exploring conflict occurrences in a nature-based …

107
EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED PRESCHOOL IN THE CONTEXT OF INDOOR AND OUTDOOR CHILD-INITIATED PLAY by Annette Pic A thesis submitted to the Faculty of the University of Delaware in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science in Human Development and Family Sciences Spring 2020 © 2020 Annette Pic All Rights Reserved

Upload: others

Post on 17-Apr-2022

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED

PRESCHOOL IN THE CONTEXT OF INDOOR AND OUTDOOR

CHILD-INITIATED PLAY

by

Annette Pic

A thesis submitted to the Faculty of the University of Delaware in partial

fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science in Human

Development and Family Sciences

Spring 2020

© 2020 Annette Pic

All Rights Reserved

Page 2: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED

PRESCHOOL IN THE CONTEXT OF INDOOR AND OUTDOOR

CHILD-INITIATED PLAY

by

Annette Pic

Approved: __________________________________________________________

Myae Han, Ph.D.

Professor in charge of thesis on behalf of the Advisory Committee

Approved: __________________________________________________________

Bahira Trask, Ph.D.

Chair of the Department of Human Development and Family Sciences

Approved: __________________________________________________________

Gary Henry, Ph.D.

Dean of the College of Education and Human Development

Approved: __________________________________________________________

Douglas J. Doren, Ph.D.

Interim Vice Provost for Graduate and Professional Education and

Dean of the Graduate College

Page 3: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

iii

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

First, I would like to thank Dr. Myae Han for her time, unwavering support,

feedback, and guidance. I would like to thank my committee members, Dr. Rena

Hallam and Dr. Jason Hustedt for their suggestions and discussions which provided

invaluable insight and direction. Thank you to my fellow graduate students, Cara for

assisting me with inter-rater reliability, and to Imani, Susan, and Nadisha for providing

encouragement. I would also like to thank Polly for encouraging me to pursue a

master’s degree. I owe a huge thank you to my husband, Chris and my four children,

Andrew, Ryan, Rebeca and David for believing in me, picking up the extra slack, and

for their unending patience. Lastly, I would like to thank my parents, Charlie and

Reita, my siblings, Chris, Carolyn and Dan, and my daughter-in-law, Kristin for their

support and encouragement throughout this process.

Page 4: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

iv

TABLE OF CONTENTS

LIST OF TABLES .................................................................................................... vi LIST OF FIGURES .................................................................................................. vii

ABSTRACT............................................................................................................ viii

1 INTRODUCTION .......................................................................................... 1

Theoretical Framework ................................................................................... 2

2 LITERATURE REVIEW ............................................................................... 9

Nature-Based Outdoor Classrooms ................................................................. 9

The Benefits of Child-Initiated Play and Outdoor Nature-Based Play ...... 11

Benefits of Child-Initiated Play. ........................................................ 11 Benefits of Outdoor Nature-Based Play. ............................................ 12

Defining Conflict .................................................................................... 15

Conflict Categories, Constructs and Rates ......................................... 16

Categories of Conflict. ................................................................. 16 Conflict Constructs. ..................................................................... 18

Rates of Conflict. ......................................................................... 20

Gaps in Current Literature ....................................................................... 21

Current Study .......................................................................................... 22

Research Questions ........................................................................... 23

3 METHOD ..................................................................................................... 25

Present Study ................................................................................................ 25

Setting .................................................................................................... 25 Participants ............................................................................................. 30

Data Collection ............................................................................................. 32

Page 5: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

v

Data Analysis/Coding ....................................................................... 37

Event Sampling ........................................................................... 37

Coding Development ................................................................... 39

Credibility ............................................................................................... 46

Ethics ...................................................................................................... 47

4 FINDINGS ................................................................................................... 48

Question 1. Frequency of Conflict Between Indoor and Outdoor Child-

initiated Play in a Nature-based Preschool .................................................... 48

Question 2. Catalyst Differences Among Preschool Children During Child-

Initiated Play Indoor and Outdoor in a Nature-Based Preschool .................... 51

Question 3: Conflict Resolution Among Children in the Context of Child-

Initiated Play in a Nature-Based Preschool .................................................... 55

