch of the MSCEITv2 (Facilitate Thought) refers to a person’s

ability to use emotions to facilitate their thinking and other cognitive processes.

The MSCEITv2 assesses this ability through the Facilitation (Task B) and the

Sensations (Task F) tasks. Indicative examples of the items for these tasks are

displayed in Table 3.4. Task B asks respondents to assess how different

moods would interact with and/or support thinking and reasoning. Task F asks

respondents to assess how different feelings are similar to other sensate

experiences.

Table 3.4 Indicative Items for Facilitation and Sensations Tasks from MSCEITv2

Branch 2: Facilitate Thought

Task B:

Facilitation

Instructions: Please select an answer for each item

What mood might be helpful when studying for an exam?

Task F:

Sensations

Instructions: For each item below, you are asked to imagine feeling a certain way. Answer as best as you can, even if you are unable to imagine the feeling.

Imagine feeling confused after having a conversation with your boss about your performance over the last year. How much is the feeling of confusion like each of the following?

(NOTE - these sample items have been created as indicative examples of format only in keeping with the request of the test publishers to avoid invalidating actual test items).

3.2.3.3 Branch 3: Understand Emotion

The third branch of the MSCEITv2 (Understand Emotion), refers to knowledge

of emotions including the stimuli for different emotional reactions, the range of

Page 100: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

98

specific emotions (i.e. from irritation to rage), and the relationship between

different emotions and how they can combine. The MSCEITv2 assesses this

ability through the Changes (Task C) and the Blends (Task G) tasks. Indicative

examples of the items for these tasks are displayed in Table 3.5.

Table 3.5 Indicative Items for Changes and Blends Tasks from the MSCEITv2

Branch 3: Understand Emotion

Task C:

Changes

Instructions: Select the best alternative for each of these questions.

Geoff felt irritated and began to feel extremely frustrated. If nothing changes, he will eventually feel _________.

a. Happy b. Disappointed c. Angry d. Disgusted e. Neutral

Task G:

Blends

Instructions: Select the best alternative for each of these questions.

A feeling of unease most closely combines the emotions of _____.

a. Love, anxiety, surprise, anger b. Surprise, pride, anger, fear c. Acceptance, anxiety, fear, anticipation d. Fear, joy, surprise, embarrassment e. Anxiety, caring, anticipation

(NOTE - these sample items have been created as indicative examples of format only in keeping with the request of the test publishers to avoid invalidating actual test items).

Task C asks respondents to assess how different emotions transition from one

to another. Task G asks respondents to assess how different feelings are

similar to other sensate experiences.

3.2.3.4 Branch 4: Manage Emotion

The fourth branch of the MSCEITv2 (Manage Emotion) refers to a person’s

ability to regulate emotions their own emotions and the emotions of others to

facilitate personal understanding and growth. The MSCEITv2 assesses this

Page 101: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

99

ability through the Emotion Management (Task D) and the Emotional Relations

(Task H) tasks. Indicative examples are displayed in Table 3.6.

Table 3.6 Indicative Items for Emotion Management and Emotional Relations

Tasks from the MSCEITv2 Branch 4 Manage Emotion

Task D:

Emotion management

Instructions: Please select a response for each action.

After a positive coaching session, David was feeling motivated and energised. How well would each action preserve his mood?

Action 1) He started doing some filing.

a. Very ineffective b. Somewhat ineffective c. Neutral d. Somewhat effective e. Very effective

Action 2) He made a list of what he wanted to accomplish this week and by the month end.

a. Very ineffective b. Somewhat ineffective c. Neutral d. Somewhat effective e. Very effective

Action 3) He decided to use the feeling to motivate himself to address a few things he had been procrastinating about.

a. Very ineffective b. Somewhat ineffective c. Neutral d. Somewhat effective e. Very effective

Action 4) He decided to go for coffee with a colleague who was frustrated with his job and try to cheer him up. a. Very ineffective b. Somewhat ineffective c. Neutral d. Somewhat effective e. Very effective

Task H:

Emotional relations

Instructions: Please select a response for each action.

Marie and Cindy are good friends. Recently, Marie felt betrayed by Cindy when she disclosed a confidence that Marie had specifically asked her to keep to herself. How effective would Marie be in maintaining a good relationship if she choose to respond in each of the following ways?

Response 1) She decided to ignore the situation.

a. Very ineffective b. Somewhat ineffective c. Neutral d. Somewhat effective e. Very effective

Response 2) She sent Marie a detailed email about how she felt. a. Very ineffective b. Somewhat ineffective c. Neutral d. Somewhat effective e. Very effective

Response 3) Marie shared her feelings with Cindy in person and asked for help in understanding why Cindy acted as she did.

a. Very ineffective b. Somewhat ineffective c. Neutral d. Somewhat effective e. Very effective

Response 4) Marie decided Cindy was not a trustworthy person and started to ignore her.

a. Very ineffective b. Somewhat ineffective c. Neutral d. Somewhat effective e. Very effective

(NOTE - these sample items have been created as indicative examples of format only in keeping with the request of the test publishers to avoid invalidating actual test items).

Page 102: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

100

Task D asks respondents to assess the effectiveness of four different actions in

regulating emotions to achieve a specific outcome. Task H asks respondents to

evaluate the effectiveness of different responses towards achieving specific

outcomes.

3.2.3.5 MSCEITv2 Scoring and Measurement Properties

The MSCEITv2 scores are based on 122 of the 141-items because a

“psychometric analysis on the normative sample suggested (the) exclusion of

19-items. These items were not deleted from the actual test so as to preserve a

balanced layout” (Lopes et al. 2004, p. 1021).

Each MSCEITv2 branch score is the average of two unadjusted raw task

scores. Similarly, each MSCEITv2 area score is the average of the four related

task scores. That is, Experiential EI consists of Tasks A, B, E and F and

Reasoning EI consists of Tasks C, D, G and H. Finally, the Total EI score is the

average of the eight unadjusted raw task scores.

The MSCEITv2 scoring algorithms are held by MHS, the test publisher, and

MSCEITv2 scores are obtained by sending completed forms, online or in hard

copy, to the MHS scoring service. While the algorithms are proprietorial, the

general protocol has been described as having six distinct stages (Mayer,

Salovey & Caruso 2002). The MSCEITv2 produces fifteen different scores

(eight task scores, four branch scores, two area scores and an overall EI score).

Each of the scores can be adjusted according to age, gender and/or ethnic

group. The scores are based on the task scores which are used to compute

branch, area and the total scores. Raw scores are converted to percentile

Page 103: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

101

scores. Percentile scores are converted to standard scores with a mean of 100

and a standard deviation of 15 (Mayer, Salovey & Caruso 2002). MHS also

provides a scatter score that is used to assess the amount of variation across

the eight tasks (the absolute value of the differential between each of the eight

task percentiles and the average, divided by eight) and a positive-negative bias

score that is based on responses to the pictorial stimuli that are used in the

MSCEITV2.

As was mentioned in Chapter 2, the MSCEITv2 uses a consensus scoring

method based on norms from the general population and/or from a panel of

experts (Mayer, Salovey & Caruso 2002). To compute the task scores, each

item response is “assigned a score based on the proportion of the consensus

sample (either general or expert) that selected that response” (Mayer, Salovey

& Caruso 2002, p. 67). The item scores are then used to compute an average

response score for each task, which is termed an unadjusted raw task score.

The expert consensus and the general population consensus have been found

to be highly correlated for the eight task scores (Palmer et al. 2005; Mayer,

Salovey & Caruso 2002). However, the use of consensus scoring continues to

be a key controversy for the MSCEITv2 (Keele & Bell 2009; MacCann et al.

2004; Roberts, Zeidner & Matthews 2001). There has been considerable

debate between those who feel large samples of individuals converge on

correct answers (Legree 1995) and those who feel consensual scores indicate

conformity and are not necessarily the correct answer (Roberts, Zeidner &

Matthews 2001).

In terms of construct validity, the original factor analysis on the MSCEIT

suggested three possible structures with increasing good fit indices. The

Page 104: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

102

solutions included a one factor structure, representing the overall EI score, a

two factor structure, representing the two area scores, and a four factor

structure, representing the four branch scores of the model (Mayer, Salovey &

Caruso 2000). Lopes et al. (2004) also used confirmatory factor analysis and

found support for the four-branch model. More recent studies, however, do not

support these results (Gignac 2005; Kafetsios 2004; Keele & Bell 2008; Palmer

et al. 2005; Rode et al. 2008). Specifically, fit indices in these later studies have

suggested a two factor structure (Day & Carroll 2004; Roberts et al. 2006) or a

three factor structure (Keele & Bell 2008; Palmer et al. 2005; Rode et al. 2008).

Another view is that three of the four MSCEITv2 EI abilities cascade from

Branch 1 (Perceive Emotion) to Branch 3 (Facilitate Thought) and then to

Branch 4 (Manage Emotion) (Joseph & Newman 2010).

The MSCEITv2 is said to have discriminant validity in relation to personality (J.

Ciarrochi, A.Y. Chan & P. Caputi 2000; Roberts, Zeidner & Matthews 2001;

Rosete & Ciarrochi 2005; J.V. Ciarrochi, A.Y. Chan & P. Caputi 2000). Low to

moderate correlations with IQ support the assertion that ability EI has

discriminant validity as an intelligence (Cote & Miners 2006; Lopes, Salovey &

Straus 2003; Roberts, Zeidner & Matthews 2001; Rode et al. 2007; Van Rooy,

Viswesvaran & Pluta 2005). In addition, low correlations between the

MSCEITv2 and mixed measures of EI, which are thought to be confounded with

personality measures, suggest the MSCEITv2 is not measuring personality

(Van Rooy, Alonso & Viswesvaran 2005). The MSCEITv2 subscales have

convergent validity in that the inter-correlations range from .27 to .51 (Mayer et

al. 2003).

Page 105: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

103

Pusey (2000) reported that the MSCEITv2 had good face validity and

concurrent validity is assumed from the moderate correlations between the

MSCEITv2 and psychological well-being scales (r= .28) (Brackett & Mayer

2003), measures of depression (r= -.33) and trait anxiety (r= -.29) (Head 2002

in Brackett, 2004 #642) , the JACBART (r= -.02 to .20) and the Vocal-I

(r= -.10 to .24) (Roberts et al. 2006).

The reliability of the MSCEITv2 scales vary. Good to high split-half reliability

scores have been reported when using both general and expert scoring for

Total EI score (r = .89 to .93), for the Strategic and Experiential Area scores (r =

.76 to .90) and for Branch 1 scale (Perceive Emotion) (r= .89 to .91) (Palmer et

al. 2005; Mayer, Salovey & Caruso 2002). The MSCEITv2 User’s Manual

reports good split-half reliability scores for Branch 2, 3 and 4, again using both

general and expert scoring (r= .76 to .79, .77 to .80 and .81 to .83 respectively)

(Mayer, Salovey & Caruso 2002). However, other studies have reported split-

half reliability scores lower than those reported for the normative sample. For

example, for Branch 2 (r= .63 to .80), Branch 3 (r= .56 to .73) and Branch 4 (r=

.60 to .76) (Clarke 2010; Lopes, Salovey & Straus 2003; Lopes et al. 2004;

Palmer et al. 2005; Weinberger 2003). The internal consistency of the

MSCEITv2 branch scales, when measured by Cronbach’s alpha, range from

moderate to good (α= .88, .65, .71 and .86 for Branch 1 through 4 respectively)

(Kafetsios 2004).

Follesdal (2009) has suggested Classical Test Theory measures of reliability,

such as split-half correlations and Cronbach’s alpha, are not appropriate given

the MSCEITv2’s multifaceted design and has argued for the use of G and D

studies from Generalisability Theory. Such an analysis of the MSCEITv2

Page 106: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

104

produced generalisability coefficients of the four branch scores that ranged from

.46 to .71, suggested low variance components associated with persons

(3% to 10%) and identified additional sources of variance that are not identified

in the current measurement design (Follesdal & Hagtvet 2009). However, this

analysis was based on the total 141-items, rather than on the subset of 122-

items that are used to produce the MSCEITv2 scores. The test-retest reliability

of the full-scale MSCEITv2 has been reported at r= .86 (Brackett & Mayer

2003).

A key issue debated in the literature concerns whether MSCEITv2 task scores,

branch scores, area scores or the total score are the most appropriate for use in

subsequent statistical analysis. This issue has both a theoretical and

measurement aspect. First, it is important to understand the theoretical basis as

to whether the dimensionality of EI is best viewed as a profile, an aggregate or

a latent construct (Law, Wong & Mobley 1998). If ability EI is comparable to

GMA, it could be considered to be a latent multidimensional construct (Wong &

Law 2002). Numerous studies have taken this approach (Cote & Miners 2006).

However, if the four branches represent individual aspects that are distinct from

each other, the score could be considered to be a profile score.

In summary, the MSCEITv2 is a relatively new measure of ability EI that seems

to have acceptable validity and varying levels of reliability. The MSCEITv2

continues to be one of the best measures of the ability model of EI (Mayer,

Caruso & Salovey 2000). Consequently, the MSCEITv2 Total EI score was

selected used in the present study. However, given the above discussion, it was

considered important to confirm the appropriate level of the construct hierarchy

to employ by examining the reliability of the MSCEITv2 subscales.

Page 107: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

105

3.2.1 The Wonderlic Personality Test

A critical control variable in the model is traditional intelligence (IQ). The

Wonderlic Personnel Test (WPT) (Wonderlic & Associates 2002) was used to

measure IQ in the present study. The WPT is a 12-minute timed test that

includes 50-items of increasing difficulty. The items represent a mixture of

numerical ability, reasoning, spatial ability, mathematical, logical and

geometrical questions. Scoring was done in two stages. First, an answer key

provided by the test publishers was used to identify correct and incorrect

responses on each protocol. Next, a score for each participant’s WPT was

calculated by subtracting the number of incorrect responses from the total

number of questions attempted/answered. For example, if a respondent

answered 35 of the 50 questions, five of which were incorrect, their WPT score

would be: 35 – 5 = 30.

The WPT has been found to be a reliable and valid measure of general mental

ability (GMA) or IQ (Dodrill 1983). Test-retest reliability has been estimated at

between .84 and .94 for up to a five-year period (Dodrill 1983). Split-half

reliability has been estimated at between .88 and .94 (Wonderlic & Associates

2002) and alternate form reliability has been estimated at between .73 and .95

(Schoenfeldt 1985). The concurrent validity of the WPT has been established

through correlations with the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale and the revised

Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-revised), ranging from .84 to .93

(Dodrill 1983; Dodrill & Warner 1988; Hawkins et al. 1990). The WPT

interpretive guide suggests people who score 20 or more have managerial

potential and that people who score 28 or more have upper level management

potential, as can be seen in Appendix B.

Page 108: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

106

3.3 METHODS

3.3.1 Sample Sources

This research study was made possible through access to a convenient sample

of managerial-leaders undertaking leadership development at the AIM-UWA

Integral Leadership Center. Two samples were procured and employed. These

are each described in turn below.

3.3.1.1 The Separate ILMDP Sample

Given the limited published information on the measurement properties of the

ILMDP a separate sample of data was deemed necessary to test these

characteristics. This sample was extracted from the data-base at the AIM-UWA

Integral Leadership Center on 10 April 2005. The total population included 460

target managerial-leaders who had participated in a leadership development

program that included the use of the ILMDP as a multi-source feedback

developmental process. This data set, referred to throughout the remainder of

the thesis as ‘the separate ILMDP sample’, consisted of individual responses

from each ‘other-rater’ on all 32-items of the ILMDP.

3.3.1.2 The Main Sample

The main sample was collected from a pool of managerial-leaders undertaking

a leadership development program through the AIM-UWA Integral Leadership

Center from September 2003 to September 2006. Specifically, managerial-

leaders participating in seven programs, total population 615, were invited to

participate in the research. In response, 259 participants provided some

element of data towards the research representing a response rate of 42%.

Page 109: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

107

The eight variables of interest in the main sample include IQ, the four EI branch

scores (Perceive Emotion, Understand Emotion, Facilitate Thought and

Manage Emotion), meaning-making structure (MMS), and MLE. Unfortunately,

not all respondents provided a complete data set. For example, while

participants in the TAFE program provided complete data responses, the

program did not utilise the ILMDP as a measure of MLE and so participants

could not be included as complete cases to test the research hypotheses. The

final size of the main sample was n=169 complete cases representing a

response rate of 27.5%.

3.3.2 Data Collection for the Main Sample

During the period of September 2003 to September 2006, ILMDP reports

administered by the AIM-UWA Integral Leadership Center included an

information sheet introducing the researcher, the research topic, eligibility

requirements for participation, assurances of confidentiality and expected

benefits to participants (Appendix C). This was consistent with the

requirements outlined by The University of Western Australia’s Human

Research Ethics Committee. Benefits to participants included a personalised

feedback report and a group debrief of these results. An invitation and faxback

form, which are shown in Appendix D, were also included with the MSF

feedback and interested people were asked to complete the form and send it

back to the researchers by fax or email. By submitting the faxback form,

potential participants confirmed their availability and preference for a data

collection session. Potential participants were then contacted by email and/or

phone to confirm a date and time of the data collection session for which they

Page 110: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

108

registered. Travel and parking instructions were also provided for participants

who attended a session at The University of Western Australia.

The majority of data collection occurred in face-to-face sessions facilitated by

the researcher. The sessions varied in size from as few as one participant to as

many as 25 participants. Sessions were held at the UWA Business School or in

a private meeting room at the participant’s place of work. Measures were taken

to ensure the data collection rooms were set apart from other activity, provided

a quiet environment with no distractions, had appropriate lighting and enough

space for each participant. However, due to geographic limitations, it was

necessary to send some participants written instructions on how to complete the

WUSCT and the online MSCEITv2 and to arrange for them to undertake the

timed WPT at a later date (generally within 30 days of completing the

MSCEITv2 and WUSCT).

The same order of administration was followed for each face-to-face data

collection workshop. At the start, participants were given two copies of the

informed consent sheet, which can be seen in Appendix E. In keeping with

UWA Human Research Ethics procedures, participants were asked to read and

complete one copy and retain the second copy for their records. The signed

sheets provided informed consent for the researcher to access each

participant’s individual MSF feedback from the AIM-UWA Integral Leadership

Centre.

Once the informed consent sheets had been collected, participants were

reminded they would be completing three different tests. The WPT timed test

which was administered in accordance with the instructions in the user’s-

Page 111: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

109

manual was completed first (Wonderlic & Associates 2002). Next participants

completed the MSCEITv2 test. Where internet access was available,

participants completed the online version of the MSCEITv2. When this was not

possible, paper and pencil versions of the MSCEITv2 were used. In both cases,

the researcher briefed participants that the MSCEITv2 was to be completed

independently without input from others and in its entirety, in accordance with

the MSCEITv2 technical manual’s requirements (Mayer, Salovey & Caruso

2002). Comparison of results from the paper and pencil version of the

MSCEITv2 with the online version suggested the two were equivalent (Mayer et

al. 2003). Finally, a paper and pencil survey, which can be seen in Appendix F,

and which included the WUSCT, was handed to and completed by participants.

The average total administration time for the three instruments was

approximately one hour and 45 minutes.

3.3.3 Data Scoring and Entry of the Main Sample

As data from each construct came from different sources, a coding structure

was of critical importance. Each participant in the study was assigned a data

entry code number that facilitated collation and ensured the anonymity of

individual responses. Specific data handling, entry and coding procedures are

discussed in turn for each construct.

The ILMDP MSF data were downloaded from the AIM-UWA Integral Leadership

Centre database. A procedure was written by the Centre’s external database

designer to facilitate the process. The data were captured in Excel files that

required sorting to identify survey respondents. After this had been done, the

data files were collated to produce a format that was appropriate for exporting

Page 112: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

110

into the SPSS computer program that was used to undertake most of the

subsequent data analysis. As noted earlier in this chapter, the ILMDP uses a 0

to anchor the end point of the scale as ‘Not Applicable’. This was recognised

as a potential limitation in terms of calculating average other-rater scores. As

such, scores of 0 were identified as missing data to facilitate missing data

analysis.

The completed WPTs were scored by hand using a scoring key provided by the

test publisher. The WPT is scored by subtracting the number of incorrect

answers from the total number of questions attempted by the respondent. This

number is known as the raw WPT score. An age correction can also be applied

to produce an age-adjusted score. These calculations were initially performed

by hand on the back of each WPT and then double-checked by the researcher

at least one day after the initial calculation. The final raw score and age-

adjusted scores were entered into the SPSS computer program. The entered

numbers were again checked against the hard copies of the WPT to ensure

they matched and that no errors had been made in data entry.

Approximately half of respondents completed the online version of the

MSCEITv2, which alleviated the need for data entry for these respondents. The

other half of the sample completed paper and pencil versions of the MSCEITv2

that the researcher entered onto the MHS website. Scoring of completed

MSCEITv2 protocols was performed on the website using the expert scoring

method. A single data file, provided in an Excel spreadsheet, was downloaded

from the MHS website on 4 June 2010. The 141-item response scores, and the

raw task, branch, area and Total EI scores, which were then imported into the

SPSS computer program.

Page 113: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

111

Two raters, including the researcher, were trained to score the WUSCT using

the self-training methodology that was provided in the WUSCT training manual

(Hy & Loevinger 1996). Both raters scored a sample of ten protocols collected

from another research project, independently. The results obtained were

debriefed. This process confirmed a high level of inter-rater agreement (80%)

and enabled the negotiation of coding procedures for the main sample. For the

main sample, both raters independently scored each item response by

applying both ego level (i.e. 2 through 9) and thematic codes (i.e. a, b, c etc.) to

a total of 6,588 item responses (183 protocols of 36-items each). The coding

from each rater was compared and areas of disagreement identified. These

responses were reviewed again, in turn, by each rater until a consensus

decision was reached. This iterative process was chosen to maximise learning

and the possibility of correctly assigning an ED level to each protocol.

3.3.4 Data Analysis

The data analysis had two distinct phases. In the first phase, preliminary data

analysis was used to obtain a feel for the data and to assess the constructs’

measurement properties. In the second stage, the hypotheses outlined in

Chapter 1 and Chapter 2 were examined.

For the separate ILMDP sample, preliminary data analysis began by ensuring

all cases had a minimum number of other-raters, identifying any missing data

and confirming the accuracy of data entry. Inter-rater similarity on each of the

items and ILMDP subscales was assessed to establish justification for

averaging other-rater scores. The other-rater average scores were tested for

the existence of outliers and the assumption of normality. Finally, the

Page 114: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

112

measurement properties of the ILMDP subscales were assessed using

confirmatory factor analysis.

Preliminary data analysis for the main sample included data preparation and

testing for normality. Once again, inter-rater agreement and the measurement

properties of the ILMDP items were assessed. In addition, inter-rater agreement

for the WUSCT coding and the reliabilities and inter-correlations of the

MSCEITv2 scales were assessed.

The second stage of data analysis involved testing the posited hypotheses

using the main sample. This required correlation analysis (H1) and hierarchical

regression analysis (H2).

Each of the above statistical processes are described in turn.

3.3.4.1 Data Preparation and Assumption Checking

Both samples were subjected to appropriate data preparation: ensuring

accuracy of data entry, identifying missing data, screening outliers and testing

the assumption of normality (Tabachnick & Fidell 2007). Univariate descriptive

statistics for each of the variables were examined to ensure accuracy of the

data file and identify any missing values. Screening for outliers was undertaken

by reviewing z scores and box-plots for each of the variables. Cases with

standardised scores in excess of 3.29 (p<.001) were identified as potential

outliers and considered for transformation and/or deletion (Tabachnick & Fidell

2007). The assumption of normality was assessed by both statistical and

graphical means: skewness, kurtosis, Kolmogorov-Smirnov statistic with a

Lilliefors significance level (p > .05), normal probability plots and detrended

Page 115: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

113

normal plots (Tabachnick & Fidell 2007). Distributions were considered highly

skewed if the skewness statistic was greater than |1|, moderately skewed when

between |1 to .5| and approximately symmetrical when between |.5 to 0|

(Bulmer 1979). Normality was indicated by kurtosis statistics equal 0 and

Kolmogorov-Smirnov statistics below 2.

3.3.4.2 Inter-Rater Similarity

Issues of inter-rater similarity are relevant for both the ILMDP and the WUSCT

as both involve the use of ratings by more than one judge to arrive at a score

for the target managerial-leaders. Establishing inter-rater similarity for the

WUSCT scores provides evidence of construct validity and gives confidence in

the consistency of the assigned ratings (Loevinger 1979). Similarly, inter-rater

similarity for the groups of other-raters on the ILMDP is necessary to justify

combining the individual scores into an average other-rater score (James &

Brett 1984; James, Demaree & Wolf 1993). In addition, analysing the

measurement properties of the ILMDP using a data set with high levels of inter-

rater similarity eliminates a potential source of ‘noise’ and therefore assists

interpretation of results. Inter-rater similarity is assessed through both inter-

rater agreement (IRA) and inter-rater reliability (IRR), which are discussed in

turn.

Inter-rater agreement (IRA) refers to the degree to which different judges

provide similar scores for the target manager being rated; more specifically, it

attempts to provide an indication of how interchangeable the scores of the

other-raters are (Tinsley & Weiss 1975). While different IRA measures have

been found to yield highly convergent results, the use of multiple indices is

Page 116: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

114

recommended to assist in interpreting such data (LeBreton & Senter 2008).

The IRA for the WUSCT coding was examined using percentage agreement

scores and Pearson correlations. In contrast, four indices were used assess

agreement on the ILMDP: the rWG and the rWG(J) (James & Brett 1984; James,

Demaree & Wolf 1993) and the ADM and the ADM(J) indexes (Burke & Dunlap

2002; Burke, Finkelstein & Dusig 1999).

The average deviation (AD) indices (i.e. ADM and ADM(J)) are pragmatic

measures of IRA that can be used when a single target is being rated (Burke,

Finkelstein & Dusig 1999). These indices represent a dispersion score that is

calculated around the mean for a group of other-raters on a single item scale

(ADM) or multi-item scales (ADM(J)). The ADM index is calculated as is shown in

equation 3.1, in which N is the number of judges assessing the target individual

on the single item j, xjk is the kth judge’s rating, and Xji is the mean of the judges’

ratings on item j.

ADM(J) is the average of the item ADM(j) scores as can be seen in equation 3.2.

Smaller scores indicate greater agreement. The recommended upper limit for

the AD indices is < c / 6, where c=the number of response options (Burke &

(3.2)

(3.1)

Page 117: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

115

Dunlap 2002). The ILMDP has 10 response options. Consequently, the

interpretive standard for examining IRA using ADM and ADM(J) is equal to or less

than 10/6 (i.e. < 1.67).

The rWG and rWG(J) indices are used to measure IRA for individual items and for

multi-item scales respectively. Both indices define agreement in terms of the

proportional reduction in error variance (LeBreton & Senter 2008). The rWG

index is appropriate when a researcher is interested in the agreement of

multiple judges on a single target for a single variable. This index is calculated

as is shown in equation 3.3, where Sx2 is the observed variance on variable X

from K raters and δE2 is the variance that would be expected when there was a

complete lack of agreement among a set of raters (James & Brett 1984; James,

Demaree & Wolf 1993).

In this study, a range of distributions for the completely random expected

variance were computed and these are reported as has been recommended by

Lebreton & Senter (2008). That is, distributions were computed for a uniform

(rWG UN), a slightly skewed (rWG_sskew) and a moderately skewed (rWG_Mskew) null

distribution (LeBreton & Senter 2008). The argument for using the uniform

distribution is based on an assumption that if raters are responding randomly

each response option has an equal chance of being selected (LeBreton &

Senter 2008). Alternatively, the restriction of variance hypothesis argues that

organisational development interventions, if even marginally successful, would

attenuate variance in job performance, resulting in a skew to the null distribution

(3.3)

Page 118: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

116

(LeBreton et al. 2003). There is an extension of the above index appropriate for

situations where multiple judges assess a single target on multiple items. It is

calculated as shown in equation 3.4 where Sx2 is the mean of the observed

variance for J items (James & Brett 1984; James, Demaree & Wolf 1993):

When other-raters are in total agreement rWG and rWG(J)=1 and when other-

raters are in total lack of agreement rWG and rWG(J)=0 (James, Demaree & Wolf

1993). It is possible to obtain negative values for rWG and rWG(J) which are then

reset to 0 (James & Brett 1984). The commonly reported interpretive standard

for establishing moderate IRA is rWG= .70 (Lance, Butts & Michels 2006).

However the number of other-raters within the group can significantly attenuate

rWG (Kozlowski & Klein 2000). This effect can be avoided by increasing the

number of other-raters to at least ten, or increasing the number of items for

rWG(j). A more-inclusive set of guidelines for IRA interpretive standards are

intended to encourage researchers to “think more globally about the necessity

of high versus low within-group agreement based on their particular research

question and composition model” (LeBreton & Senter 2008). LeBreton and

Senter (2008) suggest moderate agreement can be assumed if the index is >

.51, strong agreement can be assumed if the index is > .71, and very strong

agreement can be assumed if the index is > .91. Given the small number of

other-raters for each of the target managers (that range from 3 to 12), the

hurdle for assuming a moderate level of IRA was set at r WG > .51.

(3.4)

Page 119: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

117

Inter-rater reliability (IRR) refers to the consistency in ratings provided by

multiple other-raters of multiple target managerial-leaders (Bliese 2000; James,

Demaree & Wolf 1993; Kozlowski & Hattrup 1992; LeBreton et al. 2003).

Intraclass correlation (ICC) indices are widely used as a measure of IRR

(Shrout & Fleiss 1979). Deciding on which of the six different ICC indices is

appropriate depends on a number of factors, namely whether the analysis is

assumed to have:

• one way versus two way effects;

• random or fixed column effects;

• single or average measure;

• the existence of interaction effects (McGraw & Wong 1996).

Three different indices that are relevant within the context of this research will

be discussed in turn. ICC (1) is a one-way random effect ANOVA that provides

information on the consistency and consensus of individual judges’ ratings

(LeBreton & Senter 2008). It is computed as shown in equation 3.5 (McGraw &

Wong 1996):

In this formula MSR is the mean squares for the rows in the data file (or the

targets), MSW is the mean square within the target’s group of raters, and K is

the number of other-raters per target. ICC (1) is an estimate of the reliability of

ratings by individual judges and/or an effect size regarding how the judges

(3.5)

Page 120: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

118

rating is affected by the managerial-leader they are evaluating (LeBreton &

Senter 2008). ICC (1) can be interpreted as an effect size with the standard of

ICC (1) > .01 representing a small effect, ICC (1) > .10 representing a medium

effect, and ICC (1) > .25 representing a large effect (Murphy, Myors & Wolach

1998 in James et al 2008).

However it is not an individual judge’s MSF scores that are of interest but rather

average MSF scores from a group of judges on a range of items, which was the

case in the present study. The ICC (K), a one-way random ANOVA, is

appropriate in this situation because each target individual in this study received

feedback from a different group of judges, which means the column effect does

not vary in a systematic way. ICC (K) assesses the stability of the average

ratings for a group of judges and is computed as is shown in equation 3.6

(McGraw & Wong 1996):

In contrast, IRA analysis of the WUSCT scores requires the ICC (A, 1) index,

which is a two way mixed effect ANOVA, because the same two judges scored

each of the target managerial-leaders. As such, the column effect does vary in

a systematic way. ICC (A, 1) is an estimate of the absolute agreement of

measurements made by the two judges and is computed as shown in equation

3.7 (McGraw & Wong 1996):

(3.6)

(3.7)

Page 121: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

119

As was the case with ICC (1), ICC (A, 1) values are interpreted as the reliability

of individual judge’s scores and/or an estimate of effect size (LeBreton & Senter

2008). Traditionally, the minimum interpretive standard for all ICC indices has

been a score of .70 (Nunnally & Bernstein 1978). However, LeBreton & Senter.

(2008) express caution in this regard, as all ICC’s are measures of IRA + IRR.

Consequently, high values are only possible when both IRA and IRR are high,

while low values may indicate low IRA, low IRR or they may indicate both

(LeBreton & Senter 2008).

3.3.4.3 Measurement Properties

In order to confirm the appropriateness of employing the Total EI score, the

reliability of the each of the MSCEITv2 subscales was examined. This analysis

was undertaken on the 122-items identified as those being more valid items

(Mayer 2010). Split-half reliability coefficients, with a Spearman-Brown

correction, were calculated at the total, area and branch levels, given the item

heterogeneity for these scales (Mayer et al. 2003). In contrast, coefficient

alphas were calculated at the task level because items at this level have the

same response format (Mayer et al. 2003). All of the reliability coefficients were

calculated using MISCEITv2 raw scores.

The measurement properties of the ILMDP were analysed by examining the

construct validity, reliability and convergent validity of each of the subscales.

The ILMDP subscales were specified a priori and the AMOS structural equation

modelling program was used to undertake the confirmatory factor analysis

procedures that were used in this phase of the analysis. Construct validity can

be assessed by a range of goodness-of-fit (GOF) indices that fall roughly into

Page 122: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

120

broad categories referred to as absolute, incremental or comparative, and

parsimony fit measures (Byrne 2010). Reporting multiple indices from different

categories is recommended as being an appropriate way to establish

acceptable fit and, hence, construct validity (Hair Jr et al. 1995).

Absolute fit measures indicate how well the hypothesised model is reflected in a

particular data set and include the Chi-Square Statistic (χ2) and the Root Mean

Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), both of which were reported in the

present study (Hair Jr et al. 1995). The χ2 is the only statistically based SEM fit

measure that quantifies the difference between the observed and estimated

covariance matrices. Non-significant (large p-value) small Chi-Square Statistics

indicate that there is no significant difference between the two matrices (Hair Jr

et al. 1995). However, with sample sizes over 200 the p-values of the χ2

becomes less meaningful as they can suggest a poor fit for a trivial difference

between the two matrices, which leads to the need to report additional GOF

indices.

The RMSEA index, an alternate measure of absolute fit, indicates how well a

model with optimally chosen parameters fits a population’s covariance matrix

(Browne & Cudeck 1993). RMSEA is interpreted based on general rules of

thumb that vary depending on the number of variables, the sample size and the

value of the comparative fit index (the CFI), which is an incremental fit index

and is discussed below. To demonstrate GOF a model with less than 12

variables requires a RMSEA value that is less than .07 for sample sizes greater

than 250, and a RMSEA value that is less than .08 for sample sizes under 250,

both with a CFI that is greater than or equal to .97 (Hair Jr et al. 1995). The

advantages of the RMSEA index include sensitivity to model misspecification,

Page 123: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

121

widely used and accepted interpretive guidelines and the ability to construct

confidence intervals around the RMSEA to see if it differs significantly from zero

(MacCallum & Austin 2000).

The second category, which includes incremental or comparative fit indices,

contrasts the hypothesised model with a baseline model, usually the null model

(Byrne 2010). Commonly used and widely accepted indices include the

Comparative Fit Index (CFI) and the Tucker Lewis Index (TLI) both of which are

reported in the present study (Hair Jr et al. 1995). Again, the CFI and TLI are

interpreted based on general rules of thumb that vary depending on the number

of variables and the sample size. For example, GOF for a model with less than

12 variables requires CFI and TLI values that are greater than .95 for sample

sizes that are larger than 250, and greater than .97 for sample sizes that are

under 250 (Hair Jr et al. 1995).

Construct reliability that is greater than or equal to .70 suggests that there is a

high level of internal consistency within a scale. Construct reliability represents

the degree to which a scale is free of measurement error (Hair Jr et al. 1995)

and is computed using equation 3.8.

Convergent validity is a measure of both how strongly related the items on a

scale are and also where there is more information than there is error in a

construct. Significant item loadings (i.e. greater than .6) are indicative of

convergence of items onto their relevant subscales and also of the scale’s GOF

(Bagozzi, Yi & Phillips 1991). Fornell and Larcker (1981b) developed the

(3.8)

Page 124: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

122

average variance extracted (AVE) score as a way to assess convergent validity,

arguing scores of .50 or more imply there is more information than noise in the

construct of interest and, consequently, that a scale has convergent validity.

The AVE score is computed using equation 3.9.

3.3.4.4 Regression Analysis (H2)

To explore the potential predictive validity of the IQ, EI and ED constructs,

hierarchical regression analyses were estimated using the other-rater average

scores as the dependent variable. Based on a theoretical assumption that IQ is

the strongest single predictor of MLE, IQ was entered in the first stage of this

analysis. In the next two stages, EI and MMS were entered respectively based

on the theoretical assumption that EI would operate as a subset of MMS.

Variables needed to achieve a significant probability of .05 for them to be

considered in the forward step and entered into the regression model.

3.4 CHAPTER SUMMARY

The present chapter operationalised the constructs in the study’s research

model. As was noted, the MLE construct was measured using the ILMDP, the

EI construct was measured using the four branch scores of the MSCEITv2, IQ

was measured using the WPT and the MMS construct was measured using the

WUSCT. The type and number of items, the scoring approach that was used

and the psychometric properties of each of the measures was then discussed in

turn. Finally, an overview of the methods that were used to obtain participants,

(3.9)

Page 125: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

123

to collect data needed, to enter and score the data and to analyse the data was

provided. In the next chapter, the results obtained from the preliminary data

analysis phase are presented.

Page 126: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

124

Page 127: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

125

Chapter 4

PRELIMINARY DATA ANALYSIS

4.1 CHAPTER OVERVIEW

In Chapter 3, the measures and methods that were used in the study were

described, including the two phases of data analysis that were undertaken (the

preliminary data analysis phase and the analysis of the hypothesised

relationships). This chapter presents the results of the preliminary data analysis

phase, which consists of an assessment of the following:

• The ILMDP’s measurement properties (section 4.2).

• The MSCEITv2’s measurement properties (section 4.3).

• The inter-rater agreement on the WUSCT coding (section 4.4).

As was noted in the previous chapter, the study’s research model included the

following constructs:

• Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness (MLE) measured by the Integral

Leadership and Management Development Profile (ILMDP).

Page 128: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

126

• Emotional intelligence (EI) measured by the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso

Emotional Intelligence Test version 2 (MSCEITv2).

• Intelligence (IQ) measured by the Wonderlic Personnel Test (WPT).

• Meaning-making structure (MMS) measured by the Washington

University Sentence Completion Test (WUSCT).

As was also discussed in Chapter 3, the measurement properties of the latter

two constructs are well established in the literature. In contrast, the ILMDP has

received limited attention beyond the work of the developers in their initial

presentation of the model (Cacioppe & Albrecht 2000; Cacioppe & Albrecht

2001). Similarly, the measurement properties of the MSCEITv2 have been the

subject of some debate within the literature, particularly with regard to which

level of the construct measurement hierarchy to employ. Consequently, the

measurement properties of the ILMDP and the MSCEITv2 were explored and

the results obtained are also discussed in this chapter. A summary of the results

obtained in the examination of the inter-rater reliability of the WUSCT scores is

also provided. This preliminary data analysis provides the foundation for the

second phase of data analysis, which is discussed in Chapter 5.

4.2 THE MEASUREMENT PROPERTIES OF THE ILMDP

Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness (MLE) was operationalised using the

ILMDP (Cacioppe & Albrecht 2000). The ILMDP is a multi-source feedback

(MSF) instrument that provides self-scores and scores from other-raters. Using

scores from other-raters alleviates issues of common method variance.

Consequently, these scores were used in the present study. However, in light of

Page 129: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

127

the limited published information on the measurement properties of the ILMDP

it was seen as critical to ensure that the construct was valid and reliable. As

was noted in section 3.3.1.1, a separate sample was used for this analysis

which is briefly described in the next section, which also includes a description

of the data preparation and screening undertaken prior to this analysis. The

results of the analysis of inter-rater similarity for each of the ILMDP’s items and

subscales are then described. These other-average scores for the ILMDP

items and subscales were used to analyse the ILMDP’s convergent and

discriminant validity. Finally, the results of the factor analysis procedures that

were used to assess the ILMDP’s factor structure are discussed.

4.2.1 The Sample and Data Preparation

The separate sample (n=460) that was used to test the measurement properties

of the ILMDP was initially examined to ensure there were a minimum number of

other-raters for each target manager and each case was screened for potential

data entry errors.

To begin, the number of other-raters was calculated for each case. Only cases

that had a minimum of three other-raters for all of the ILMDP’s items were

retained. This analysis resulted in a useable sample of 364 of the initial 460

managerial-leaders with a range of three to twelve other-raters for each of the

32-items on the ILMDP. This sample size is more than adequate for the

analysis of a scale’s measurement properties (Mendoza, Stafford & Stauffer

2000). The items’ scores in each of the 364 cases fell within the expected

ranges of 1 to 10, confirming no instances of missing data and suggesting input

errors were unlikely.