5 DISCUSSION .............................................................................................. 63

Frequency of Indoor and Outdoor Conflict .................................................... 63

Catalysts Which Initiate Conflict .................................................................. 65 Interventionist Types and Resolutions ........................................................... 66

Limitations ................................................................................................... 67 Conclusion and Implications ......................................................................... 68

REFERENCES ........................................................................................................ 71

Appendix

A PARENTAL CONSENT LETTERS ............................................................. 82

B PARENTAL SCRIPT ................................................................................... 88 C CHILD SCRIPT ........................................................................................... 89

D FIELD NOTES FORM ................................................................................. 90 E CONFLICT LOG INDOOR/OUTDOOR ...................................................... 91

F CODE BOOK ............................................................................................... 93 G IRB FORMS ................................................................................................. 96

H INTERVENTIONIST TYPE DATA ............................................................. 99

Page 6: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

vi

LIST OF TABLES

Table 1 Characteristics of Focal Children ......................................................... 31

Table 2 Catalyst Categories .............................................................................. 42

Table 3 Resolution Intervention Types .............................................................. 43

Table 4 Resolution Outcome ............................................................................. 45

Table 5 Focal Child Conflict Events Indoor and Outdoor .................................. 49

Table 6 Interventionist Type by Setting ............................................................ 56

Table 7 Frequency of Indoor and Outdoor Resolution by Interventionist Type .. 57

Page 7: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

vii

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure 1 Indoor Setting Layout .......................................................................... 28

Figure 2 Outdoor Setting Layout ........................................................................ 29

Figure 3 Conflict Identification and Transcript Coding Map............................... 38

Figure 4 Conflict Event Coding.......................................................................... 40

Figure 5 Frequency of Catalyst that Initiates Conflict ......................................... 52

Page 8: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

viii

ABSTRACT

The social nature of preschool play provides a rich context to study conflict.

Play in the outdoor environment affords children the opportunity to engage in deep

sustained play (Bohling et al., 2010) making outdoor nature-based preschools a rich

setting for children to engage in conflict in the context of play. This descriptive

qualitative study explored peer conflict and resolution in the context of free play in a

culturally diverse nature-based preschool both in the indoor and the outdoor setting. In

this nature-based preschool, children spend two-thirds of their school day outside

engaging in free play and forty-five minutes indoors engaging in free play and teacher

directed activities. For this study focal child (n=16) video observations were

conducted. Each focal child was video recorded for 30 minutes of their outdoor free

play and 30 minutes of their indoor free play. Event coding analysis was used to

examine peer conflict frequency, catalysts, interventionist types, and resolution or no

resolution. Data analysis showed that in this nature preschool the setting (indoor,

outdoor) played a role in conflict frequency rates, duration, catalyst, interventionist

type patterns, and children’s opportunity to negotiate and resolve peer conflict. Boys

engaged in more conflict indoors while girls engaged in conflict outdoors. The outdoor

conflict events were more likely to be initiated by a play idea, lasted longer and

involved more complex interventionist patterns.

Page 9: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

1

Chapter 1

INTRODUCTION

The Federal Mandate No Child Left Behind stressed standardized testing of

students in grades 3-8, incentivized schools to focus on math, reading, and writing;

and created a teach-to-the-test climate extending down to preschool. Common Core

State Standards Initiative’s kindergarten guidelines led to increased “seat work” for

children and scripted curricula, also known as direct instruction, for teachers. In a

comparative study which examined teachers’ attitudes nationwide in 1998 and 2010,

Bassok, Latham and Rorem (2016) noted Kindergarten children spent more time with

workbooks and worksheets and less time with music and art. The authors concluded

kindergarten was the new first grade, shifting the expectations for kindergarten

readiness to preschool. Kindergarten skills taught in preschool limits children’s

opportunities to play freely, engage in child-initiated learning (Bassok, et al., 2016;

Gray, 2011), and play in the outdoor environment (Davies & Hamilton, 2018; Kuo,

Barnes & Jordan, 2019). This decrease in child-initiated play may decrease the

opportunity for children to gain necessary social skills such as sharing, turn taking,

conflict negotiation and resolution.