Page 130: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

128

4.2.2 Inter-Rater Agreement and Reliability

As was also described in Chapter 3, before the ILMDP data could be used in

the second phase of the analysis, it was important to establish the ILMDP items’

and subscales’ inter-rater agreement (IRA) (through the rWG and ADM indices)

and inter-rater reliability (through the ICC (1) and ICC (K) indices). The IRA

analysis and the IRR analysis were undertaken using the SPSS program and

the results obtained are described in subsequent sections.

4.2.2.1 IRA and IRR for the ILMDP Items

As was noted in Chapter 3, the rWG statistic for each of the raters on each of the

32-items were computed using the uniform (rWG_UN), slightly skewed (rWG_skew)

and moderately skewed (rWG_Mskew) distributions. Table 4.1 provides a summary

of the mean scores for rWG_UN, rWG_skew, rWG_Mskew, ADM, ICC (1) and ICC (K) for

each of the 32-items. As was recommended by LeBreton and Senter (2008),

measures of central tendency and dispersion for each item were computed and

reported for rWG_UN in Appendix G, for rWG_skew in Appendix H and for rWG_Mskew

in Appendix I.

The rWG statistics for the individual raters on all 32-items ranged from perfect

agreement to perfect non-agreement (i.e. from 0 to 1, with negative values reset

to 0 for the reasons noted in Chapter 3). Despite the variance in the scores, the

mean rWG_UN values for each of the 32-items ranged from .65 to .81 and all were

above the recommended .51 interpretive standard. Assuming a uniform null

distribution, there was an acceptable level of inter-rater agreement between the

other-raters about their managerial-leaders.

Page 131: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

129

Table 4.1 Mean IRR and IRA Statistics for the ILMDP Items

ITEM Agreement Reliability

rWG_UN rWG_skew rWG_Mskew ADM Mean ICC (1) Mean ICC (K)

1 .71 .62 .53 1.05 .15 .51 2 .76 .68 .61 .95 .14 .50 3 .80 .74 .68 .85 .18 .55 4 .81 .75 .69 .84 .14 .52 5 .81 .75 .69 .87 .12 .47 6 .71 .62 .54 1.05 .21 .63 7 .72 .64 .55 1.03 .13 .50 8 .72 .64 .55 1.03 .13 .49 9 .75 .67 .59 .98 .08 .36 10 .80 .74 .68 .87 .11 .43 11 .67 .57 .47 1.14 .09 .38 12 .65 .54 .43 1.17 .08 -.11 13 .78 .72 .65 .91 .18 .58 14 .76 .69 .61 .96 .10 .39 15 .69 .60 .50 1.09 .12 .45 16 .74 .66 .58 .99 .12 .43 17 .72 .63 .55 1.03 .09 .38 18 .79 .72 .65 .88 .13 .49 19 .78 .71 .64 .93 .11 .46 20 .77 .70 .63 .94 .08 .21 21 .77 .70 .62 .93 .09 .37 22 .76 .68 .61 .95 .19 .50 23 .70 .61 .52 1.09 .12 .42 24 .71 .62 .53 1.05 .10 .31 25 .75 .67 .60 .97 .19 .60 26 .69 .60 .51 1.08 .13 .44 27 .74 .66 .58 1.01 .14 .52 28 .76 .68 .61 .95 .27 .59 29 .78 .71 .64 .92 .18 .55 30 .72 .63 .54 1.03 .12 .44 31 .76 .69 .61 .95 .18 .59 32 .78 .71 .65 .92 .09 .38

Page 132: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

130

The same analysis was repeated for the rWG_skew and rWG_Mskew null distributions.

The mean rWG_skew scores for the items ranged from .54 to .75. Once again, all

of the mean scores were above the recommended interpretive standard. The

mean rWG_Mskew scores ranged from .43 to .69. In this case, three of the items

fell below the recommended interpretive standard. If a slightly-skewed null

distribution is assumed, there was an acceptable level of inter-rater agreement

between the other-raters regarding their managerial-leaders. With the exception

of three items, this was also true for a moderately-skewed null distribution.

The mean ADM indexes for the 32-items ranged from .84 to 1.17 and all were

below the 1.67 recommended hurdle. Based on the ADM index, it can also be

seen that there was an acceptable level of inter-rater agreement in the present

sample.

The ICC (1) scores ranged from .08 to .27, indicating a medium to large effect

at an individual level. The implication is that group membership, has only a

moderate effect on the scores provided by other-raters and that mean scores

between target managerial-leaders can be compared. However, the ICC (K)

scores ranged from -.11 to .63, suggesting the item means were not as stable

as had been hoped.

Given the variation in these results, the percentage of indices meeting the

respective interpretive standards were determined. The number of items

meeting the rWG hurdle ( > .51) and the ADM hurdle ( < 1.67) was summed for

each case, for each of the three null distributions. The results that were

obtained indicated the modal case in the sample had 100% of the 32-items

meeting all of the IRA hurdles, as can be seen in Table 4.2.

Page 133: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

131

Table 4.2 Central Tendency and Dispersion Total # of ILMDP Items Meeting IRA

Interpretive Standards

# Items meeting ADM standard

# Items meeting rwg standard for

uniform null distribution

slightly skewed null distribution

moderately skewed null distribution

# % # % # % # %

Mean 30 97 28 86 25 79 23 72 Median 32 100 31 97 28 88 26 80 Mode 32 100 32 100 32 100 32 100 Std. Deviation 4 13 6 20 7 23 8 26 Percentile 25 29 91 25 79 21 66 18 56

50 32 100 31 97 28 88 26 80 75 32 100 32 100 31 97 30 94

As can also be seen in Table 4.2, the mean number of items (and their

percentage out of 32 total items) ranged from ADM=30 (97%), rWG_UN=28 (86%),

rWG_skew=25 (79%), to rWG_Mskew=23 (72%). Further examination of the dispersion

showed that the lowest 25th percentile of the sample had large numbers (and

percentage) of items meeting the interpretive standard: ADM=29 (91%),

rWG_UN=25 (79%), rWG_skew=21 (66%), and rWG_Mskew=18 (56%).

The analysis of the mean rWG, ADM and ICC statistics suggested that the

sample of other-raters had moderate to high levels of IRA and IRR on each of

the 32-items within the ILMDP. Consequently, all of the cases and items in the

present sample were included in the subsequent analysis.

4.2.2.2 IRA and IRR for the ILMDP Scales

As was also noted in Chapter 3, the ILMDP has a total of eight role subscales

and a number of aggregations of these scales (i.e. the Management Function

Scale, the Leadership Function Scale, and the total MLE Scale). Given the

limited information on the measurement properties of the ILMDP, it was decided

Page 134: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

132

to examine all of these scales at this early stage in the analysis. Consequently

the rWG(J), ADM(J), ICC (1) and ICC (K) statistics were computed for the eleven

scales. Once again, the rWG(J) statistics were computed using the uniform,

slightly skewed and moderately skewed distributions. The mean rWG(J)_UN for

each the eleven scales was above the recommended interpretive standard,

ranging from .87 to .98. The mean rWG(J)_skew for 10 of the 11 scales was also

above the recommended interpretive standard, ranging from .68 to 1.00. The

exception was the Coaching Scale, which had an rWG(J)_skew score of -.04.

The mean rWG(J)_Mskew was above the recommended interpretive standard for

eight of the 11 scales, with these scores ranging from .61 to .98. The three

exceptions were the Monitoring Scale (rWG(J)_Mskew = .43), the Facilitating Scale

(rWG(J)_Mskew = .23) and, once again, the Coaching Scale (rWG(J)_Mskew = .19). It is

not unusual for rWG(J) scores to be larger than rWG scores, as the process of

averaging parallel items reduces the influence of measurement error (James &

Brett 1984). These results can be seen in Table 4.3. The minimum, maximum

and standard deviations for each scale were again computed and, for rWG_UN the

results are reported in Appendix J, for rWG_skew the results are reported in

Appendix K, and for rWG_Mskew the results are reported in Appendix L.

The eleven scales’ ADM(J) scores met the recommended standard, with values

ranging from .09 to 1.06. Similarly, the ICC (1) scores ranged from .48 to .67

and the ICC (K) scores ranged from .79 to .97, indicating high levels of rater

consistency and stability in the mean ratings respectively. The analysis of the

mean rWG(J) and the ICC (1) and ICC (K) statistics suggested the sample of

other-raters had moderate to high levels of IRA and IRR for the ILMDP scales,

with the three possible exceptions noted. The potential problem of varying IRA

Page 135: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

133

with the Coaching, Monitoring and Facilitating scales is noted and may

contribute to the decision to ultimately exclude them. In the interest of exploring

the measurement properties of the ILMDP further, they are currently retained for

further analysis.

Table 4.3 Mean IRA and IRR Statistics for the ILMDP Scales

Scale Agreement Reliability

rWG_UN rWG_skew rWG_Mskew ADM(J)

Mean ICC (1)

Mean ICC (K)

1 Brokering .90 .84 .92 .96 .63 .87 2 Directing .88 .68 .83 1.00 .51 .81 3 Achieving .92 .86 .61 .94 .48 .79 4 Monitoring .92 .81 .43 .93 .55 .83 5 Facilitating .89 .78 .23 1.05 .54 .82 6 Visioning .89 .85 .85 1.00 .67 .89 7 Stewarding .90 .90 .90 .92 .56 .84 8 Coaching .87 -.04 .19 1.06 .58 .85 9 Management .96 .98 .94 .96 .54 .95 10 Leadership .97 .95 .94 1.01 .55 .95 11 32-item Scale .98 1.01 .98 .98 .53 .97

4.2.3 Testing for Outliers and Normality

Prior to further analysis, the other-average scores were examined to identify

outliers and see whether they were normally distributed. Outliers were

identified by examining boxplots and z scores for each of the items. A total of

49 cases were identified as outliers on one or more items. A total of 14 cases

were deleted as they appeared to be outliers on more than three of the 32

items. The remaining sample (n=350) was then examined for normality.

Normality was examined by looking at histograms, normal and de-trended

normal plots, Kolmogorov-Smirnov z-statistics and the skewness of each of the

Page 136: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

134

32 items (Tabachnick & Fidell 2007). The skewness, and the Kolmogorov-

Smirnov statistics for the 32 items can be seen in Table 4.4.

Table 4.4 Descriptive statistics for the IMLDP Items

Item # Mean Std Dev Skewness Statistic

Kolmogorov-Smirnov Z

Statistic Sig.

1 7.65 .86 -.40 1.24 .10 2 7.89 .86 -.78 2.01 .00 3 8.24 .74 -.48 1.56 .02 4 8.33 .71 -.67 1.28 .08 5 8.11 .66 -.55 1.33 .06 6 7.79 .92 -.45 1.59 .01 7 7.77 .87 -.43 1.13 .16 8 7.54 .84 -.31 1.35 .05 9 7.74 .78 -.29 1.43 .03

10 8.18 .67 -.32 1.99 .00 11 7.21 .94 -.37 0.98 .30 12 7.25 .91 -.37 1.35 .05 13 8.11 .70 -.78 1.23 .10 14 7.79 .70 -.42 1.22 .10 15 7.40 .86 -.36 1.65 .01 16 7.98 .87 -.46 1.21 .11 17 7.86 .79 -.39 1.35 .05 18 8.14 .76 -.57 1.89 .00 19 7.98 .73 -.38 1.14 .15 20 7.96 .74 -.25 1.10 .18 21 8.17 .73 -.49 1.36 .05 22 7.74 .76 -.18 1.21 .11 23 7.59 .91 -.22 1.00 .27 24 7.72 .87 -.35 1.23 .10 25 7.95 .78 -.39 1.52 .02 26 7.56 .84 -.14 1.25 .09 27 7.74 .83 -.43 1.42 .04 28 7.87 .74 -.34 1.52 .02 29 7.86 .75 -.18 0.89 .41 30 7.53 .79 -.32 1.08 .19 31 8.11 .85 -.26 1.31 .06 32 7.85 .72 -.20 1.29 .07

The skewness statistics for most items (27 of 32) suggested they were

approximately symmetrical, as they were between |.5 to 0|. The remaining five

items had only a moderate level of skewness. In addition the Komolgorov-

Smirnoff z-scores for most items (22 of 32) suggest normality could be assumed

Page 137: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

135

(p>.05). The moderate skewness is likely the cause of the test failure, but it

was not seen to be a major concern. Consequently, all of the items in the

ILMDP were included in the next stage of the analysis.

4.2.4 Measurement Properties of the ILMDP Subscales

As was noted in Chapter 3, the measurement properties of each of the eight

ILMDP role subscales were analysed by examining their unidimensionality,

reliability and convergent validity. The four-item subscales were specified as

suggested by Cacioppe and Albrecht (2000) and the AMOS Structural Equation

Modelling (SEM) program’s Maximum Likelihood Analysis procedure was used

to estimate the relevant confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) for each. An

iterative process, described in detail subsequently, was followed if the initially

suggested model did not fit the data.

4.2.4.1 The Brokering Subscale

The four items that were used to measure the ILMDP Brokering role were:

I appropriately influence others to benefit the interests of our division,

section or workgroup.

I effectively represent the interests and achievements of our work group or section to higher levels of management.

I negotiate effectively in order to obtain resources and outcomes

which help the overall success of our section.

I am effective when speaking and presenting ideas at meetings or

forums held outside of our work group.

Page 138: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

136

The results of the CFA for these four items suggested the model was a good fit

to the data (χ2=2.80, df=2, p<.25, RMSEA= .03, CFI=1.00, TLI=1.00), with

loadings ranging from .71 to .84 as can be seen in Figure 4.1. The subscale’s

construct reliability was .87 and its AVE score was .63, suggesting the construct

was reliable and had convergent validity. Consequently the four-item Brokering

subscale was retained for subsequent analysis.

* item loading

Figure 4.1 ILMDP Brokering Subscale Item Loadings and Fit Statistics

4.2.4.2 The Directing Subscale

The four items that were used to measure the ILMDP Directing role were:

I keep a focus on important, high priority activities.

I effectively delegate responsibility by giving people challenging jobs

and the freedom they need to do the job.

I develop plans which clearly set out goals, tasks and timelines.

I clearly communicate to people in the work group how their tasks and

activities fit into the broader organisational goals and objectives.

Influence others

Represent the workgroup

Negotiate for the section

Speaking and presenting

.84*

.81*

.83*

.71*

Brokering

chi-square = 2.80 df = 2 p < .25 rmsea = .03 cfi = 1.00 tli = 1.00

Page 139: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

137

The results of the CFA suggested the model was not a good fit to the data

(χ2=26.51, df=2, p<.01), despite item loadings that ranged between .69 and .79.

An examination of the modification indices suggested this was due to the error

term for the first item (I keep a focus on important, high priority activities) and

the error term for third item (I develop plans which clearly set out goals, tasks

and timelines) being correlated. Correlated errors can cause problems in

subsequent analysis and, as Byrne (2001, p. 110) has noted, “the specification

of correlated errors for purposes of achieving a better fit is not an acceptable

practice.”

Consequently, following Simon and Usunier’s (2007) suggestion, it was decided

to remove one of the items. This change made theoretical sense as the two

items were similar as they both relate to priorities and planning. To this end, the

model was re-run with the correlated error and the item with the lowest loading

was deleted. However, its removal left no degrees of freedom with which to

assess model fit. An examination of the error variances suggested two could be

made equal without affecting model fit greatly, which provided the degree of

freedom required to examine the construct’s measurement properties.

The resulting CFA had item loadings ranging from to .69 to .87 and an

acceptable fit (χ2= .01, df=1, p<.91, RMSEA= .00, CFI=1.00, TLI=1.01), as can

be seen in Figure 4.2. Construct reliability in this case was .81 and the AVE was

score was .59, suggesting the revised construct was reliable and had

convergent validity. Consequently, the revised three-item Directing subscale

was retained for subsequent analysis.

Page 140: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

138

* item loading

Figure 4.2 ILMDP Directing Subscale Item Loadings and Fit Statistics

4.2.4.3 The Monitoring Subscale

The four items used to measure the ILMDP Monitoring role were:

I make sure that our decisions and actions comply with the

organisation’s policies, standards and rules.

I make sure our section has good information about how we are

progressing toward our targets and goals.

I monitor activities and procedures to ensure that our branch or

section is working as effectively and efficiently as possible.

I follow up on decisions to make sure they are implemented.

The results of the CFA suggested the four-item model was not a good fit to the

data (χ2=12.90, df=2, p<.01), although the item loadings ranged from .61 to .84.

The poor fit was again due to correlations between some of the error terms. An

examination of the modification indices indicated the correlated error between

the first item (I make sure that our decisions and actions comply with the

organisation’s policies, standards and rules) and the second item (I make sure

our section has good information about how we are progressing toward our

Page 141: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

139

targets and goals) was the issue. Consistent with the process described in

Section 4.2.4.2, the model was re-run with this correlation included in order to

identify an item to delete. This change made theoretical sense as the two items

relate to external standards. The first item had the lowest loading and was

deleted, again leaving no degrees of freedom to assess model fit.

An examination of the remaining error variances suggested two could be made

equal, providing the degree of freedom needed to examine the construct’s

measurement properties. The construct had an acceptable fit

(χ2= .80, df=1, p< .37, RMSEA= .00, CFI=1.00, TLI=1.00), with loadings ranging

from .80 to .82, as can be seen in Figure 4.3. Construct reliability was .86 and

the AVE score was .66, suggesting the construct was reliable and that it had

convergent validity. Consequently the revised three-item Monitoring subscale

was retained for subsequent analysis.

* item loading

Figure 4.3 ILMDP Monitoring Subscale Item Loadings and Fit Statistics

4.2.4.4 The Achieving Subscale

The four items that were used to measure the ILMDP Achieving scale were:

I come up with good solutions when problems arise that might get in

the way of achieving our goals.

Page 142: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

140

I promote and support attempts to improve the standards of our

products and services.

I provide people within our section with clear, accurate and timely

feedback on their performance.

I create and develop new ideas and ways of approaching things.

The results of the CFA suggested the model was not an acceptable fit to the

data (χ2=19.58, df=2, p<.01), despite items loading ranging between

.60 and .85. The poor fit was again due to correlations between some of the

error terms. An examination of the modification indices suggested the

correlation between the error term for the second item (I promote and support

attempts to improve the standards of our products and services) and the error

term for the third item (I provide people within our section with clear, accurate

and timely feedback on their performance) was the problem. Consistent with

the process described above in Section 4.2.4.2, the model was re-run with this

correlation. This change made theoretical sense, as both related to achieving

results by enabling others. The third item had the lowest loading and was

deleted, leaving no degrees of freedom left to assess model fit.

An examination of the remaining error variances suggested two of the items

could be made equal, which provided the degree of freedom required to

examine the construct’s measurement properties. The resulting CFA had an

acceptable fit (χ2=1.55, df=1, p< .21, RMSEA= .04, CFI=1.00, TLI=1.00), with

loadings ranging from to .79 to .88, as scan be seen in Figure 4.4. The

construct reliability was .86 and the AVE score was .67, suggesting the

construct was reliable and had convergent validity. Consequently, the revised

three-item Achieving subscale was retained for subsequent analysis.

Page 143: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

141

* item loading

Figure 4.4 ILMDP Achieving Subscale Item Loadings and Fit Statistics

4.2.4.5 The Stewarding Subscale

The four items that were used to measure the ILMDP Stewarding role were:

I develop positive relations with people who use our services and/or

products (e.g. external clients, internal customers, etc.)

I make sure our section gets feedback from our customers about how

well we are meeting their needs.

I liaise and cooperate with other divisions, departments or sections so

the organisation, overall, provides good service.

I demonstrate a strong commitment to customer satisfaction in my

day-to-day activities (e.g. respond to customer complaints and ideas

on how we can improve our service).

The results of the CFA suggested the model was not a good fit to the data

(χ2 =18.91, df=2, p<.01), although the item loadings ranged from .68 to .85. The

poor fit was again due to correlations between some of the error terms. An

examination of the modification indices suggested the correlation between the

error term for the second item (I make sure our section gets feedback from our

customers about how well we are meeting their needs) and the error term for

Page 144: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

142

the fourth item (I demonstrate a strong commitment to customer satisfaction in

my day-to-day activities) was the problem. Consistent with the process

described in Section 4.2.4.2, the model was re-run with this correlation. The

second item had the lowest loading and was deleted, leaving no degrees of

freedom left to assess model fit. This change made theoretical sense, as the

two items ask about engaging with and responding to customer complaints.

An examination of the remaining error variances suggested two could be made

equal, providing the degree of freedom required to examine the construct’s

measurement properties. The resulting CFA had an acceptable fit

(χ2=2.62, df=1, p<.10, RMSEA= .07, CFI=1.00, TLI=1.00), with loadings ranging

from to .76 to .85, as can be seen in Figure 4.5. Construct reliability was .85 and

the AVE score was .65, suggesting the construct was reliable and had

convergent validity. The revised three-item Stewarding subscale was therefore

retained for subsequent analysis.

* item loading

Figure 4.5 ILMDP Stewarding Subscale Item Loadings and Fit Statistics

4.2.4.6 The Coaching Subscale

The four items that were used to measure the ILMDP Coaching role were:

Page 145: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

143

I help and encourage people within the Division/Section develop their

skills and potential (e.g. share my skills, discuss training opportunities,

suggest ways to improve etc.).

I demonstrate team leadership and build good team relations.

I effectively manage people not performing to the required standard

(e.g. help them find solutions, set agreed outcomes, coaching etc.).

I praise people for their positive contributions and achievements.

The results of the CFA suggested the model was not a good fit to the data

(χ2=7.50, df=2, p<.02), despite item loadings ranging from .75 to .83. The poor

fit was again due to correlations between some of the error terms. An

examination of the modification indices suggested the correlation between the

error term for the third item (I effectively manage people not performing to the

required standard) and the error term for the fourth item (I praise people for their

positive contributions and achievements) was the problem. Consistent with the

process described in Section 4.2.4.2, the model was re-run with this correlation

in place. The third item had the lowest loading and was deleted, which meant

there were no degrees of freedom left to assess model fit. This change made

theoretical sense, as the two items are concerned with aspects of performance

management.

An examination of the remaining error variances suggested two could be made

equal, which provided the degree of freedom required to examine the

construct’s measurement properties. The resulting CFA had an acceptable fit

(χ2= .08, df=1, p<.78, RMSEA= .00, CFI=1.00, TLI=1.01), with loadings ranging

from to .80 to .83, as can be seen in Figure 4.6. Construct reliability was .85

Page 146: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

144

and the construct’s AVE score was .66, suggesting the construct was reliable

and had convergent validity. Consequently the revised three-item Coaching

subscale was retained for subsequent analysis.

* item loading

Figure 4.6 ILMDP Coaching Subscale Item Loadings and Fit Statistics

4.2.4.7 The Facilitating Subscale

The four items that were used to measure the ILMDP Facilitating role were:

I manage meetings effectively (e.g. set realistic agendas, encourage

participation, keep to time, set action items).

I use a participative style of management by involving others when

planning, goal setting, decision making etc.

I effectively address and manage conflicts and disagreements as they

arise within our section.

I facilitate group discussions effectively (e.g. clarify issues, help get

consensus, use brainstorming etc.).

The results of the CFA suggested the model was a good fit to the data

(χ2=2.65, df=2, p<.27, RMSEA= .03, CFI=1.00, TLI=1.00), with item loadings

ranging from .69 to .85, as can be seen in Figure 4.7. Construct reliability was

Encourage people to develop

Team leadership

Praise positive contributions

.80*

.83*

.81* Coaching

chi-square = .08 df = 1 p < .78 rmsea = .00 cfi = 1.00 tli = 1.01

Page 147: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

145

.85 and the AVE score was .60, suggesting the construct was reliable and had

convergent validity. The four-item Facilitating subscale was therefore retained

for subsequent analysis.

* item loading

Figure 4.7 ILMDP Facilitating Subscale Item Loadings and Fit Statistics

4.2.4.8 The Visioning Subscale

The four items that were used to measure the ILMDP Visioning role were:

I inspire others to believe in and have enthusiasm for the organisation’s

values and its vision.

I communicate a long range vision for the section so people have a

clear sense of direction and purpose.

I work at developing possibilities and new opportunities that

contribute to our vision or goals.

I initiate and implement changes necessary to achieve our vision and

benefit the organisation as a whole.

The results of the CFA suggested the model was a good fit to the data

(χ2=5.61, df=2, p<.06, RMSEA= .07, CFI=1.00, TLI=1.00), with item loadings

Page 148: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

146

ranging from .77 to .86, as can be seen in Figure 4.8. Construct reliability was

.77 and the AVE score was .68, suggesting the construct was reliable and had

convergent validity. The four-item Visioning subscale was therefore retained for

subsequent analysis.

* item loading

Figure 4.8 ILMDP Visioning Subscale Item Loadings and Fit Statistics

4.2.4.9 Summary Statistics and Correlation Analysis with Original Scales

The results of the eight CFAs described above are summarised in Table 4.5,

which includes the fit statistics, the final number of items retained, the range of

item loadings, the reliability and the AVE score for each scale. It can be seen

that each of the eight ILMDP subscales can be modelled as a one-factor

congeneric model. Each of the subscales met the required criteria for

unidimensionality, reliability and convergent validity; although in some cases

items had to be deleted from the analysis to achieve this outcome. To ensure

there had been no loss in information in reducing the number of items,

correlations were computed between the revised scales and the initially

suggested scales, as suggested by Thomas, Soutar and Ryan (2001). As can

be seen in Table 4.5, these correlations ranged from 1.00 (for the scales in

Page 149: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

147

which the four items were retained) to .96, suggesting no information was lost in

ensuring appropriate measurement properties in the present study.

Table 4.5 CFA Summary Statistics for the Eight ILMDP Subscales

Chi square

Sig df # Items

Item loadings

AVE Construct Reliability

Correlation with Original Subscale

Brokering 2.80 .25 2 4 .71 to .84 .63 .87 1.00**

Directing .01 .91 1 3 .72 to .87 .59 .81 .97**

Monitoring .080 .37 1 3 .80 to .82 .66 .86 .96**

Achieving 1.55 .21 1 3 .79 to .88 .67 .86 .97**

Stewarding 2.62 .10 1 3 .76 to .85 .65 .85 .97**

Coaching .08 .78 1 3 .80 to .83 .66 .85 .98**

Facilitating 2.65 .27 2 4 .69 to .75 .60 .85 1.00**

Visioning 5.61 .06 2 4 .77 to .86 .68 .77 1.00**

4.2.5 Discriminant Validity of the ILMDP Subscales

Discriminant validity is the extent to which different constructs (in this case, the

ILMDP subscales) are distinct from one another and provide unique information.

It can be assessed by comparing the squared correlations between the various

construct pairs with their AVE scores (Fornell & Larcker 1981a). Discriminant

validity can be assumed if the squared correlation between two constructs is

less than the AVE scores of the individual constructs.

The AVE scores for the various roles ranged from .59 to .68. However, many of

the subscales’ AVE scores were less than the squared correlations between

them as these correlations ranged from .51 to .78. These results are reported in

Table 4.6, in which the AVE scores are shown in the main diagonal and the

squared correlations are shown in the upper triangle of the table.

Page 150: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

148

Table 4.6 AVE and Squared Correlations for the ILMDP Subscales

Broker Director Monitor Achieve Steward Coach Facilitate Vision

Broker .64 .70 .60 .73 .66 .62 .65 .77

Director .59 .65 .72 .59 .65 .70 .75

Monitor .66 .72 .59 .51 .56 .57

Achiever .67 .63 .65 .61 .78

Steward .65 .65 .62 .63

Coach .66 .77 .66

Facilitate .60 .60

Vision .68

Thus, while the eight role subscales had convergent validity and reliability, they

did not seem to have discriminant validity. The eight factor structure suggested

by the scale’s developers does not appear to be appropriate to the data that

were obtained in the present study. Consequently, the structure of the ILMDP in

the present context was explored further and the results obtained in this phase

of the analysis are discussed in the next section.

4.2.6 Exploring the ILMDP’S Structure in the Present Study

As the hypothesised structure of the ILMDP is a circumplex, it was decided the

underlying structure should be first analysed using the PREFSCAL program

contained in SPSS (Busing 2006) to see if this was the case. However, this

analysis led to a degenerate solution suggesting the ILMDP is not circular, as

suggested by Cacioppe and Albrecht (2000; 2001), at least in the current

research context. Consequently, exploratory factor analysis (EFA), using the

same 350 respondents, was undertaken to identify the underlying structure of

Page 151: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

149

the ILMDP in the present study. The Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin measure of sampling

adequacy, which provides an indication of the presence of underlying factors,

was .97, which strongly suggested there were factors and that undertaking a

factor analysis would be useful (Hair Jr et al. 1995). Contrary to the model

suggested by the ILMDP’s developers, the EFA results did not support an eight-

component structure, as only three factors had eigenvalues greater than one,

which is the most common rule for determining the number of factors (Hair Jr et

al. 1995). Further, an examination of the scree diagram suggested there may

be only two components, as can be seen in Figure 4.9.

Figure 4.9 Scree Diagram of ILMDP Items

The two-component solution was therefore initially rotated to obtain simple

structure using the oblimin oblique rotation procedure. However, as the two

components were not correlated (-.002), the EFA was re-estimated using the

varimax rotation procedure, with the resulting factor loadings being shown in

Table 4.7. The analysis suggested the rotated components explained similar

amounts of the variance in the data set (33.3% component one and 32% for

component two) and this solution was accepted as a revised measure of MLE.

Page 152: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

150

Table 4.7 Rotated Factor Loadings for the ILMDP Items

Leadership Effectiveness

Managerial Effectiveness

Team Leadership .83

Participative Style .83

Positive Relationships with Customers .74

Inspires Vision .74

Praises Positive Contributions .74

Manages Conflicts Well .73

Encourages People’s Development .73

Delegate_Responsibility .72

Manage_Not_Performing .70

Communicate_Fit .69

Facilitate_Discussion .69

Influence_Others .64 .59 Provide_Feedback .63 .51 Liase_Cooperate .62 .50 Feedback_From_Cust .59

Represent_Workgroup .59 .54 Follow_Up .78 Focu_High_Priority .75 Monitor_Activities .74 Support_Att_Improve .72 Develop_New_Ideas .72 Negotiate_Effectively .72 Plans_Goals_Tasks .71 Good_Solutions .71 Dev_New_Opportunities .71 Initiate_Changes .70 Comply_With_Policies .66 Info_Progressing .65 Communicate_Vision .55 .65 Commitment_Customer .51 .59 Manage_Meetings .56 Speaking_Presenting .54

As can be seen in Table 4.7, the first component is most closely related to items

that asked about the target manager’s leadership style (e.g. provides team

leadership, inspires a vision and has a participative style). On the other hand,

Page 153: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

151

the second component was most closely related to items that asked about the

managerial side of the task (e.g. follows up, focus on high priority tasks and

monitors activities). These relationships suggested the two components should

be labelled Leadership Effectiveness (LE) and Management Effectiveness (ME)

and that the ILMDP scale should be examined further using these two aspects.

Only items loading highly on one component (greater than .60) were retained

for this further analysis, which is discussed in the next section.

4.2.7 Confirming the Revised MLE Scales in the Present Study

As was the case with the original eight subscales, a CFA was used to examine

the revised LE and revised ME scale’s measurement properties. In this case, a

different data set was used to ensure there were no biases in the resulting

constructs. The data set in this case was obtained from the 169 respondents

who had answered the IQ, EI and MMS questions and who had not been

included in the initial data set from which the eight factors’ measurement

properties had previously been assessed. Data for 31 of the 169 cases were

only available as the average other-rater scores, as this was the way the data

were provided to the researcher. However an IRA analysis was undertaken on

the ME and LE data for which actual other-rater scores were available (n=127)

before the CFA analysis was undertaken using all of the other-rater average

scores (n=169). The IRA and CFA results are reported for the revised LE and

ME scales in turn and more detail on the main sample characteristics is

provided in Chapter 5.

Page 154: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

152

4.2.7.1 The Revised LE Scale

Based on the decisions about the items that were retained, the revised

Leadership Effectiveness (LE) scale was measured using 11 items:

I demonstrate team leadership and build good team relations.

I use a participative style of management by involving others when

planning, goal setting, decision making etc.

I develop positive relations with people who use our services and/or

products (e.g. external clients, internal customers, etc.).

I inspire others to believe in and have enthusiasm for the organisation’s

values and its vision.

I praise people for their positive contributions and achievements.

I effectively address and manage conflicts and disagreements as they

arise within our section.

I help and encourage people within the Division/Section develop their

skills and potential (e.g. share my skills, discuss training opportunities,

suggest ways to improve etc.).

I effectively delegate responsibility by giving people challenging jobs

and the freedom they need to do the job.

I effectively manage people not performing to the required standard

(e.g. help them find solutions, set agreed outcomes, coaching etc.).

I clearly communicate to people in the work group how their tasks and

activities fit into the broader organisational goals and objectives.

I facilitate group discussions effectively (e.g. clarify issues, help get

consensus, use brainstorming etc.).

Page 155: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

153

As was noted in Chapter 3, before averaging the other rater scores for the

above 11 items, it was important to ensure there was acceptable inter-rater

agreement. The results of the analysis undertaken to ensure this was the case

are described in the next section.

4.2.7.1.1 IRA: The revised 11-item LE scale

The ADM and the rWG (rWG_UN, rWG_skew and rWG_Mskew) statistics for each of the

raters on each of the above items were computed. Table 4.8 provides a

summary of the mean scores for all three distributions for each of the 11-items.

Table 4.8 Mean IRR Statistic for Items in Revised LE Scale

rWG_UN rWG_skew rWG_Mskew ADM

 Team leadership .67 .56 .46 1.12

 Participative style .67 .56 .46 1.12

 Positive relations customers .83 .77 .72 .81

 Inspire vision .68 .59 .49 1.09

 Praise positive contributions .70 .61 .52 1.02

 Manage conflicts .69 .60 .50 1.08

 Encourage people develop .71 .62 .52 1.07

 Delegate responsibility .67 .56 .46 1.10

 Manage not performing .68 .57 .47 1.06

 Communicate fit .72 .63 .54 1.02

 Facilitate discussions .71 .62 .54 1.05

n  =  127    

The rWG statistics for the individual raters on all 11-items ranged from perfect

agreement to perfect non-agreement (i.e. from 0 to 1, with negative values reset

to 0). Despite the variance in the scores, the mean rWG_UN values, ranging from

.66 to .83, and the mean rWG_skew values, ranging from .56 to .83, were above

the recommended .51 hurdle. The mean rWG_Mskew ranged from .45 to .77 with

three of the items just below the recommended hurdle. As was recommended

by LeBreton and Senter (2008), measures of central tendency and dispersion

for each item were computed for rWG_UN, rWG_skew, and for rWG_Mskew, as reported

Page 156: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

154

in Appendices O, P and Q respectively. Finally, mean ADM indexes for all 11-

items were above the recommended 1.67 recommended hurdle.

Given the variation in these results, the percentage of indices meeting the

respective interpretive standards were determined. The number of items

meeting the rWG hurdle ( > .51) and the ADM hurdle ( < 1.67) was summed for

each case, for each of the three null distributions. The modal case in the sample

had 100% of the 32-items meeting all of the IRA hurdles, as can be seen in

Table 4.9.

Based on the above analysis, it seems there was acceptable inter-rater

agreement between the other-raters in the main sample on the 11-items of the

revised LE scale. Consequently, the other-rater scores for these 11-items were

averaged and used in the CFA.

Table 4.9 Central Tendency and Dispersion of Total # of Revised LE Scale Items

Meeting IRA Interpretive Standards

# Items

meeting ADM standard

# Items meeting rwg standard for

uniform null distribution

slightly skewed null distribution

moderately skewed null distribution

# % # % # % # %

Mean 10 91 9 79 8 71 7 64

Median 11 100 10 91 9 82 8 73

Mode 11 100 11 100 11 100 8a 73

Std. Deviation 2 18 2 18 3 29 3 30

Percentile

25 10 91 8 73 6 55 4 36

50 11 100 10 91 9 82 8 73

75 11 100 11 100 11 100 10 91 n = 127 a lowest of multiple modes

Page 157: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

155

4.2.7.1.2 CFA: The revised 11-item LE scale

The CFA suggested the 11 items were not a good fit to the data

(χ2=241.66, df=44, p<.01) despite item loadings ranging from .78 to .86. The

same iterative process that was used to examine the previous subscales (i.e.

examining modification indices and item loadings) was followed to improve the

subscale’s fit. This approach led to a significant reduction in the number of

items, as only four of the 11 original items were retained. However, the

resulting scale had an acceptable fit

(χ2=6.01, df=2, p<.35, RMSEA= .03, CFI= .99, TLI= .99), with loadings ranging

from to .82 to .91, as can be seen in Figure 4.10. Construct reliability in this

case was .94 and the scale’s AVE score was .72, suggesting the construct was

reliable and had convergent validity. The revised four-item scale for the LE

construct was therefore retained for subsequent analysis.

* item loading n=169

Figure 4.10 Revised LE Scale Item Loadings and Fit Statistics

4.2.7.2 Revised ME Scale

Based on the decisions about the items that were retained, the revised

Management Effectiveness (ME) scale was measured using 12 items:

Team Leadership

Participation Style

Delegate Responsibility

Communicate Fit

.91*

.88*

.82*

.82*

LE

chi-square = 6.01 df = 2 p < .35 Rmsea = .03 cfi = .99 tli = .99

Page 158: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

156

I follow up on decisions to make sure they are implemented.

I keep a focus on important, high priority activities.

I monitor activities and procedures to ensure that our branch or

section is working as effectively and efficiently as possible.

I promote and support attempts to improve the standards of our

products and services.

I create and develop new ideas and ways of approaching things.

I negotiate effectively in order to obtain resources and outcomes

which help the overall success of our section.

I develop plans which clearly set out goals, tasks and timelines.

I come up with good solutions when problems arise that might get in

the way of achieving our goals.

I work at developing possibilities and new opportunities that

contribute to our vision or goals.

I initiate and implement changes necessary to achieve our vision and

benefit the organisation as a whole.

I make sure that our decisions and actions comply with the

organisations policies, standards and rules.

I make sure our section has good information about how we are

progressing toward our targets and goals.

Once again, before averaging the other rater scores for these 12 items, it was

important to ensure there was acceptable inter-rater agreement. The results of

this analysis are described in the next section.

Page 159: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

157

4.2.7.2.1 IRA: revised 12-item ME scale

Prior to averaging the other-rater scores in the main sample, the ADM and the

rWG (rWG_UN, rWG_skew and rWG_Mskew) statistics for each of the raters on each of

the above items were computed. Table 4.10 provides a summary of the mean

scores for all three distributions for each of the items. The rWG statistics for the

individual raters on all items ranged from perfect agreement to perfect non-

agreement (i.e. from 0 to 1, with negative values reset to 0). Despite the

variance in the scores, the mean rWG_UN values, ranging from .70 to .82, the

mean rWG_skew values, ranging from .61 to .76, and the mean rWG_Mskew ranged

from .52 to .70 were all above the recommended .51 hurdle. As was

recommended by LeBreton and Senter (2008), measures of central tendency

and dispersion for each item were computed for rWG_UN, rWG_skew, and for

rWG_Mskew, as reported in Appendices R, S and T respectively. Finally, mean

ADM indexes for all items were above the recommended 1.67 recommended

hurdle.

Table 4.10 Mean IRR Statistic for Items in Revised ME Scale

rWG_UN rWG_skew rWG_Mskew ADM

Follow up decisions .78   .71   .64   .92   High Priority .75   .68   .60   .94   Monitor activities .75   .67   .60   .96   Support attempts improve .82   .76   .70   .81   Develop new ideas .78   .72   .65   .89   Negotiate effectively .77   .70   .63   .91   Plans goals, tasks and timelines .71   .61   .52   1.07   Good solutions problems .80   .74   .67   .86   Developing new opportunities .79   .72   .66   .91   Initiative changes .77   .69   .62   .93   Comply organisation policies .82   .76   .70   .85   Information about progress .76   .69   .61   .92  n  =  127    

Page 160: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

158

The number of items meeting the rWG hurdle ( > .51) and the ADM hurdle

( < 1.67) was summed for each case, for each of the three null distributions.

The modal case in the sample had 100% of the 12-items meeting all of the IRA

hurdles, as can be seen in Table 4.11.

 Table  4.11  Central  Tendency  and  Dispersion  of  Total  #  of  Revised  ME  Scale  Items  Meeting  IRA  Interpretive  Standards  

# Items meeting ADM standard

# Items meeting rwg standard for uniform null distribution

slightly skewed null distribution

moderately skewed null distribution

# % # % # % # % Mean 11 94 11 92 10 81 9 76

Median 12 100 11 92 11 92 10 83

Mode 12 100 12 100 12 100 12 100

Std. Deviation 1 8 3 22 2 16 3 22

Percentile

25 12 100 10 83 9 75 8 67

50 12 100 11 92 11 92 10 83 75 12 100 12 100 12 100 12 100

n = 127

Based on the above analysis, it was assumed there was an acceptable level of

inter-rater agreement between the other-raters in the main sample on all items

of the revised ME scale. Consequently, the other-rater scores for these items

were averaged and used in the CFA.

4.2.7.2.2 CFA: revised 12-item ME scale

The CFA again suggested that the 12 items were not a good fit to the data

(χ2=380.76, df = 44, p<.01), despite item loadings ranging from .74 to .84. The

same iterative process was followed to improve the scale’s fit. This approach

again led to a significant reduction in the number of items, as only five of the

original twelve items were retained. However, the resulting scale had an

acceptable fit (χ2=6.01, df=5, p= .31, RMSEA= .03, CFI= .99, TLI= .99), with

loadings ranging from to .77 to .87, as can be seen in Figure 4.11. Construct

Page 161: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

159

reliability in this case was .92 and the scale’s AVE score was .69, suggesting

the construct was reliable and had convergent validity. Consequently, the

revised five-item ME construct was retained for subsequent analysis.

* item loading n = 169

Figure 4.11 Revised Management Effectiveness Scale Item Loadings and Fit Statistics

To ensure there had been no loss in information in reducing the number of

items, correlations were computed between the revised scales and the initially

suggested scales (Thomas, Soutar & Ryan 2001). In this case, the correlation

between the two leadership effectiveness scores was .96 and the correlation

between the two management effectiveness scores was .97, suggesting no

information was lost in ensuring the two scales had appropriate measurement

properties. Finally, the revised leadership effectiveness and management

effectiveness scales were analysed for discriminant validity. The squared

correlation between the two constructs (r= .65) was below both AVE scores.

Consequently, discriminant validity can be assumed and the revised Leadership

Effectiveness (LE) and Management Effectiveness (ME) Scales were retained

for use as the dependent variables in the subsequent analysis.

Initiate Changes

Developing New Opportunties

Focus on High Priority Issues

. 89*

. 85*

.72* ME

chi-square = 5.72 df = 5 p < .33 cfi = 1.00 tli = 1.00

Good Solutions

Negotiate Effectively

.81*

.88*

Page 162: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

160

4.3 THE RELIABILITY OF THE MSCEITV2

This section reports the results of the analyses that were undertaken to

examine the reliability of the MSCEIT branch scales that were used in this

study. The subsection of the main sample that was used in this analysis, and

which is described in more detail in the next chapter, included 255 respondents.

Inter-correlations were first computed at the task level and the results obtained

are summarised in Table 4.13. It was expected that the two tasks that were

used for each branch would have the highest correlations. For Branch 1

(Perceive Emotion) the correlation between Task A (Faces) and Task E

(Pictures) was r = .24 (p < .01). However, Task A (Faces) had a slightly higher

correlation with Task B (Facilitation) (r = .26, p < .01) and similarly, Task E had

a higher correlation with Task F (Sensations) (r = .28, p < .01).

For Branch 2 (Facilitate Thought) the correlation between Task B (Facilitation)

and Task F (Sensations) was r = .31 (p < .01). Again, however, there was a

higher correlation between Task F (Sensations) and Task C (Changes)

(r = .33, p < .01). For Branch 3 (Understand Emotion) the correlation between

Task C (Changes) and Task G (Blends) was the highest (r = .40, p < .01). This

was also true for Branch 4 (Manage Emotion) as the correlation between Task

D (Emotion Management) and Task H (Emotion Relations) was the highest

(r = .31, p < .01). With the three exceptions that were noted, which may be due

to sampling error alone, this pattern of correlations was consistent with the

theory underlying the MSCEITv2 and with previous results (Palmer et al. 2005).

Page 163: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

161

Table 4.12 MSCEITv2 Task Level Correlations

Branch 1

Perceive Emotion

Branch 2

Facilitate Thought

Branch 3

Understand Emotion

Branch 4

Manage Emotion

Task A Task E Task B Task F Task C Task G Task D

Branch 1: Task_E_Pictures .24**

Branch 2:

Task_B_Facilitation .26** .24**

Branch 2: Task_F_Sensations .18* .28** .31**

Branch 3 Task_C_Changes .19* .24** .19* .33**

Branch 3: Task_G_Blends .10 .21** .01 .28** .40**

Branch 4: Task_D_Emotion Management

.14

.22**

.15*

.15*

.16*

.05

Branch 4: Task_H_Emotion Relations

.09 .22** .15 .11 .05 .10 .31**

**p<.01; *p<.05

The correlations between the four branch scores of the MSCEITv2 ranged from

.12 to .33, as can be seen in Table 4.14, suggesting the branches are related,

but distinct, factors. The correlation between the MSCEITv2 area scores of

Strategic EI and Experiential EI was .40 (p < .01), again indicating a positive

relationship, but not so strong as to suggest they might be redundant

constructs.

Table 4.13 MSCEITv2 Branch Level Correlations

MSCEITv2 Branch Perceive Emotion

Facilitate Thought

Understand Emotion

Branch 2: Facilitate Thought .33**

Branch 3: Understand Emotion .24** .30**

Branch 4: Manage Emotion .26** .19** .12**

**p<.01; *p<.05

Page 164: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

162

The reliability of the MSCEITv2 was assessed at the task, branch, area and

total levels. Scale reliability for the Total EI score was .70, while the scale

reliabilities for Experiential EI and Strategic EI were .88 and .53 respectively.

The scale reliabilities of each of the four branch scores ranged from .50 to .88,

while the scale reliabilities of each of the MSCEITv2 task scores ranged from

.21 to .90, as can be seen in Table 4.15. Some of these reliabilities are lower

than those reported by the test’s authors (Mayer et al. 2003). While the

reliability of Branch 3 (Facilitate Thought), which clearly follows from the lower

reliability of the two tasks on which it is based, is of particular concern, it will be

retained for further analysis.

TABLE 4.14 Comparison of the Reliabilities for the MSCEITv2 Total EI,

Branch and Task Subscales across Three Studies

The Current Study

Fiori et al (2011)

Maul (2010)

Palmer et al. (2005)

Mayer et al. (2003)

Total EI .70 .66 .85 .89 .91

Area Scores

Experiential .88 --- --- .90 .90

Strategic .53 --- --- .76 .86

Branch Scores

1 Perceive Emotion .88 .53 .87 .89 .90

2 Facilitate Thought .70 .63 .62 .67 .76

3 Understand Emotion .50 .73 .62 .69 .77

4 Manage Emotion .69 .77 .66 .66 .81

Branch 1 Tasks

A Faces .90 --- .76 .84 .82

E Pictures .81 --- .82 .85 .87

Branch 2 Tasks

B Facilitation .63 --- .56 .48 .62

F Sensations .57 --- .50 .48 .56

Branch 3 Tasks

C Changes .21 --- .45 .60 .68

G Blends .35 --- .49 .54 .62

Branch 4 Tasks

D Emotion Mgmt .56 --- .55 .48 .64

H Emotion Relations .47 --- .55 .51 .64

Page 165: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

163

However, the lower reliabilities reported in the present study are consistent with

results obtained by Palmer et al. (2005), although some of the subscales’

reliabilities in the current study are lower than those reported by Palmer et al.

(2005) (i.e. the Total EI score, the Strategic EI area score, the Understand

Emotion branch and the Changes and Blends tasks). Given these results, a

further analysis of the MSCEITv2 measurement properties was undertaken

using Maximum Likelihood Confirmatory Factor Analysis. However, the results

of this analysis did not lead to an improvement in the subscale’s reliabilities, as

can be seen in Appendix M. Consequently, a decision was made to use the

MSCEITv2 branch scales in the subsequent analysis. The low reliabilities of the

MSCEITv2 task subscales are a limitation of the present study and it is clear

more work is needed to understand why such results were obtained in the

present research context.

4.4 THE INTER-RATER SIMILARITY OF THE WUSCT SCORES

This section reports the results of the analysis undertaken to assess the inter-

rater similarity of the WUSCT scores that were used in this study. The

subsection of the main sample used for this analysis, which is described in

more detail in the next chapter, included 183 respondents.

As was described in Chapter 3, the WUSCT is made up of 36-items. Each of

the 6,588 item responses (183 protocols x 36 items=6,588 item responses) was

scored by two raters. Inter-rater agreement on the first round of scoring was

moderately high, with the two raters independently agreeing on the score for

75% of the individual item responses (4,924 item responses) and 73% of the

Page 166: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

164

total protocol ratings of respondents’ ED levels (135 total protocol ratings).

Similarly, the correlation between the raters on individual items was .76 and on

total protocol rating (or ED level) was .86 (p<.001). These results are similar to

those reported elsewhere in the literature as noted in Chapter 3.

The ICC (A,1) for this first round of scoring was above .74 for all of the items

except item seven, as can be seen in Appendix N. For item seven, the total

number of responses that raters disagreed upon was consistent with the other

35 items on the WUSCT. However, where the raters disagreed on item seven

responses, the mean difference in the assigned scores was larger, which in turn

led to a decrease in ICC for this item. Regardless, the iterative process of

assessing disagreements mitigated this as a concern.

To maximise the possibility of correctly assigning an ED level to the items and

total protocols an iterative discussion process that was described in Chapter 3

(p. 101) was followed that enabled the raters to reach agreement on the

remaining items. Agreement was reached on a further 20% of the items in the

second round, 3% in the third round and 2% in the fourth and final round. For

148 protocols the final TPR was the same using both the ogive and the item

sum method. The remaining 35 protocols were reviewed visually by both raters

and assigned a final TPR.

4.5 CHAPTER SUMMARY

The present chapter presented results from the preliminary data analysis phase.

First, the measurement properties of the ILMDP were assessed, with the results

suggesting the eight-factor model proposed by Cacioppe and Albrecht (2001)

Page 167: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

165

was not a good fit to the data in the present study. As such, an exploratory

factor analysis was undertaken that suggested a two-factor model. The items

that were related to the two factors, which were labelled Leadership

Effectiveness (LE) and Management Effectiveness (ME), were analysed and

revised to ensure they were unidimensional and reliable and that they had

convergent and discriminant validity. These constructs were alternately used as

the dependent variable of MLE.

Results of the examination of the internal consistency of the MSCEITv2 total,

area, branch and task scales were then outlined. A lower than previously

reported set of internal reliabilities were identified, which was seen to be a

limitation in the present study. Finally, the inter-rater similarity of the two-person

WUSCT rating team was examined. This analysis suggested there was a high

level of reliability and agreement in the raters’ scoring of the WUSCT item

responses and of the total protocol ratings.

In the next chapter, these constructs were used in the second data analysis

phase that was undertaken to examine the hypotheses that arose from the

literature review and that were discussed in Chapter 2.

Page 168: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

166

Page 169: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

167

Chapter 5

RESULTS

5.1 INTRODUCTION

The previous chapter presented the results of the preliminary data analysis phase

of the present study. This phase included an analysis of the measurement

properties for the measure of MLE, the reliability of the MSCEITv2 branch scores

and the inter-rater reliability on the MMS data. This chapter reports the results of

the data analysis that was undertaken to examine the hypotheses that were

developed in Chapter 2. The chapter begins with a discussion of the initial analysis

of the main research sample (section 5.2) and of its background characteristics

(section 5.3). Next a summary of the descriptive statistics for the model’s

constructs is provided (section 5.4) including the correlations between each of the

constructs. The final sections of the chapter summarise the results that were

obtained from testing the study’s various hypotheses (section 5.5 to section 5.9).

5.2 THE INITIAL ANALYSIS OF THE MAIN SAMPLE

The main research sample, which is described in Chapter 3 (section 3.3.1.2 The

Main Research Sample), was examined first. The ways in which respondents were

approached and the data were collected were discussed in detail in Section 3.3.2.

This process led to a final sample of 169 managers that was used to examine the

study’s various research hypotheses.

Page 170: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

168

Data preparation of the main sample was undertaken as described in section

3.3.4.1. As already noted, the sample was checked for the existence missing data.

However, there were no missing data in this sample. An examination of univariate

descriptive statistics for each variable found scores that fell within the expected

ranges, suggesting input errors were unlikely. Z-scores for all the constructs were

below the recommended hurdle of 3.29 (Tabachnick & Fidell 2007) with very few

exceptions. Consequently, all of the cases were retained. Skewness, kurtosis and

Kolmogorov-Smirnov Z-scores for each of the constructs can be seen in Table 5.1.

Table 5.1 Normality Statistics for Main Research Sample

Skewness Statistic

Kurtosis Statistic

Kolmogorov-Smirnov Z

Statistic Sig.

IQ -.08 -.55 1.17 .13 MMS – total item score .26 .45 .99 .28 Perceive Emotion (Branch 1) -.20 -1.03 1.31 .06 Facilitate Thought (Branch 2) -1.08 .81 1.83 .00 Understand Emotion (Branch 3) -.60 -.10 1.20 .11 Manage Emotion (Branch 4) -.88 1.35 1.14 .15 LE -.57 .30 .98 .29 ME -.34 -.02 .68 .75

n=169

The skewness for the IQ, MMS, and Perceive Emotion data suggested the

distribution was approximately normal. The constructs of Understand Emotion,

Manage Emotion and LE exhibited a moderate level of skewness. Facilitate

Thought exhibited a significant amount of skewness. However, the kurtosis was

within an acceptable level. The Kolmorgorov-Smirnov Z-scores suggested all but

one of the constructs (Facilitate Thought) could be considered to be normal. The

skewness for Facilitate Thought is likely the cause of its non-normality. Given the

skewness of this construct, it was decided to drop Facilitate Thought from

subsequent analysis. Consequently, this analysis was based on the MLE

Page 171: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

169

antecedents of IQ, MMS, Perceive Emotion, Understand Emotion and Manage

Emotion.

5.3 SAMPLE BACKGROUND CHARACTERISTICS

The respondents’ background characteristics can be seen in Table 5.2. There

were 103 males (61%) and 66 females (39%) in the sample, which is similar to the

gender break-down of full-time employed professionals in Australia for the period

from 2003-2005 (56% males and 44% females respectively) (ABS 2003 - 2005).

Respondents’ ages ranged from 25 to 58 years of age, with a mean age of 39 and

a standard deviation of 7.7 years. The sample was predominantly Caucasian

(90%), but also included a small number of Asian (7%) and Middle Eastern (1%)

participants.

Respondents’ education attainment ranged from primary school to having a post-

graduate qualifications. The sample was generally well educated, as the modal

response was a post-graduate degree (46% of the sample), while the next most

common response was an undergraduate qualification (Bachelor’s degree) (36% of

the sample). Most respondents were employed, with 81% working full-time and 8%

working part-time. The remaining 10% were not currently employed and were

pursuing an MBA degree. Respondents’ work experience ranged from a minimum

of 2.5 years to a maximum of 41 years, with a mean work experience of 18 years

and a standard deviation of nine years.

Page 172: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

170

Table 5.2 Background Characteristics of the Sample

n % Gender Male 103 61

Female 66 39 Age Under 25 1 1

26-30 years 23 14 31-35 years 42 25 36-40 years 40 23 41-45 years 25 15 46-50 years 25 15 51-55 years 9 5 56-60 years 4 2

Ethnicity Asian 11 7 Caucasian 152 90 Middle Eastern 2 1 Missing 4 2

Education Primary School 1 1 3 years or less high school 3 2 Completed high school 8 5 Business college 9 5 Technical college 4 2 Trade qualification 3 2 Undergrad/Bachelor’s degree

61 36 Post-graduate/Master’s degree

79 46 Missing 1 1

Employment Status

Employed full-time 137 81 Employed part-time 13 8 Not currently employed 18 10 Missing 1 1

Years of Work Experience 2.5 – 4 9 5

5 – 9 18 11 10 – 14 31 18 15 – 19 30 18 20 – 24 33 20 25 + 46 27 Missing 2 1

Manage Staff Yes 122 72 No 46 27 Missing 1 1

Years Managing Staff

0 11 7 1 – 5 61 36 6 - 10 39 23 11 – 19 39 23 20 + 18 11 Missing 1 1

Page 173: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

171

Most participants had responsibility for managing staff (72%) and there was a wide

range (zero to 40 years) of managerial experience within the sample. A more

detailed look at the eleven individuals with zero years of management experience

found they were in professional roles that enabled multi-source feedback to be

provided on the skills within the ILMDP. The mean managerial experience was nine

years, with a standard deviation of eight years.

As can be seen in Table 5.3, respondents came from a range of industries. The

property and business services, government and health-community sectors

employed a third of the sample, while the mining and utility industries were also

strongly represented, which is not surprising as respondents were from Western

Australia.

Table 5.3 Industries Represented in the Sample

n % Agriculture, Forestry, Fishing and Hunting 5 3 Mining 12 7 Manufacturing 6 3 Electricity, Gas and Water Supply 17 10 Construction 5 3 Retail Trade 4 2 Accommodation, Cafes and Restaurants 9 5 Transport and Storage 3 2 Communication Services 3 2 Finance and Insurance 10 6 Property and Business Services 37 22 Government Administration and Defence 26 15 Education 10 6 Health and Community Services 18 11 Cultural and Recreational Services 1 1 Personal and Other Services 1 1 Missing 2 1 TOTAL 169 100

Page 174: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

172

As can be seen in Table 5.4, the sample included a broad cross section of

executives, senior managers, managers and non-managerial professionals.

However, senior managers were the largest group (31%) and managers were the

second largest group (29%).

Table 5.4 Frequency of Job Titles

n %

Managing Director/CEO/Director 19 11

Senior Manager 53 31

Manager 49 29

Professional Specialist 36 21

Student 6 4

Missing 6 4

5.4 DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS

Descriptive statistics, including the range, mean and standard deviation were

calculated for each of the key constructs. These included MLE (measured using

the revised Leadership Effectiveness scale and the revised Management

Effectiveness scale), IQ, MMS (measured as the ED total item sum using the

WUSCT) and the three MSCEITv2 branch scores (Perceive Emotion, Understand

Emotion and Manage Emotion). The results obtained for the various constructs are

discussed below and are summarised in Table 5.5 and Table 5.6.

Respondents’ scores on the revised Leadership Effectiveness scale ranged from

4.99 to 9.25, while scores on the Management Effectiveness scale ranged from

5.82 to 9.30. The mean Leadership Effectiveness score was 7.59, with a standard

deviation of .89, while the mean Management Effectiveness score was 7.84, with a

standard deviation of .71.

Page 175: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

173

Table 5.5 Descriptive Statistics for IQ, ED and MLE Variables

N Minimum Maximum Mean SD

IQ 169 17 39 28.3 5.13

MMS (TIS)1 169 159 244 199 15

LE2 169 4.99 9.25 7.59 .89

ME2 169 5.82 9.30 7.84 .71

1 MMS operationalised as Ego Development total item score (TIS); 2 MLE operationalised as Management Effectiveness (ME) and Leadership Effectiveness (LE).

As was noted in Chapter 3, IQ was measured using the Wonderlic Personality Test

(WPT) (Wonderlic & Associates, 2002). Scores ranged from 17 to 39, with a mean

score of 28.3 and a standard deviation of 5.13. Given the education and

management experience of the present sample, the mean score is consistent with

norms obtained in previous studies that used the WPT, as was noted in Chapter 3.

MMS was operationalised as Ego-development (ED), which was measured using

the Washington University Sentence Completion Test (WUSCT) (Loevinger, 1998).

As was noted in Chapter 3, WUSCT scores can be used to determine a person’s

ED level or can be used to compute an ED total item sum score. The ED levels in

the present sample which are shown in Table 5.6 and ranged from four to nine,

with a mean level of six. The ED total item sum scores (TIS) ranged from 159 to

244, with a mean of 199 and a standard deviation of 15, which was consistent with

prior research that had suggested 80% of the population is within a similar range

(Cook-Greuter, 2000). As was expected, ED (TIS) and ED level in the present

sample had a correlation of .89 (p<.01) and as such, only ED (TIS) is reported

throughout the remainder of the thesis.

Page 176: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

174

Table 5.6 Frequency of ED Level

Current Study Rooke (2005) 1

N % %

3 0 0 5

4 1 1 12

5 15 9 38

6 93 55 30

7 48 28 10

8 10 6 4

9 2 1 1 1 Based population of “thousands of managers and professionals, most between the ages of 25 and 55’ (pg 68).

Emotional Intelligence (EI) was measured using the Mayer, Salovey and Caruso

(2002) Emotional Intelligence Test version 2 (MSCEITv2). As was noted in

Chapter 3, raw scores, rather than percentiles, were used in this study. The raw

scores for the Perceive Emotion Branch (Branch 1) ranged from .30 to .75, with a

mean score of .55 and standard deviation of .12. The raw scores for the

Understand Emotion Branch (Branch 3) ranged from .38 to .81, with a mean of .66

and standard deviation of .09. The raw scores for the Manage Emotion Branch

(Branch 4) ranged from .25 to .57, with a mean of .46 and standard deviation of

.06. As can be seen in Table 5.7, the mean score for each of the MSCEITv2

branch scales in this study were slightly higher than were the mean scores that

were reported by the test authors in their norming data set (Mayer et al., 2003).

Page 177: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

175

Table 5.7 Descriptives on MSCEITv2 Subscales and Comparison to Norming Sample

Current Study

MSCEITv2 Norming Sample1

Min Max Mean SD Mean SD

Perceive Emotion (Branch 1) .30 .75 .55 .12 .49 .11

Understand Emotion (Branch 2) .38 .81 .66 .09 .62 .11

Manage Emotion (Branch 4) .25 .57 .46 .06 .41 .07

 1  (Mayer  et  al.,  2003)  

5.5 TESTING HYPOTHESIS ONE

Hypothesis one (H1) suggested the individual difference constructs of EI, MMS and

IQ would be positively correlated, but that they would have discriminant validity.

That is:

H1a: IQ, EI and MMS are positively correlated.

H1b: IQ, EI and MMS have discriminant validity from each other.

In order to test H1a, correlations were computed to investigate the nature of the

bivariate relationships between the three independent variables. The results

obtained can be seen in Table 5.8. IQ was positively correlated with Understand

Emotion (r= .30; p<.01). Perceive Emotion was positively correlated with the other

MSCEITv2 branch Understand Emotion (r= .26; p<.01) and also Manage Emotion

(r= .27; p<.01)]. However, Understand Emotion and Manage Emotion were not

significantly related.

Page 178: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

176

Table 5.8 Correlations of Independent Variables

IQ MMS

(TIS)1 Perceive 2 Understand 4

MMS (TIS) 1 .07

Perceive Emotion 2 .13 -.10

Understand Emotion 4 .30** .06 .26**

Manage Emotion 5 .03 .04 .27** .13

1 MMS operationalised as Ego Development (ED) total item score (TIS); 2 MSCEIT Branch 1 Perceive Emotion; 3 MSCEIT Branch 2 Facilitate Thought; 4 MSCEIT Branch 3 Understand Emotion; 5 MSCEIT Branch 4 Manage Emotion; **p<.01; *p<.05

While IQ was significantly correlated with Understand Emotion, as was

hypothesised, it was not significantly correlated to any of the other independent

variables that were included in the study. Further, MMS was not significantly

related to IQ or to any of the MSCEITv2 branches. Based on this analysis, H1a was

rejected at an overall sample level. However, the lack of significant relationships

between the independent variables, combined with significant correlations between

the two ED scoring methods and between the MSCEIT branches, provides partial

evidence of discriminant validity. Based on this analysis, H1b was accepted at an

overall sample level.

5.6 TESTING HYPOTHESIS TWO

Hypothesis two (H2) had suggested the individual difference constructs of IQ, EI

and MMS would have incremental predictive validity. That is:

H2a: IQ is a significant predictor of MLE.

H2b: EI is a significant incremental predictor of MLE, beyond IQ.

H2c: MMS is a significant incremental predictor of MLE, beyond IQ and EI.

Page 179: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

177

Hierarchical regression analyses were used to explore the potential predictive

validity of the IQ, EI and MMS constructs, using the other-rater average scores on

the revised ME and LE scales as the dependent variables. Based on a theoretical

assumption that IQ is the strongest single predictor of MLE (Van Rooy &

Viswesvaran 2004), IQ was entered in the first stage of this analysis. In the next

two stages, EI and MMS were entered respectively based on the theoretical

assumption that EI would operate as a subset of MMS (Lane & Schwartz 1987).

The regression analysis found that none of the variables were significantly related

to the dependent variable of management effectiveness with the final equation

explaining almost none of the variation in ME scores (R2= .03, F=1.00, p<.43).

Similar results were obtained when the same analysis was undertaken to examine

the relationship between IQ, EI, MMS and leadership effectiveness. The regression

analysis found that none of the variables were significantly related to the dependent

variable of leadership effectiveness, with the final equation explaining almost none

of the variation in LE scores (R2= .04, F=1.02, p<.41). Consequently, the suggested

relationships were not evident in the sample obtained for the present study and

both H2a and H2b were rejected at an overall sample level.

5.7 TESTING FOR SUBGROUPS

Despite the theoretical underpinnings that were outlined in Chapter 2, the

regression analyses failed to support the hypothesised relationships. One possible

explanation of these results is the issue of unobserved heterogeneity. As Sarsted

(2008, p. 228), noted, regression analysis :

…is usually based on the assumption that the analysed data originate from a single population, that is, a unique global model represents all the observations well. In many real-world

Page 180: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

178

applications however, this assumption of homogeneity is unrealistic, as individuals (or groups) are likely to be heterogeneous.

However, there may be unobserved subgroups for whom the relationships of

interest differ, which would explain the results obtained from the sample as a

whole. Sometimes this heterogeneity is dealt with in an a prior fashion by

estimating regressions at a subgroup level based on demographic or other

background information, but this may not be sufficient to explain the sample’s

heterogeneity. As Coltman, Devinney and Midgley (2003, p. 2) pointed out, what is

often required is ‘a more sophisticated approach that moves beyond data pooling

and aggregation techniques towards approaches that enable us to capture the

heterogeneity that actually exists’.

The mixture regression or latent class (LC) regression approach overcomes this

problem, as it assumes the data comes from a number of subgroups (or segments)

‘that are homogeneous with respect to the within-segment regression

coefficients…(and) identifies latent (unobserved) groups of individuals who differ in

the effects of predictor variables on outcome’ (Jaccard 2012, p. 63). In LC

regression ‘the latent variable is a predictor that interacts with the observed

predictors which means that it serves as a moderator variable’ (Magidson &

Vermunt 2004, p. 18) as can be seen in Figure 5.1.

This approach assumes the relationships of interest cannot be explained “with a

single distribution of probabilities; rather, it requires a mixture of them…It is

necessary to separate the samples by identifying the number of segments and

estimating the parameters (regression coefficients, in this case) that define each of

Page 181: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

179

them” (Castro, Martín Armario & Martín Ruiz 2007, pp. 179-180), which is seen as

advantageous, as the subgroups are not determined by the researcher.

Figure 5.1: A Representation of Latent Class Regression

The goal of LC regression is to determine the smallest number of latent classes

that are sufficient to explain the variance in a dependent variable within each of the

subgroups (Magidson & Vermunt 2002). “This ability to separate and extract

segments and then determine a statistical (regression) model for each one,

enables…researchers to develop more realistic and complex theoretical models,

while maintaining parsimony” (Taylor, Garver & Williams 2010, p. 217).

Consequently, it was decided to follow this approach to see whether there were

subgroups in the present research context that had led to the lack of significant

results at an overall sample level.

The analysis typically begins by fitting a single class baseline model (which

suggests there are no subgroups and that the relationship between a set of

predictor variables and a dependent variable is the same for the sample as a

whole). Assuming the null model does not provide an adequate fit to the data, a LC

Page 182: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

180

regression model with two classes is fitted to the data to see whether there is a

significant improvement in fit. This process continues by fitting successive LC

models with more classes (or subgroups) to the data, until the simplest model that

provides an adequate fit is found (Magidson & Vermunt 2002). Choosing the

correct number of classes requires balancing the amount of heterogeneity that is

explained with model parsimony. To assist with this, log-likelihood and a number of

chi-squared based statistics provide information on the fit of a particular model

(Vermunt & Magidson 2005). In particular:

• The L-squared (L2), or likelihood–ratio, is a goodness of fit value for the

current model. When there is perfect fit, this statistic will equal zero. A

model with a higher value of L2 is a poorer model in terms of explaining less

of the association in the data

• The Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) is based on the L2 but also takes

model parsimony into account. Again, when comparing models, a lower BIC

is indicative of a better fit.

• The Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) is also based on the L2 statistic. The

lower the value the better the model fit.

Classification statistics provide information about how well the model classifies

individual cases into the relevant classes (Vermunt & Magidson 2005):

• Classification Errors measure the proportion of cases that are misclassified.

As such, the closer to zero the better.

• Reduction of Errors (R2) indicates how well a model predicts class

membership. The closer to one the better the prediction.

Finally, output from a LC regression analysis includes a number of parameter

estimates (Vermunt & Magidson 2005). In particular:

Page 183: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

181

• Betas, which are indicators of the class-specific effect each predictor

variable has on the dependent variable.

• Sigmas, which are the error variances for a continuous dependent variable.

• Two Wald statistics, Wald and Wald (=), are provided each with an

associated p-value. The Wald statistic assesses the statistical significance of

the set of parameter estimates associated with a given variable. Wald (=)

assesses the equality of each set of regression effects across classes. As

such Wald (=) statistics that are not statistically different suggest that

different suggest the betas for that variable are same across the classes.

The Latent Gold 4.0 computer program (Vermunt and Magidson, 2005), was used

to estimate the various regressions, using the random seed default, which meant

ten starting points were randomly selected for each analysis which is “best

practice” as it minimises the chance of obtaining a local solution (Garver, Williams

& Taylor 2008). Following Vermunt and Magidson’s (2000) suggestion, and as

noted earlier, a 1-class model was initially estimated, after which additional models

that had more classes were estimated, until the simplest acceptable model was

found.

As LC regression cannot be undertaken in a hierarchical manner all of the

independent variables (i.e. IQ, the three EI branches and MMS) were included in

this phase of the analysis. The two aspects of MLE (i.e. management

effectiveness and leadership effectiveness) were again examined separately and

are discussed in turn. Further, as the number of cases in the smallest subgroups

were very low when the number of groups were greater than four for both the ME

and LE analysis (less than 20 in each case), it was decided to look only at the

results for the one, two, three and four-group solutions.

Page 184: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

182

5.7.1 Latent Class Regression: LE

The fit statistics obtained for the leadership effectiveness (LE) analysis are shown

in Table 5.9. As can be seen from the Table, the three group solution seems to be

the best, as there was a clear slowing in the improvements in the various fit

statistics, the entropy R2 statistic only changed by .03 and the R2 statistic only

changed by .05 from the three group to the four group solution and the number of

parameters that had to be estimated increased by 8. As the smallest group

included 43 respondents, the three-group solution was accepted in this case.

Table 5.9 Latent Class Regression Fit Statistics: Leadership Effectiveness

Number of Segments

Log Likelihood

BIC AIC Number of Parameters

Entropy R2

R2

1 -217.99 471.88 449.98 7 .04

2 -203.45 483.84 436.89 15 .40 .48

3 -189.38 496.74 424.76 23 .54 .80

4 -176.48 511.99 414.96 31 .57 .85

5.7.2 Latent Class Regression: ME

The fit statistics obtained for the managerial effectiveness analysis (ME) are shown

in Table 5.10. As can be seen from the Table, the four-group solution seems to be

the best, as while the BIC scores increased across the various solutions, the log

likelihood and AIC statistics were minimised and the entropy R2 and R2 statistics

were maximised at this point. However, in this case, the fourth group had only 27

respondents, which meant it was too small to be used in any subsequent analysis.

Consequently, it was decided to use the three larger groups suggested within the

four group solution and to treat the members of the fourth group as outliers and not

include these respondents in any of the subsequent analyses. This seemed

Page 185: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

183

especially appropriate as an examination their backgrounds suggested they did not

have any unique characteristics. A series of chi-squared tests suggested the group

had similar distributions of gender, age and years of managerial experience to the

rest of the sample.

Table 5.10 Latent Class Regression Fit Statistics: Managerial Effectiveness

Number of Segments

Log Likelihood

BIC AIC Number of Parameters

Entropy R2

R2

1 -178.76 393.44 371.53 7 .03

2 -163.71 414.63 359.46 15 .36 .59

3 -142.45 418.28 360.10 23 .46 .82

4 -130.97 441.48 355.17 31 .63 .90

In summary, results of the LC regression analysis suggested there were in fact

subgroups within the sample. The optimal solution for the LE dependent variable

suggested three subgroups. The optimal solution for the ME data suggested four

subgroups, although the fourth is too small for further statistical analysis and so

these cases are excluded as outliers. Consequently, the remaining group sizes

ranged from 35 to 80 as can be seen in Table 5.11.

Table 5.11 Sizes of the Latent Classes

MLE Scale Group # n %

Management Effectiveness Scale 1 55 32.5 2 52 30.8 3 35 20.7

4 27 16.0

Leadership Effectiveness Scale 1 80 47.3 2 46 27.2 3 43 25.4

The existence of subgroups within the sample would in fact have an impact on the

results obtained in the initial regression. However, before exploring the regressions

Page 186: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

184

at the subgroup level, the background characteristics and mean scores on the

variables of interest were examined further so as to better understand the

differences between the subgroups.

5.7.3 Mean Differences within the LE Subgroups

Descriptive statistics for the constructs and the univariate analysis of variance

results for the Leadership Effectiveness (LE) subgroups can be seen in Table 5.12.

Table 5.12 Descriptive Statistics on Variables for Leadership Effectiveness Groups

LE Group One (LE1)

LE Group Two (LE2)

LE Group Three (LE3) F

Statistic Sig.

Mean Std. Dev Mean Std. Dev Mean Std. Dev IQ 29.21 5.32 27.73 4.94 28.16 5.14 1.26 .29 MMS (TIS) 1 201 14 199 17 199 13 .26 .79 MLE2 6.82 .88 7.97 .57 7.92 .73 44.23 .00 Perceive Emotions3 .55 .12 .55 .12 .55 .13 .02 .98 Understand Emotion4 .66 .09 .64 .10 .67 .08 1.22 .30 Manage Emotion5 .46 .05 .46 .06 .45 .06 .21 .81 1 MMS operationalised as Ego Development (ED) total item score (TIS); 2 MLE operationalised as Leadership Effectiveness

(LE); 3 MSCEIT Branch 1 Perceive Emotion; 4 MSCEIT Branch 3 Understand Emotion; 5 MSCEIT Branch 4 Manage Emotion; **p<.01; *p<.05

As can be seen in the Table, the only variable for which there was a significant

difference across the three groups was the effectiveness variable itself, suggesting

the differences in the various relationships were not due to differences in the mean

scores of the predictor variables.

5.7.4 Mean Differences within the ME Subgroups

Descriptive statistics for the constructs and the univariate analysis of variance

results in the Management Effectiveness subgroups can be seen in Table 5.13. As

can be seen in the Table, the only variable for which there was a significant

difference across the three groups was the effectiveness variable itself, suggesting

Page 187: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

185

that, once again, differences in the various relationships were not due to

differences in the mean scores of the predictor variables.

Table 5.13 Descriptive Statistics on Variables for Management Effectiveness Groups

ME Group One (ME1)

ME Group Two (ME2)

ME Group Three (ME3 )

F Statistic

Sig.

Mean Std. Dev Mean Std. Dev Mean Std. Dev

IQ 28.40 4.79 27.43 5.09 29.13 5.93 1.07 .34 MMS (TIS) 1 202 16 196 13 196 15 2.48 .09 MLE2 7.97 .55 7.85 .89 6.91 .31 11.48 .00 Perceive Emotions3 .56 .11 .56 .13 .52 .13 1.11 .33 Understand Emotion4 .65 .10 .67 .09 .65 .10 .63 .53 Manage Emotion5 .46 .06 .45 .06 .46 .05 .41 .66 1 MMS operationalised as Ego Development (ED) total item score (TIS); 2 MLE operationalised as Management Effectiveness (ME); 3 MSCEIT Branch 1 Perceive Emotion; 4 MSCEIT Branch 3 Understand Emotion; 5 MSCEIT Branch 4 Manage Emotion; **p<.01; *p<.05

Given the existence of subgroups and a lack of subgroup differences between the

mean scores of the independent variables within the sample, it was considered

necessary to re-examine H1a, H1b, H2a and H2b at a subgroup level. The intention

was to explore the nature of the relationships between the independent variables

and their incremental validity in predicting MLE for each of the subgroups. The

results obtained are discussed next.

5.8 TESTING THE HYPOTHESES FOR THE SUBGROUPS

As was described earlier in this chapter, correlation analysis was used to test H1a

(IQ, EI and MMS are positively related) and H1b (IQ, EI and MMS have discriminant

validity), while regression analysis was used to test H2a (IQ predicts MLE), H2b (EI

has incremental validity in predicting MLE, beyond IQ), and H2c (MMS has

incremental validity in predicting MLE, beyond IQ and EI). These processes were

Page 188: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

186

repeated at a subgroup level and the results obtained from these analyses are

reported in subsequent sections.

5.8.1 H1 and H2 for Leadership Effectiveness Group One

The results of the correlation analysis for Leadership Effectiveness Group One

(LE1) can be seen in Table 5.14. For LE1 managerial-leaders there were significant

relationships between all three of the EI variables and MLE: Perceive Emotion

(r= -.36, p<.01), Understand Emotion (r= .34, p<.01) and between Manage Emotion

(r= .34, p<.01). IQ was not significantly correlated with any other constructs. MMS

correlated positively with Manage Emotion (r= .23, p<.05). Two of the three

MSCEITv2 branch correlations were significant with a non-significant correlation

between Understand Emotion and Manage Emotion.

Table 5.14 Correlations for Leadership Effectiveness Group One

MLE1 IQ MMS2 (TIS)

Perceive Emotion3

Understand Emotion4

IQ .16

MMS (TIS)2 -.05 .11

Perceive Emotion3 -.36** .19 -.10

Understand Emotion4 .34** .19 .10 .24*

Manage Emotion5 .34** -.02 .23* .27* .07

1 MLE operationalised as Leadership Effectiveness (LE); 2 MMS operationalised as Ego Development (ED) total item score (TIS); 3 MSCEIT Branch 1 Perceive Emotion; 4 MSCEIT Branch 3 Understand Emotion; 5 MSCEIT Branch 4 Manage Emotion; **p<.01; *p<.05

The regression analysis undertaken for this group suggested all of the independent

variables were significantly related to respondents’ perceptions of MLE

(R2= .62; F=28.88; p<.01). The standardised regression coefficients obtained in this

case can be seen in Table 5.15. For this subgroup of managers, Perceive Emotion

(β= -.70; p<.01) was the single largest negative predictor of MLE and Manage

Page 189: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

187

Emotion (β= .58; p<.01) was the single largest positive predictor. The remaining

relationships were moderately strong and also varied in direction: positive between

MLE and IQ (β= .24; p<.01) and Understand Emotion (β= .45; p<.01), and negative

between MLE and MMS (β= -.32; p<.01).

Table 5.15 Regression for Leadership Effectiveness Group One

Standardised Coefficient

Sig. Collinearity Statistics

Beta Tolerance VIF

IQ .24 .00 .92 1.09

MMS (TIS) -.32 .00 .88 1.13

Perceive Emotion -.70 .00 .81 1.23

Understand Emotion .45 .00 .91 1.10

Manage Emotion .58 .00 .85 1.18

5.8.2 and H2 for Leadership Effectiveness Group Two

The results of the correlation analysis for Leadership Effectiveness Group Two

(LE2) can be seen in Table 5.16. For this group of managerial-leaders, there was

a significant positive relationship between MLE and Perceive Emotion

(r= .43, p<.01) and between IQ and Understand Emotion (r= .42, p<.01). MMS was

negatively correlated with Perceive Emotion (r= -.32, p<.01) and Manage Emotion

(r= -.41, p<.01). Two of the three MSCEITv2 branch correlations were significant

with again the non-significant correlation between Understand Emotion and

Manage_Emotion.

Page 190: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

188

Table 5.16 Correlations for Leadership Effectiveness Group Two

MLE1 IQ MMS2 (TIS)

Perceive Emotion3

Understand Emotion4

IQ .08

MMS (TIS)2 -.15 -.02

Perceive Emotion3 .43** .23 -.32**

Understand Emotion4 -.06 .42** -.12 .45**

Manage Emotion5 -.25 .03 -.41** .39** .19

1 MLE operationalised as Leadership Effectiveness (LE); 2 MMS operationalised as Ego Development (ED) total item score (TIS); 3 MSCEIT Branch 1 Perceive Emotion; 4 MSCEIT Branch 3 Understand Emotion; 5

MSCEIT Branch 4 Manage Emotion; **p<.01; *p<.05

The regression analysis undertaken for the second subgroup suggested only three

of the independent variables were significantly related to perceptions of MLE

(R2= .45; F=8.34; p<.01). The standardised regression coefficients that were

obtained in this case can be seen in Table 5.17. For this subgroup of managerial-

leaders, IQ and MMS are not significant predictors. Perceive Emotion

(β= .73; p<.01) is the strongest positive indicator and Manage Emotion

(β= -.55; p<.01) is the strongest negative indicator. Understand Emotion

(β= -.34; p<.02) was also negatively related to perceptions of MLE.