The teach-to-the-test climate generated pushback from early childhood

educators, especially supporters and researchers of play, child-initiated learning, and

the social-emotional well-being of children. The push back against formal

assessments, standardized testing, and structured curriculum began in Europe in the

early 1990’s and spread to the United States. The movement was fueled by Richard

Page 10: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

2

Louv’s (2008) book, Last Child in the Woods, in which Louv coined the term ‘nature-

deficit disorder.’ His work generated renewed interest in outdoor free play, child-

initiated learning, and outdoor nature preschools.

Play is a natural activity pursued by children that encourages independent skill

acquisition, allows free expression of their ideas, enables them to change their minds

and disagree, and suggest what is both possible and impossible because they are

playing (Brown & Vaughan, 2010). Nature-based outdoor classrooms combine

education and encourage play in a naturalistic environment. The natural outdoor

environment provides children with the opportunity to engage in conflict occurrences

and resolutions (Bohling, Saarela, & Miller, 2010). Conflicts are naturally occurring

events (Chen, Fein, Killen & Tam, 2001) which provide children the opportunity to

acquire and practice social skills such as negotiation, resource sharing, cooperation,

emotional regulation, and conflict management (Aasen, Grindheim & Waters, 2009;

Wheeler, 2004). Nature-based preschools offer larger play spaces compared to

standard classrooms, tend to use curriculum rich in child-lead learning and free play,

and combine education and play in a naturalistic environment which provides an ideal

setting for investigating peer conflict during free play.

Theoretical Framework

This present study is informed by Lev Vygotsky’s (1966, 1978) sociocultural

theory and plays role in development, and Urie Bronfenbrenner’s (1977, 1979, 1986)

bioecological systems theory, primarily Phase 2 (1980-1993) as defined by Rosa and

Tudge (2013). Vygotsky (1966, 1978) acknowledged the important role of play in

child development and proposed that play provides the context for learning

opportunities to develop cognitively. Vygotsky stated make-believe play is essential

Page 11: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

3

for children to develop the ability to think abstractly. Children create learning

opportunities and experiences in the context of free play which provides them with

necessary tools (e.g., problem solving, self-regulation) to achieve higher cognitive

development (Bodrova, Germeroth & Leong; Tovey, 2007). Bronfenbrenner contends

the child’s whole environment must be considered in their developmental processes

suggesting a bidirectional interconnectedness between the child’s lived experiences,

their surroundings, peers, teachers, and their own biology. These constructs will

provide a foundation in which to investigate peer conflict and resolutions among

preschool children in a nature-based preschool during indoor and outdoor child-

initiated play.

Key concepts of Vygotsky’s sociocultural theory include social interaction as

the fundamental role in children’s construction of knowledge, language as a mediator

of development, and the role of play and culture in children’s development of higher

order processes. Vygotsky proposed human development and learning originate in

social, historical, and cultural interactions suggesting learning and development are

interrelated (Green, & Piel 2010; Vygotsky 1966, 1978). Vygotsky saw play as a

context for thought creation and a leading source of cognitive development during the

preschool years. Play provides children the opportunity to practice what they

previously learned and to construct new knowledge. For Vygotsky it is through play

children reach higher levels of thinking by using objects symbolically. The stick from

a tree becomes the horse that the child uses to gallop around. The act of symbolic

thinking involves abstract thinking. According to Vygotsky the stick is a ‘pivot’ that

allows children to explore the meaning of horse. Meaning exploration contributes to

children’s development of abstract thought (Tovey, 2007). Additionally, through the

Page 12: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

4

act of play children learn to act in a cognitive realm relying on their internal

tendencies and motives within their cultural context.

According to Vygotsky social interactions are shaped by cultural and historical

settings. For Vygotsky the cultural setting of the classroom and the relationships

between peers and teachers must be taken into account (Bodrova, Germeroth &

Leong, 2013). Peer social interactions are dependent on the setting context. The

context in which play occurs is different dependent upon the setting such as an indoor

preschool classroom or an outdoor nature-based classroom. Vygotsky believes

language exchanged in the social interactions accelerates cognitive development thus

verbal interaction which occur during conflict affords children the opportunity for

cognitive growth (Green, & Pie, 2010). The natural outdoor environment allows

children to engage in deep imaginative play (Bohling, Saarela, & Miller, 2010).