Table 5.17 Regression for Leadership Effectiveness Group Two

Standardised Coefficient

Sig. Collinearity Statistics

Beta Tolerance VIF

IQ .07 .60 .82 1.22

MMS (TIS) -.18 .15 .80 1.25

Perceive Emotion .73 .00 .67 1.50

Understand Emotion -.34 .02 .69 1.45

Manage Emotion -.55 .00 .75 1.33

Page 191: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

189

5.8.3 H1 and H2 for Leadership Effectiveness Group Three

The results of the correlation analysis for the Leadership Effectiveness Group

Three (LE3) can be seen in Table 5.18.

Table 5.18 Correlations for Leadership Effectiveness Group Three

MLE1 IQ MMS2 (TIS)

Perceive Emotion3

Understand Emotion4

IQ .30*

MMS (TIS)2 .20 .10

Perceive Emotion3 .33 .00 .10

Understand Emotion4 -.55** .36* .19 .16

Manage Emotion5 -.36* .09 .03 .17 .21

1 MLE operationalised as Leadership Effectiveness (LE); 2 MMS operationalised as Ego Development (ED) total item score (TIS); 3 MSCEIT Branch 1 Perceive Emotion; 4 MSCEIT Branch 3 Understand Emotion; 5 MSCEIT Branch 4 Manage Emotion; **p<.01; *p<.05

For this group of managerial-leaders there was a significant positive relationship

between MLE and IQ (r= .30, p<.05) and a significant negative relationship

between MLE and both Understand Emotion (r= -.55, p<.01) and Manage Emotion

(r= -.36, p<.05). IQ was positively correlation with Understand Emotion

(r= .36, p<.05). For this group of managerial-leaders there were no significant

correlations with MMS or between the MSCEITv2 subscales.

The model summary of the regression analysis undertaken for the third subgroup

suggested all of the independent variables were significantly related to perceptions

of MLE (R2= .95; F=176.62; p<.01). The standardised regression coefficients

obtained in this case can be seen in Table 5.19.

Page 192: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

190

Table 5.19 Regression for Leadership Effectiveness Group Three

Standardised Coefficient

Sig. Collinearity Statistics

Beta Tolerance VIF

IQ .60 .00 .87 1.15

MMS (TIS) .25 .00 .96 1.04

Perceive Emotion .49 .00 .95 1.06

Understand Emotion -.82 .00 .81 1.24

Manage Emotion -.33 .00 .94 1.07

For this subgroup of managerial-leaders, the strongest positive predictor of MLE is

IQ (β= .60; p<.01) followed by Perceive Emotion (β= .49; p<.01) and then MMS

(β= .25; p<.01). Understand Emotion (β= -.82; p<.02) and Manage Emotion

(β= .33; p<.01) were both negatively related to perceptions of MLE.

5.8.4 H1 and H2 for Management Effectiveness Group One

The results of the correlation analysis for Management Effectiveness Group One

(ME1) can be seen in Table 5.20. There were significant relationships between all

three of the EI variables and MLE: Perceive Emotion (r= -.45, p<.01), Understand

Emotion (r= .51, p<.01) and Manage Emotion (r= .27, p<.01). There was a

significant positive relationship between MMS (TIS) and Perceive Emotion

(r= .38, p<.01). For this group of managerial-leaders there were no significant

correlations with IQ or between the MSCEITv2 subscales.

Page 193: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

191

Table 5.20 Correlations for Management Effectiveness Group One

MLE1 IQ MMS2 (TIS)

Perceive Emotion3

Understand Emotion4

IQ -.07

MMS (TIS)2 -.02 .07

Perceive Emotion3 -.45** .25 .38**

Understand Emotion4 .51** .25 .17 .04

Manage Emotion5 .27** .12 .11 .23 .23

1 MLE operationalised as Management Effectiveness (ME); 2 MMS operationalised as Ego Development (ED) total item score (TIS); 3 MSCEIT Branch 1 Perceive Emotion; 4 MSCEIT Branch 3 Understand Emotion; 5 MSCEIT Branch 4 Manage Emotion; **p<.01; *p<.05

The regression analysis undertaken for the first ME subgroup suggested four of the

independent variables were significantly related to perceptions of MLE

(R2= .67; F=22.87; p<.01). The standardised regression coefficients obtained in

this case can be seen in Table 5.21.

Table 5.21 Regression for Management Effectiveness Group One

Standardised Coefficient

Sig. Collinearity Statistics

Beta Tolerance VIF

IQ -.04 .62 .86 1.16

MMS (TIS) -.41 .00 .78 1.28

Perceive Emotion -.70 .00 .72 1.38

Understand Emotion .54 .00 .88 1.14

Manage Emotion .36 .00 .87 1.15

The independent variables significantly related to perceptions of MLE differed in the

direction and strength of these relationships. Both MMS (β= -.41; p<.01) and

Perceive Emotion (β= -.70 p<.01) were negatively related to perceptions of MLE. In

contrast, Understand Emotion (β= .54, p<.01) and Manage Emotion (β= .36, p<.01)

were positively related to perceptions of MLE. IQ was not significant for this

subgroup of managers.

Page 194: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

192

5.8.5 H1 and H2 for Management Effectiveness Group Two

The results of the correlation analysis for the Management Effectiveness Group

Two (ME2) can be seen in Table 5.22. For ME2 managerial-leaders, there was a

significant relationship between MLE and MMS (r= -.33, p<.05), Understand

Emotion (r= -.37, p<.01) and Manage Emotion (r= .45, p<.01). A significant positive

relationship existed between IQ and Understand Emotion (r= .42, p<.01). Perceive

Emotion was positively and significantly related to Understand Emotion

(r= .45, p<.01) and Management Emotion (r= .35, p<.05).

Table 5.22 Correlations for Management Effectiveness Group Two

MLE1 IQ MMS2 (TIS)

Perceive Emotion3

Understand Emotion4

IQ -.06

MMS (TIS)2 -.33* -.15

Perceive Emotion3 .27 .16 -.02

Understand Emotion4 -.37** .42** -.08 .45**

Manage Emotion5 .45** .14 .04 .35* .10

1 MLE operationalised as Management Effectiveness (ME); 2 MMS operationalised as Ego Development (ED) total item score (TIS); 3 MSCEIT Branch 1 Perceive Emotion; 4 MSCEIT Branch 3 Understand Emotion; 5 MSCEIT Branch 4 Manage Emotion; **p<.01; *p<.05

The regression analysis undertaken for the second ME subgroup suggested all the

independent variables were significantly related to perceptions of MLE

(R2= .90; F=91.04; p<.01). The standardised regression coefficients that were

obtained in this case can be seen in Table 5.23.

As was the case with the first group, while each of the independent variables was

significantly related to perceptions of MLE, for this subgroup, they differed in the

direction and strength of these relationships. MMS was again negatively related to

MLE perceptions in this subgroup (β= -.32, p<.01). In this subgroup, Understand

Page 195: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

193

Emotion (β= -.77, p<.01) and Manage Emotion (β= -.68, p<.01) were negatively

related to MLE, while Perceive Emotion was positively related to MLE

(β= .82, p<.01). This was opposite to the results that were obtained for subgroup

ME1, providing one reason for the lack of significant relationships in the overall

sample. Another difference for this subgroup was that IQ was significantly and

positively related to respondents’ perceptions of MLE (β= .19, p<.01).

Table 5.23 Regression for Management Effectiveness Group Two

Standardised Coefficient

Sig. Collinearity Statistics

Beta Tolerance VIF

IQ .19 .00 .79 1.26

MMS (TIS) -.32 .00 .97 1.03

Perceive Emotion .82 .00 .70 1.43

Understand Emotion -.77 .00 .67 1.50

Manage Emotion -.68 .00 .85 1.17

5.8.6 H1 and H2 for Management Effectiveness Group Three

The results of the correlation analysis for the Management Effectiveness Group

Three (ME3) can be seen in Table 5.24. For the ME3 group of managerial-leaders

there are only two significant relationships, which are both positive: between MLE

and both IQ (r= .38, p<.05) and Perceive Emotion (r= .58, p<.01).

Page 196: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

194

Figure 5.24 Correlations for Management Effectiveness Group Three

MLE1 IQ MMS2 (TIS)

Perceive Emotion3

Understand Emotion4

IQ .38*

MMS (TIS)2 -.21 .07

Perceive Emotion3 .58** .08 .18

Understand Emotion4 -.31 .30 .10 .10

Manage Emotion5 -.22 -.12 .28 .10 .01

1 MLE operationalised as Management Effectiveness (ME); 2 MMS operationalised as Ego Development (ED) total item score (TIS); 3 MSCEIT Branch 1 Perceive Emotion; 4 MSCEIT Branch 3 Understand Emotion; 5 MSCEIT Branch 4 Manage Emotion; **p<.01; *p<.05

The regression analysis undertaken for the third ME subgroup suggested all but

one of the independent variables were significantly related to perceptions of MLE

(R2= .76; F=22.63; p<.01). The standardised regression coefficients that were

obtained in this case can be seen in Table 5.25.

Table 5.25 Regression for Management Effectiveness Group Three

Standardised Coefficient

Sig. Collinearity Statistics

Beta Tolerance VIF

IQ .48 .00 .89 1.13

MMS (TIS) -.27 .01 .89 1.13

Perceive Emotion .65 .00 .96 1.05

Understand Emotion -.50 .00 .90 1.11

Manage Emotion -.14 .13 .90 1.11

For this group of managers, Perceive Emotion (β= .65, p<.01) and IQ

(β= .48, p<.01) had positive relationships with perceptions of MLE. Understand

Emotion (β= -.50, p<.01) and MMS (β= -.27, p<.01) were negatively related to MLE

perceptions. Manage Emotion was not significant.

In summary, the independent variables in different combinations accounted for

between .45 to .95 of the variance in respondents’ perceptions of MLE as

Page 197: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

195

measured by the revised Management Effectiveness and Leadership Effectiveness

scales. However, the different independent variables varied in the strength and the

direction of their relationship with both dependent variables across the sub-groups.

A discussion of the significant regression coefficients relevant to each of the sub-

groups with sufficient numbers to undertake the regression analysis is provided in

Chapter 6. However, before these results were interpreted in more detail, it was

considered important to see if there were background variables that differed across

the various groups. The analysis that was undertaken to see if this was the case

and the results obtained from this analysis are discussed in the next section.

5.9 SUBGROUP BACKGROUND DIFFERENCES

In order to examine the background differences between the groups in a

multivariate way, a discriminant analysis was undertaken in which the background

characteristics were included as potential group differentiators (Klecka 1980). In

addition to age, years of work experience and years of managing staff, a number of

nominally scaled characteristics were obtained from respondents, as can be seen

in Table 5.26. These nominal variables were converted into dummy (i.e. zero-one)

variables before being included in the discriminant analysis.

As two sets of groups had been obtained (a set of three groups for Leadership

Effectiveness and a set of three useable groups for Management Effectiveness), a

discriminant analysis was estimated for both the LE and ME contexts. The results

that were obtained in each analysis are discussed in turn.

Page 198: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

196

5.9.1 Background Differences in the LE Groups

As was also noted earlier in Section 5.6, three groups were found that differed in

the way MLE was predicted when measured with the revised Leadership

Effectiveness scale. The means of these three groups’ background variables can

be seen in Table 5.27, which also shows the F-statistics that were obtained from

the equivalent of a one-way ANOVA on each of these constructs (Soutar & Clarke

1981).

As can be seen in the Table, there were a number of statistically significant

differences between the groups. For example, while the mean scores for years of

work experience were all greater than 15, group two had the least such experience

(16 years), while group three had the greatest such experience (21 years).

Table 5.26 Nominally Scaled Background Variables

Gender Male

Female

Ethnicity Asian Caucasian Middle Eastern

Education All Other Undergrad/Bachelor’s degree Post-graduate/Master’s degree

Employment Status

Employed full-time Employed part-time Not currently employed

Manage Staff Yes No

Page 199: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

197

Consequently, it was seen as worthwhile to examine these background differences

in a multivariate way.

Table 5.27 Frequency (or Mean) of Background Variables for the LE Groups

Background Characteristic LE 1 n=53

LE 2 n=66

LE 3 n= 50

F- Statistic Sig.

Caucasian Ethnicity 51 55 46 3.00 .05

Currently Working 49 56 45 1.17 .31

Currently Managing Staff 42 41 39 3.25 .04

Education Level

All other 9 10 9 .09 .91

Bachelor Degree 21 20 20 .76 .47

Masters/PG 23 36 20 1.12 .33

Gender M

F

33

20

36

30

34

16

.86

-------

.42

-------

Age1 39.5 37.0 40.3 2.77 .06

Work Experience1 18.8 16.3 20.6 3.38 .03

Management Experience1 8.5 7.7 11.6 3.79 .03

1 Figures represent the group mean measured in years.

Two significant discriminant functions were found that the I2 statistic (Peterson &

Mahajan 1976) suggested explained eight per cent of the differences between the

three groups. Clearly, any multivariate differences are marginal and this needs to

be kept in mind. However, it was decided to examine these differences further to

see if they provided any useful insights.

Consequently, the structural correlations between the background characteristics

and the estimated discriminant functions were computed and varimax rotated to

better understand the groups’ differences (Clarke-Murphy & Soutar 2005). The

Page 200: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

198

resulting structural correlations, which are ordered in terms of the strength of the

relationships with the two discriminant functions, are shown in Table 5.28.

Table 5.28 Structural Correlations After Varimax Rotation

Background Characteristic Discriminant Function

1 2

Caucasian Ethnicity .77 .11

Managing Staff .63 .41

Currently Working .32 .24

Education: Bachelor Degree .03 -.001

Management Experience -.04 .98

Years Work Experience .09 .76

Age .03 .72

Education: All Others .08 .25

Gender: Male .13 .24

Education: Masters/Post-Graduate -.00 -.19

As can be seen in the Table, the first function was most related to whether the

leader had a Caucasian background and whether they managed staff. The second

function was most related to the leader’s years of management and work

experience and age. Consequently, the first function was termed Caucasian

Managers, while the second function was termed Experience.

The three groups’ positions in the space created by the estimated functions can be

shown by their centroid (or average) values (Johnson 1977). These values are

shown in Table 5.29. As can be seen in the Table, members of LE1 and LE2 were

Page 201: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

199

more likely to be Caucasian managers, while LE3 members were more likely to be

experienced managers.

Table 5.29 Group Centroid Values

Leadership Effectiveness Group Caucasian Managers Experience

1 .76 -.09

2 .64 .17

3 -.36 .96

5.9.2 Background Differences in the ME Groups

As was also noted earlier, four groups were found that differed in the way MLE was

predicted when measured with the revised Management Effectiveness scale.

However, only three groups had sufficient numbers to be used in subsequent

analysis. Consequently, only these three groups were included in the discriminant

analysis. The means of the three groups’ background variables can be seen in

Table 5.30, which also shows the F-statistics obtained from the equivalent of a one-

way ANOVA on each of these constructs.

As can be seen in the Table, there were no statistically significant differences

between the Management Effectiveness groups’ background variables.

Differences in the estimated regression coefficients of the independent variables for

each of the Management Effectiveness groups were not a result of differences in

these background variables.

Page 202: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

200

Table 5.30 Frequency (or Mean) of Background Variables for the ME Groups

Background Characteristic

ME 1 n=81

ME 2 n=46

ME 3 n= 31

F- Statistic Sig.

Caucasian Ethnicity 71 42 29 .50 .61

Currently Working 72 41 27 .16 .85

Managing Staff 56 35 24 .78 .46

Education Level

All other 15 9 4 .34 .71 Bachelor Degree 28 17 13 .23 .79

Masters/PG 38 19 14 .09 .91

Gender M

F

45 36

32 14

13 18 .99 .37

Age1 38.3 39.7 40 .68 .51

Work Experience1 18.0 19.8 18.9 .59 .55

Management Experience1 8.9 10.4 8.2 .81 .45

1 Figures represent the group mean and is measured in years.

In summary, the discriminant analyses suggested there were some marginal

differences in the Leadership Effectiveness groups’ backgrounds, as there were

significant differences in the groups’ ethnicity and work experience. LE1 and LE2

members were more likely to be Caucasian managers, while LE3 members were

more likely to be experienced managers. However, there were no differences in the

backgrounds of the Management Effectiveness groups.

5.10 SUMMARY

This chapter outlined the results of testing the study’s three research hypotheses.

In H1a it was posited that there was a positive relationship between the predictor

variables of IQ, EI and MMS. H1a was tested using correlational analysis and the

Page 203: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

201

results suggested the hypothesis should be rejected at a full sample level for this

data set. In H1b it was posited that there would be discriminant validity between the

IQ, EI and MMS constructs. H1b was also tested using correlational analysis and

results suggested there was support for this hypothesis.

Hierarchical regression analysis was then used to test H2a (IQ predicts MLE), H2b

(EI has incremental validity in predicting MLE, beyond IQ), and H2c (MMS has

incremental validity in predicting MLE, beyond IQ and EI). However, this analysis

found that none of the variables were significantly related to either measure of MLE

(i.e. ME or LE). It was thought this outcome may have been due to the presence of

subgroups in the sample and it was decided to undertake a post-hoc Latent Class

Regression Analysis to see if this was the case.

This analysis suggested subgroups did exist: three were suggested for the

dependent variable of LE and four were suggested for the dependent variable of

ME, although one of these groups was too small to examine further.

Consequently, differences between the subgroups were examined by testing H2a,

H2b and H2c for each of the subgroups. The results obtained from this analysis

found the independent variables accounted for between 45% and 95% of the

variance in respondents’ perceptions for the ME and LE subgroups. Further, the

independent constructs varied in the combination, strength and direction of their

relationships, with perceptions of ME and LE across the subgroups. In summary, at

a subgroup level, support was found for H2.

In order to see whether subgroups differed in their background characteristics

within the sample, a discriminant analysis was undertaken for both the LE and ME

subgroups. This analysis suggested some differences for the LE groups. Members

Page 204: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

202

of LE1 and LE2 were more likely to have a Caucasian background, while members

of LE3 tended to have more years of work experience. There were no statistically

significant differences between the ME groups in terms of the background

variables. In summary, the differences between the subgroups on the background

characteristics were marginal to non-existent and so can be ruled out as the source

of differences between the subgroups. In the next, and final, chapter, the results

from these analyses are discussed and conclusions drawn for the research as a

whole.

Page 205: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

203

Chapter 6

DISCUSSION

6.1 INTRODUCTION

The previous chapter presented the results of the analysis that examined the core

hypotheses that were outlined in Chapter 3. This chapter discusses these results

and provides a conclusion to the thesis. This discussion begins with a summary of

the thesis (section 6.2) and the inferences that were drawn from the analysis of the

data that were collected within the study (section 6.3). The chapter then explores

the study’s implications for practice (section 6.4) and for future research and theory

(section 6.5). Finally, some limitations of the research are outlined (section 6.6)

and the chapter is concluded (section 6.7).

6.2 A SUMMARY OF THE THESIS

The present study was undertaken to improve our understanding of the

relationships between emotional intelligence (EI), meaning-making structure

(MMS), intelligence (IQ) and managerial-leadership effectiveness (MLE). It has

been argued that today’s managerial-leaders need emotional abilities if they are to

successfully navigate the diverse and constantly changing context of modern

organisational life (Pfeiffer 2001; Price 2003; Quatro, Waldman & Galvin 2007;

Reams 2005; Zohar & Marshall 2000). Further, significant amounts of time and

money are being invested in leadership development initiatives that focus on

Page 206: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

204

developing managerial-leaders’ EI capacities. However, the role EI plays in MLE

continues to be poorly understood (Antonakis, Ashkanasy & Dasborough 2009;

Walter, Cole & Humphrey 2011). In addition, other aspects of personality, such as

capacity for meaning-making, may contribute to individual and leadership

effectiveness beyond EI (Rooke & Torbert 2005; Raelin 2006). Modern

organisational life is becoming increasingly complex and, consequently,

managerial-leaders who have a greater capacity to deal with the complexity of

current and emerging meaning-making structures will have an advantage in all

aspects of perception, cognition and processing (Caruso & Wolfe 2004; George

2000; Humphrey, Pollack & Hawver 2008; Young 2002b).

Despite this, very few empirical studies have examined the relationship between

ability EI and MLE (i.e. studies with valid samples of managerial-leaders and ability-

based measures of EI that have addressed issues of common method variance)

(Antonakis, Ashkanasy & Dasborough 2009; Walter, Cole & Humphrey 2011).

Further, to the researcher’s knowledge, no published studies have explored the

relationship between ability EI and MMS using a sample of practising managerial-

leaders, which made the present study an important one. Based on a review of the

literature, three hypotheses were suggested that were tested within the study,

namely:

H1: The individual difference constructs of EI, MMS and IQ are positively

correlated and have discriminant validity. More specifically:

H1a EI, MMS and IQ are positively correlated.

H1b EI, MMS and IQ have discriminant validity from each other.

Page 207: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

205

H2: The individual difference constructs of EI and MMS have incremental

predictive validity beyond IQ. More specifically:

H2a IQ is a significant predictor of MLE.

H2b EI is significant incremental predictor of MLE, beyond IQ.

H2c MMS is significant incremental predictor of MLE, beyond IQ and EI.

Based on a review of the literature, three existing measures were selected to

measure the independent variables included in the research model. Mayer,

Salovey, and Caruso’s (2002) Emotional Intelligence Test version 2 (MSCEITv2)

was used to measure ability Emotional Intelligence, the Washington University

Sentence Completion Test (WUSCT) (Hy and Loevinger, 1996) was used to

measure meaning-making structure and IQ was measured using the Wonderlic

Personality Test (WPT) (Wonderlic & Associates, 2002). The measure used to

operationalise the dependent MLE construct was the Integral Leadership and

Management Development Profile (ILMDP) (Cacioppe, 2000).

The study was made possible when access was made available to two

convenience samples of managerial-leaders who were undertaking leadership

development at the AIM-UWA Integral Leadership Centre. The first sample was

obtained from a group of 460 target managerial-leaders who had participated in a

leadership development program that included the ILMDP as a multi-source

feedback assessment. This data set, referred to throughout the thesis as ‘the

separate ILMDP sample’, included individual responses from each ‘other-rater’ on

the 32 items that make up the ILMDP. After removing respondents who had

missing data, a total of 364 cases were retained. The second sample, referred to

Page 208: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

206

as the main research sample, also included managerial-leaders who had

undertaken a leadership development program through the AIM-UWA Integral

Leadership Centre. This sample included part or whole responses from 259

managerial-leaders. However, after removing respondents with missing data, the

main sample included 169 complete cases.

The initial phase of data analysis examined the measurement properties of the

ILMDP, the internal consistency of the MSCEITv2 scales and the inter-rater

similarity for the WUSCT data. The separate sample of 364 target managerial-

leaders, who had three to twelve other-raters, was used to examine the

measurement properties of the ILMDP. The analysis of the ILMDP measurement

properties suggested the proposed eight-factor model was not a good fit to the data

in the present study. Consequently, an exploratory factor analysis was undertaken

that resulted in a two factor MLE model that included:

1. An 11-item Leadership Effectiveness (LE) factor and

2. A 12-item Management Effectiveness (ME) factor.

Further examination of these factors using confirmatory factor analysis and the

data in the main sample identified a one-factor congeneric model for each. The

one-factor congeneric models for the revised LE and the revised ME scales had

appropriate unidimensionality, reliability and validity. However, each scale had a

smaller number of items than had been the case at the beginning of the analysis.

To ensure there had been no loss in information in reducing the number of items,

correlations were computed between the revised scales and the initially suggested

scales (Thomas, Soutar & Ryan 2001). In this case, the correlation between the

Page 209: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

207

two leadership effectiveness scores was .96 and the correlation between the two

management effectiveness scores was .97, suggesting no information was lost in

ensuring that the two scales had appropriate measurement properties. The four-

item LE scale and the five-item ME scale had discriminant validity and,

consequently, were retained for use as dependent variables in the subsequent

analysis.

A subsection of the main research sample, consisting of 255 completed surveys,

was used to analyse the MSCEITv2 reliability. The reliability estimates for the

MSCEITv2 Scales ranged from an acceptable reliability of .88 for Perceive Emotion

through to an unacceptable reliability of .50 for Understanding Emotion. The

reliabilities for the Strategic EI area score and many of the task level scores were

also unacceptably low. However, a further analysis of the MSCEITv2 subscales

using confirmatory factor analysis failed to suggest a better alternate structure.

Consequently, the original MSCEITv2 branch scales were retained for use as the

ability-measure of EI and the reliability of the EI measures was noted as a limitation

to the present study.

Another subsection of the main research sample, consisting of 183 completed

protocols, was used to analyse the WUSCT inter-rater similarity. The inter-rater

similarity on the WUSCT data suggested there was a high level of reliability and

agreement in the scoring of the item-responses and the total protocol ratings and

that, consequently, they could be used in the subsequent analysis with confidence.

The main research sample, consisting of 169 LE and ME scores, the MSCEITv2

branch scores, the WUSCT and the WPT scores, was used to test H1, and H2,

which were not supported, despite the theoretical foundation that had led to their

Page 210: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

208

suggestion. These findings were inconsistent with previous research that had

supported the existence of a relationship between IQ and EI (Cote & Miners 2006;

Rode et al. 2007; Rosete & Ciarrochi 2005; Van Rooy, Viswesvaran & Pluta 2005)

between IQ and MMS (Hauser 1976; Loevinger 1976; McCrae & Costa 1980;

Westenberg & Block 1993) and between EI and MLE (Byron 2007; Kerr et al. 2006;

Rosete & Ciarrochi 2005).

One possible explanation for these results was thought to be unobserved

heterogeneity (Sarstead 2008). Consequently, the data were analysed using Latent

Class (LC) Regression Analysis, which can be used to determine the smallest

number of latent classes that are sufficient to explain the variance in a dependent

variable within each of the subgroups (Magidson & Vermunt 2002). In the current

research, the LC Regression Analysis results suggested there were three

subgroups that differed in the way the independent variables related to perceptions

of LE and four subgroups that differed in the way these same variables related to

perceptions of ME. However, the fourth ME subgroup was too small to be retained

for further analysis. The remaining six subgroups (three in the LE case and three in

the ME case) were used to re-examine H1 and H2 at a subgroup level.

6.3 DISCUSSION

6.3.1 The Relationships Between IQ, EI and MMS (H1)

The results of the correlation analysis found limited support for the hypothesised

relationships between EI, MMS and IQ. The only significant correlation at the

overall sample level was a positive relationship between IQ and Understand

Emotion which replicates results found by others (David 2005’, in Brackett, 2006,

Page 211: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

209

Lopes et al., 2003; Lam & Kirby 2002). In contrast MMS, which was measured by

as ED (TIS), was not significantly related to any of the other independent variables.

However, this was not the case at a subgroup level.

Using the LE subgroups identified through the Latent Class Regression Analysis, a

number of significant relationships were identified, as can be seen in Table 6.1.

For the LE groups, significant relationships existed between MLE and IQ (for

subgroup LE3), Perceive Emotion (for subgroups LE1 and LE2), Understand

Emotion (for subgroups L1 and L3) and Manage Emotion (for subgroups LE1 and

LE3). IQ was also significantly related to Understand Emotion for two subgroups

(for subgroups LE2 and LE3). Finally, MMS was significantly related to Perceive

Emotion for subgroup LE2 and to Manage Emotion for subgroup LE1 and LE2.

Interestingly, Understand Emotion and Manage Emotion were not significantly

correlated for any of the subgroups and none of the EI branch scores were

correlated for subgroup LE3.

Table 6.1 Summary of Correlations for the Leadership Effectiveness Subgroups

IQ MMS MLE Perceive Understand

IQ1

MMS2

MLE3 LE3: .30*

Perceive4 LE2: .32** LE1: -.36** LE2: .43**

Understand5 LE2: .42** LE1: .34** LE1: .24* LE3: .36* LE3: -.55** LE2: .45**

Manage5 LE1: .23* LE1: .34** LE1: .27* LE2: -.41** LE3: -.36* LE2: .39** 1 Traditional intelligence; 2 MMS operationalised as Ego Development total item score (TIS); 3 MLE operationalised as Leadership Effectiveness (LE); 4 MSCEIT Branch 1 Perceive Emotion; 5 MSCEIT Branch 3 Understanding Emotion; 6MSCEIT Branch 4 Manage Emotion; **p<.01; *p<.05

Page 212: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

210

Similarly, using the ME subgroups identified through the Latent Class Regression

Analysis, a number of significant relationships were identified, as can be seen in

Table 6.2. Significant positive correlations existed between MLE and IQ (for

subgroups ME1 and ME3), MLE and MMS (for subgroup ME2), MLE and Perceive

Emotion (for subgroup ME3), MLE and both Understand Emotion and Manage

Emotion (for subgroup ME2). MMS was significantly correlation with Perceive

Emotion for subgroup ME1. Significant correlations also existed between IQ and

Understand Emotion (ME1 and ME2) and Manage Emotion (ME1). Once again,

Understand Emotion and Manage Emotion were not significantly correlated for any

of the subgroups. Finally, none of the EI branch scores were correlated for ME1 or

ME3.

Table 6.2 Summary of Correlations for the Management Effectiveness Subgroups

IQ MMS MLE Perceive Understand

IQ1

MMS2

MLE3 ME1: -.45** ME2: -.33*

ME3: .38*

Perceive4 ME1: .38** ME3: .58**

Understand5 ME1: .51** ME2: -.37** ME2: .45** ME2: .42**

Manage6 ME1: .27** ME2: .45** ME2: .35* 1 Traditional intelligence; 2 MMS operationalised as Ego Development total item score (TIS); 3 MLE operationalised as Management Effectiveness (ME); 4 MSCEIT Branch 1 Perceive Emotion; 5 MSCEIT Branch 3 Understanding Emotion; 6 MSCEIT Branch 4 Manage Emotion; **p<.01; *p<.05

In summary, a correlation analysis of the research constructs at a subgroup level

suggested there was partial support for H1a. The constructs of IQ, EI and MMS

were sometimes moderately correlated. However, the strength and direction of

these relationships varied across the subgroups. Further, the pattern and strength

Page 213: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

211

of correlations suggest the constructs of IQ, EI, MMS and MLE are distinct and

thus, provide support for H1b.

6.3.2 Incremental Predictive Validity of MMS (H2)

The results of the regression analysis provided partial support for the hypothesis

that MMS, operationalised as ED, had incremental predictive validity beyond IQ

and EI in predicting perceptions of MLE. While the regression at an overall sample

level did not suggest any significant relationships, at a subgroup level there were

differences between the predictor variables in the model and their patterns of

relationships with the dependent variables, as can be seen in Table 6.3. However,

before discussing the results at the construct and subgroup level, larger scale

patterns that emerged are described.

The first pattern to emerge was the positive and negative polarity of the regression

coefficients. With the exception of IQ, the explanatory variables had both positive

and negative instances of polarity in different subgroups. It is interesting that some

of the relationships were negative. At least three explanations can be offered,

namely:

1. The ability constructs (IQ, EI or MMS) may not be effective in all managerial

contexts.

2. The skill may not be done well, although the use of ability measures should

mitigate this issue

3. An ability may be over-used by a manager.

Page 214: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

212

Table 6.3 Summary of Standardised Regression Coefficients for all Subgroups

Leadership Effectiveness Management Effectiveness

Construct Group 1 Group 2 Group 3 Group 1 Group 2 Group 3

IQ .24** --- .60** --- .19** .48**

MMS -.32** --- .25** -.41** -.32** -.27**

Perceive Emotion -.70** .73** .49** -.70** .82** .65**

Understand Emotion .45** -.34** -.82** .54** -.77** -.50**

Manage Emotion .58** -.55** -.33** .36** -.68** --- **p<.01; *p<.05

There was a pattern to the polarity between the first and second strongest predictor

variables. If the strongest predictor variable was positive, the next strongest

predictor variable was negative, and visa versa. Two additional patterns regarding

polarity of the independent predictor variables were evident, as:

1. The Perceive Emotion and Understand Emotion coefficients had consistently

opposite polarities.

2. The Understand Emotion and Manage Emotion coefficients had consistently

the same polarity.

Theoretically, it is understandable that the Understand Emotion and Manage

Emotion abilities vary similarly as they combine to form the MSCEITv2 Strategic

Reasoning Area of emotional Intelligence (Mayer, Salovey & Caruso 2002).

However, why these abilities are opposite to Perceive Emotion is not clear. What is

clear is that when a relationship is positive, the independent variable contributes to

effectiveness and when it is negative, it detracts from effectiveness. We first look at

the LC Regression Analysis results through the lens of the independent predictor

variables, including the significance and polarity of each.

Page 215: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

213

IQ was significant and positive for four groups of managerial-leaders

(β= .19 to .60, p<.01). It was the only construct to be consistent in the direction of

the relationship with MLE. The interpretation is that IQ is either not a significant

predictor or is a positive predictor of MLE. These results support both sides of the

IQ versus EI debate, as IQ was a significant antecedent of perceived effectiveness

in some, but not all, of the subgroups. IQ is important in some cases, but it is not

the whole story (Kellet, Humphrey & Sleeth 2006; Kellet, Humphrey & Sleeth 2002;

Riggio 2002).

MMS was significant for five of the subgroups, being positive for one and negative

for four. This suggests that, for many managerial-leaders, an increasing capacity to

think and act with more complexity detracts from their perceived effectiveness. At

an absolute level, the strength of the significant standardised betas was relatively

consistent, varying from |.25| to |.41|. One interpretation of the varied direction in

the relationship between MMS and MLE is the ‘match’ between the managerial-

leaders capacity for meaning-making and the context in which they operate. As

such, where MMS negatively predicts MLE, there may be a mismatch between the

target managerial-leader’s current capacity for meaning-making and the

requirement of their context. There is empirical evidence, albeit limited, to support

the need for such a match between the MMS and a managerial-leader’s context

(Rooke & Torbet 1998). A lack of a significant relationship between MMS and MLE

might indicate a neutral effect of the capacity for meaning-making in the current

context. Regardless of whether it was in synch or not with the context it was not

relevant. Finally, when the relationship was positive, the managerial-leader was

able to leverage their current MMS into MLE.

Page 216: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

214

At a conceptual level, it was not expected that some of the EI abilities would have

negative relationships with perceptions of MLE. The obtained results contribute to

the ongoing debate about the existence and nature of the relationship between EI

and managerial-leadership. Much of the literature exploring the link between EI and

MLE has assumed a positive relationship (George 2000; Lam & Kirby 2002; Rosete

& Ciarrochi 2005). This belief continues to hold despite the mixed empirical

evidence. However, it may be the case that the direction of the relationship is both

ability and context specific (Karim & Weisz 2010; Palmer et al. 2008). Alternative

explanations for inconsistent results regarding the EI and MLE relationship include

compensatory effects of IQ or other personality constructs. As was discussed in

Chapter 2, Cote and Miners (2006, p. 2) suggested ‘emotional intelligence should

predict job performance only some of the time, depending on the other ability that

individuals possess.’ This is explored further in sections 6.3.3 and 6.3.4. However,

the patterns of relationships that emerged in the results are described first.

Perceive Emotion and Understand Emotion were significantly related to the

dependent variable for all of the subgroups. Further, Perceive Emotion was the

strongest absolute predictor in five of the six subgroups. At a theoretical level, the

Perceive Emotion results are consistent with the foundational nature of this EI

ability (Joseph & Newman 2010; Mayer, Roberts & Barsade 2008) and replicate

results obtained in other studies (e.g. Rosete and Ciarrochi, 2005). At an

operational level, this may result from a higher internal validity for this subscale that

was found in both the present study and within the broader range of studies that

have used the MSCEITv2 measure (Maul 2011; Karim & Weisz 2010; Mayer et al.

2003; Palmer et al. 2005). For two of the groups, the ability to Perceive Emotion

was negatively related to people’s perceptions of effectiveness while, for the

Page 217: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

215

remaining groups the relationship was positive. When Perceive Emotion was a

positive predictor, the implication was managerial-leaders were perceived as more

effective when they had a greater ability to read the emotional landscape.

Conversely, when Perceive Emotion was a negative predictor, it suggested

managerial leaders were perceived as less effective when they had a greater ability

to read the emotional landscape. Job requirements may expose them to emotional

information that is best disregarded in order to be effective. This is one of many

possible explanations and further research is required to gain a better

understanding of this issue.

Understand Emotion was the strongest predictor for one subgroup and the second

strongest predictor for three of the remaining five subgroups. When Understand

Emotion was a positive predictor, the implication was managerial-leaders were

perceived as more effective when they had a great knowledge of the stimuli for

different emotional reactions, the relationship between emotions and how they can

combine. Conversely, when Understand Emotion was a negative predictor, it may

be the case that this ability is not effective, the ability is not effectively used and/or

the skill is over-used by the manager in their work context.

The ability to Manage Emotion was significant for five subgroups, positive for two

and negative for three. As already mentioned, the polarity of Manage Emotion

consistently mirrored that of Understand Emotion. This is evidence of the

theoretical assumption that the two abilities work in tandem as the two aspects of

Strategic EI (Mayer & Salovey 1997). Manage Emotion refers to a person’s ability

to regulate their own emotions, and the emotions of others, towards growth and

understanding. When the relationship with MLE is negative, the suggestion is that

an ability to Manage Emotion detracted from MLE. Again, it may be the case that

Page 218: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

216

the ability to Manage Emotion is not effective, the ability is not effectively used

and/or the skill is over-used by the manager in their work context.

It is interesting that the direction of the coefficients for these two abilities was

consistently opposite to that of Perceive Emotion. It may be the case that an

increased ability to Perceive Emotion may lead to managerial-leaders being

overwhelmed with emotional information and, thus, being less effective at

Understanding Emotion or Managing Emotion. For most of the groups the

coefficients were negative. Again, a negative relationship suggested these two

abilities detracted from the managerial-leader being perceived as effective within

their work environment.

In summary, each of the independent variables had a range of significant

relationships when predicting perceptions of Management Effectiveness and

Leadership Effectiveness. The results of the LC Regression Analyses at a

subgroup level revealed partial support for H2a, H2b, and H2c. The IQ, EI and MMS

constructs were incrementally significant in predicting MLE at the subgroup level.

However, the strength and direction of these relationships varied. Consequently,

the unique patterns of the relationships within the different groups identified were

explored to obtain further insight.

6.3.3 The Three Leadership Effectiveness Groups

The results of the LC Regression Analysis suggested the existence of three

statistically different groups of managerial-leaders that differed in terms of the ways

EI, MMS and IQ acted as antecedents to perceptions of MLE. Based on the

pattern of relationships with the independent variables, these groups were renamed

Page 219: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

217

Manage the Emotional Landscape Leaders, Read the Emotional Landscape

Leaders, and Intelligent Ego Leaders.

6.3.3.1 Manage the Emotional Landscape Leaders

The perceived effectiveness of the Manage the Emotional Landscape Leaders was

a function of all the independent predictor variables. However, for this group of

managerial-leaders the strongest positive predictor of perceptions of MLE was their

ability to Manage Emotion and so the label Manage the Emotional Landscape

Leaders was used. The next strongest relationship for Manage the Emotional

Landscape Leaders was Perceive Emotion, which was a negative predictor. For

this subgroup, IQ and the ability to Understand Emotion were weak to moderate

positive predictors of MLE. In contrast, increases in MMS detracted from being

seen as effective in their environment.

Manage the Emotional Landscape Leaders were rated as more effective when they

engaged their ability to regulate emotions, their own emotions and the emotions of

others, to facilitate personal understanding and growth. However, their ability to

accurately observe the emotional content in this landscape detracted from their

effectiveness. This was not due to mean differences in ability between the groups

for any of the independent variables. Something else created a negative

relationship between their ability to Perceive Emotion and how effective they are

perceived to be. One possible explanation could be the context for Manage the

Emotional Landscape Leaders. It may be rife with emotional information that is

considered superfluous, requiring them to disregard such information in order to be

effective. Regardless, based on the results of this research, the Manage the

Page 220: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

218

Emotional Landscape Leaders subgroup would be best advised to downplay their

ability to Perceive Emotion and exercise their ability to Manage Emotion.

Manage the Emotional Landscape Leaders would benefit from understanding how

their worldview (MMS) could better leverage perceptions of their MLE. For some

reason, the MMS level of Manage the Emotional Landscape Leaders was

negatively related to ratings of MLE. We do not have enough information to identify

the nature of the mismatch between the work context and the managerial-leader’s

worldview. However, given the negative relationship, we could infer the managerial-

leader’s worldview is not leveraging their effectiveness. It may be the worldview is

either more, or less, complex than what is required in their work context. This

could be either a positive or negative experience for them.

A more complex worldview would facilitate greater awareness and complexity in

thinking, which would include an increased capacity to Perceive Emotion and other

subtle dynamics (Young 2002b). In contexts where this is valued, an individual

managerial-leader would flourish. However, in contexts where this is not supported,

where the managerial-leader is not empowered or appreciated, or where the

preference is to view things simplistically, an increased capacity for complexity

could be seen as a negative attribute. Alternatively, the managerial-leader may

have a worldview that is less complex than their work context. This too could be

experienced as a challenge or a threat depending on the level of support for the

managerial-leader. Finally, the managerial-leader may be aligned with their work

context, but their ‘other-raters’ may not have a matching worldview and/or things

may be rapidly changing. Regardless of the root cause, the level of MMS is

detracting from the managerial-leader’s perceived MLE.

Page 221: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

219

6.3.3.2 Read the Emotional Landscape Leaders

For the Read the Emotional Landscape Leaders, perceptions of MLE were most

strongly, and positively, predicted by their ability to Perceive Emotion. This ability

involves ‘paying attention to and accurately decoding emotional signals in facial

expressions, tone of voice and artistic expressions’ (Mayer, Salovey & Caruso

2002, p. 19). An increasing ability to Manage Emotion was the next strongest, but

negative, predictor. These were opposite results than for the Manage Emotion

Leaders subgroup which was described in the previous section.

This group of managerial-leaders was seen as more effective when they had a

greater ability to pick up on the emotional cues around them, but less effective

when they had a greater ability to manage emotions. As was noted in Chapter 5,

this is not due to mean differences in EI abilities between the groups. Rather,

something in these managerial-leaders’ contexts contributed to the negative

relationship between their ability to Manage Emotion and perceptions of MLE

(Salovey & Grewal 2005).

6.3.3.3 Intelligent Ego Leaders

The strongest positive predictor of MLE for Intelligent Ego Leaders was IQ. In

addition, this was the only group of managerial-leaders for whom MMS positively

predicted perceptions of Leadership Effectiveness. The MLE of Intelligent Ego

Leaders was positively related to IQ, MMS and the ability to Perceive Emotion.

Increasing levels of IQ facilitate speed of cognitive processing. Increasing levels of

MMS facilitate a person’s ability to think with more complexity and increasing levels

of the ability to Perceive Emotion enable more information about the emotional

environment to be considered. It is also possible IQ, MMS and Perceive Emotion

Page 222: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

220

mutually reinforce each other (Cote & Miners 2006). This combination of

antecedents suggests the context in which Intelligent Ego Leaders operate requires

them to have a broader cognitive skill set to be perceived as effective.

As already discussed, we do not know whether Intelligent Ego Leaders’ personal

MMS are in sync or out of step with their context. Rooke and Torbert’s (1998)

research into CEOs and organisational transformation would suggest it is the

former. Regardless, the positive relationship between MMS and MLE suggests

Intelligent Ego Leaders are coping in their environment and can leverage

differences between their capacity and their environment. It may be the case that

the greater importance of IQ facilitates this process. The abilities to Understand

Emotion and Manage Emotion detracted from the perceived effectiveness of

Intelligent Ego Leaders, which also contrasts with the Manage Emotion Leaders.

Despite differences in the strength and direction of the relationships with the

predictor variables, the similarity in the mean MLE ratings between the statistically

distinct Intelligent Ego Leaders and Read the Emotional Landscape Leaders

groups supports the view that there are multiple paths towards excellence as a

leader (Quinn et al. 1996).

6.3.4 The Three Management Effectiveness Groups

The results of the LC Regression Analysis suggested the existence of three

statistically different groups of managerial-leaders that differed in terms of the way

EI, MMS and IQ act as antecedents of perceptions of MLE. Based on the pattern

of relationships with the independent variables, these groups were renamed

Strategise the Emotional Landscape Managers, Experience Emotion Managers

Page 223: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

221

and I See But I Don’t Understand Managers. Each of these groups is discussed in

turn.

6.3.4.1 Strategise the Emotional Environment Managers

The strongest positive predictor of MLE for Strategise the Emotional Environment

Managers was their ability to Understand Emotion. In addition, Manage Emotion

was a positive predictor. These two abilities form the Strategic Reasoning area of

the ability model of EI (Mayer, Salovey & Caruso 2002). Perceive Emotion and

MMS were negative predictors of MLE. The antecedent profile for Strategise the

Emotional Environment Managers was similar to that of the Manage Emotion

Leaders with a few key exceptions. For the Strategise the Emotional Environment

Managers IQ was not a significant predictor, Understand Emotion was a slightly

stronger predictor and Manage Emotion was a slightly weaker predictor. Based on

these results, Strategise the Emotional Environment Managers would do well to

use their ability to Understand Emotion and Manage Emotions while downplaying

their ability to Perceive Emotion. More abstractly, using ability-EI theory (Mayer,

Salovey & Caruso 2002), Strategise the Emotional Environment Managers should

emphasise their strategic reasoning with and minimise their experiential use of

emotion (more thinking less feeling).

6.3.4.2 Intelligent Perception Managers

For the Intelligent Perception Managers, all of the hypothesised antecedents were

significant predictors of perceptions of MLE. Perceive Emotion and IQ were the

only positive predictors of MLE with the ability to Perceive Emotion being the

strongest predictor overall. Exactly opposite of the previous Strategise the

Page 224: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

222

Emotional Landscape Managers subgroup, this subgroup should do less thinking

and use their IQ to do more feeling.

These managerial-leaders were rated as being more effective when they had a

greater ability to pick up on the emotional cues around them. Understand Emotion

and Manage Emotion, which again form the Strategic Reasoning area of EI (Mayer

et al., 2002), were both significant and negatively related to perceptions of

effectiveness, suggesting increasing levels of MMS detracted from the Intelligent

Perception Managers’ rated effectiveness.

Again, the similarity in the mean ratings of effectiveness between Strategise the

Emotional Environment Managers and Intelligent Perception Managers despite the

very different profiles of the importance of the predictor variables, supported the

view that there are multiple paths towards excellence as a manager.

6.3.4.3 I See It But I Don’t Understand It Managers

For the I See It But I Don’t Understand It Managers, increases in perceptions of

MLE were strongly predicted by increases in IQ and the ability to Perceive Emotion.

However, the ability to Understand Emotion detracted from perceptions of

Management Effectiveness. The pattern of antecedents for this group was similar

to that of the statistically different Intelligent Perception Managers group, with the

exception that for this group IQ was a much stronger predictor, Perceive Emotion

and Understand Emotion were slightly weaker and Manage Emotion was not

significant. Once again, MMS was a negative predictor of MLE. This group of

managerial-leaders was on average perceived as less effective than the other two

Page 225: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

223

groups, supporting the view that IQ is important but may be more of a threshold

ability. MLE also requires emotional and meaning-making abilities.

6.4 IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE

As is mentioned in the subsequent limitations section, this study was conducted

using a convenience sample drawn from a pool of managerial-leaders who had

participated in leadership development programs through the AIM–UWA Integral

Leadership Centre in Western Australia. As such, the results and implications of

this study are not generalisable beyond this population. However, results from this

study do have implications for practice, including providing:

1. Support for focussing on multiple abilities in leadership development,

including, in varying degrees, IQ, EI and MMS.

2. Evidence of the relevance and potential untapped resource of MMS for MLE.

3. Additional information about the reliability of the MSCEITv2, and supporting

evidence of the importance of ensuring fit for purpose when using the

MSCEITv2.

4. Additional information about the psychometric properties of the ILMDP for

HRM practitioners using this measure of MLE.

First and foremost, support for H1 and H2 at the subgroup level provides support for

the importance of individual differences beyond IQ, such as EI and MMS, in

understanding MLE. Further, the existence of subgroups, with different patterns

between these constructs and MLE, suggests the importance understanding the

Page 226: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

224

context within which individual managerial-leaders operate (Zaccaro 2007). This is

particularly relevant for leadership selection and development. We know that

without consideration of the match between skills and context requirements, some

managerial-leaders find it hard to succeed. In addition, the context in which

managerial-leaders operate may preclude an opportunity to apply new skills and

abilities that are important to leadership development. In contrast, recognising the

existence of different groups of managerial-leaders can help in the design of more

effective placement and leadership development programs.

A second implication worth further consideration by practitioners concerns the

relationship between MMS and perceptions of MLE. The findings in this study

suggest MMS varies in its relationship with MLE. It can be negatively related,

positively related or not significant at all. In this study, MMS was more commonly

negatively related to perceptions of MLE. The implication is that simplicity in

thinking and acting is valued. Whether or not this is productive is another question.

It may be the case that there is an untapped potential within managerial-leaders

MMS. However, further research is required to ascertain this. HRM practitioners

are uniquely placed to identify and diagnose situations in which managerial-leaders

require more support to leverage their MMS. It may be the case that ‘difficult’

employees represent a potential contribution in terms of their meaning-making

capacity.

Another implication from this study is that the results lend support to an ongoing

concern about the commercial use of the MSCEITv2. As was discussed in Chapter

1 and 2, the EI construct has enjoyed widespread popularity with individuals and

organisations in recent years, resulting in a demand for usable EI measures. The

MSCEITv2 is widely seen as the most scientifically valid measure available. HR

Page 227: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

225

practitioners have chosen to use the MSCEITv2, believing it is the gold standard in

measuring EI. However, this study contributes to a growing body of research that

suggests the limitations of the MSCEITv2 may outweigh its benefits. First, the

MSCEITv2 reliabilities appear to be lower than reported by the test’s authors.

Second, the inter-correlations of the four abilities measured by the MSCEITv2 vary

and contribute differently to perceived effectiveness, depending on the specific

context of the target managerial-leader. There are situations in which the

MSCEITv2 will be the best fit for the leadership development purpose, including

when it is used as part of a battery of tests. However, given the cost of the

MSCEITv2 and mixed support for its reliability, it may be prudent for HR

practitioners to choose additional or alternative ways of measuring EI until some of

these problems are overcome.

Finally, this study contributes to our understanding of the measurement properties

of the ILMDP. It would be possible to improve the measurement properties of the

ILMDP by revising the items and the scale overall. The revised ME and LE scales

identified in this study provide a good start for the latter. However, practitioners

might argue for a full array items and feel the ILMDP as a circumplex model that

includes the multiple roles of managerial-leadership provides a useful map for

teaching behaviourally complex MLE.

6.5 IMPLICATIONS FOR THEORY AND FUTURE RESEARCH

This research also has implications for theory and future research. In terms of

theory, the study’s results have implications for:

1. Theories of MLE based on individual-differences.

Page 228: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

226

2. The ability-based theory of EI.

3. The application of constructive developmental theory to understanding MLE.

4. The use of the ILMDP.

This research contributes support to the important role that individual differences

play in predicting MLE. The study contributes to the suggestion that multivariate

antecedents and the context within which managerial-leaders operate are important

in building an appropriate MLE theory (Zaccaro 2007). Both EI and MMS were

antecedents to MLE, but their significance, relative strength and the direction of

their relationship to MLE differed for broad groups of managerial-leaders. Again,

the importance of context to the relationship between IQ, EI and MMS with MLE is

stressed (Karim & Weisz 2010; Parker et al. 2005).

This research also has implications for EI theory. Specifically, the results from this

research support the increasing call for a better understanding of ability-based EI.

There appears to be an uncritical acceptance of a positive relationship between

overall EI and MLE. However, it is clear EI abilities ‘cannot exist outside of the

social context in which they operate’ (Salovey & Grewal 2005, pg 282). Initial

results from this research replicated some other studies, as none of the MSCEIT

scores were significantly related to perceptions of MLE (Weinberger 2009; Byron

2007). Further, it is important to examine EI at a branch level given the individual

and changing contributions each branch makes in different managerial-leader

contexts. In addition to theoretical considerations, there are a number of

implications for future EI research. First, future researchers interested in exploring

relationships between EI and other constructs should be vigilant in choosing their

Page 229: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

227

measure of EI and heed concerns about the MSCEITv2 (Antonakis, Ashkanasy &

Dasborough 2009; Fiori & Antonakis 2011; Maul 2011). This is a critical issue in

the field as noted by Fiori and Antonakis (2011): ‘a practical problem that

researcher willing to study EI as an ability are confronted with is that there are

basically no alternative to the MSCEIT’ (pg 333). The MSCEITv2 has contributed

significantly to our ability to explore and understand the construct of EI. However, if

future research is to advance the field, a measure with better measurement

properties is required. Future research should:

• Use alternate measures of ability-based EI.

• Rely on the Perceive Emotion branch of the MSCEITv2 as a measure of

ability-based EI, given the higher reliabilities for this scale.

• Explore alternate scoring methodologies (i.e. optimal scoring) for the existing

MSCEITv2 (Keele & Bell 2009).

• Design new ability-based measures of EI taking advantage of the knowledge

we have gained from the use of the MSCEITv2 over the past eight years.

This research has implications for furthering the application of constructive

developmental theory to MLE. Differential results for the relationship between

MMS and MLE, despite no mean differences on the independent variables between

the groups, challenges the assumption argued in the literature that higher levels of

MMS will translate into MLE. Instead, it seems this relationship is more complex

and depends on other aspects, such as other individual differences and the

requisite skills for particular work contexts. This result is more in line with the

Page 230: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

228

theoretical argument that each level of MMS has contributions and limitations

(Drath 1990). As such, it is important for theory to explore both the context in

which level of MMS operates most effectively and the support structures (i.e.

coaching, job-design, leadership-development) that leverage any differences

between MMS and the required context.

Further qualitative exploration of the impact MMS level has on MLE, particularly

with managerial-leaders at the higher stages of MMS, would improve our

understanding of the contextual factors that contribute to this becoming a negative

relationship. Why and when does MMS detract from perceptions of MLE? Is it that

complex thinking is not valued, or does complex thinking require mediation with

other abilities or skills? Alternatively, what is the relationship between EI and

MMS? What does EI look like at different levels of MMS? Unfortunately, the

WUSCT is such a labour intensive instrument that it is difficult to use with large

samples.

Finally, the examination of the measurement properties of the ILMDP suggested

many of the roles did not have discriminant validity. Consequently, researchers

interested in exploring MLE from a behavioural complexity perspective need to be

aware that the ILMDP may not have acceptable measurement properties in its

current form. Further research is needed to create an underlying structure that

reflects the suggested circumplex, as well as refinement of some of the items that

may be ambiguous.

Page 231: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

229

6.6 LIMITATIONS TO THE PRESENT STUDY

The present study had a number of limitations. The scale that was used to

measure managerial-leadership effectiveness was both a contribution and a

limitation. The decision to use the Integral Leadership and Management

Development Profile (ILMDP) as a measure of managerial-leadership effectiveness

was made because of the availability of a sample of managerial-leaders who had

used the ILMDP. However, the ILMDP’s measurement properties have received

limited academic attention, which represented both an opportunity and a risk. The

specific limitations of the ILMDP include the double-barrelled nature of some of the

items and the use of an uneven 10-point bi-polar scale. The opportunity was to

contribute to the discussion of EI and managerial-leadership beyond the Multi-

Factor Leadership Questionnaire (Avolio, Bass & Jung 1995). The limited use of

alternate MLE measures is recognised as a limitation in the EI research (Walter,

Cole & Humphrey 2011). Further, the use of other-average scores from multiple-

source feedback alleviated issues of common method variance.

The use of a convenience sample was also a limitation. While the sample included

a variety of managerial-leaders at different levels and from different organisations,

participants were approached after completing the multiple-source feedback and so

there may be aspects of self-selection at play.

The sample size at the whole sample level was more than adequate to test the

hypotheses. However, when the analysis shifted to the subgroup level, the

subgroup sizes ranged from 35 to 80, which was a limiting factor.

Another potential limitation was the length of the data collection process itself, as

this may have contributed to cognitive fatigue (Rode et al. 2008). Most participants

Page 232: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

230

completed all of the measures in one setting that lasted approximately two hours.

Breaks were built into the process to mitigate this concern, but it may have led to a

lack of motivation in some cases.

The lack of a personality measure, such as the Big Five, could also be perceived

as a limitation to the current study. Because the primary interest was in MMS and

EI and the length of time required to complete these measures, it was decided not

to include additional constructs. However, the inclusion of a personality measure

may have provided additional information.

The measure used to operationalise emotional intelligence (the Mayer-Salovey-

Caruso-Emotional-Intelligence-Test version 2 (MSCEITv2)) (Mayer, Salovey &

Caruso 2002) was also a limitation and may have impacted on the study’s results.

The MSCEITv2 is not without controversy (as was discussed in detail in Chapter 2).

At the time the data were collected, the MSCEITv2 was seen as the best measure

of ability EI available. It is still widely used in research and practice, although this

research contributes to the growing debate about the measure. However, the

reliabilities of the MSCEITv2 subscales in the present study were not as strong as

had been hoped. The difference in the administration methods of the MSCEITv2

was noted (i.e. online versus paper-and-pencil), but this was not considered to be a

limitation as the two methods have been shown to be virtually identical (Mayer et

al. 2003).

A limitation for much of the research on meaning-making structure is that samples

often have a restricted range of development orders (McCauley et al. 2006). In

order to manage this issue a sample size of more than 150 was targeted.

However, the range of MMS levels was still restricted and may have contributed to

Page 233: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

231

the present results. Finally, the cross-sectional design of this study is a limitation

(Locke 2005), as it limited the interpretation of causality in the identified

relationships.

6.7 CONCLUSIONS

The growing importance of emotions and meaning-making within organisational life

has meant EI and MMS are seen as increasingly relevant to understanding

managerial-leadership effectiveness. The present study provides some insights

into the relationship between EI, MMS, IQ and followers’ perceptions of MLE. The

present results suggest the relationships between these constructs are not

universal phenomena. Each construct is relevant. However, a managerial-leaders’

work context impacts on the relative importance these antecedents have in

predicting people’s perceptions of MLE. This is also true for the abilities of EI

(Perceive Emotion, Understand Emotion and Manage Emotion). The existence of

groups of managerial-leaders with similarities in how EI, MMS and IQ contribute to

MLE is consistent with the reality underpinning the theory of behaviourally complex

MLE. There is great importance in recognising the uniqueness of an individual

managerial-leader’s situation and the existence of similar groups of managerial-

leaders. Finally, the results of the present research support concerns about the

MSCEITv2’s effectiveness in measuring EI.

Both the scientific and the practitioner communities continue to grapple with the EI

construct, its relationship to meaning-making and, ultimately, its impact on MLE. It

is hoped the present study has contributed to the empirical evidence of this

important issue and suggested the need for more research into heterogeneity as

this seems to be crucial to our understanding of these relationships.

Page 234: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

232

REFERENCES REFERENCES

ABS,  Labour  Force  and  Demography  Australian  Bureau  of  Statistics.  Available  from:  <http://www.abs.gov.au/%3E.  [11  July  2007].  

Adams,  GR  &  Fitch,  SA  1982,  'Ego  stage  and  identity  status  development:  a  cross-­‐sequential  analysis',  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  vol.  43,  no.  3,  pp.  574-­‐583.  

Antonakis,  J  2003,  'Why"  Emotional  Intelligence"  Does  Not  Predict  Leadership  Effectiveness:  A  Comment  on  Prati,  Douglas,  Ferris,  Ammeter,  and  Buckley  (2003)',  International  Journal  of  Organizational  Analysis,  vol.  11,  no.  4,  pp.  355-­‐362.  

Antonakis,  J  2004,  'On  why  "emotional  intelligence"  will  not  predict  leadership  effectiveness  beyond  IQ  or  the  "big  five":  an  extension  and  rejoinder',  Organizational  Analysis,  vol.  12,  no.  2,  pp.  171-­‐182.  

Antonakis,  J,  Ashkanasy,  NM  &  Dasborough,  MT  2009,  'Does  leadership  need  emotional  intelligence?',  The  Leadership  Quarterly,  vol.  20,  no.  2,  pp.  247-­‐261.  

Argyris,  C  &  Schön,  DA  1974,  Theory  in  practice:  increasing  professional  effectiveness,  Jossey-­‐Bass  San  Francisco.  

Ashkanasy,  NM  &  Dasborough,  MT  2003,  'Emotional  awareness  and  emotional  intelligence  in  leadership  teaching',  Journal  of  Education  for  Business,  vol.  79,  no.  1,  pp.  18-­‐22.  

Ashkanasy,  NM  &  Daus,  CS  2005,  'Rumors  of  the  death  of  emotional  intelligence  in  organizational  behavior  are  vastly  exaggerated',  Journal  of  Organizational  Behavior,  vol.  26,  no.  4,  pp.  441-­‐452.  

Ashkanasy,  NM  &  Tse,  B  2000,  'Transformational  leadership  as  management  of  emotion:  a  conceptual  review',  Emotions  in  the  workplace:  Research,  theory,  and  practice,  pp.  221–235.  

Ashton-­‐James,  C  2003,  Is  emotional  intelligence  a  viable  construct?,  MS    thesis,  University  of  Queensland  Business  School.  

Atwater,  LE  &  Yammarino,  FJ  1992,  'Does  self-­‐other  agreement  on  leadership  perceptions  moderate  the  validity  of  leadership  and  performance  predictions?',  Personnel  Pyschology,  vol.  45,  no.  1,  pp.  141-­‐164.  

Atwater,  LE  &  Yammarino,  FJ  1997,  'Self-­‐other  rating  agreement:  a  review  and  a  model',  Research  in  Personnel  and  Human  Resources  Management,  vol.  15,  pp.  121-­‐174.  

Avolio,  B,  Bass,  BM  &  Jung,  DI  1995,  MLQ:  Multifactor  leadership  questionnaire:  technical  report,  Mind  Garden  Palo  Alto,  CA.  

Bagozzi,  R,  Yi,  Y  &  Phillips,  L  1991,  'Assessing  construct  validity  in  organizational  research',  Administrative  Science  Quarterly,  vol.  36,  no.  3.  

Bar-­‐On,  R  1997,  The  Bar-­‐On  emotional  quotient  inventory  (EQ-­‐i):  a  test  of  emotional  intelligence,  Multi-­‐Health  Systems,  Toronto.  

Page 235: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

233

Barbuto  Jr,  JE  &  Burbach,  ME  2006,  'The  emotional  intelligence  of  transformational  leaders:  a  field  study  of  elected  officials',  The  Journal  of  Social  Psychology,  vol.  146,  no.  1,  pp.  51-­‐64.  

Barling,  J,  Slater,  F  &  Kelloway,  E  2000,  'Transformational  leadership  and  emotional  intelligence:  an  exploratory  study',  Leadership  Organisation  Development  Journal,  vol.  21,  no.  3,  pp.  157-­‐161.  

Barsade,  SG  2002,  'The  ripple  effect:  emotional  contagion  and  its  influence  on  group  behavior',  Administrative  Science  Quarterly,  vol.  47,  pp.  644-­‐675.  

Barsalou,  LW  2008,  'Cognitive  and  neural  contributions  to  understanding  the  conceptual  system',  Current  Directions  in  Psychological  Science,  vol.  17,  no.  2,  p.  91.  

Bass,  B  1998,  Transformational  leadership:  industrial,  military  and  educational  impact,  Lawrence  Earlbaum  Associates,  Inc.,  Mahwah,  NJ.  

Bass,  BM  1990,  Bass  and  Stogdill's  handbook  of  leadership:  theory,  research  and  managerial  applications  3edn,  The  Free  Press,  New  York.  

Bass,  BM  &  Steidlmeier,  P  1999,  'Ethics,  character,  and  authentic  transformational  leadership  behaviour',  Leadership  Quarterly,  vol.  10,  no.  2,  pp.  181-­‐208.  

Bastian,  VA,  Burns,  NR  &  Nettelbeck,  T  2005,  'Emotional  intelligence  predicts  life  skills,  but  not  as  well  as  personality  and  cognitive  abilities',  Personality  and  Individual  Differences,  vol.  39,  no.  6,  pp.  1135-­‐1145.  

Baumeister,  RF  1991,  Meanings  of  life,  The  Guilford  Press.  Bay,  D  &  McKeage,  K  2006,  'Emotional  Intelligence  in  undergraduate  accounting  

students:  preliminary  assessment',  Accounting  Education,  vol.  15,  no.  4,  pp.  439-­‐454.  

Bennis,  W  2003,  On  becoming  a  leader:  the  leadership  classic,  Perseus  Publishing,  Cambridge.  

Bertua,  C,  Anderson,  N  &  Salgado,  J  2005,  'The  predictive  validity  of  cognitive  ability  tests:  a  UK  meta-­‐analysis',  Journal  of  Occupational  and  Organizational  Psychology,  vol.  78,  no.  3,  pp.  387-­‐409.  

Binet,  A  &  Simon,  T  1905,  'New  methods  for  the  diagnosis  of  the  intellectual  level  of  subnormals',  L’annee  Psychologique,  vol.  12,  pp.  191–244.  

Blasi,  A  1971,  A  developmental  approach  to  responsibility  training,  PhD    thesis,  Washingon  University.  

Blasi,  A  1998,  'Loevinger's  theory  of  ego  development  and  Its  relationship  to  the  cognitive  -­‐  developmental  approach',  in  Theoretical,  empirical  and  clinical  investigations  of  Loevinger's  conception  of  ego  development,  eds  PM  Westenberg,  A  Blasi  &  LD  Cohn,  Lawrence  Erlbaum  Associates,  Mahwah,  New  Jersey,  pp.  13-­‐26.  

Bliese,  P  2000,  'Within-­‐group  agreement,  non-­‐independence,  and  reliability:  implications  for  data  aggregation  and  analysis',  in  Multilevel  Theory,  Research,  and  Methods  in  Organizations,  eds  K  Klein  &  S  Kozlowski,  Jossey-­‐  Bass,  San  Francisco,  pp.  349-­‐381.  

Page 236: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

234

Block,  J  1982,  'Assimilation,  accommodation  and  the  dynamics  of  personality  development',  Child  Development,  pp.  281-­‐295.  

Boal,  KB  &  Hooijberg,  R  2000,  'Strategic  leadership  research:  moving  on',  The  Leadership  Quarterly,  vol.  11,  no.  4,  pp.  515-­‐549.  

Brackett,  MA  &  Mayer,  JD  2003,  'Convergent,  discriminant  and  incremental  validity  of  competing  measures  of  emotional  itnelligence',  Personality  and  Social  Psychology  Bulletin,  vol.  29,  pp.  1147-­‐1158.  

Brackett,  MA,  Mayer,  JD  &  Warner,  RM  2004,  'Emotional  intelligence  and  its  relation  to  everyday  behaviour',  Personality  and  Individual  Differences,  vol.  36,  no.  6,  pp.  1387-­‐1402.  

Brackett,  MA,  Rivers,  SE,  Shiffman,  S,  Lerner,  N  &  Salovey,  P  2006,  'Relating  emotional  abilities  to  social  functioning:  a  comparison  of  self-­‐report  and  performance  measures  of  emotional  intelligence',  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  vol.  91,  no.  4,  pp.  780-­‐795.  

Brannick,  M,  Wahi,  M,  Arce,  M,  Johnson,  HA,  Nazian,  S  &  Goldin,  S  2009,  'Comparison  of  trait  and  ability  measures  of  emotional  intelligence  in  medical  students',  Medical  Education,  vol.  43,  no.  11,  pp.  1062-­‐1068.  

Broughton,  J  1978,  'The  cognitive-­‐developmental  approach  to  morality:  A  reply  to  Kurtines  and  Greif',  Journal  of  Moral  Education,  vol.  7,  no.  2,  pp.  81-­‐96.  

Browne,  M  &  Cudeck,  R  1993,  'Alternative  ways  of  assessing  model  fit',  Testing  structural  equation  models,  vol.  154,  pp.  136–162.  

Browning,  DL  1987,  'Ego  development,  authoritarianism,  and  social  status:  an  investigation  of  the  incremental  validity  of  Loevinger’s  Sentence  Completion  Test  (short  form)',  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  vol.  53,  no.  1,  pp.  113–118.  

Buenger,  V,  Daft,  RL,  Conlon,  EJ  &  Austin,  J  1996,  'Competing  values  in  organizations:  contextual  influences  and  structural  consequences',  Organization  Science,  vol.  7,  no.  5,  pp.  557-­‐576.  

Bulmer,  MG  1979,  Principles  of  statistics,  Dover  Pubns.  Burke,  M  &  Dunlap,  W  2002,  'Estimating  interrater  agreement  with  the  average  

deviation  index:  A  user's  guide',  Organizational  Research  Methods,  vol.  5,  no.  2,  pp.  159-­‐172.  

Burke,  M,  Finkelstein,  L  &  Dusig,  M  1999,  'On  average  deviation  indices  for  estimating  interrater  agreement',  Organizational  Research  Methods,  vol.  2,  no.  1,  pp.  49-­‐68.  

Burns,  J  1978,  Leadership,  Harper  &  Row,  New  York.  Bushe,  GR  &  Gibbs,  BW  1990,  'Predicting  organization  development  consulting  

competence  from  the  Myers-­‐Briggs  Type  Indicator  and  stage  of  ego  development',  The  Journal  of  Applied  Behavioral  Science,  vol.  26,  no.  3,  pp.  337-­‐357.  

Busing,  FMTA  2006,  'Avoiding  degeneracy  in  metric  unfolding  by  penalizing  the  intercept',  British  Journal  of  Mathematical  &  Statistical  Psychology,  vol.  59,  no.  2,  pp.  419-­‐427.  Available  from:  pbh.  

Page 237: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

235

Byrne,  B  2010,  Structural  equation  modeling  with  AMOS:  basic  concepts,  applications,  and  programming,  2nd  edn,  Routledge,  Mahwah,  New  Jersey.  

Byron,  K  2007,  'Male  and  female  managers'  ability  to  'read'  emotions:  relationships  with  supervisor's  performance  ratings  and  subordinate's  satisfaction  ratings',  Journal  of  Occupational  and  Organizational  Psychology,  vol.  80,  pp.  713-­‐733.  

Cacioppe,  R  &  Albrecht,  S  2000,  'Using  360  degree  feedback  and  the  Integral  Model  to  develop  leadership  and  management  skills',  Leadership  &  Organization  Development  Journal,  vol.  21,  no.  8,  pp.  390-­‐404.  

Cacioppe,  R  &  Albrecht,  S  2001,  'Understanding  and  developing  leadership  and  management  using  the  Holon  model',  in  Leadership  and  the  Anitpodes:  findings,  implications  and  a  leader  profile,  ed.  KW  Parry,  Institute  of  Policy  Studies  [and]  Centre  for  the  Study  of  Leadership,  Wellington,  pp.  124-­‐142.  

Carlozzi,  A,  Gaa,  J  &  Liberman,  D  1983,  'Empathy  and  ego  development',  Journal  of  Counseling  Psychology,  vol.  30,  no.  1,  pp.  113-­‐116.  

Carlyle,  T,  Goldberg,  M,  Brattin,  J  &  Engel,  M  1840/1993,  On  heroes,  hero-­‐worship  &  the  heroic  in  history,  Univ  of  California  Press  on  Demand.  

Carroll,  J  1993,  Human  cognitive  abilities:  a  survey  of  factor  analytic  studies,  Cambridge  University  Press,  New  York.  

Cartwright,  S,  Pappas,  C  &  West,  BS  2008,  'Emotional  intelligence,  its  measurement  and  implications  for  the  workplace',  International  Journal  of  Management  Reviews,  vol.  10,  no.  2,  pp.  149-­‐171.  

Caruso,  D  &  Salovey,  P  2004,  The  emotionally  intelligent  manager,  Jossey-­‐Bass.  Caruso,  DR  &  Wolfe,  CJ  2004,  'Emotional  intelligence  and  leadership  development',  

Leader  development  for  transforming  organizations:  Growing  leaders  for  tomorrow,  pp.  237-­‐263.  

Castro,  CB,  Martín  Armario,  E  &  Martín  Ruiz,  D  2007,  'The  influence  of  market  heterogeneity  on  the  relationship  between  a  destination's  image  and  tourists’  future  behaviour',  Tourism  Management,  vol.  28,  no.  1,  pp.  175-­‐187.  

Cattell,  R  1963,  'Theory  of  fluid  and  crystallized  intelligence:  a  critical  experiment',  Journal  of  Educational  Psychology,  vol.  54,  no.  1,  pp.  1-­‐22.  

Cattell,  R  1987,  Intelligence:  its  structure,  growth,  and  action,  Elsevier  Science  Ltd.  Ceci,  S  &  Williams,  W  2007,  'Little  g:  prospects  and  constraints  in  discussion  on  'the  g-­‐

factor  of  international  cognitive  ability  comparison:  the  homogeneity  of  results  in  PISA,  TIMSS,  PIRLS  and  IQ-­‐Tests  across  nations',  European  Journal  of  Personality,  vol.  21,  pp.  716-­‐718.  

Ciarrochi,  J,  Chan,  AY  &  Caputi,  P  2000,  'A  critical  evaluation  of  the  emotional  intelligence  construct',  Personality  and  Individual  Differences,  vol.  28,  no.  3,  pp.  539-­‐561.  

Ciarrochi,  JV,  Chan,  AY  &  Caputi,  P  2000,  'A  critical  evaluation  of  the  emotional  intelligence  construct:  Erratum',  Personality  and  Individual  Differences,  vol.  29,  no.  5,  p.  1001.  

Page 238: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

236

Clarke,  N  2010,  'Emotional  intelligence  and  its  relationship  to  transformational  leadership  and  key  project  manager  competencies',  Project  Management  Journal,  vol.  41,  no.  2,  pp.  5-­‐20.  

Clarke-­‐Murphy,  M  &  Soutar,  G  2005,  'Individual  investor  preferences:  a  segmentation  analysis',  The  Journal  of  Behavioural  Finance,  vol.  6,  no.  1,  pp.  6-­‐14.  

Cohn,  LD  &  Westenberg,  PM  2004,  'Intelligence  and  maturity:  Meta-­‐analytic  evidence  for  the  incremental  and  discriminant  validity  of  Loevinger's  measure  of  ego  development',  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  vol.  86,  no.  5,  pp.  760-­‐772.  

Collins,  VL  2001,  Emotional  intelligence  and  leadership  success,  PhD    thesis,  The  University  of  Nebraska  -­‐  Lincoln.  

Coltman,  T,  Devinney,  T  &  Midgley,  D  2003,  'Strategic  Drivers  and  Organizational  Impediments  to  E-­‐Business  Performance:  A  Latent  Class  Assessment'.  

Conger,  J  &  Kanungo,  R  1988,  Charismatic  Leadership:  the  elusive  factor  in  organizational  effectiveness,  Jossey-­‐Bass,  San  Francisco.  

Conger,  JA  1990,  'The  dark  side  of  leadership',  Organizational  Dynamics,  vol.  19,  no.  2,  pp.  44-­‐55.  

Conger,  JA  1994,  Spirit  at  work:  Discovering  the  spirituality  in  leadership.  Conte,  JM  2005,  'A  review  and  critique  of  emotional  intelligence  measures',  Journal  of  

Organizational  Behavior,  vol.  26,  no.  4,  pp.  433-­‐440.  Cook-­‐Greuter,  S  2005,  Ego  development:  nine  levels  of  increasing  embrace,  Il  

Psychology  Center  Wayland,  MA,  pp.  1-­‐36.  Cook-­‐Greuter,  SR  1990,  'Maps  for  living:  ego-­‐development  stages  from  symbiosis  to  

conscious  universal  embeddedness',  Adult  Development,  vol.  2,  pp.  79-­‐104.  Cook-­‐Greuter,  SR  1994,  'Rare  forms  of  self-­‐understanding  in  mature  adults',  in  

Transcendence  and  mature  thought  in  adulthood:  The  further  reaches  of  adult  development,  eds  ME  Miller  &  SR  Cook-­‐Greuter,  Rowman  &  Littlefield  Pub  Inc,  New  York  pp.  119-­‐147.  

Cook-­‐Greuter,  SR  2004,  'Making  the  case  for  a  developmental  perspective',  Industrial  and  Commercial  Training,  vol.  36,  no.  6,  pp.  275-­‐281.  

Cooper,  RK  &  Sawaf,  A  1997,  Executive  EQ:  emotional  intelligence  in  leadership  and  organizations,  Grosset/Putnam,  New  York.  

Cote,  S,  Lopez,  PN,  Salovey,  P  &  Miners,  CTH  2010,  'Emotional  intelligence  and  leadership  emergence  in  small  groups',  Leadership  Quarterly,  vol.  21,  pp.  496-­‐508.  

Cote,  S  &  Miners,  CTH  2006,  'Emotional  intelligence,  cognitive  intelligence  and  job  performance',  Administrative  Science  Quarterly  vol.  51,  pp.  1-­‐28.  

Cramer,  P  1999,  'Ego  functions  and  ego  development:  defense  mechanisms  and  intelligence  as  predictors  of  ego  level',  Journal  of  Personality,  vol.  67,  no.  5,  pp.  735-­‐760.  

Cronbach,  LJ  1960,  Essentials  of  psychological  testing,  2nd  edn,  Harper  &  Row,  New  York.  

Page 239: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

237

Crosby,  BC  &  Kiedrowski,  J  2008,  'Integrative  leadership:  observations  from  a  University  of  Minnesota  seminar  series',  Integral  Leadership  Review,  vol.  8,  no.  3,  pp.  1-­‐13.  

Daft,  R  &  Pirola-­‐Merlo,  A  2009,  The  leadership  experience,  1st  Asia  Pacific  edn,  Cengage  Learning  Australia,  Melbourne.  

Dasborough,  MT  &  Ashkanasy,  NM  2002,  'Emotion  and  attribution  of  intentionality  in  leader–member  relationships',  The  Leadership  Quarterly,  vol.  13,  no.  5,  pp.  615-­‐634.  

Daus,  CS  &  Ashkanasy,  NM  2005,  'The  case  for  the  ability  based  model  of  emotional  intelligence',  Journal  of  Organizational  Behavior  vol.  26,  no.  4,  pp.  453-­‐466.  

Daus,  CS  &  Harris,  A  2003,  'Emotional  intelligence  and  transformational  leadership  in  groups.  Paper/presentation  of  symposium',  in  Multilevel  Perspectives  on  Emotions  in  Organizations  at  the  18th  Annual  Meeting  of  the  Society  for  Industrial  and  Organizational  Psychologists,  Orlando,  FL.  

David,  S  2005,  Emotional  intelligence:  conceptual  and  methodological  issues,  and  its  role  in  coping  and  well-­‐being,  Doctoral    thesis,  University  of  Melbourne.  

Davies,  M,  Stankov,  L  &  Roberts,  RD  1998,  'Emotional  intelligence:  In  search  of  an  elusive  construct',  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  vol.  75,  pp.  989-­‐1015.  

Dawda,  D  &  Hart,  SD  2000,  'Assessing  emotional  intelligence:  reliability  and  validity  of  the  Bar-­‐On  Emotional  Quotient  Inventory  (EQ-­‐i)  in  university  students',  Personality  and  Individual  Differences,  vol.  28,  no.  4,  pp.  797-­‐812.  

Day,  AL  &  Carroll,  SA  2004,  'Using  an  ability-­‐based  measure  of  emotional  intelligence  to  predict  individual  performance,  group  performance,  and  group  citizenship  behaviours',  Personality  and  Individual  Differences,  vol.  36,  no.  6,  pp.  1443-­‐1458.  

Day,  AL  &  Carroll,  SA  2008,  'Faking  emotional  intelligence  (EI):  comparing  response  distortion  on  ability  and  trait-­‐based  EI  measures',  Journal  of  Organizational  Behavior,  vol.  29,  no.  6,  pp.  761-­‐784.  

Day,  DV  &  Zaccaro,  SJ  2007  'Leadership:  a  critical  historical  analysis  of  the  influence  of  leader  traits',  in  Historical  Perspectives  in  Industrial  and  Organizational  Psychology,  ed.  L  Koppes,  Mahwah,  NJ:  Erlbaum,  pp.  383–405.  

Debats,  DL  1999,  'Sources  of  meaning:  An  investigation  of  significant  commitments  in  life',  Journal  of  Humanistic  Psychology,  vol.  39,  no.  4,  pp.  30-­‐57.  

Denison,  DR,  Hoojiberg,  R  &  Quinn,  RE  1995,  'Paradox  and  performance:  toward  a  theory  of  behavioral  complexity  in  managerial  leadership  ',  Organization  Science,  vol.  6,  no.  5,  pp.  524-­‐540.  

Dodrill,  C  1983,  'Long-­‐term  reliability  of  the  Wonderlic  Personnel  Test',  Journal  of  Consulting  and  Clinical  Psychology,  vol.  51,  no.  2,  pp.  316-­‐317.  