Vygotsky proposed learning itself was not development but rather it is through

social interactions with peers and teachers that development and learning occurred. In

other words, there is a social nature to learning. Play among peers occurs in a social

context. Conflict that arises between preschoolers during play allows children the

opportunity to learn through shared problem solving experiences. Vygotsky viewed

these experiences as a dialectical process which occurs in cognitive development. The

current teach-to-the test climate which emphasizes outcomes and increases seatwork

for children leads to a decrease in children’s opportunity to play (Bassok, Latham, &

Rorem, 2016; Gray, 2011).

Bronfenbrenner originally proposed the ecological systems theory (Phase 1)

(Bronfenbrenner, 1977, 1979), which stated an individual’s environment influences

development, and development occurs within nested systems in which the individual

Page 13: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

5

resides: microsystem, mesosystem, exosystem, and macrosystem (Bronfenbrenner,

1977). Phase 2 of Bronfenbrenner’s bioecological systems theory built on the

constructs of Phase 1 and encompasses Bronfenbrenner’s writings from 1980-1993

(Rosa & Tudge, 2013). Bronfenbrenner acknowledged short-comings in his original

work and later expounded upon his original theory: the role of the individual on

development, focus on the passage of time, and greater concern with developmental

processes (Rosa & Tudge, 2013). The microsystem is the first layer in the nested

system, and it refers to the direct relationships between the child, and the child’s

immediate surroundings including people, places, and objects; and the interpersonal

bidirectional interactions between the individuals, places and objects. Examples of the

microsystem are the child’s relationship with their parents, siblings, peers, and

teachers as well as, their neighborhood and school. The mesosystem is the second

layer of the nested system; it encompasses the direct interactions between two or more

aspects of the microsystem that influence the child’s development. An example of the

mesosystem is the relationships between the child’s parents and their teacher.

Bronfenbrenner (1986) proposed a child’s home life can impact the child’s progress in

school, and a child’s school experience can impact the child’s home life. This suggests

mesosystem interactions impact a child’s development. The third layer of the nested

system is the exosystem; the distal influences of the microsystem elements that do not

interact with the child directly. For example, parent’s employment status or job

security impacts both family income, and the parent’s physical and emotional

availability to the child. Bronfenbrenner (1986) proposed although children do not

interact with their parent’s place of employment or social systems directly their

development is affected by the distal influences. The fourth layer of the nested system

Page 14: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

6

is the macrosystem. The macrosystem includes the cultural and societal beliefs,

ideologies, and programming which influence a child’s development. Societal gender

norms and religious influences are examples of the macrosystem. The fifth layer of the

nested system, chronosystem, was added in Phase 2 of Bronfenbrenner’s (1986)

writings. Bronfenbrenner proposed the chronosystem is the dimension of time as it

relates to the child’s internal (physiological and cognitive changes), and external

(specific event) environment, historical time, past and present. Examples of the

chronosystem are family morals and values handed down through the generations,

sudden death of a parent, and the child’s physiological and cognitive stage.

Bronfenbrenner (1986) proposed development is influenced by the child’s reaction to

the event or value system.

In Bronfenbrenner’s (1986) Phase 2 writings, he stressed the bi-directionality

of the nested system, the importance of time, and the role of the child in development.

A key construct of bioecological systems theory is the reciprocal interactions between

the child and other persons and places (Bronfenbrenner, 1986). A child’s development

is dependent on the child’s immediate environment, their biology, and the

bidirectional interactions within the layered system. According to bioecological

systems theory, the child’s personal interaction within their environment, the

relationships the child forms within the nested system, and the outside influences of

sociohistorical conditions should be taken into account to understand a child’s

development (Bronfenbrenner, 1986). The child’s environment includes interactions

and relationships between the child and other individuals (e.g. parents, siblings,

relatives, peers, teachers, religious leaders, etc.) who surround the child. The nature

and quality of the interactions throughout the system result in positive and/or negative

Page 15: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

7

influences on the child’s cognitive development. An underlying assumption of

Bronfenbrenner’s Phase 2 writings, is that development is influenced by both the

child’s biology and the environment in which the child resides (Rosa & Tudge, 2013).