Dodrill,  C  &  Warner,  M  1988,  'Further  studies  of  the  Wonderlic  Personnel  Test  as  a  brief  measure  of  intelligence',  Journal  of  Consulting  and  Clinical  Psychology,  vol.  56,  no.  1,  pp.  145-­‐147.  

Drath,  WH  1990,  'Managerial  strengths  and  weaknesses  as  functions  of  the  development  of  personal  meaning',  The  Journal  of  Applied  Behavioral  Science,  vol.  26,  no.  4,  pp.  483-­‐499.  

Page 240: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

238

Drath,  WH  1994,  Making  common  sense.  Duchon,  D  &  Plowman,  DA  2005,  'Nurturing  the  spirit  at  work:  Impact  on  work  unit  

performance',  The  Leadership  Quarterly,  vol.  16,  no.  5,  pp.  807-­‐833.  Dulewicz,  V  &  Higgs,  M  1998,  'Emotional  intelligence:  can  it  be  measured  reliably  and  

validly  using  competency  data?',  Competency,  vol.  6,  no.  1,  pp.  28-­‐37.  Dulewicz,  V  &  Higgs,  M  1999,  'Can  emotional  intelligence  be  measured  and  developed?  

',  Leadership  &  Organization  Development  Journal,  vol.  20,  no.  5,  pp.  242-­‐252.  Dulewicz,  V  &  Higgs,  M  2000,  'Emotional  intelligence:  a  review  and  evaluation  study',  

Journal  of  Managerial  Psychology,  vol.  15,  no.  4,  pp.  341-­‐373.  Dulewicz,  V,  Higgs,  M  &  Slaski,  M  2003,  'Measuring  emotional  intelligence:  content,  

construct  and  criterion-­‐related  validity  ',  Journal  of  Managerial  Psychology,  vol.  18,  no.  5,  pp.  405-­‐421.  

Ebersole,  P  1998,  'Types  and  depth  of  written  life  meanings',  The  human  quest  for  meaning:  A  handbook  of  psychological  research  and  clinical  applications,  pp.  179-­‐191.  

Eigel,  KM  &  Kuhnert,  KW  2005,  'Authentic  development:  leadership  development  level  and  executive  effectiveness',  Authentic  Leadership  Theory  and  Practice:  Origins,  Effects  and  Development,  vol.  3,  pp.  357-­‐385.  

Elfenbein,  HA,  Foo,  MD,  White,  J,  Tan,  HH  &  Aik,  VC  2007,  'Reading  your  counterpart:  the  benefit  of  emotion  recognition  accuracy  for  effectiveness  in  negotiation',  Journal  of  Nonverbal  Behavior,  vol.  31,  no.  4,  pp.  205-­‐223.  

Eysenck,  H  2000,  Intelligence:  a  new  look,  Transaction  Publishers,  London.  Fairholm,  M  2004,  'A  new  sciences  outline  for  leadership  development',  Leadership  &  

Organization  Development  Journal,  vol.  25,  no.  4,  pp.  369-­‐383.  Farrelly,  D  &  Austin,  EJ  2007,  'Ability  EI  as  an  intelligence?  Associations  of  the  MSCEIT  

with  performance  on  emotion  processing  and  social  tasks  and  with  cognitive  ability',  Cognition  &  Emotion,  vol.  21,  no.  5,  pp.  1043-­‐1063.  

Fiedler,  F  1967,  A  theory  of  leadership  effectiveness,  McGraw-­‐Hill  New  York,  New  York.  Fiedler,  F  &  Garcia,  J  1987,  New  approaches  to  effective  leadership:  cognitive  resources  

and  organizational  performance,  Wiley  New  York.  Fiori,  M  &  Antonakis,  J  2011,  'The  ability  model  of  emotional  intelligence:  searching  for  

valid  measures',  Personality  and  Individual  Differences,  vol.  50,  pp.  329-­‐334.  Fisher,  D  &  Torbert,  WR  1991,  'Transforming  managerial  practice:  Beyond  the  

achiever  stage',  Research  in  Organizational  Change  and  Development,  vol.  5,  pp.  143-­‐173.  

Flynn,  J  1984,  'The  mean  IQ  of  Americans:  massive  gains  1932  to  1978',  Psychological  Bulletin,  vol.  95,  no.  1,  pp.  29–51.  

Follesdal,  H  &  Hagtvet,  KA  2009,  'Emotional  intelligence:  the  MSCEIT  from  the  perspective  of  generalizability  theory',  Intelligence,  vol.  37,  no.  1,  pp.  94-­‐105.  

Forgas,  JP  2001,  Handbook  of  affect  and  social  cognition,  L.  Erlbaum  Associates  Mahwah,  NJ.  

Page 241: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

239

Fornell,  C  &  Larcker,  DF  1981a,  'Evaluating  structural  equation  models  with  unobservable  variables  and  measurement  error',  Journal  of  Marketing  Research,  pp.  39-­‐50.  

Fornell,  C  &  Larcker,  DF  1981b,  'Structural  equation  models  with  unobservable  variables  and  measurement  error:  Algebra  and  statistics',  Journal  of  Marketing  Research,  vol.  18,  no.  3,  pp.  382-­‐388.  

Frank,  S  &  Quinlan,  DM  1976,  'Ego  development  and  female  delinquency:  a  cognitive-­‐developmental  approach',  Journal  of  Abnormal  Psychology,  vol.  85,  no.  5,  pp.  505-­‐510.  

Gammons,  AW  1993,  Ego  development  and  teachers  in  peer  leadership  positions:  implications  for  effectiveness,  PhD    thesis,  Cleveland  State  University.  

Gardner,  H  1983,  Frames  of  mind:  the  theory  of  multiple  intelligences,  Basic  Books,  New  York.  

Gardner,  L  &  Stough,  C  2002,  'Examining  the  relationship  between  leadership  and  emotional  intelligence  in  senior  level  managers',  Leadership  Organization  Development  Journal,  vol.  23,  no.  1/2,  pp.  68-­‐79.  

Garver,  MS,  Williams,  Z  &  Taylor,  GS  2008,  'Employing  latent  class  regression  analysis  to  examine  logistics  theory:  an  application  of  truck  driver  retention',  Journal  of  Business  Logistics,  vol.  29,  no.  2,  pp.  233-­‐257.  

Gehrke,  SJ  &  Gehrke,  S  2008,  'Leadership  through  meaning-­‐making:  An  empirical  exploration  of  spirituality  and  leadership  in  college  students',  Journal  of  college  student  development,  vol.  49,  no.  4,  p.  351.  

George,  JM  1995,  'Leader  positive  mood  and  group  performance:  the  case  of  customer  service',  Journal  of  Applied  Social  Psychology,  vol.  25,  no.  9,  pp.  778-­‐794.  

George,  JM  2000,  'Emotions  and  leadership:  the  role  of  emotional  intelligence',  Human  Relations,  vol.  53,  no.  8,  pp.  1027-­‐1055.  

George,  JM  &  Bettenhausen,  K  1990,  'Understanding  prosocial  behavior,  sales  performance,  and  turnover:  A  group-­‐level  analysis  in  a  service  context',  Journal  of  Applied  Psychology,  vol.  75,  no.  6,  pp.  698-­‐709.  

Giesbrecht,  N  &  Walker,  LJ  2000,  'Ego  development  and  the  construction  of  a  moral  self',  Journal  of  College  Student  Development,  vol.  41,  no.  2,  pp.  157-­‐71.  

Gignac,  G  2005,  'Evaluating  the  MSCEITv2.0  via  CFA:  comment  on  Mayer  et  al  (2003)',  Emotion,  vol.  5,  pp.  233-­‐235.  

Gohm,  CL  2004,  'Moving  forward  with  emotional  intelligence',  Psychological  Inquiry,  vol.  15,  no.  3,  pp.  222-­‐227.  

Goleman,  D  1995,  Emotional  intelligence,  Bantam  Books,  New  York.  Goleman,  D  1998a,  'What  makes  a  leader?',  Harvard  Business  Review,  vol.  76,  no.  6,  pp.  

93-­‐104.  Goleman,  D  1998b,  Working  with  emotional  intelligence,  Bantam  Books,  New  York.  Gottfredson,  L  2005,  'What  if  the  hereditarian  hypothesis  is  true',  Psychology,  Public  

Policy,  and  Law,  vol.  11,  no.  2,  pp.  311-­‐319.  Gratch,  A  1987,  The  interrelationships  among  ego  development,  managerial  level  and  

decision-­‐making  style,  PhD    thesis,  Columbia  University    

Page 242: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

240

Greenleaf,  R  2007,  'The  servant  as  leader',  Corporate  Ethics  and  Corporate  Governance,  pp.  79-­‐85.  

Guilford,  JP  1967,  'Three  faces  of  intellect',  American  Psychologist,  vol.  14,  pp.  469-­‐479.  Haan,  N,  Stroud,  J  &  Holstein,  C  1973,  'Moral  and  ego  stages  in  relationship  to  ego  

processes:  A  study  of  'hippies'',  Journal  of  Personality,  vol.  41,  no.  4,  pp.  596-­‐612.  

Hair  Jr,  J,  Anderson,  R,  Tatham,  R  &  Black,  W  1995,  Multivariate  data  analysis:  with  readings,  Prentice-­‐Hall,  Inc.  Upper  Saddle  River,  NJ,  USA.  

Handy,  C  1994,  The  age  of  paradox,  Harvard  Business  Press,  Boston.  Hansell,  S,  Sparacino,  J,  Ronchi,  D  &  Strodtbeck,  FL  1984,  'Ego  development  responses  

in  written  questionnaires  and  telephone  interviews',  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  vol.  47,  no.  5,  pp.  1118-­‐1128.  

Harris,  LS  2005,  An  examination  of  executive  leadership  effectivness  using  constructive  developmental  theory,  Masters    thesis,  University  of  Georgia,  Athens.  

Harris,  LS  &  Kuhnert,  KW  2008,  'Looking  through  the  lens  of  leadership:  a  constructive  developmental  approach',  Leadership  &  Organization  Development  Journal,  vol.  29,  no.  1,  pp.  47-­‐67.  

Hart,  SL,  Hart,  SL  &  Quinn,  RE  1993,  'Roles  executives  play:  CEOs,  behavioral  complexity,  and  firm  performance',  Human  Relations,  vol.  46,  no.  5,  pp.  543-­‐575.  

Hauser,  ST  1976,  'Loevinger's  model  and  measure  of  ego  development:  A  critical  review',  Psychological  Bulletin,  vol.  83,  no.  5,  pp.  928-­‐955.  

Hauser,  ST  1993,  'Loevinger's  model  and  measure  of  ego  development:  a  critical  review,  II',  Psychological  Inquiry,  vol.  4,  no.  1,  pp.  23-­‐30.  

Hawkins,  KA,  Faraone,  SV,  Pepple,  JR,  Seidman,  LJ  &  Tsuang,  MT  1990,  'WAIS-­‐R  validation  of  the  Wonderlic  Personnel  Test  as  a  brief  intelligence  measure  in  a  psychiatric  sample',  Psychological  Assessment,  vol.  2,  no.  2,  pp.  198-­‐201.  

Head,  C  2002,  Revealing  moods:  a  diary  study  of  everyday  events,  personality  and  mood,    thesis,  Yale  University.  

Helson,  R  &  Wink,  P  1987,  'Two  conceptions  of  maturity  examined  in  the  findings  of  a  longitudinal  study',  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  vol.  53,  no.  3,  pp.  531-­‐541.  

Hersey,  P  &  Blanchard,  K  1969,  'Life  cycle  theory  of  leadership',  Training  and  Development  Journal,  vol.  23,  no.  5,  pp.  26-­‐34.  

Hesselbein,  F,  Goldsmith,  M  &  Beckhard,  R  (eds)  1996,  The  leader  of  the  future,  Jossey-­‐Bass  Publishers,  San  Francisco.  

Hirsch,  JA  1988,  Toward  a  cognitive-­‐developmental  theory  of  strategy  formulation  among  practicing  physicians,    thesis,  Boston  University.  

Hooijberg,  R  &  Choi,  J  2000,  'Which  leadership  roles  matter  to  whom?  An  examination  of  rater  effects  on  perceptions  of  effectiveness',  The  Leadership  Quarterly,  vol.  11,  no.  3,  pp.  341-­‐364.  

Hooijberg,  R,  Hunt,  JG  &  Dodge,  GE  1997,  'Leadership  complexity  and  development  of  the  leaderplex  model',  Journal  of  Management,  vol.  23,  no.  3,  pp.  375-­‐408.  

Page 243: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

241

Hooijberg,  R  &  Quinn,  RE  1992,  'Behavioral  complexity  and  the  development  of  effective  managers',  in  Strategic  management:  a  multiorganizational-­‐level  perspective,  eds  RL  Phillips  &  JG  Hunt,  Quorum,  New  York,  pp.  161-­‐176.  

Hoojiberg,  R  1996,  'A  multidirectional  approach  toward  leadership:  an  extension  of  the  concept  of  behavioral  complexity',  Human  Relations,  vol.  49,  no.  7,  pp.  917-­‐946.  

Hoppe,  CF  1972,  Ego  development  and  conformity  behaviors,    thesis,  Washington  University.  

House,  R  1971,  'A  path  goal  theory  of  leader  effectiveness',  Administrative  Science  Quarterly,  pp.  321-­‐339.  

House,  RJ  &  Aditya,  RN  1997,  'The  social  scientific  study  of  leadership:  quo  vadis?',  Journal  of  Management,  vol.  23,  no.  3,  pp.  409-­‐473.  

Howard,  A  &  Bray,  D  1988,  Managerial  lives  in  transition:  advancing  age  and  changing  times,  Guilford  Press,  New  York.  

Howard,  LW  1998,  'Validating  the  competing  values  model  as  a  representation  of  organizational  cultures',  International  Journal  of  Organizational  Analysis,  vol.  6,  pp.  231-­‐250.  

Hülsheger,  U,  Maier,  G  &  Stumpp,  T  2007,  'Validity  of  general  mental  ability  for  the  prediction  of  job  performance  and  training  success  in  Germany:  a  meta  analysis',  International  Journal  of  Selection  and  Assessment,  vol.  15,  no.  1,  pp.  3-­‐18.  

Humphrey,  RH  2002,  'The  many  faces  of  emotional  leadership',  The  Leadership  Quarterly,  vol.  13,  no.  5,  pp.  493-­‐504.  

Humphrey,  RH,  Pollack,  JM  &  Hawver,  T  2008,  'Leading  with  emotional  labor',  Journal  of  managerial  psychology,  vol.  23,  no.  2,  p.  151.  

Hunt,  E  &  Carlson,  J  2007,  'Considerations  relating  to  the  study  of  group  differences  in  intelligence',  Perspectives  on  Psychological  Science,  vol.  2,  no.  2,  pp.  194-­‐213.  

Hunt,  JG  1999,  'Transformational/charismatic  leadership's  tranformation  of  the  field  An  historical  essay',  The  Leadership  Quarterly,  vol.  10,  no.  2,  pp.  129-­‐144.  

Hunter,  J  1986,  'Cognitive  ability,  cognitive  aptitudes,  job  knowledge,  and  job  performance',  Journal  of  Vocational  Behavior,  vol.  29,  no.  3,  pp.  340-­‐362.  

Hunter,  J  &  Hunter,  R  1984,  'Validity  and  utility  of  alternative  predictors  of  job  performance',  Psychological  Bulletin,  vol.  96,  no.  1,  pp.  72-­‐98.  

Hunter,  J,  Schmidt,  F  &  Judiesch,  M  1990,  'Individual  differences  in  output  variability  as  a  function  of  job  complexity',  Journal  of  Applied  Psychology,  vol.  75,  no.  1,  pp.  28-­‐42.  

Hy,  LX  &  Loevinger,  J  1996,  Measuring  ego  development,  Lawrence  Erlbaum  Associates,  Mahwah,  NJ.  

Irwing,  P  &  Lynn,  R  2006,  'Intelligence:  is  there  a  sex  difference  in  IQ  scores?',  Nature,  vol.  442.  

Izard,  C,  Fine,  S,  Schultz,  D,  Mostow,  A,  Ackerman,  B  &  Youngstrom,  E  2001,  'Emotion  knowledge  as  a  predictor  of  social  behavior  and  academic  competence  in  children  at  risk',  Psychological  Science,  vol.  12,  no.  1,  pp.  18-­‐23.  

Page 244: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

242

Jaccard,  J  2012,  'The  Reasoned  Action  Model:  Directions  for  future  research',  The  ANNALS  of  the  American  Academy  of  Political  and  Social  Science,  vol.  640,  pp.  58-­‐81.  

Jackson,  D  &  Rushton,  J  2006,  'Males  have  greater  g:  sex  differences  in  general  mental  ability  from  100,000  17  to  18  year-­‐olds  on  the  Scholastic  Assessment  Test',  Intelligence,  vol.  34,  no.  5,  pp.  479-­‐486.  

James,  L,  Demaree,  R  &  Wolf,  G  1993,  'rwg:  an  assessment  of  within-­‐group  interrater  agreement',  Journal  of  Applied  Psychology,  vol.  78,  no.  2,  pp.  306-­‐309.  

James,  LR  &  Brett,  JM  1984,  'Mediators,  moderators,  and  tests  for  mediation',  Journal  of  Applied  Psychology,  vol.  69,  no.  2,  pp.  307-­‐321.  

Janovics,  J  &  Christiansen,  ND  2001,  'Emotional  Intelligence  at  the  workplace',  Paper  presented  at  the  16th  Annual  Converence  of  the  Society  of  Industrial  and  Organzational  Psychology.  

Johnson,  RM  1977,  'Multiple  discriminant  analysis:  marketing  research  applications',  in  Multivariate  Methods  for  Market  and  Survey  Research,  ed.  J  Sheth,  American  Marketing  Association,  Chicago,  IL.  

Joiner,  B  &  Josephs,  SA  2007,  Leadership  agility:  five  levels  of  mastery  for  anticipating  and  initiating  change,  Jossey-­‐Bass,  San  Francisco.  

Jordan,  PJ,  Ashkanasy,  NM,  Härtel,  CEJ  &  Hooper,  GS  2002,  'Workgroup  emotional  intelligence  scale  development  and  relationship  to  team  process  effectiveness  and  goal  focus',  Human  Resource  Management  Review,  vol.  12,  no.  2,  pp.  195-­‐214.  

Jordan,  PJ,  Ashton-­‐James,  CE,  Ashkanasy,  NM  &  Murphy,  KR  2006,  'Evaluating  the  claims',  in  A  critique  of  emotional  intelligence:  what  are  the  problems  and  how  can  they  be  fixed?  ,  Lawrence  Erlbaum  Associates,  Mahwah,  NJ,  pp.  189-­‐210.  

Joseph,  DL  &  Newman,  DA  2010,  'Emotional  intelligence:  an  integrative  meta-­‐analysis  and  cascading  model',  Journal  of  Applied  Psychology,  vol.  95,  no.  1,  pp.  54-­‐78.  

Judge,  T,  Colbert,  A  &  Ilies,  R  2004,  'Intelligence  and  leadership:  a  quantitative  review  and  test  of  theoretical  propositions',  Journal  of  Applied  Psychology,  vol.  89,  no.  3,  pp.  542-­‐552.  

Kafetsios,  K  2004,  'Attachment  and  emotional  intelligence  abilities  across  the  life  course',  Personality  and  Individual  differences,  vol.  37,  no.  1,  pp.  129-­‐145.  

Kafetsios,  K  &  Zampetakis,  LA  2008,  'Emotional  intelligence  and  job  satisfaction:  testing  the  mediatory  role  of  positive  and  negative  affect  at  work',  Personality  and  Individual  Differences,  vol.  44,  no.  3,  pp.  712-­‐722.  

Kahn,  R,  Katz,  D,  Cartwright,  D  &  Zander,  A  1960,  Group  dynamics:  research  and  theory,  Row,  Peterson  Elmsford,  NY.  

Kalliath,  TJ,  Bluedorn,  AC  &  Strube,  MJ  1999,  'A  test  of  value  congruence  effects',  Journal  of  Organizational  Behavior,  pp.  1175-­‐1198.  

Kang,  SM  &  Shaver,  PR  2004,  'Individual  differences  in  emotional  complexity:  their  psychological  implications',  Journal  of  Personality,  vol.  72,  no.  4,  pp.  687-­‐726.  

Page 245: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

243

Karim,  J  &  Weisz,  R  2010,  'Cross-­‐Cultural  Research  on  the  Reliability  and  Validity  of  the  Mayer-­‐Salovey-­‐Caruso  Emotional  Intelligence  Test  (MSCEIT)',  Cross-­‐cultural  research,  vol.  44,  no.  4,  pp.  374-­‐404.  

Keele,  S  &  Bell,  R  2009,  'Consensus  scoring,  correct  responses  and  reliability  of  the  MSCEITv2',  Personality  and  Individual  Differences,  vol.  47,  no.  7,  pp.  740-­‐747.  

Keele,  SM  &  Bell,  RC  2008,  'The  factorial  validity  of  emotional  intelligence:  An  unresolved  issue',  Personality  and  Individual  Differences,  vol.  44,  no.  2,  pp.  487-­‐500.  

Kegan,  R  1980,  'Making  meaning:  the  constructive-­‐developmental  approach  to  persons  and  practice',  Personnel  and  Guidance  Journal,  vol.  58,  no.  5,  pp.  373-­‐80.  

Kegan,  R  1982,  The  evolving  self:  problem  and  process  in  human  development,  Harvard  University  Press,  Cambridge,  MA.  

Kegan,  R  1995,  In  over  our  heads:  the  mental  demands  of  modern  life,  Harvard  University  Press,  Cambridge,  MA.  

Kegan,  R  &  Lahey,  LL  1984,  'Adult  leadership  and  adult  development:  a  constructivist  view',  in  Leadership:  multidisciplinary  perspectives,  pp.  199-­‐230.  

Kellet,  JB,  Humphrey,  R  &  Sleeth,  RG  2006,  'Empathy  and  the  emergence  of  task  and  relations  leaders',  The  Leadership  Quarterly,  vol.  17,  pp.  146-­‐162.  

Kellet,  JB,  Humphrey,  RH  &  Sleeth,  RG  2002,  'Empathy  and  complex  task  performance:  two  routes  to  leadership',  The  Leadership  Quarterly,  vol.  13,  pp.  523-­‐544.  

Kerr,  R,  Garvin,  J,  Heaton,  N  &  Boyle,  E  2006,  'Emotional  intelligence  and  leadership  effectiveness',  Leadership  &  Organization  Development  Journal,  vol.  27,  no.  4,  pp.  265-­‐279.  

Kerr,  S,  Schriesheim,  C,  Murphy,  C  &  Stogdill,  R  1974,  'Toward  a  contingency  theory  of  leadership  based  upon  the  consideration  and  initiating  structure  literature',  Organizational  Behavior  and  Human  Performance,  vol.  12,  no.  1,  pp.  62-­‐82.  

Kim,  K  2008,  'Meta-­‐analyses  of  the  relationship  of  creative  achievement  to  both  IQ  and  divergent  thinking  test  scores',  The  Journal  of  Creative  Behavior,  vol.  42,  no.  2,  pp.  106-­‐130.  

King,  S  &  Nicol,  DM  1999,  'Organizational  enhancement  through  recognition  of  individual  spirituality:  reflections  of  Jaques  and  Jung',  Journal  of  Organizational  Change  Management,  vol.  12,  no.  3,  pp.  234-­‐243.  

Kirkpatrick,  S  &  Locke,  E  1991,  'Leadership:  do  traits  matter?',  The  Executive,  vol.  5,  no.  2,  pp.  48-­‐60.  

Klecka,  WR  1980,  Discriminant  Analysis,  Sage  Publications,  New  Delhi.  Kohlberg,  L  1969,  'Stage  and  sequence:  the  cognitive-­‐developmental  approach  to  

socialization',  in  Handbook  of  socialization  theory  and  research,  ed.  D  Goslin,  Rand  McNally,  Chicago,  pp.  357-­‐480.  

Kotter,  JP  1990,  A  force  for  change:  how  leadership  differs  from  management,  The  Free  Press,  New  York.  

Kozlowski,  S  &  Hattrup,  K  1992,  'A  disagreement  about  within-­‐group  agreement:  disentangling  issues  of  consistency  versus  consensus',  Journal  of  Applied  Psychology,  vol.  77,  no.  2,  pp.  161-­‐167.  

Page 246: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

244

Kozlowski,  S  &  Klein,  K  2000,  'A  multilevel  approach  to  theory  and  research  in  organizations:  contextual,  temporal,  and  emergent  processes',  Multilevel  theory,  research,  and  methods  in  organizations:  Foundations,  extensions,  and  new  directions,  pp.  3–90.  

Kuhnert,  KW  1994,  'Transforming  leadership:  developing  people  through  delegation',  in  Improving  organizational  effectiveness  through  transformational  leadership,  eds  B  Bass  &  BJ  Avolio,  Sage,  Thousand  Oaks,  pp.  10-­‐25.  

Kuhnert,  KW  &  Lewis,  P  1987,  'Transactional  and  transformational  leadership:  aconstructive/developmental  analysis',  Academy  of  Management  Review,  pp.  648-­‐657.  

Lam,  L  &  Kirby,  S  2002,  'Is  emotional  intelligence  an  advantage?  An  exploration  of  the  impact  of  emotional  and  general  intelligence  on  individual  performance',  Journal  of  Social  Psychology,  vol.  142,  no.  1,  pp.  133-­‐143.  

Lambert,  HV  1972,  A  comparison  of  Jane  Loevinger's  theory  of  ego  development  and  Lawrence  Kohlberg's  theory  of  moral  development,    thesis,  University  of  Chicago.  

Lamond,  D  2003,  'The  value  of  Quinn's  Competing  Values  Model  in  an  Australian  context',  Journal  of  Managerial  Psychology,  vol.  18,  no.  1,  pp.  46-­‐59.  

Lance,  CE,  Butts,  MM  &  Michels,  LC  2006,  'The  sources  of  four  commonly  reported  cutoff  criteria:  what  did  they  really  say?',  Organizational  Research  Methods,  vol.  9,  no.  2,  pp.  202-­‐220.  

Landy,  FJ  2005,  'Some  historical  and  scientific  issues  related  to  research  on  emotional  intelligence',  Journal  of  Organizational  Behavior,  vol.  26,  no.  4,  pp.  411-­‐424.  

Lane,  RD,  Quinlan,  DM,  Schwartz,  GE,  Walker,  PA  &  Zeitlin,  SB  1990,  'The  Levels  of  Emotional  Awareness  Scale:  a  cognitive-­‐developmental  measure  of  emotion',  Journal  of  Personal  Assessment,  vol.  55,  pp.  124-­‐134.  

Lane,  RD  &  Schwartz,  GE  1987,  'Levels  of  emotional  awareness:  a  cognitive-­‐developmental  theory  and  its  application  to  psychopathology',  The  American  Journal  of  Psychiatry,  vol.  144,  no.  2,  pp.  133-­‐143.  

Law,  KS,  Wong,  CS  &  Mobley,  WH  1998,  'Toward  a  taxonomy  of  multidimensional  constructs',  Academy  of  Management  Review,  vol.  23,  no.  4,  pp.  741-­‐755.  

Leban,  W  &  Zulauf,  C  2004,  'Linking  emotional  intelligence  abilities  and  transformational  leadership  styles',  Leadership  &  Organization  Development  Journal,  vol.  25,  no.  7,  pp.  554-­‐564.  

LeBreton,  JM,  Burgess,  JRD,  Kaiser,  RB,  Atchley,  EK  &  James,  LR  2003,  'The  restriction  of  variance  hypothesis  and  interrater  reliability  and  agreement:  are  ratings  from  multiple  sources  really  dissimilar?',  Organizational  Research  Methods,  vol.  6,  no.  1,  pp.  80-­‐128.  

LeBreton,  JM  &  Senter,  JL  2008,  'Answers  to  20  questions  about  interrater  reliability  and  interrater  agreement',  Organizational  Research  Methods,  vol.  11,  no.  4,  pp.  815-­‐852.  

Legree,  PJ  1995,  'Evidence  for  an  oblique  social  intelligence  factor  established  with  a  Likert-­‐based  testing  procedure',  Intelligence,  vol.  21,  no.  3,  pp.  247-­‐266.  

Page 247: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

245

Lewis,  M  2000,  'Exploring  paradox:  toward  a  more  comprehensive  guide',  Academy  of  Management  Review,  vol.  25,  no.  4,  pp.  760-­‐776.  

Lewis,  P,  Forsythe,  GB,  Sweeney,  P,  Bartone,  P,  Bullis,  C  &  Snook,  S  2005,  'Identity  development  during  the  college  years:  findings  from  the  West  Point  longitudinal  study',  Journal  of  College  Student  Development,  vol.  46,  pp.  357-­‐373.  

Lichtenstein,  BMS,  B.  A.  and  Torbert,  W.R.  1995,  'Leadership  and  ethical  development:  balancing  light  and  shadow',  Business  Ethics  Quarterly,  vol.  5,  no.  1,  pp.  97-­‐116.  

Lindebaum,  D  2009,  'Rhetoric  or  remedy?  A  critique  on  developing  emotional  intelligence',  Academy  of  Management  Learning  &  Education,  vol.  8,  no.  2,  pp.  225-­‐237.  

Locke,  E,  Kirkpatrick,  S,  Wheeler,  J,  Schneider,  J,  Niles,  K,  Goldstein,  H,  Welsh,  K  &  Chah,  D  1991,  The  essence  of  leadership,  Lexington  Books,  New  York.  

Locke,  EA  2005,  'Why  emotional  intelligence  is  an  invalid  concept',  Journal  of  Organizational  Behavior,  vol.  26,  no.  4.  

Loevinger,  J  1976,  Ego  Development,  Jossey-­‐Bass,  San  Francisco.  Loevinger,  J  1979,  'Construct  validity  of  the  sentence  completion  test  of  ego  

development',  Applied  Psychological  Measurement,  vol.  3,  no.  3,  pp.  281-­‐311.  Loevinger,  J  1997,  'Stages  of  personality  development',  Handbook  of  Personality  

Psychology,  pp.  199–208.  Loevinger,  J  1998,  Technical  foundations  for  measuring  ego  development:  the  

Washington  University  Sentence  Completion  Test,  Lawrence  Erlbaum.  Loevinger,  J,  Cohn,  LD,  Bonneville,  LP,  Redmore,  CD,  Streich,  DD  &  Sargent,  M  1985,  

'Ego  development  in  college',  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  vol.  48,  no.  4,  pp.  947-­‐962.  

Loevinger,  J,  Wessler,  R  &  Redmore,  C  1970,  Measuring  ego  development,  Jossey-­‐Bass,  San  Francisco.  

Loevinger,  JJ  1966,  'The  meaning  and  measurement  of  ego  development',  The  American  Psychologist,  vol.  21,  no.  3,  pp.  195-­‐206.  

Loevinger,  JJ  1993,  'Measurement  of  personality:  true  or  false',  Psychological  Inquiry,  vol.  4,  no.  1,  pp.  1-­‐16.  

Lopes,  PN,  Brackett,  MA,  Nezlek,  JB,  Schutz,  A,  Sellin,  I  &  Salovey,  P  2004,  'Emotional  intelligence  and  social  interaction',  Personality  and  Social  Psychology  Bulletin,  vol.  30,  pp.  1018-­‐1034.  

Lopes,  PN,  Grewal,  D,  Kadis,  J,  Gall,  M  &  Salovey,  P  2006,  'Evidence  that  emotional  intelligence  is  related  to  job  performance  and  affect  and  attitudes  at  work',  Psicothema,  vol.  18,  no.  1,  pp.  132–138.  

Lopes,  PN,  Salovey,  P,  Côté,  S,  Beers,  M  &  Petty,  RE  2005,  'Emotion  regulation  abilities  and  the  quality  of  social  interaction',  Emotion,  vol.  5,  no.  1,  pp.  113-­‐118.  

Lopes,  PN,  Salovey,  P  &  Straus,  R  2003,  'Emotional  intelligence,  personality,  and  the  perceived  quality  of  social  relationships',  Personality  and  Individual  Differences,  vol.  35,  no.  3,  pp.  641-­‐658.  

Page 248: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

246

Lord,  R,  De  Vader,  C  &  Alliger,  G  1986,  'A  meta-­‐analysis  of  the  relation  between  personality  traits  and  leadership  perceptions:  an  application  of  validity  generalization  procedures',  Journal  of  Applied  Psychology,  vol.  71,  no.  3,  pp.  402-­‐410.  

Lussier,  R  &  Achua,  C  2009,  Leadership:  theory,  application,  &  skill  development,  South-­‐Western  Cengage  Learning,  Mason,  OH.  

Lynn,  R  &  Irwing,  P  2004,  'Sex  differences  on  the  progressive  matrices:  a  meta-­‐analysis',  Intelligence,  vol.  32,  no.  5,  pp.  481-­‐498.  

Lyons,  JB  &  Schneider,  TR  2005,  'The  influence  of  emotional  intelligence  on  performance',  Personality  and  Individual  Differences,  vol.  39,  no.  4,  pp.  693-­‐703.  

MacCallum,  R  &  Austin,  J  2000,  'Applications  of  structural  equation  modeling  in  psychological  research',  Annual  Review  of  Psychology,  vol.  51,  no.  1,  pp.  201-­‐226.  

MacCann,  C,  Matthews,  G,  Zeidner,  M  &  Roberts,  RD  2003,  'Psychological  assessment  of  emotional  intelligence:  a  review  of  self-­‐report  and  performance-­‐based  testing',  International  Journal  of  Organizational  Analysis,  vol.  11,  no.  3,  pp.  247-­‐274.  

MacCann,  C,  Roberts,  R,  Matthews,  G  &  Zeidner,  M  2004,  'Consensus  scoring  and  empirical  option  weighting  of  performance-­‐based  emotional  intelligence  (EI)  tests',  Personality  and  Individual  Differences,  vol.  36,  no.  3,  pp.  645-­‐662.  

Magidson,  J  &  Vermunt,  JK  2002,  'Latent  class  models  for  clustering:  a  comparison  with  K-­‐means',  Canadian  Journal  of  Marketing  Research,  vol.  20,  pp.  37-­‐44.  

Magidson,  J  &  Vermunt,  JK  2004,  'Latent  class  models',  The  Sage  handbook  of  quantitative  methodology  for  the  social  sciences,  pp.  175-­‐198.  

Mandell,  B  &  Pherwani,  S  2003,  'Relationship  between  emotional  intelligence  and  transformational  leadership  style:  a  gender  comparison',  Journal  of  Business  and  Psychology,  vol.  17,  no.  3,  pp.  387-­‐404.  

Mann,  R  1959,  'A  review  of  the  relationships  between  personality  and  performance  in  small  groups',  Psychological  Bulletin,  vol.  56,  no.  4,  pp.  241-­‐270.  

Manners,  J  &  Durkin,  K  2000,  'Processes  involved  in  adult  ego  development:  a  conceptual  framework',  Developmental  Review,  vol.  20,  no.  4,  pp.  475-­‐513.  

Manners,  J  &  Durkin,  K  2001,  'A  critical  review  of  the  validity  of  ego  development  theory  and  its  measurement  ',  Journal  of  Personality  Assessment  vol.  77,  no.  3,  pp.  541-­‐567.  

Matsumoto,  D,  LeRoux,  JA,  Wilson-­‐Cohn,  C,  Raroque,  J,  Kooken,  K  &  al,  e  2000,  'A  new  test  to  measure  emotion  recognition  ability:  Matsumoto  and  Ekman's  Japanese  and  Caucasian  Brief  Affect  Recognition  Test  (JACBART)',  Journal  of  Nonverbal  Behavior,  vol.  24,  pp.  179-­‐209.  

Matthews,  G,  Roberts,  RD  &  Zeidner,  M  2004,  'Seven  myths  About  emotional  intelligence',  Psychological  Inquiry,  vol.  15,  no.  3,  pp.  179-­‐196.  

Matthews,  G,  Zeidner,  M  &  Roberts,  RD  2002,  Emotional  intelligence:  science  and  myth,  MIT  Press,  Cambridge,  MA.  

Page 249: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

247

Maul,  A  2011,  'The  factor  structure  and  cross-­‐test  convergence  of  the  Mayer-­‐Salovey-­‐Caruso  model  of  emotional  intelligence',  Personality  and  Individual  Differences,  vol.  50,  pp.  457-­‐463.  

Mayer,  JD  2001,  'Emotion,  intelligence  and  emotional  intelligence',  in  Handbook  of  affect  and  cognition,  ed.  JP  Forgas,  Lawrence  Erlbaum,  Mahwah  NJ,  pp.  415-­‐433.  

Mayer,  JD  2010,  Personal  communication  clarifying  the  nineteen  items  ommitted  from  MSCEIT  score  calculations,  S  Chappell.  

Mayer,  JD,  Caruso,  D  &  Salovey,  P  1998,  Multifactor  Emotional  Intelligence  Test  (MEIS)  Available  from  John  D.  Mayer  Department  of  Psychology    Conant  Hall,  University  of  New  Hampshire,  Durham,  NH  03824  USA.  

Mayer,  JD,  Caruso,  D  &  Salovey,  P  1999a,  'Emotional  intelligence  meets  standards  for  a  traditional  intelligence',  Intelligence,  vol.  27,  no.  4,  pp.  267-­‐298.  

Mayer,  JD,  Caruso,  D  &  Salovey,  P  1999b,  'Emotional  intelligence  meets  standards  for  a  traditional  intelligence',  Intelligence,  vol.  27,  pp.  267-­‐298.  

Mayer,  JD,  Caruso,  DR  &  Salovey,  P  2000,  'Selecting  a  measure  of  emotional  intelligence:  The  case  for  ability  scales',  in  The  handbook  of  emotional  intelligence:  theory  development,  assessment  and  application  at  home,  school  and  in  the  workplace,  eds  R  Bar-­‐On  &  JDA  Parker,  Jossey-­‐Bass,  San  Francisco,  pp.  320–342.  

Mayer,  JD  &  Geher,  G  1996,  'Emotional  intelligence  and  the  identification  of  emotion',  Intelligence,  no.  22,  pp.  89-­‐113.  

Mayer,  JD,  Perkins,  DM,  Caruso,  DR  &  Salovey,  P  2001,  'Emotional  intelligence  and  giftedness',  Roeper  Review,  vol.  23,  no.  3,  pp.  131-­‐137.  

Mayer,  JD,  Roberts,  RD  &  Barsade,  SG  2008,  'Human  abilities:  emotional  intelligence',  Annual  Review  of  Psychology,  vol.  59,  pp.  507-­‐536.  

Mayer,  JD  &  Salovey,  P  1993,  'The  intelligence  of  emotional  intelligence',  Intelligence,  vol.  17,  pp.  433-­‐442.  

Mayer,  JD  &  Salovey,  P  1997,  'What  is  emotional  intelligence?',  in  Emotional  development  and  emotional  intelligence:  educational  implications,  eds  P  Salovey  &  D  Sluyter,  Basic  Books,  New  York.  

Mayer,  JD,  Salovey,  P,  Caruso,  D  &  Sitaremos,  G  2003,  'Measuring  emotional  intelligence  with  the  MSCEITv2.0',  Emotion,  vol.  3,  no.  1,  pp.  97-­‐105.  

Mayer,  JD,  Salovey,  P  &  Caruso,  DR  2000,  'Competing  models  of  emotional  intelligence',  in  Handbook  of  human  intelligence,  vol.  396-­‐420,  ed.  RJ  Sternberg,  Cambridge  University  Press,  New  York.  

Mayer,  JD,  Salovey,  P  &  Caruso,  DR  2002,  Technical  Manual  for  Mayer-­‐Salovey-­‐Caruso  Emotional  Intelligence  Test  (MSCEIT)  Multi-­‐Health  Systems  Toronto,  Ontario.  

Mayer,  JD,  Salovey,  P  &  Caruso,  DR  2004,  'Emotional  intelligence:  theory,  findings  and  implications',  Psychological  Inquiry,  vol.  15,  no.  3,  pp.  197-­‐215.  

Mayer,  JD,  Salovey,  P  &  Caruso,  DR  2008,  'Emotional  intelligence:  new  ability  or  eclectic  traits?',  American  Psychologist,  vol.  63,  no.  6,  pp.  503-­‐517.  

Page 250: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

248

McCauley,  CD,  Drath,  WH,  Palus,  CJ,  O'Connor,  PMG  &  Baker,  BA  2006,  'The  use  of  constructive-­‐developmental  theory  to  advance  the  understanding  of  leadership',  The  Leadership  Quarterly,  vol.  17,  pp.  643-­‐653.  

McClelland,  D  1973,  'Testing  for  competence  rather  than  for  'intelligence'',  American  Psychologist,  vol.  28,  no.  1,  pp.  1-­‐14.  

McCrae,  RR  &  Costa,  PT  1980,  'Openness  to  experience  and  ego  level  in  Loevinger's  Sentence  Completion  Test:  dispositional  contributions  to  developmental  models  of  personality',  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  vol.  39,  no.  6,  pp.  1179-­‐1190.  

McGraw,  K  &  Wong,  S  1996,  'Forming  inferences  about  some  intraclass  correlation  coefficients',  Psychological  Methods,  vol.  1,  no.  1,  pp.  30-­‐46.  

Megerian  &  Sosik  1996,  'An  affair  of  the  heart:  emotional  intelligence  and  transformational  leadership',  Journal  of  Leadership  Studies,  no.  3,  pp.  31-­‐48.  

Mehltretter,  GW,  Jr  1996,  The  contribution  of  complexity  of  mental  processing  and  stage  of  ego  development  to  transforming  leadership,  PhD    thesis,  North  Carolina  State  University.  

Mendoza,  J,  Stafford,  K  &  Stauffer,  J  2000,  'Large-­‐sample  confidence  intervals  for  validity  and  reliability  coefficients',  Psychological  Methods,  vol.  5,  no.  3,  pp.  356-­‐369.  

Merron,  K  1985,  The  relationship  between  ego  development  and  managerial  effectiveness  under  conditions  of  high  uncertainty,  Doctoral    thesis,  Harvard  Graduate  School  of  Education.  

Merron,  K,  Fisher,  D  &  Torbert,  W  1986,  'Meaning  making  and  managerial  effectiveness:  a  developmental  perspective',  in  Academy  of  Management  Annual  Meeting,  Chicago.  

Merron,  K,  Fisher,  D  &  Torbert,  WR  1987,  'Meaning  making  and  management  action',  Group  &  Organization  Management,  vol.  12,  no.  3,  pp.  274-­‐286.  

Merron,  K  &  Torbert,  WR  1984,  Offering  managers  feedback  on  Loevinger's  ego  development  measure,  School  of  Management,  Boston  College.  

Mintzberg,  H  1973,  The  nature  of  managerial  work,  Harper  &  Row,  New  York.  Moss,  FA  &  Hunt,  T  1927,  'Are  you  socially  intelligent?',  Scientific  American,  vol.  137,  

pp.  108-­‐110.  Mowrer,  OH  1960,  Learning  theory  and  behavior,  Wiley,  New  York.  Mueller,  J  &  Curhan,  J  2006,  'Emotional  intelligence  and  counterpart  mood  induction  in  

a  negotiation',  International  Journal  of  Conflict  Management  vol.  17,  pp.  110-­‐128.  

Mumford,  M,  Zaccaro,  S,  Harding,  F,  Jacobs,  T  &  Fleishman,  E  2000,  'Leadership  skills  for  a  changing  world:  solving  complex  social  problems',  The  Leadership  Quarterly,  vol.  11,  no.  1,  pp.  11-­‐35.  

Murphy,  K,  Myors,  B  &  Wolach,  A  1998,  Statistical  power  analysis,  L.  Erlbaum  Associates.  

Murray,  FB  1983,  'Equilibration  as  cognitive  conflict',  Developmental  Review,  vol.  3,  pp.  54-­‐61.  

Page 251: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

249

Neisser,  U  1976,  Cognition  and  reality,  Freeman,  San  Francisco.  Neisser,  U,  Boodoo,  B,  Bouchard,  TJ,  Boykin,  A,  Brody,  B,  Ceci,  S,  Halpern,  D,  Loehlin,  J,  

Perloff,  R,  Sternberg,  S  &  Urbina,  S  1996,  'Intelligence:  knowns  and  unknowns',  American  Psychologist,  vol.  5,  no.  2,  pp.  77-­‐101.  

Nettelbeck,  T  &  Wilson,  C  2005,  'Intelligence  and  IQ:  what  teachers  should  know',  Educational  Psychology,  vol.  25,  no.  6,  pp.  609-­‐630.  

Newcombe,  MJ  &  Ashkanasy,  NM  2002,  'The  role  of  affect  and  affective  congruence  in  perceptions  of  leaders:  an  experimental  study',  The  Leadership  Quarterly,  vol.  13,  no.  5,  pp.  601-­‐614.  

Newman,  DL,  Tellegen,  A  &  Bouchard,  TJ  1998,  'Individual  differences  in  adult  ego  development:  sources  of  influence  in  twins  reared  apart',  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  vol.  74,  pp.  985-­‐995.  

Novy,  D  &  Francis,  D  1992,  'Psychometric  properties  of  the  Washington  University  Sentence  Completion  Test',  Educational  and  Psychological  Measurement,  vol.  52,  no.  4,  pp.  1029-­‐1039.  

Novy,  DM,  Franklowicz,  RG,  Francis,  DJ,  Llberman,  D  &  Vincent,  KR  1994,  'An  investigation  of  the  structural  validity  of  Loevinger's  model  and  measure  of  ego  development',  Journal  of  Personality,  vol.  62,  no.  1,  pp.  87-­‐118.  

Nowicki,  SJ  &  Duke,  MP  1994,  'Individual  differences  in  the  nonverbal  communication  of  affect:  the  Diagnostic  Analysis  of  Nonverbal  Accuracy  Scale',  Journal  of  Nonverbal  Behavior,  vol.  19,  pp.  9-­‐35.  

Nunnally,  JC  &  Bernstein,  IH  1978,  Psychometric  theory,  McGraw-­‐Hill,  Sydney.  O'Boyle  Jr,  EH,  Humphrey,  RH,  Pollack,  JM,  Hawver,  TH  &  Story,  PA  2010,  'The  relation  

between  emotional  intelligence  and  job  performance:  a  meta  analysis',  Journal  of  Organizational  Behavior.  

O'Connor,  K  &  Chamberlain,  K  1996,  'Dimensions  of  life  meaning:  A  qualitative  investigation  at  mid-­‐life',  British  Journal  of  Psychology,  vol.  87,  no.  3,  pp.  461-­‐477.  

O'Connor,  RMJ  &  Little,  IS  2003,  'Revisiting  the  predicitve  validity  of  emotional  intelligence:  self-­‐report  vs  ability-­‐based  measures',  Personality  and  Individual  Differences,  vol.  35,  pp.  1893-­‐1902.  

Ones,  D,  Viswesvaran,  C  &  Dilchert,  S  2005,  'Cognitive  ability  in  selection  decisions',  in  Handbook  of  understanding  and  measuring  intelligence,  eds  O  Wilhelm  &  R  Engle,  Sage  Publications,  Thousand  Oaks,  California,  pp.  431–468.  

Palmer,  B,  Gignac,  G,  Manocha,  R  &  Stough,  C  2008,  'A  comprehensive  framework  for  emotional  intelligence',  in  Emotional  Intelligence:  Theoretical  and  Cultural  Perspectives,  eds  RJ  Emmerling,  VK  Shanwal  &  MK  Mandal,  Nova  Science,  New  York,  pp.  17-­‐38.  

Palmer,  B,  Walls,  M,  Burgess,  Z  &  Stough,  C  2001,  'Emotional  intelligence  and  effective  leadership',  Leadership  &  Organization  Development  Journal,  vol.  22,  no.  1,  pp.  5-­‐10.  

Palmer,  BR,  Gignac,  F,  Manocha,  R  &  Stough,  C  2005,  'A  psychometric  evaluation  of  the  Mayer-­‐Salovey-­‐Caruso  Emotional  Intelligence  Test  Version  2.0',  Intelligence,  vol.  33,  no.  3,  pp.  285-­‐305.  

Page 252: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

250

Park,  CL  &  Folkman,  S  1997,  'Meaning  in  the  context  of  stress  and  coping',  Review  of  General  Psychology,  vol.  1,  no.  2,  p.  115.  

Parker,  JDA,  Saklofske,  DH,  Shaughnessy,  PA,  Huang,  SHS  &  Wood,  LM  2005,  'Generalizability  of  the  emotional  intelligence  construct:  A  cross-­‐cultural  study  of  North  American  aboriginal  youth',  Personality  and  Individual  Differences,  vol.  39,  no.  1,  p.  215.  

Pérez,  JC,  Petrides,  KV  &  Furnham,  A  2005,  'Measuring  trait  emotional  intelligence',  International  handbook  of  emotional  intelligence,  pp.  123-­‐153.  

Pescosolido,  AT  2002,  'Emergent  leaders  as  managers  of  group  emotion',  The  Leadership  Quarterly,  vol.  13,  no.  5,  pp.  583-­‐599.  

Peterson,  R  &  Mahajan,  V  1976,  'Practical  significance  and  partitioning  variance  in  discriminant  analysis',  Decision  Sciences,  vol.  7,  no.  4,  pp.  649-­‐658.  

Petrides,  KV  &  Furnham,  A  2001,  'Trait  emotional  intelligence:  psychometric  investigation  with  reference  to  established  trait  taxonomies',  European  Journal  of  Personality,  vol.  15,  no.  6,  pp.  425-­‐448.  

Pfaffenberger,  AH  2005,  'Optimal  adult  development:  an  inquiry  into  the  dynamics  of  growth',  Journal  of  Humanistic  Psychology,  vol.  45,  no.  3,  pp.  279-­‐301.  

Pfeiffer,  SI  2001,  'Emotional  intelligence:  Popular  but  elusive  construct',  Roeper  Review,  vol.  23,  no.  3,  pp.  138-­‐142.  

Piaget,  J  1954,  The  construction  of  reality  in  the  child,  Basic  Books,  New  York    Prati,  LM,  Douglas,  C,  Ferris,  GR,  Ammeter,  AP  &  Buckley,  MR  2003,  'Emotional  

intelligence,  leadership  effectiveness,  and  team  outcomes',  International  Journal  of  Organizational  Analysis,  vol.  11,  no.  1,  pp.  21-­‐40.  

Price,  T  2003,  'The  ethics  of  authentic  transformational  leadership',  The  Leadership  Quarterly,  vol.  14,  no.  1,  pp.  67-­‐81.  

Pusey,  F  2000,  Emotional  intelligence  and  success  in  the  workplace:  The  MSCEIT  and  EQi,  Master's  thesis,  Guildhall  University.  

Quatro,  SA,  Waldman,  DA  &  Galvin,  BM  2007,  'Developing  holistic  leaders:  four  domains  for  leadership  development  and  practice',  Human  Resource  Management  Review,  vol.  17,  no.  4,  pp.  427-­‐441.  

Quinn,  R  &  Torbert,  W  1987,  Who  is  an  effective,  transforming  leader  in  Unpublished  paper.  University  of  Michigan  School  of  Business,  Ann  Arbor,  MI.  

Quinn,  RE  1984,  'Applying  the  competing  values  approach  to  leadership:  toward  an  integrative  framework',  in  Managers  and  leaders:  an  international  perspective,  eds  JG  Hunt,  R  Stewart,  C  Schriesheim  &  D  Hosking,  Pergamon,  New  York  pp.  117-­‐141.  

Quinn,  RE  1988,  Beyond  rational  management:  mastering  the  paradoxes  and  competing  demands  of  high  performance,  Jossey-­‐Bass,  San  Francisco.  

Quinn,  RE,  Faerman,  SR,  Thompson,  MP  &  McGrath,  MR  1996,  Becoming  a  master  manager,  2nd  edn,  John  Wiley  &  Sons,  Brisbane.  

Quinn,  RE  &  McGrath,  MR  1982,  'Moving  beyond  the  single-­‐solution  perspective:  the  competing  values  approach  as  a  diagnostic  tool',  The  Journal  of  Applied  Behavioral  Science,  vol.  18,  no.  4,  pp.  463-­‐472.  

Page 253: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

251

Quinn,  RE  &  Rohrbaugh,  J  1983,  'A  spatial  model  of  effectiveness  criteria:  towards  a  competing  values  approach  to  organizational  analysis',  Management  Science,  vol.  29,  pp.  363-­‐377.  

Raelin,  J  2006,  'Finding  meaning  in  the  organization',  MIT  Sloan  Management  Review,  vol.  47,  no.  3,  p.  64.  

Raelin,  JA  2003,  Creating  leaderful  organizations:  how  to  bring  out  leadership  in  everyone,  Berrett-­‐Koehler  Publishers.  

Reams,  J  2005,  'What’s  integral  about  leadership?  A  reflection  on  leadership  and  integral  theory',  Integral  Review,  vol.  1,  pp.  118-­‐131.  

Redmore,  C  &  Waldman,  K  1975,  'Reliability  of  a  sentence  completion  measure  of  ego  development',  Journal  of  Personality  Assessment,  vol.  39,  no.  3,  pp.  236-­‐243.  

Redmore,  CD  1983,  'Ego  development  in  the  college  years:  two  longitudinal  studies',  Journal  of  Youth  and  Adolescence,  vol.  12,  no.  4,  pp.  301-­‐306.  

Redmore,  CD  &  Loevinger,  J  1979,  'Ego  development  in  adolescence:  longitudinal  studies',  Journal  of  Youth  and  Adolescence,  vol.  8,  no.  1,  pp.  1-­‐20.  

Reker,  GT  2005,  'Meaning  in  life  of  young,  middle-­‐aged,  and  older  adults:  factorial  validity,  age,  and  gender  invariance  of  the  Personal  Meaning  Index  (PMI)',  Personality  and  Individual  Differences,  vol.  38,  no.  1,  pp.  71-­‐85.  

Riggio,  RE  2002,  'Multiple  Intelligences  and  Leadership:  An  Overview',  in  Multiple  Intelligences  and  Leadership  eds  S  Murphy  &  F  Pirozzolo,  Lawrence  Erlbaum  Associates,  Inc,  New  Jersey.  

Roberts,  RD,  Schulze,  R,  O'Brien,  K,  MacCann,  C,  Reid,  J  &  Maul,  A  2006,  'Exploring  the  validity  of  the  Mayer-­‐Salovey-­‐Caruso  Emotional  Intelligence  Test  (MSCEIT)  with  established  emotions  measures',  Emotion,  vol.  6,  pp.  663-­‐669.  

Roberts,  RD,  Zeidner,  M  &  Matthews,  G  2001,  'Does  emotional  intelligence  meet  traditional  standards  for  an  intelligence?  Some  new  data  and  conclusions',  Emotion,  vol.  1,  no.  3,  pp.  196-­‐231.  

Rode,  JC,  Mooney,  CH,  Arthaud-­‐Day,  ML,  Near,  JP,  Baldwin,  TT,  Rubin,  RS  &  Bommer,  WH  2007,  'Emotional  intelligence  and  individual  performance:  evidence  of  direct  and  moderated  effects',  Journal  of  Organizational  Behavior,  vol.  28,  no.  4,  pp.  399-­‐421.  

Rode,  JC,  Mooney,  CH,  Arthaud-­‐Day,  ML,  Near,  JP,  Rubin,  RS  &  Baldwin,  TT  2008,  'An  examination  of  the  structural,  discriminant,  nomological,  and  incremental  predictive  validity  of  the  MSCEIT©  V2.0',  Intelligence,  vol.  36,  no.  4,  pp.  350-­‐366.  

Rooke,  D  &  Torbert,  WR  2005,  'The  seven  transformations  of  leadership  ',  Harvard  Business  Review,  vol.  Vol.  83,  no.  Issues  4,  pp.  66-­‐76.  

Rooke,  D  &  Torbet,  WR  1998,  'Organizational  transformation  as  a  function  of  CEO's  developmental  stage',  Organization  Development  Journal,  vol.  16,  pp.  11-­‐28.  

Rosenthal,  R  1979,  Sensitivity  to  nonverbal  communication:  The  PONS  test,  Johns  Hopkins  University  Press,  Baltimore.  

Page 254: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

252

Rosete,  D  &  Ciarrochi,  J  2005,  'Emotional  intelligence  and  its  relationship  to  workplace  performance  of  leadership  effectiveness',  Leadership  and  Organizational  Development  Journal,  vol.  26,  pp.  388-­‐399.  

Rossen,  E  &  Kranzler,  JH  2008,  'Confirmatory  factor  analysis  of  the  Mayer  Salovey  Caruso  Emotional  Intelligence  Test  v  2.0  (MSCEIT)',  Personality  and  Individual  Differences,  vol.  44,  no.  5,  pp.  1258-­‐1269    

Rossen,  E  &  Kranzler,  JH  2009,  'Incremental  validity  of  the  Mayer-­‐Salovey-­‐Caruso  Emotional  Intelligence  Test  Version  2.0  (MSCEIT)  after  controlling  for  personality  and  intelligence',  Journal  of  Research  in  Personality,  vol.  43,  no.  1,  pp.  60-­‐65.  

Rozsnafszky,  J  1981,  'The  relationship  of  level  of  ego  development  to  Q-­‐sort  personality  ratings',  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  vol.  41,  pp.  99-­‐120.  

Rubin,  R,  Bartels,  L  &  Bommer,  W  2002,  'Are  leaders  smarter  or  do  they  just  seem  that  way:  exploring  perceived  intellectual  competence  and  leadership  emergence',  Social  Behavior  and  Personality,  vol.  30,  no.  2,  pp.  105-­‐118.  

Rubin,  RS,  Munz,  DC  &  Bommer,  WH  2005,  'Leading  From  Within:  The  Effects  of  Emotional  Recognitian  and  Personality  on  Transformational  Leadership  Behaviour  ',  Academy  of  Management  Journal,  vol.  48,  no.  5,  pp.  845-­‐858.  

Rushton,  J  &  Jensen,  A  2005,  'Thirty  years  of  research  on  race  differences  in  cognitive  ability',  Psychology,  Public  Policy,  and  Law,  vol.  11,  no.  2,  pp.  235-­‐294.  

Rushton,  J  &  Jensen,  A  2006,  'The  totality  of  available  evidence  shows  the  race  IQ  gap  still  remains',  Psychological  Science,  vol.  17,  no.  10,  pp.  921-­‐922.  

Saklofske,  D,  Austin,  E  &  Minski,  P  2003,  'Factor  structure  and  validity  of  a  trait  emotional  intelligence  measure',  Personality  and  Individual  Differences,  vol.  34,  no.  4,  pp.  707-­‐721.  

Sala,  F  2002,  Emotional  competence  inventory:  technical  manual,  McClelland  Center  for  Research:  Hay  Group.  

Salgado,  J,  Anderson,  N,  Moscoso,  S,  Bertua,  C  &  FRUYT,  F  2003,  'International  validity  generalization  of  GMA  and  cognitivie  abilities:  a  Eurpoean  Community  meta  analysis  ',  Personnel  Psychology,  vol.  56,  no.  3,  pp.  573-­‐605.  

Salovey,  P,  Bedell,  BT,  Detweiler,  JB  &  Mayer,  JD  1999,  'Coping  intelligently:  emotional  intelligence  and  the  coping  process',  in  Coping:  the  psychology  of  what  works,  ed.  CR  Snyder,  Oxford  University  Press,  New  York,  pp.  141-­‐164.  

Salovey,  P  &  Grewal,  D  2005,  'The  science  of  emotional  intelligence',  Current  Directions  in  Psychological  Science,  vol.  14,  no.  6,  pp.  281-­‐285.  

Salovey,  P  &  Mayer,  JD  1990,  'Emotional  intelligence',  Imagination,  Cognition  and  Personality,  vol.  9,  pp.  185-­‐211.  

Salovey,  P,  Mayer,  JD  &  Caruso,  D  2002,  'The  positive  psychology  of  emotional  intelligence',  Handbook  of  positive  psychology,  pp.  159-­‐171.  

Salovey,  P,  Mayer,  JD,  Goldman,  SL,  Turvey,  C  &  Palfai,  TP  1995,  'Emotional  attention,  clarity,  and  repair:  exploring  emotional  intelligence  using  the  Trait  Meta  mood  Scale',  in  Emotion,  disclosure  and  health,  ed.  JW  Pennebaker,  American  Psychological  Association,  Washington  DC,  pp.  125-­‐154.  

Page 255: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

253

Salovey,  P  &  Sluyter,  DJ  1997,  Emotional  development  and  emotional  intelligence:  educational  implications,  Basic  Books,  New  York.  

Sarstedt,  M  2008,  'Market  segmentation  with  mixture  regression  models:  Understanding  measures  that  guide  model  selection',  Journal  of  Targeting,  Measurement  and  Analysis  for  Marketing,  vol.  16,  no.  3,  pp.  228-­‐246.  

Schmidt,  F  &  Hunter,  J  1998,  'The  validity  and  utility  of  selection  methods  in  personnel  psychology:  practical  and  theoretical  implications  of  85  years  of  research  findings',  Psychological  Bulletin,  vol.  124,  pp.  262-­‐274.  

Schmidt,  F  &  Hunter,  J  2004,  'General  mental  ability  in  the  world  of  work:  occupational  attainment  and  job  performance',  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  vol.  86,  no.  1,  pp.  162-­‐173.  

Schnell,  T  2009,  'The  Sources  of  Meaning  and  Meaning  in  Life  Questionnaire  (SoMe):  Relations  to  demographics  and  well-­‐being',  The  Journal  of  Positive  Psychology,  vol.  4,  no.  6,  pp.  483-­‐499.  

Schoenfeldt,  LF  1985,  'Review  of  the  Wonderlic  Personnel  Test',  in  The  nineth  mental  measurements  yearbook,  vol.  2,  ed.  JV  Mitchell  Jr,  University  of  Nebraska  Press,  Lincoln,  NE.  

Schulte,  MJ,  Ree,  MJ  &  Carretta,  TR  2004,  'Emotional  intelligence:  not  much  more  than  g  and  personality',  Personality  and  Individual  Differences,  vol.  37,  no.  5,  pp.  1059-­‐1068.  

Schutte,  NS,  Malouff,  JM,  Bobik,  C,  Coston,  TD,  Greeson,  C,  Jedlicka,  C,  Rhodes,  E  &  Wendorf,  G  2001,  'Emotional  intelligence  and  interpersonal  relations',  Journal  of  Social  Psychology,  vol.  141,  no.  4,  pp.  523-­‐36.  

Schutte,  NS,  Malouff,  JM,  Hall,  LE,  Haggerty,  DJ,  Cooper,  JT,  Golden,  CJ  &  Dornheim,  L  1998,  'Development  and  validation  of  a  measure  of  emotional  intelligence',  Personality  and  Individual  Differences,  vol.  25,  pp.  167-­‐177.  

Sears,  G  &  Holmvall,  C  2009,  'The  joint  Influence  of  supervisor  and  subordinate  emotional  intelligence  on  leader–member  exchange',  Journal  of  Business  and  Psychology,  pp.  1-­‐13.  

Senge,  PM  1990,  The  fifth  discipline,  Doubleday,  New  York.  Shaffer,  RD  2005,  'Emotional  intelligence  abilities,  personality  and  workplace  

performance',  in  Academy  of  Management  Annual  Meeting,  Honolulu.  Shipper,  F,  Kincaid,  J,  Rotondo,  D  &  Hoffman,  R  2003,  'A  cross-­‐cultural  exploratory  

study  of  the  linkage  between  emotional  intelligence  and  managerial  effectiveness',  International  Journal  of  Organizational  Analysis,  vol.  11,  no.  3,  pp.  171-­‐191.  

Shrout,  PE  &  Fleiss,  JL  1979,  'Intraclass  correlations:  uses  in  assessing  rater  reliability',  Psychological  Bulletin,  vol.  56,  no.  2,  pp.  420-­‐428.  

Simonton,  D  2008,  'Genius,  creativity,  and  leadership',  in  The  Routledge  companion  to  creativity,  eds  T  Rickards,  MA  Runco  &  S  Moger,  Routledge,  New  York  pp.  247-­‐255.  

Skinner,  C  &  Spurgeon,  P  2005,  'Valuing  empathy  and  emotional  intelligence  in  health  leadership:  a  study  of  empathy,  leadership  behaviour  and  outcome  effectiveness',  Health  Services  Management  Research,  vol.  18,  no.  1,  pp.  1-­‐12.  

Page 256: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

254

Smith,  SE  1980,  Ego  development  and  the  problems  of  power  and  agreement  in  organizations,  DBA    thesis,  George  Washington  University.  

Snarey,  J,  Kohlberg,  L  &  Noam,  G  1983,  'Ego  development  in  perspective:  Structural  stage,  functional  phase,  and  cultural  age-­‐period  models',  Developmental  Review,  vol.  3,  no.  3,  pp.  303-­‐338.  

Snarey,  J  &  Lydens,  L  1990,  'Worker  equality  and  adult  development:  the  kibbutz  as  a  developmental  model',  Psychology  and  Aging,  vol.  5,  no.  1,  pp.  86-­‐93.  

Sommers,  S  1981,  'Emotionality  reconsidered:  the  role  of  cognition  in  emotional  responsiveness',  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  vol.  41,  no.  3,  pp.  553-­‐561.  

Sosik,  JJ  &  Megerian,  LE  1999,  'Understanding  leader  emotional  intelligence  and  performance:  the  role  of  self-­‐other  agreement  on  transformational  leadership  perceptions',  Group  &  Organization  Management,  vol.  24,  no.  3,  pp.  367-­‐390.  

Soutar,  G  &  Clarke,  Y  1981,  'Life  style  and  television  viewing  behaviour  in  Perth,  Western  Australia',  Australian  Journal  of  Management,  vol.  6,  no.  2,  pp.  110-­‐123.  

Spearman,  C  1904,  'General  intelligence,  objectively  determined  and  measured',  The  American  Journal  of  Psychology,  vol.  15,  no.  2,  pp.  201-­‐292.  

Spearman,  C  1927,  The  abilities  of  man,  Macmilan,  New  York.  Spence,  KK  2005,  An  empirical  exploration  of  the  relationship  between  

transformational  leadership  and  stages  of  ego  development,  PhD    thesis,  University  of  Massachusetts  Amherst.  

Spillett,  MA  1995,  Women  student  leader's  constructions  of  leadership:  a  developmental  perspective,  PhD    thesis,  Harvard  Graduate  School  of  Education.  

Steeves,  RA  1997,  Why  leaders  are  effective:  an  examination  into  leader  developmental  level  and  leader–follower  developmental  fit  as  predictors  of  effective  leadership,  PhD    thesis,  The  Fielding  Institute.  

Sternberg,  R  1990,  Metaphors  of  mind:  conceptions  of  the  nature  of  intelligence,  Cambridge  University  Press.  

Sternberg,  R  2005,  'There  are  no  public-­‐policy  implications:  a  reply  to  Rushton  and  Jensen  (2005)',  Psychology,  Public  Policy,  and  Law,  vol.  11,  no.  2,  pp.  295-­‐301.  

Sternberg,  R,  Wagner,  R,  Williams,  W  &  Horvath,  J  1995,  'Testing  common  sense',  American  Psychologist,  vol.  50,  no.  11,  pp.  912-­‐927.  

Stogdill,  R  1948,  'Personal  factors  associated  with  leadership:  a  survey  of  the  literature',  Journal  of  Psychology:  Interdisciplinary  and  Applied,  vol.  25,  no.  1,  pp.  35-­‐71.  

Stogdill,  RM  1963,  Manual  for  the  leader  behavior  description  questionnaire-­‐form  XII,  Bureau  of  Business  Research,  College  of  Commerce  and  Administration,  Ohio  State  University,  Columbus.  

Strang,  SE  2006,  Big  five  personality  and  leadership  development  levels  as  predictors  of  leader  performance,  Master's    thesis,  University  of  Georgia,  Athens.  

Sullivan,  C,  Grant,  MQ  &  Grant,  JD  1957,  'The  development  of  interpersonal  maturity:  applications  to  delinquency',  Psychiatry:  Journal  for  the  Study  of  Interpersonal  Processes,  vol.  20,  no.  4,  pp.  373-­‐386.  

Page 257: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

255

Sundet,  J,  Barlaug,  D  &  Torjussen,  T  2004,  'The  end  of  the  Flynn  effect?  A  study  of  secular  trends  in  mean  intelligence  test  scores  of  Norwegian  conscripts  during  half  a  century',  Intelligence,  vol.  32,  no.  4,  pp.  349-­‐362.  

Sutton,  PM  &  Swensen,  CH  1983,  'The  reliability  and  concurrent  validity  of  alternative  methods  for  assessing  ego  development',  Journal  of  Personality  Assessment,  vol.  47,  no.  5,  pp.  468-­‐475.  

Sweeney,  D  1991,  A  study  of  the  relationship  between  the  level  of  ego  development  and  chosen  leadership  styles  of  school  principals,  PhD    thesis,  University  of  Denver.  

Sy,  T,  Tram,  S  &  O’Hara,  LA  2006,  'Relation  of  employee  and  manager  emotional  intelligence  to  job  satisfaction  and  performance',  Journal  of  Vocational  Behavior,  vol.  68,  no.  3,  pp.  461-­‐473.  

Tabachnick,  B  &  Fidell,  L  2007,  Using  multivariate  statistics,  5th  edn,  Pearson,  Sydney.  Tacey,  D  2003,  Spirituality  revolution,  Harper  Collins,  Sydney.  Taylor,  GS,  Garver,  MS  &  Williams,  Z  2010,  'Owner  operators:  employing  a  

segmentation  approach  to  improve  retention',  International  Journal  of  Logistics  Management,  The,  vol.  21,  no.  2,  pp.  207-­‐229.  

Taylor,  SE  1983,  'Adjustment  to  threatening  events:  A  theory  of  cognitive  adaptation',  American  Psychologist,  vol.  38,  no.  11,  p.  1161.  

Thomas,  R  2008,  Crucibles  of  leadership:  how  to  learn  from  experience  to  become  a  great  leader,  Harvard  Business  Press,  Boston.  

Thomas,  RW,  Soutar,  GN  &  Ryan,  MM  2001,  'The  selling  orientation-­‐customer  orientation  (SOCO)  scale:  A  proposed  short  form',  The  Journal  of  Personal  Selling  and  Sales  Management,  pp.  63-­‐69.  

Thompson,  SC  &  Janigian,  AS  1988,  'Life  schemes:  A  framework  for  understanding  the  search  for  meaning',  Journal  of  Social  and  Clinical  Psychology,  vol.  7,  no.  2-­‐3,  pp.  260-­‐280.  

Thorndike,  EL  1920,  'Intelligence  and  its  uses',  Harper's,  vol.  140,  pp.  227-­‐235.  Thurstone,  LL  1938,  Primary  mental  abilities,  The  University  of  Chicago  Press,  Chicago.  Tinsley,  H  &  Weiss,  D  1975,  'Interrater  reliability  and  agreement  of  subjective  

judgments',  Journal  of  Counseling  Psychology,  vol.  22,  no.  4,  pp.  358-­‐376.  Tischler,  L,  Biberman,  J  &  McKeage,  R  2002,  'Linking  emotional  intelligence,spirituality  

and  workplace  performance',  Journal  of  Managerial  Psychology,  vol.  17,  no.  3,  p.  203.  

Toor,  SR  &  Ofori,  G  2008,  'Leadership  versus  management:  how  they  are  different,  and  why',  Leadership  and  Management  in  Engineering,  vol.  8,  no.  2,  pp.  61-­‐71.  

Torbert,  W  &  Fisher,  D  1992,  'Autobiographical  awareness  as  a  catalyst  for  managerial  and  organisational  development',  Management  Learning,  vol.  23,  no.  3,  pp.  184-­‐198.  

Torbert,  W  &  Livne-­‐Tarandach,  R  2009,  'Reliability  and  validity  tests  of  the  Harthill  Leadership  Development  Profile  in  the  context  of  Developmental  Action  Inquiry  theory,  practice  and  method',  Integral  Review,  vol.  5,  no.  2,  pp.  133-­‐151.  

Page 258: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

256

Torbert,  WR  1983,  'Identifying  and  cultivating  professional  effectiveness:  'bureaucratic  action'  at  one  professional  business  school',  Annual  Meeting  for  the  American  Society  for  Public  Administration.  

Torbert,  WR  1985,  'On-­‐line  reframing:  an  integrative  approach  to  organizational  management',  Organizational  Dynamics,  vol.  14,  no.  1,  pp.  60-­‐79.  

Torbert,  WR  1987,  Managing  the  corporate  dream:  restructuring  for  long-­‐term  success,  Dow  Jones-­‐Irwin,  Homewood,  IL.  

Torbert,  WR  1994,  'Cultivating  postformal  adult  development:  higher  stages  and  contrasting  interventions',  Transcendence  and  mature  thought  in  adulthood:  the  further  reaches  of  adult  development,  pp.  181-­‐203.  

Torbert,  WR  1996,  'The  'chaotic'  action  awareness  of  transformational  leaders',  International  Journal  of  Public  Administration,  vol.  19,  no.  6,  pp.  911-­‐939.  

Vaill,  P  1998,  Spirited  leading  and  learning,  Jossey-­‐Bass  Publishers,  San  Francisco.  Van  Rooy,  DL,  Alonso,  A  &  Viswesvaran,  C  2005,  'Group  differences  in  emotional  

intelligence  scores:  theoretical  and  practical  implications',  Personality  and  Individual  Differences,  vol.  38,  no.  3,  pp.  689-­‐700.  

Van  Rooy,  DL  &  Viswesvaran,  C  2004,  'Emotional  intelligence:  a  meta-­‐analytic  investigation  of  predictive  validity  and  nomological  net',  Journal  of  Vocational  Behaviour,  vol.  65,  pp.  71-­‐95.  

Van  Rooy,  DL,  Viswesvaran,  C  &  Pluta,  P  2005,  'An  evaluation  of  construct  validity:  what  is  this  thing  called  emotional  intelligence?',  Human  Performance,  vol.  18,  no.  4,  pp.  445-­‐462.  

Vermunt,  JK  &  Magidson,  J  2005,  Latent  Gold  4.0  User's  Guide,  Statistical  Innovations  Inc,  Belmont  Massachusetts.  

Vernon,  PE  1933,  'Some  characteristics  of  the  good  judge  of  personality',  Journal  of  Social  Psychology,  vol.  4,  pp.  42-­‐57.  

Vilkinas,  T  &  Cartan,  G  2001,  'The  behavioural  control  room  for  managers:  the  integrator  role',  Leadership  &  Organization  Development  Journal,  vol.  22,  no.  4,  pp.  175-­‐185.  

Vilkinas,  T  &  Cartan,  G  2006,  'The  integrated  competing  values  framework:  its  spatial  configuration',  Journal  of  Management  Development,  vol.  25,  no.  6,  pp.  505-­‐521.  

Viswesvaran,  C  &  Ones,  D  2002,  'Agreements  and  disagreements  on  the  role  of  general  mental  ability  (GMA)  in  industrial,  work,  and  organizational  psychology',  Human  Performance,  vol.  15,  no.  1,  pp.  211-­‐231.  

Walter,  F,  Cole,  M  &  Humphrey,  R  2011,  'Emotional  intelligence:  sine  qua  non  of  leadership  or  folderol?',  Academy  of  Management  Perspectives,  vol.  25,  no.  1,  pp.  45-­‐59.  

Warwick,  J  &  Nettelbeck,  T  2004,  'Emotional  intelligence  is....?',  Personality  and  Individual  Differences,  vol.  37,  no.  5,  pp.  1091-­‐1100.  

Wechsler,  D  1944,  The  measurement  of  adult  intelligence,  Williams  &  Wilkins,  Baltimore.  

Wedeck,  J  1947,  'The  relationship  between  personality  and  psychological  ability',  British  Journal  of  Psychology,  vol.  37,  pp.  133-­‐151.  

Page 259: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

257

Weinberger,  LA  2003,  An  examination  of  the  relationship  between  emotional  intelligence,  leadership  style  and  perceived  leadership  effectiveness,  PhD  thesis,  University  of  Minnesota.  

Weinberger,  LA  2009,  'Emotional  intelligence,  leadership  style,  and  perceived  leadership  effectiveness',  Advances  in  Developing  Human  Resources,  vol.  11,  no.  6,  pp.  747-­‐772    

Weiss,  D,  Zilberg,  N  &  Genevro,  J  1989,  'Psychometric  properties  of  Loevinger  s  Sentence  Completion  Test  in  an  adult  psychiatric  outpatient  sample',  Journal  of  Personality  Assessment,  vol.  53,  no.  3,  pp.  478-­‐486.  

Westenberg,  PM  &  Block,  J  1993,  'Ego  development  and  individual  differences  in  personality',  Journal  of  Personality  and  Social  Psychology,  vol.  65,  pp.  792-­‐792.  

Wheatley,  M  2009,  Turning  to  one  another:  Simple  conversations  to  restore  hope  to  the  future,  Berrett-­‐Koehler  Publishers  Inc.,  San  Francisco.  

Wheatley,  MJ  2006,  Leadership  and  the  new  science:  Discovering  order  in  a  chaotic  world,  Berrett-­‐Koehler  Publishers  Inc.,  San  Francisco.  

Wilber,  K  1993,  The  spectrum  of  consciousness,  2nd  edn,  Quest  Books,  Wheaton,  IL.  Wilber,  K  1996,  A  brief  history  of  everything,  1  edn,  8  vols,  Shambala    Publications,  

Boston  &  London.  Wilber,  K  2000,  A  Brief  History  of  Everything,  1  edn,  8  vols,  Shambala    Publications,  

Boston  &  London.  Wonderlic  &  Associates  2002,  Wonderlic  Personnel  Test  &  Scholastic  Level  Exam  user's  

manual,  Wonderlic,  Inc.,  Libertyville,  Illinois.  Wong,  CS  &  Law,  KS  2002,  'The  effects  of  leader  and  follower  emotional  intelligence  on  

performance  and  attitude:  an  exploratory  study',  The  Leadership  Quarterly,  vol.  13,  no.  3,  pp.  243-­‐274.  

Wyse,  A  &  Vilkinas,  T  2004,  'Executive  leadership  roles  in  the  Australian  public  service',  Women  in  Management  Review,  vol.  19,  no.  4,  pp.  205-­‐211.  

Yammarino,  FJ  &  Atwater,  LE  1997,  'Do  managers  see  themselves  as  other  see  them?  Implications  of  self-­‐other  rating  agreement  for  human  resources  management',  Organizational  Dynamics,  vol.  25,  no.  4,  pp.  35-­‐44.  

Young,  J  2002a,  'A  spectrum  of  consciousness  for  CEO's:  A  business  application  of  Ken  Wilber's  spectrum  of  consciousness',  The  International  Journal  of  Organizational  Analysis,  vol.  10,  no.  1,  pp.  30-­‐54.  

Young,  J  2002b,  'A  spectrum  of  consciousness  for  CEOs:  a  business  application  of  Ken  Wilber's  spectrum  of  consciousness',  The  International  Journal  of  Organizational  Analysis,  vol.  10,  no.  1,  pp.  30-­‐54.  

Young,  M  &  Dulewicz,  V  2008,  'Similarities  and  differences  between  leadership  and  management:  high  performance  competencies  in  the  British  Royal  Navy  ',  British  Journal  of  Management,  vol.  19,  pp.  17-­‐31.  

Yukl,  G  1989,  'Managerial  leadership:  a  review  of  theory  and  research',  Journal  of  Management  vol.  15,  no.  2,  pp.  251-­‐289.  

Yukl,  G  2006,  Leadership  in  organizations,  6th  Edition  edn,  Pearson  Prentice  Hall,  Upper  Saddle  River,  New  Jersey.  

Page 260: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

258

Zaccaro,  S  2007,  'Trait-­‐based  perspectives  of  leadership',  American  Psychologist,  vol.  62,  no.  1,  pp.  6-­‐16.  

Zaccaro,  S,  Kemp,  C  &  Bader,  P  2004,  'Leader  traits  and  attributes',  in  The  nature  of  leadership,  eds  J  Antonakis,  A  Cianciolo  &  R  Sternberg,  Sage  Publications,  Thousand  Oaks,  CA,  pp.  101-­‐124.  

Zeidner,  M,  Matthews,  G  &  Roberts,  RD  2004,  'Emotional  intelligence  in  the  workplace:  a  critical  review',  Applied  Psychology,  vol.  53,  no.  3,  pp.  371-­‐399.  

Zohar,  D  &  Marshall,  I  2000,  Spiritual  intelligence:  the  ultimate  intelligence,  Bloomsbury  Publishing,  London.  

 

Page 261: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

259

APPENDICES

Appendix A Studies Exploring Ability-EI and MLE

Appendix B Wonderlic Personnel Test Norms

Appendix C Information Sheet for Participants

Appendix D Invitation and Faxback Form

Appendix E Informed Consent Form

Appendix F Survey

Appendix G Central Tendency and Dispersion of rWG_un for 32 ILMDP Items

Appendix H Central Tendency and Dispersion of rWG_skew for 32 ILMDP Items

Appendix I Central Tendency and Dispersion of rWG_Mskew for 32 ILMDP Items

Appendix J Central Tendency and Dispersion of rWG_UN for the ILMDP Scales: 8 role scales, 2 function scales and a 32-item scale.

Appendix K Central Tendency and Dispersion of rWG_skew for the ILMDP Scales: 8 role scales, 2 function scales and a 32-item scale.