Examples of the child’s individual biology are hair color, weight, height, gender

identity, ethnicity, sex, and physical and mental health.

Phase 2 of the bioecological systems theory provides a framework in which to

identify multiple factors which may influence a child’s initiation of conflict or

response to conflict in a nature-based preschool. The model offers a broad, holistic

approach to researching, and analyzing the multiple interactive influences on peer

conflict and resolution occurrences among preschool children. On the microsystem

level the child’s surroundings such as forested area, meadow, or creek and natural

lighting would impact a child differently than a classroom with four walls, fluorescent

lighting, tables, and chairs. The outdoor natural environment has the potential to

provide unlimited resources to manipulate, explore, and discover. The materials

available for children to interact with are different in the indoor space compared to the

outdoor space. Legos, blocks, puzzles, and a fish in a tank provide a different

experience than logs, stones, dirt, leaves, plants, and insects. Static playgrounds with

fixed structures do not offer changing landscapes as do natural areas affected by

seasonal and weather changes.

On the mesosystem level the interactions with peers and the proximity of

teachers in the indoor space compared to the outdoor natural space may be different.

The larger more open space of the outdoor environment may allow children more

freedom to resolve peer conflict independently compared to the close proximity of

teachers and children in a classroom setting.

Page 16: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

8

An example of the exosystem is parental employment and income which are

likely to determine if a child has access to a nature-based preschool. Policies and

belief systems which support outdoor nature-based play for all children are examples

of the macrosystem. National policies and belief in the value of play can impact the

availability of outdoor nature play for children. Providing children with the

opportunity to play freely in the outdoor environment may provide them with

opportunities to develop conflict resolution skills. The child’s past experience with

peer conflict and the passing of time which produce physiological and cognitive

changes, and the changing seasons are examples of the chronosystem. In nature-based

preschools children’s experiences are rooted in seasonal changes, landscape changes

and personal growth.

Page 17: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

9

Chapter 2

LITERATURE REVIEW

This literature review acknowledges the importance of children’s participation

in peer conflict occurrences and resolutions in the context of child-initiated play in a

nature-based preschool.

Nature-Based Outdoor Classrooms

The terms “nature-based preschool” and “nature preschool” are used

interchangeably in the literature (Larimore, 2016). Nature-based outdoor classrooms

are designed for use in all types of weather and throughout the seasons. The materials

provided are natural, open-ended, renewable, evolving, and plentiful. A stick becomes

a wand, a cane, a broom, a pencil, or a spatula, the child’s imagination is the limit. The

natural environment provides a different setting in comparison to standard indoor

classrooms (controlled climates, four walls, predictable, designated spaces, and

fluorescent lighting) with natural borders, child-sized places, unique exploration

opportunities (Bohling, Saarela, & Miller, 2010), and changing landscape. In the

outdoor environment voices can be louder and movement can be bigger, engaging

whole body movement (Frost, Wortham, & Reifel, 2012).

Nature-based outdoor classrooms support full body learning, child-initiated

learning and afford children the opportunity to interact directly with nature promoting

environmental stewardship (Kuo, Barnes, & Jordan, 2019). In a qualitative post-

occupancy study, Dennis, Wells, and Bishop (2014) examined 11 nature-based

Page 18: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

10

outdoor early childhood education sites in various regions in the U.S through

structured semi-structured telephone interviews of involved teachers and

administrators. Interview data was used to evaluated six existing theories linking

nature-based outdoor classrooms to positive learning and developmental outcomes

(p.38):

• The use of open-ended materials stimulates imaginative play;

• Using “loose parts” and sensory materials contributes to full-bodied

experiential learning and physical development;

• Incorporating as much open space as possible promotes physical

activity;

• Creating multiple places for activity increases learning opportunities;

• Providing “safe” risks allows children to learn to handle risk

successfully;

• The replication of rich, natural environment contributes to stewardship

Their findings support the above theories, noting nature-based outdoor

classrooms afforded children many opportunities for explorations of patterns of

change (life cycles of plants and animals), longer engagement with materials, more

cooperative play, and cozy nooks and child-sized spaces compared to indoor settings.