Appendix L Central Tendency and Dispersion of rWG_Mskew for the ILMDP Scales: 8 role scales, 2 function scales and a 32-item scale.

Appendix M Exploratory Factor Analysis of the MSCEITv2

Appendix N IRA on WUSCT Items

Appendix O Central Tendency and Dispersion of rwg_UN for Items in Revised LE Scale

Appendix P Central Tendency and Dispersion of rwg_skew for Items in Revised LE Scale

Appendix Q Central Tendency and Dispersion of rwg_Mskew for Items in Revised LE Scale

Appendix R Central Tendency and Dispersion of rwg_UN for Items in Revised ME Scale

Appendix S Central Tendency and Dispersion of rwg_skew for Items in Revised ME Scale

Appendix T Central Tendency and Dispersion of rwg_Mskew for Items in Revised ME Scale

Page 262: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

260

Studies Exploring Ability-EI and MLE

Study EI Measure

Sample Measure of Leadership Results of testing EI and MLE relationship

Collins (2001)

MSCEIT N= 91 Managers

Customised 360o feedback Relationship not supported

Leban & Zulauf (2004)

MSCEIT N=24

Project Managers

Transformational leadership (MLQ) other-raters average scores

Partial support

Rubin et al (2005)

DANVA

N=177 Transformational leadership style (MLQ) subordinate scores

Partial support

Rosete & Ciarriochi (2005)

MSCEIT N=41

Managers

Customised 360o feedback (supervisors)

Partial support

Kerr et al (2006)

MSCEIT N=38

Supervisors

Customised 360o feedback (subordinates)

Partial support

Byron (2007) DANVA N=112

Managers

Satisfaction with supervisor (Hackman and Oldham 1975) (subordinates)

Managerial performance (Mount 1984) (supervisors)

Direct relationship not supported.

Support for relationship when mediated by managers’ supportiveness and for relationships when mediated by managers’ persuasiveness.

Partial support for relationship when moderated by gender – holds for female managers only.

Jin et al (2008)

MSCEIT N=178

Managers

Transformational leadership (MLQ) (subordinate)

Support for relationship

Weinberger (2009)

MSCEIT

N=151

Managers

Transformational leadership (MLQ) (subordinate)

Relationship not supported

Cote et al (2010)

MSCEIT

N=138

(study 1)

N=165 (study 2)

Undergrad students

Leadership emergence (peers)

Support for relationship

Clarke (2010) MSCEIT N=67

Project Managers

Transformational leadership (MLQ) (self ratings)

Project manager competencies (self-ratings)

Support for relationship

Appendix A

Page 263: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

261

Wonderlic Personnel Test Norms

Test Scores Job Potential Education Potential

28 and over

Upper level management; only upper 17% of population score within this range.

College graduate mean IQ 120; WPT 29. Central tendency for graduate students is WPT 30.

26 to 30

Managerial potential and upper level clerical positions; 24% of the population score within this range; gathers information; analyses and makes decisions from a limited number of choices.

May enter college; mean score for college freshmen 1Q 115; WPT 24

20 to 26

General clerical and first line supervisors; able to train other for routine positions; gather information; may require help with making decisions. 29% of the population score within this range.

Mean for High School grads is IQ 110; WPT 21; Central tendency for College Freshmen WPT 24 - have a better than average chance of completing High School curriculum, 50/50 change of graduating from college.

16 to 22

Routine office worker can run routinized equipment; 27% of the population scores in this range. Given enough time can learn and perform jobs with length routinized steps; perform simple operations with lists of names and numbers.

May enter High School; will probably select classes which are on a less academic track; Central tendency for High School Junior, WPT 16.

10 to 17

Operate simple process equipment 21% of the population score within this range. Given ample time can learn limited number of steps for routinized jobs; if deviations occur on the job will have difficulty establishing or using contingencies.

Slightly better than average chance of reaching the 9th grade or entering high school. Central tendency for High School Sophmore is WPT 14, High School Freshmen WPT 13, 8th grade WPT 11.

12 or less

Use very simple tools and equipment; repair furniture; assist electrician; simple carpentry; domestic work. 13 % of the population score within this range. '

Armed forces IQ cut off score between 75-80. Central tendency for 7th grade WPT 9.

Appendix B

Page 264: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

262

INFORMATION SHEET

The Graduate School of Management The University of Western Australia 35 Stirling Highway CRAWLEY WA 6009 AUSTRALIA

RESEARCH PROJECT: Multiple Intelligences Underpinning Effective Leadership

The intention of this research is to explore the relationships between traditional intelligence (IQ), emotional intelligence (EI), spiritual intelligence (SI) and effective leadership. It is also intended to explore the concept of spiritual intelligence in more detail.

Who are the researchers in this study? This study is being conducted by Stacie Chappell, Lecturer and Doctoral Student at the GSM and Dr. Renu Burr, Lecturer at the Graduate School of Management (GSM).

Who can participate in this study? Participation in this study is limited to clients of the AIM-UWA Senior Management Centre (SMC) who have completed the Integral Leadership and Management Development 360 Profile (ILMDP 360). The ILMDP 360 results will provide a standard measure of leadership effectiveness for the testing of the research hypothesis.

What about confidentiality and security of the data? All data collected will be held in the strictest confidence adhering to UWA ethics standards, kept in secure premises at UWA and accessed only by the researcher and her supervisor. Upon completion of the research project, the data will be destroyed. All data will be coded to eliminate the need for using specific names in the database.

What do I have to do to ‘participate?’ Participants in this study will participate in one a 2hr data collection workshop. Over the course of the 2 hours, participants will complete:

• the Wonderlic Personnel Test, a measure of traditional cognitive intelligence (IQ);

• Mayer, Salovey and Caruso’s Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT), a measure of emotional intelligence (EI);

• Loevinger’s Sentence Completion Test, a measure of ego development;

• Psychomatrix Spirituality Indicator, (PSI) a profile of if/how you experience / incorporate spirituality into your life.

In addition, you will be asked to release the results of your ILMDP 360 for the purposes of this study.

When and where are the data collection workshops? The data collection workshop will be held at the UWA Graduate School of Management and will be scheduled at a time that suits you.

Appendix C Page 1 of 2

Page 265: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

263

What’s in it for me? If you are interested in your own leadership development, participating in this study will provide you with a number of direct benefits:

• You will receive a validated rating of your traditional IQ using one of the most common measures employed by corporate recruiters;

• You will receive a validated rating and individualised feedback report on your EI profile using one of the leading measures of EI;

• You will receive a validated rating and individualised feedback report on your SI profile; and

• You will receive a complimentary seat for the ½ day workshop titled ‘Leveraging EI and SI for Effective Leadership’. This workshop will be presented in the first ½ of 2004 and will debrief the results of the research and explore strategies for developing and leveraging your current levels of EI and SI.

As well, by participating in this study you will be contributing to the growing body of knowledge on EI and SI. It is important that we continue to explore these new concepts through research in order for them to become useful to us as practitioners. As a participant in this study, you will receive a summary report of the findings from the research.

Can I say yes now, and change my mind? Yes! It is important that you know that your participation in this study should be completely voluntary and you can withdraw your participation at any point during the process. If you should withdraw from the study, the data collected from you, if any, will be destroyed. Your participation in this study does not prejudice any right to compensation, which you may have under statute or common law.

Can I ask a few more questions? Yes! We would be happy to answer any and all questions you might have on this research project. For more information, or to register your interest in participating, please contact:

Stacie Chappell

Lecturer and Doctoral Student

UWA Graduate School of Management

[email protected] 61 8 6488 1019 phone

61 8 6488 1072 fax

Thanks in advance for your consideration!

Appendix C Page 2of 2

Page 266: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

Invitation and Faxback Form

The Graduate School of Management The University of Western Australia 35 Stirling Highway CRAWLEY WA 6009 AUSTRALIA PH: 61 8 6488 1019 FX: 61 8 6488 1072

Dear ILMDP 360 Client,

Invitation to Participate in Research Study on Multiple Intelligences and Leadership Effectiveness

The UWA Graduate School of Management is conducting a study on the role of multiple intelligence and effective leadership. Specifically, we want to explore the role of traditional intelligence, emotional intelligence and spiritual intelligence on leadership effectiveness. Because you have recently completed the Integral Leadership and Management 360 Profile (ILMDP), we would like to invite you to participate in this study. This would consist of:

• participating in a 2 hr data collection workshop; and • enabling us to access your ILMDP profile for the purposes of this research study.

PLEASE NOTE: All information gathered for the use of this study will be held in the strictest of confidence. Best practice security measures are used in the storage of hard copy and soft copy data. All data is coded and specific reference to individuals is not necessary.

In addition to contributing to our understanding of multiple intelligence and leadership effectiveness, you would gain:

• individualised feedback on each of the three intelligences measured; • access to a free ½ day workshop on multiple intelligences and leadership

effectiveness; and • a summary report of the research findings.

If you would like to sign up to participate, or would like more information on the proposed study, please complete the enclosed faxback form and return it to us. Alternatively, you can contact us via Stacie Chappell at [email protected] or 6488 1019. Thanks in advance for your consideration and time. Renu Burr Stacie Chappell Lecturer, UWA GSM Doctoral Student, UWA GSM

Appendix D Page 1 of 2

Page 267: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

265

The Graduate School of Management The University of Western Australia 35 Stirling Highway CRAWLEY WA 6009 AUSTRALIA

Fax Back Form To: Stacie Chappell and Renu Burr

UWA Graduate School of Management Fax: 9380 1072

From: Date:

Pages: 1

Re: Research Study on Multiple Intelligences and Effective Leadership

I would like to participate in this study, please reserve a spot for me in the following data collection workshop (Circle one):

Tuesday 23 March 3-5pm Tuesday 23 March 6-8pm

Tuesday 30 March 3-5pm Tuesday 30 March 6-8pm

Tuesday 6 April 3-5pm Tuesday 6 April 6-8pm

Tuesday 13 April 3-5pm Tuesday 13 April 6-8pm

Tuesday 20 April 3-5pm Tuesday 20 April 6-8pm

Tuesday 27 April 3-5pm Tuesday 27 April 6-8pm

Tuesday 4 May 3-5pm Tuesday 4 May 6-8pm

Tuesday 18 May 3-5pm Tuesday 18 May 6-8pm

Tuesday 25 May 3-5pm Tuesday 25 May 6-8pm

Tuesday 1 June 3-5pm Tuesday 1 June 6-8pm

Tuesday 8 June 3-5pm Tuesday 8 June 6-8pm

Tuesday 22 June 3-5pm Tuesday 22 June 6-8pm

Tuesday 29 June 3-5pm Tuesday 29 June 6-8pm NOTE: spaces are limited to 10 people per session. As such, we will confirm your spot (via phone or email) or contact you to reschedule if your preference is not available.

My contact details are as follows:

Daytime phone contact: Email:

I would like to speak with someone in more detail about this research project.

Appendix D Page 2 of 2

Page 268: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

CONSENT FORM

UWA Business School The University of Western Australia 35 Stirling Highway CRAWLEY WA 6009 AUSTRALIA

VOLUNTARY PARTICIPATION IN RESEARCH STUDY:

Multiple Intelligences Underpinning Effective Leadership I (the participant) have read the information provided and any questions I have asked

have been answered to my satisfaction. I agree to participate in this activity, realising that I may withdraw at any time without reason and without prejudice.

I have given permission for the researcher to access my 360 feedback and/or MSCEIT

results. I understand that all information provided is treated as strictly confidential and will not be released by the investigator unless required to by law. I have been advised as to what data is being collected, what the purpose is, and what will be done with the data upon completion of the research.

I agree that research data gathered for the study may be published provided my name or other identifying information is not used.

____________________________________________________________________ Participant Name Signature Date

(Please note that as this document is not a contract between parties, it is not necessary that the researcher sign it. Nor is it necessary to have a witness.)

The Human Research Ethics Committee at the University of Western Australia requires that all participants are informed that, if they have any complaint regarding the manner, in which a research project is conducted, it may be given to the researcher or, alternatively to the Secretary, Human Research Ethics Committee, Registrar’s Office, University of Western Australia, 35 Stirling Highway, Crawley, WA 6009 (telephone number 6488-3703). All study participants will be provided with a copy of the Information Sheet and Consent Form for their personal records.

Copies of this form are available for you to take home.

For more information on this research project, please contact:

Stacie Chappell Lecturer and Doctoral Student

UWA Business School [email protected]

61 8 6488 1019 phone 61 8 6488 1004 fax

Appendix E

Page 269: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

267

Appendix F

Page 1 of 9

Page 270: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

268

Appendix F

Page 2 of 9

Page 271: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

269

Appendix F Page 3 of 9

Page 272: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

270

Appendix F Page 4 of 9

Page 273: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

271

Appendix F Page 5 of 9

Page 274: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

272

Appendix F Page 6 of 9

Page 275: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

273

Appendix F

Page 7 of 9

Page 276: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

274

Appendix F Page 8 of 9

Page 277: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

275

Appendix F

Page 9 of 9

Page 278: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

276

Central Tendency and Dispersion of rWG_un for ILMDP 32-Items

ILMDP Item # Mean Median Mode Std.

Deviation Minimum Maximum

1 .71 .80 .87 .27 -.66 1.00

2 .76 .85 .92 .25 -.66 1.00

3 .80 .88 .90 .23 -.73 1.00

4 .81 .89 .92 .21 -.54 1.00

5 .81 .87 .92* .18 -.43 1.00

6 .71 .83 .92 .28 -.62 1.00

7 .72 .82 .92 .26 -.36 1.00

8 .72 .81 .96 .26 -.58 1.00

9 .75 .82 .92 .24 -.31 1.00

10 .80 .86 .92 .19 -.07 1.00

11 .67 .76 .92 .28 -.54 1.00

12 .65 .73 .92 .30 -.89 1.00

13 .78 .85 .92 .22 -.54 1.00

14 .76 .83 .92* .20 -.18 .98

15 .69 .79 .92 .27 -.66 1.00

16 .74 .84 .85 .25 -.48 1.00

17 .72 .81 .92 .28 -1.02 1.00

18 .79 .88 .92 .25 -.74 1.00

19 .78 .84 .92 .20 -.15 1.00

20 .77 .83 .92 .21 -.09 1.00

21 .77 .85 .92 .24 -.70 1.00

22 .76 .83 .92 .24 -.49 1.00

23 .70 .78 .92 .27 -.72 1.00

24 .71 .81 .89 .28 -.58 1.00

25 .75 .83 .92 .25 -.40 1.00

26 .70 .79 .79 .29 -.66 1.00

27 .74 .82 .90* .24 -.25 1.00

28 .76 .84 .90* .24 -.66 1.00

29 .78 .84 .90* .22 -.64 1.00

30 .72 .81 .97 .27 -.66 1.00

31 .76 .85 .90 .26 -.81 1.00

32 .78 .84 .84 .21 -.45 1.00

*  Multiple  modes  exist.  The  smallest  value  is  shown.

Appendix G

Page 279: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

277

Central Tendency and Dispersion of rWG_skew for ILMDP 32-Items

ILMDP Item # Mean Median Mode Std.

Deviation Minimum Maximum

1 .62 .74 .83 .36 -1.17 1.00

2 .68 .80 .89 .33 -1.17 1.00

3 .74 .85 .87 .30 -1.27 1.00

4 .75 .85 .89 .28 -1.02 1.00

5 .75 .82 .89* .23 -.87 1.00

6 .62 .78 .89 .37 -1.12 1.00

7 .64 .76 .89 .34 -.78 1.00

8 .64 .75 .95 .34 -1.06 1.00

9 .67 .76 .89 .31 -.71 1.00

10 .74 .82 .89 .25 -.40 1.00

11 .57 .68 .89 .37 -1.02 1.00

12 .54 .65 .89 .40 -1.47 1.00

13 .72 .81 .89 .29 -1.02 1.00

14 .69 .78 .89* .27 -.55 .97

15 .60 .73 .89 .36 -1.17 1.00

16 .66 .79 .81 .33 -.94 1.00

17 .63 .75 .89 .37 -1.65 1.00

18 .72 .84 .89 .33 -1.28 1.00

19 .71 .79 .89 .26 -.50 1.00

20 .70 .78 .89 .28 -.43 1.00

21 .70 .81 .89 .32 -1.22 1.00

22 .68 .78 .89 .31 -.96 1.00

23 .61 .71 .89 .35 -1.25 1.00

24 .62 .75 .85 .37 -1.06 1.00

25 .67 .78 .89 .33 -.84 1.00

26 .60 .73 .73 .38 -1.17 1.00

27 .66 .77 .87* .31 -.64 1.00

28 .68 .79 .87* .32 -1.17 1.00

29 .71 .79 .87* .28 -1.14 1.00

30 .63 .75 .96 .36 -1.17 1.00

31 .69 .81 .87 .34 -1.37 1.00

32 .71 .79 .79 .28 -.90 1.00

*  Multiple  modes  exist.  The  smallest  value  is  shown.    

Appendix H

Page 280: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

278

Central Tendency and Dispersion of rWG_Mskew for ILMDP 32-Items

ILMDP Item # Mean Median Mode Std.

Deviation Minimum Maximum

1 .53 .68 .79 .44 -1.69 1.00

2 .61 .75 .87 .40 -1.69 1.00

3 .68 .81 .84 .38 -1.81 1.00

4 .69 .81 .87 .34 -1.50 1.00

5 .69 .78 .87* .29 -1.32 1.00

6 .54 .73 .87 .45 -1.63 1.00

7 .55 .71 .86 .43 -1.20 1.00

8 .55 .69 .94 .43 -1.55 1.00

9 .59 .71 .87 .39 -1.12 1.00

10 .68 .78 .87 .31 -.73 1.00

11 .47 .61 .86 .46 -1.50 1.00

12 .43 .56 .87 .49 -2.06 1.00

13 .65 .76 .86 .35 -1.50 1.00

14 .61 .73 .86* .33 -.92 .97

15 .50 .66 .87 .44 -1.69 1.00

16 .58 .74 .76 .41 -1.40 1.00

17 .55 .69 .86 .45 -2.27 1.00

18 .65 .80 .87 .41 -1.82 1.00

19 .64 .74 .86 .32 -.86 1.00

20 .63 .73 .87 .34 -.77 1.00

21 .62 .76 .86 .40 -1.75 1.00

22 .61 .73 .86 .38 -1.42 1.00

23 .52 .64 .86 .43 -1.79 1.00

24 .53 .69 .82 .45 -1.55 1.00

25 .60 .73 .87 .41 -1.28 1.00

26 .51 .67 .67 .47 -1.69 1.00

27 .58 .71 .84* .39 -1.03 1.00

28 .61 .74 .84* .39 -1.69 1.00

29 .64 .74 .84* .35 -1.65 1.00

30 .54 .69 .95 .44 -1.69 1.00

31 .61 .76 .84 .42 -1.93 1.00

32 .65 .74 .74 .35 -1.36 1.00

*  Multiple  modes  exist.  The  smallest  value  is  shown.  

Appendix I

Page 281: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

279

Central Tendency and Dispersion of rWG_UN for ILMDP Subscales: eight roles,

two functions and the full 32-item scale

Mean Median Mode

Std. Deviation

Minimum Maximum

Management Function .95 .97 .95* .07 .03 1.00

Broker .90 .94 .98* .15 -1.00 1.00

Director .88 .94 .85* .28 -3.72 .99

Achiever .92 .95 .95* .09 .33 .99

Monitor .92 .95 .95 .10 .00 .99

Leadership Function .94 .97 .96* .11 -.15 1.00

Facilitator .89 .93 .93 .22 -.54 3.54

Visioner .89 .94 .97 .15 -.20 .99

Steward .90 .95 .96 .25 -2.56 1.00

Coach .87 .93 .93* .26 -2.58 .99

32-Item Scale .98 .99 .99* .05 .12 1.00

* Multiple modes exist. The smallest value is shown.

Appendix J

Page 282: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

280

Central Tendency and Dispersion of rWG_skew for ILMDP Subscales: eight roles,

two functions and the full 32-item scale

Mean Median Mode

Std. Deviation

Minimum Maximum

Management Function .91 .95 .91* .23 -1.67 3.12

Broker .84 .92 .97* .44 -3.88 4.06

Director .68 .92 .77* 3.46 -59.76 16.61

Achiever .86 .93 .93* .23 -1.31 .99

Monitor .81 .93 .93 1.01 -17.33 .99

Leadership Function .90 .95 .93* .48 -6.06 3.81

Facilitator .78 .90 .9 1.19 -11.59 13.78

Visioner .85 .91 .97 1.52 -17.33 16.61

Steward .90 .94 .95 .49 -1.82 7.62

Coach -.04 .91 .91* 17.60 -334.67 6.67

32 Item Scale 1.01 .99 .99* .34 .64 7.15

* Multiple modes exist. The smallest value is shown.

Appendix K

Page 283: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

281

Central Tendency and Dispersion of rWG_Mkew for ILMDP Subscales: eight roles,

two functions and the full 32-item scale

Mean Median Mode

Std. Deviation

Minimum Maximum

Management Function .96 .97 .95 .29 -2.37 3.59

Broker .95 .91 .96 1.73 -1.79 26.94

Director .87 .90 .66 1.05 -5.93 16.76

Achiever .90 .91 .91 1.23 -6.56 19.93

Monitor .84 .91 .91 .50 -7.09 3.40

Leadership Function .94 .97 .96 .29 -3.62 2.00

Facilitator .19 .89 .86 1.74 -192.57 11.94

Visioner .81 .90 .96 1.70 -27.55 9.41

Steward .89 .92 .93 .46 -2.85 5.97

Coach .69 .88 .88 1.04 -15.22 2.74

32-Item Scale .98 .99 .98 .08 .54 2.04

* Multiple modes exist. The smallest value is shown.

Appendix L

Page 284: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

Exploratory Factor Analysis on MSCEIT Task A

# items

chi-square

df p< TLI CFI RMSEA

NOTES

Step 1: 20 640.851 170 .001 .680 .714 .104 deleted items < .4 (#1, 7, 12 & 16) run with all items

Step 2: 16 499.314 104 .001 .699 .739 .112 std resid over 2.58 for: 8&13, 11&13, 17&19, 19&20, 17&18, 17&9, 2&4, 11&15

deleted items #1, 7, 12 & 16

MI highest for 19&20

covary 19&20 and deleted 19 Bc lowest loading

Step 3: 15 397.907 90 .001 .742 .779 .112 std resid over 2.58 for: 8&13, 11/&13, 17&18, 18&20, 17&20, 15&11, 2&4

deleted 19

MI highest for 17&18

covary 17&18 and deleted 17 Bc lowest loading

Step 4: 14 288.868 77 .001 .787 .819 .104 std resid over 2.58 for: 8&13, 11&13, 18&20, 15&11, 2&4 deleted 17

MI for 18&20 highest

covary 18&20 and deleted 18 bc lowest loading Step 5: 13 243.734 65 .001 .799 .832 .104 std resid over 2.58 for: 8&13, 11&13, 15&11, 2&/4 deleted 18

MI for 11&15 highest

covary 11&15 and deleted 11 bc lowest loading Step 6: 12 186.866 54 .001 .831 .862 .098 std resid over 2.58 for: 8&13, 2&4 deleted 11

MI for 2&4 highest

covary 2&4 and deleted 2 bc lowest loading Step 7: 11 131.898 44 .001 .871 .897 .089 std resid over 2.58 for: 8&13 deleted 2

MI for 8&13 highest

covary8&13 and deleted 13 bc lowest loading Step 8: 10 99.087 35 .001 .895 .918 .085 std resid all < 2.58 deleted 13

MI for 8&9 highest

covary 8&9 and deleted 8 bc lowest loading Step 9: 9 74.166 27 .001 .908 .931 .083 std resid all < 2.58 deleted 8

MI for 9&15 highest

covary 9&15 and deleted 15 bc lowest loading Step 10: 8 47.386 20 .001 .935 .954 .073 std resid all < 2.58 deleted 15

MI for 3&10 highest

covary 3&10and deleted 10 bc lowest loading Step 11 7 25.478 14 .030 .964 .976 .057 std resid all < 2.58 deleted 10

MI for 4&6highest

covary 4&6 and deleted 6 bc lowest loading Step 12: 6 1.534 9 .309 .994 .996 .026 accept fit deleted 6

* EFA steps and cut-off criteria were based on processes outlined by Byrne (2010)

Appendix M Page 1 of 8

Page 285: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

283

Exploratory Factor Analysis on MSCEIT Task B

# items

chi-square df p< TLI CFI RMSEA

NOTES

Step 1: 15 177.09 90 .001 .564 .626 .062

deleted items <.3 loading (3,4, 5, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13, 14)

Run with all items

Step 2: 6 21.809 9 .010 .835 .901 .075 std resid over 2.58 for 1&7 (2.613) - deleted items 3,4, 5, 9, 10,

MI for 1&7 highest

11, 12, 13, 14

Covary 1& 7 and deleted #7

Step 3:

Deleted 7 5 4.822 5 .438 1.003 1.000 .000 accept model fit - 1 Step 4:

deleted 1 4 .179 2 .914 1.068 1.000 .000 accept model fit - 2 Step 5:

deleted 15 replaced 1 4 3.65 2 .161 .940 .980 .057 accept model fit - 3 Step 6:

deleted 1&15, 3 1.836 1 .175 .953 .984 .057 accept model fit - 4 error on 2&6 set to unity

* EFA steps and cut-off criteria were based on processes outlined by Byrne (2010)

Appendix M Page 2 of 8

Page 286: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

284

Exploratory Factor Analysis on MSCEIT Task C

# items

chi-square df p< TLI CFI RMSEA

NOTES

Step 1: 20 212.66 170 .015 .798 .820 .031 deleted items loading < .3 (2, 3, 5, 8,

Run with all items 11, 13, 16, 17, 18 and 19) Step 2: 9 33.401 27 .184 .944 .958 .031 no std resid > 2.5ish 2, 3, 5, 8, 11, 12, 13, 16,

MI for 1-14 highest

17, 18 and 19 deleted

delete # 1 with lower std reg weight

Step 3: 8 24.093 20 .238 .959 .971 .028 no std resid > 2.5ish Deleted 1

MI for 7-15 highest

delete #7 with lower std reg weight Step 4: 7 16.226 14 .300 .971 .981 .025 no std resid > 2.5ish Deleted 7

MI for 4 - 20 highest

delete # 20 with lower std reg weight

Step 5: 6 8.199 9 .514 1.014 1.000 .000 no std resid > 2.5ish Deleted 20

MI for 4-9 highest

delete 9 with lower std reg weight Step 6: 5 2.35 5 .799 1.065 1.000 .000 no std resid > 2.5ish Deleted 9

MI for 4-10 highest

delete #4 with lower std reg weight

Step 7: 4 .604 2 .793 1.061 1.000 .000 Accept model fit - 1 Deleted 4

no std resid > 2.5ishMI for 10-15 highest

delete #10 with lower std reg weight

Step 8: 3 1.624 1 .203 .967 .989 .050 Accept model fit - 2 Deleted 10, set variance of

6&15 to unity

* EFA steps and cut-off criteria were based on processes outlined by Byrne (2010)

Appendix M Page 3 of 8

Page 287: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

285

Exploratory Factor Analysis on MSCEIT Task D

# items

chi-square df p< TLI CFI RMSEA

NOTES

Step 1: 20 262.35 170 .001 .496 .549 .046 deleted items with < .3 loading (#1, 2, 4, 5, 6, 7, 9,

10, 11, 12, 13, 18, 19) Run with all items

Step 2: 7 26.754 14 .021 .813 .875 .060 std resid > 2.5ish for 15/16 (3.037)

Deleted: 1,2,4,5,6,7,9,10, 11,

MI for 15&16highest

12,13,18,19

covary 15&16 and deleted 15 bc lowest loading Step 3: 6 5.971 9 .743 1.068 1.000 .000 no std resid > 2.5ish Deleted #15

MI for 3&8 highest

covary 3&8 and deleted 3 bc lowest loading Step 4: 5 2.083 5 .838 1.094 1.000 .000 no std resid > 2.5ish Deleted #3

MI for 16&20 highest

covary 16&20 and deleted 20 bc lowest loading

Step 5: 4 1.046 2 .593 1.054 1.000 .000 Accept Model Fit- 1 Deleted 20

no std resid > 2.5ish, MI for 8&14 highest

covary 8&14 and deleted 14 bc lowest loading Step 6: 3 7.262 1 .007 .585 .862 .157

Deleted 14

set variance on 8&16 equal

* EFA steps and cut-off criteria were based on processes outlined by Byrne (2010)

Appendix M Page 4 of 8

Page 288: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

286

Exploratory Factor Analysis on MSCEIT Task E

# items

chi-square df p< TLI CFI RMSEA

NOTES

Step 1: 30 1111.27 405 .001 .592 .620 .083 Deleted items < .40 (#1,2,4,5,6, 8,10,11,18,21,26,27)

run with all items

Step 2: 18 461.13 135 .001 .723 .755 .097 std resid > 2.518 for 28&30, 24&25, 22&25, 14&29, 7&9 and 13&14

#1,2,4,5,,6,8,10,11,18,21,26,27

MI for 7&9 highest deleted above items

delete # 9 with lower std reg weight & reran model

Step 3: 17 338.027 119 .001 .787 .813 .085 std resid > 2.518 for 28&30, and 14&/29 deleted #9

MI for 28&30 highest

delete #28 with lower std reg weight & reran model

Step 4: 16 271.733 104 .001 .811 .836 .080 std resid > 2.518 for 14&29 deleted #28

MI for 14&29 highest

delete #29 with lower std reg weight & reran model

Step 5: 15 223.1 90 .001 .837 .860 .076 no std resid > 2.5ish deleted #29

MI for 13&14 highest

delete #14 with lower std reg weight & reran model

Step 6: 14 181.453 77 .001 .859 .880 .073 no std resid > 2.5ish deleted #14

MI for 20&22 highest

delete #22 with lower std reg weight & reran model

Step 7: 13 145.962 65 .001 .880 .900 .070 no std resid > 2.5ish deleted #22

MI for 24&25 highest

delete #25 with lower std reg weight & reran model

Step 8: 12 105.937 54 .001 .914 .929 .061 no std resid > 2.5ish deleted #25

MI for 23&24 highest

delete #24 with lower std reg weight & reran model

Step 9: 11 76.096 44 .002 .933 .947 .053 no std resid > 2.5ish deleted #24

MI for 19&23 highest

delete #19 with lower std reg weight & reran model

Step 10: 10 56.447 35 .012 .951 .962 .049 no std resid > 2.5ish deleted #19

MI for 13&17 highest

delete #13 with lower std reg weight & reran model

Step 11: 9 33.58 27 .179 .980 .985 .031 Accepted Model Fit - 1 deleted #13

deleted items < .50 (#3, 15 & 16)

Step 12 6 11.994 9 .214 .984 .990 .036 Accept model fit - 2 deleted #3, 15, 16

* EFA steps and cut-off criteria were based on processes outlined by Byrne (2010)

Appendix M Page 5 of 8

Page 289: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

287

Exploratory Factor Analysis on MSCEIT Task F

#

items chi-

square df p< TLI CFI RMSEA

NOTES

Step 1: 15 158.868 90 .001 .503 .574 .055 deleted items <.3 (1,2,3,4,5,7,8,10,11,13) Run with all items

Step 2 5 3.112 5 .683 1.049 1.000 .000 no std resid > 2.5ish deleted #1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 7, 8, 10, 11, 13

Accept model fit -1

Ave low so deleted item<.4 (#12)

Step 3: 4 2.327 2 .312 .985 .995 .025 Accept model fit -2 deleted #12

* EFA steps and cut-off criteria were based on processes outlined by Byrne (2010)

Appendix M Page 6 of 8

Page 290: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

288

Exploratory Factor Analysis on MSCEIT Task G

# items

chi-square

df p< TLI CFI RMSEA

NOTES

Step 1: 12 72.301 54 .049 .740 .787 .036 deleted items < .3 loading (1,2,3,5,7,10) Run with all items Step 2: 6 8.292 9 .505 1.021 1.000 .000 no std resid > 2.5ish

deleted 1,2,3,5,7,10

MI for 4&9 highest

delete #9 with lower std reg weight & reran model

Step 3: 5 3.04 5 .694 1.085 1.000 .000 no std resid > 2.5ish deleted #9

MI for 6&8 highest

delete # 8 with lower std reg weight & reran model

Step 4: 4 1.145 2 .564 1.065 1.000 .000 no std resid > 2.5ish deleted #8

MI for 4&11 highest

delete #11 with lower std reg weight & reran model

Step 5: 4 .215 1 .643 1.068 1.000 .000 Accept model fit deleted 11

set error 6&12 to equal

* EFA steps and cut-off criteria were based on processes outlined by Byrne (2010)

Appendix M Page 7 of 8

Page 291: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

289

Exploratory Factor Analysis on MSCEIT Task H

# items

chi-square df p< TLI CFI RMSEA

NOTES

Step 1: 9 25.559 27 .543 1.024 1.000 .000 Delete items loading <.30 (#4, 7, 8 &9)

run with all items

Step 2: 5 10.05 5 .074 .865 .932 .063 no std resid > 2.5ish

deleted 4,7 8 &9

MI for 5&6 highest

delete #6 with lower std reg weight & reran model

Step 3: 4 .47 2 .791 1.088 1.000 .000 accept model fit - 1

deleted 6

Low AVE so deleted low items #3

Step 4: 3 1.567 1 .211 .949 .983 .047 accept model fit - 2 deleted #3 set 2&5 to unity

Step 5:

deleted #5, replace 3 3 10.78 1 .001 .144 .715 .196 reject model fit - 3

* EFA steps and cut-off criteria were based on processes outlined by Byrne (2010)

Appendix M Page 8 of 8

Page 292: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

290

Inter-rater Agreement on 32 WUSCT Items

Item

Cron Alpha

1 .95 2 .92 3 .85 4 .87 5 .89 6 .86 7 .45 8 .87 9 .92

10 .92 11 .86 12 .89 13 .88 14 .92 15 .88 16 .74 17 .91 18 .90 19 .80 20 .90 21 .89 22 .87 23 .90 24 .92 25 .79 26 .87 27 .82 28 .93 29 .80 30 .90 31 .91 32 .88 33 .86 34 .89 35 .87 36 .81

Appendix N

Page 293: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

291

Central Tendency and Dispersion of rWG_un for Items in Revised LE Scale

Mean Median Mode

Std. Deviation

Minimum Maximum

Team leadership .67 .77 .89 .32 -­‐.94 1.00

Participative style .67 .76 .92 .35 -­‐1.18 1.00

Positive relations customer .83 .89 .96 .19 -­‐.17 1.00

Inspire vision .68 .77 .92a .30 -­‐.49 1.00

Praise positive contribution .70 .83 .96 .37 -­‐1.21 .98

Manage conflicts .69 .78 .76 .29 -­‐.61 1.00

Encourage people develop .71 .81 .92 .30 -­‐.97 .98

Delegate responsibility .67 .79 .96 .35 -­‐1.18 1.00

Manage not performing .68 .79 .76 .30 -­‐.52 1.00

Communicate fit .72 .82 .96 .26 -­‐.37 1.00

Facilitate discussions .71 .81 .84a .28 -­‐.54 1.00

* Multiple modes exist. The smallest value is shown.

Appendix O

Page 294: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

292

Central Tendency and Dispersion of rWG_sskew for Items in Revised LE Scale

Mean Median Mode

Std. Deviation

Minimum Maximum

Team leadership .56 .70 .85 .41 -­‐1.54 1.00

Participative style    .56 .69 .89 .46 -­‐1.86 1.00

Positive relations customer .77 .86 .95 .25 -­‐.53 1.00

Inspire vision .59 .70 .89a .40 -­‐.96 1.00

Praise positive contribution .61 .78 .95 .49 -­‐1.90 .97

Manage conflicts .60 .71 .68 .37 -­‐1.11 1.00

Encourage people develop .62 .75 .89 .39 -­‐1.58 .98

Delegate responsibility .56 .73 .95 .45 -­‐1.86 1.00

Manage not performing .57 .72 .68 .40 -­‐.98 1.00

Communicate fit .63 .76 .95 .34 -­‐.80 1.00

Facilitate discussions .62 .75 .79 .36 -­‐1.01 1.00

* Multiple modes exist. The smallest value is shown.

1

Appendix P

2

Page 295: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

293

Central Tendency and Dispersion of rWG_Mskew for Items in Revised LE Scale

Mean Median Mode

Std. Deviation

Minimum Maximum

Team leadership .46 .63 .82 .51 -­‐2.14 1.00

Participative style .46 .61 .87 .57 -­‐2.54 1.00

Positive relations customer .72 .82 .95 .31 -­‐.90 1.00

Inspire vision .49 .63 .86a .49 -­‐1.42 1.00

Praise positive contribution .52 .73 .94 .61 -­‐2.59 .97

Manage conflicts .50 .64 .61 .46 -­‐1.61 1.00

Encourage people develop .52 .69 .87 .48 -­‐2.19 .97

Delegate responsibility .46 .67 .93 .56 -­‐2.54 1.00

Manage not performing .47 .65 .61 .49 -­‐1.46 1.00

Communicate fit .54 .71 .93 .42 -­‐1.23 1.00

Facilitate discussions .54 .69 .74a .45 -­‐1.49 1.00

* Multiple modes exist. The smallest value is shown.

Appendix Q

Page 296: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

294

Central Tendency and Dispersion of rWG_un for Items in Revised ME Scale

Mean Median Mode Std.

Deviation Minimum Maximum

Follow up decisions .78 .87 .88 .22 -­‐.40 1.00

High priority .75 .87 .96 .28 -­‐.43 .98

Monitor activities .75 .86 .89a .27 -­‐.82 1.00

Support attempts improve .82 .88 .92 .29 -­‐1.97 1.00

Develop new ideas .78 .85 .92 .23 -­‐.66 1.00

Negotiate effectively .77 .85 .86a .27 -­‐1.18 1.00

Plans goals, task and timelines .71 .80 .76a .30 -­‐.97 .98

Good solutions problems .80 .87 .90 .21 -­‐.32 1.00

Developing new opportunities .79 .85 .92a .20 -­‐.45 1.00

Initiate changes .77 .83 .92a .22 -­‐.05 1.00

Comply organisation policies .82 .88 .90a .18 -­‐.03 1.00

Information about progress .76 .84 .96 .29 -­‐.98 1.00

* Multiple modes exist. The smallest value is shown.

Appendix R

Page 297: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

295

Central Tendency and Dispersion of rWG_sskew for Items in Revised ME Scale

Mean Median Mode

Std. Deviation

Minimum Maximum

Follow up decisions .71 .83 .84 .29 -­‐.84 1.00

High priority .68 .83 .95 .37 -­‐.87 .98

Monitor activities .67 .82 .85a .36 -­‐1.38 1.00

Support attempts improve .76 .84 .89 .38 -­‐2.89 1.00

Develop new ideas .72 .80 .89 .31 -­‐1.17 1.00

Negotiate effectively .70 .81 .82a .36 -­‐1.86 1.00

Plans goals, task and timelines .61 .74 .68a .39 -­‐1.58 .97

Good solutions problems .74 .83 .87 .27 -­‐.73 1.00

Developing new opportunities .72 .81 .89a .26 -­‐.90 1.00

Initiate changes .69 .78 .89a .29 -­‐.38 1.00

Comply organisation policies .76 .84 .87a .24 -­‐.35 1.00

Information about progress .69 .79 .95 .38 -­‐1.59 1.00

* Multiple modes exist. The smallest value is shown.

Appendix S

Page 298: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

296

Central Tendency and Dispersion of rWG_Mskew for Items in Revised ME Scale

Mean Median Mode

Std. Deviation

Minimum Maximum

Follow up decisions .64 .79 .80 .36 -­‐1.28 1.00

High priority .60 .79 .86a .46 -­‐1.32 .97

Monitor activities .60 .78 .82 .44 -­‐1.95 1.00

Support attempts improve .70 .80 .87 .47 -­‐3.81 1.00

Develop new ideas .65 .75 .87 .38 -­‐1.69 1.00

Negotiate effectively .63 .76 .78 .45 -­‐2.54 1.00

Plans goals, task and timelines .52 .68 .61a .49 -­‐2.19 .97

Good solutions problems .67 .79 .84 .33 -­‐1.14 1.00

Developing new opportunities .66 .76 .87a .33 -­‐1.36 1.00

Initiate changes .62 .72 .87a .35 -­‐.70 1.00

Comply organisation policies .70 .80 .86 .29 -­‐.67 1.00

Information about progress .61 .74 .74a .47 -­‐2.21 1.00

* Multiple modes exist. The smallest value is shown.

Appendix T

Page 299: Exploring the Relationship of Meaning Making Structure ... · Exploring the Relationship of Meaning-Making Structure, Emotional Intelligence, IQ and Managerial-Leadership Effectiveness

297