Their findings also noted nature-based outdoor classrooms provide a flexible changing

environment through seasonal variation, changes in weather and naturally occurring

environmental changes. Additionally, Dennis and colleagues’ results suggest that

children in the outdoor setting were more relaxed, happy and less impulsive compared

to children in the indoor setting.

Page 19: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

11

A qualitative case study exploring preschool children’s (ages 3-5) skill

development in a Nature Explore Classroom in Minnesota was conducted by Bohling,

Saarela, and Miller (2010). Bohling and colleagues noted seasonal changes impact

play among children. The authors concluded winter contained the most complex and

creative play frames as well as the largest number of children playing together. The

authors observed on eleven occasions dramatic/creative play frames that involved five

or more children. Bohling and colleagues reported only 18% of the sixty Nature Notes

completed by teachers represented adult-directed activities in the nature-based outdoor

classroom, the remaining were child-initiated. These findings support literature

suggesting the larger degree of freedom afforded by the natural outdoor environment

lends itself to child-initiated learning and a smaller degree of adult control (Sutterby,

& Frost, 2006; Maynard, Waters, & Clement, 2013a); and in the outdoor environment

teachers tend to plan activities which have a greater emphasis on children’s

collaboration and social skills (Maynard et al., 2013a).

The Benefits of Child-Initiated Play and Outdoor Nature-Based Play

Benefits of Child-Initiated Play. Empirical research has established that

children develop and learn through play. According to Aras (2016), play provides a

context for children to develop social and emotional, physical, and cognitive skills that

cannot be acquired through teacher-directed instruction. Johnson, Christie and Wardle

(2005) state, “development is served by play and development is seen in play” (p.56).

Play provides children a non-threatening environment in which to explore, discover,

and test new skills through a process of trial and error. Child-initiated play provides

children an important context in which to acquire skills (Cordoni et al., 2016).

Qualitative research studies acknowledge the positive role child-initiated learning

Page 20: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

12

plays in children’s overall development (Berkhout, Bakkers, Hoekman & Goorhuis-

Brouwer, 2013; Maynard, Waters, & Clement, 2013b), and acknowledge children’s

ability to self-select learning activities and manage their choices (Wood, 2014).

Through this process, development (defined as changes over time) evolves into

processes that become predictable, coordinated, and transferable to other contexts.

Owens (1996, p.229) states, “much of the child’s language develops with the context

of play with an adult or with other children.” Play is a natural context in which

children can explore new ways to merge language and thought (Pelletier, 2011).

Cordoni et al., (2016) suggests, “managing new playful interactions requires

sophisticated communicative, emotional and social skills” (p. 1076). Play affords

children the opportunity to practice necessary fundamental social skills such as

conflict management. Providing children the opportunity to engage in child-initiated

play where conflicts may arise affords children the opportunity to develop and practice

communication, and emotional and social skills (perspective taking, negotiation,

resolution).

Benefits of Outdoor Nature-Based Play. The limitations placed on children’s

free play by the formal assessment climate directly impact play opportunities afforded

to child-initiated play. The decrease is most notable in the reduced exposure to play in

the outdoor environment (Gray, 2011). Several qualitative studies note outdoor nature-

based play presents children with multiple opportunities and freedom to learn, explore

and develop skills loudly, wildly and without direct adult control (Maynard, Waters, &

Clement, 2013a; Stack, & Nikiforidou, 2019; Tovey, 2007). Nature provides children

with an abundance of materials for manipulation and investigation (Bohling, Saarela,

& Miller, 2010) and supports increased opportunity for independent exploration and

Page 21: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

13

risk taking (Stephenson, 2003). Outdoor nature-based play provides children with an

endless supply of natural loose parts: stones, sand, dirt, water, logs, sticks, leaves, seed

pods, and living creatures (Craft, McConnon, & Paige-Smith, 2012; Maynard et al.,

2013a). In nature-based environments children have the freedom to use their

imagination and inventiveness to create their own play, engage in deep play with

sustained attention (Bohling et al., 2010), and investigate, construct and reorganize

their surroundings (Fjørtoft, 2004). A quantitative study done by Wells and Evans

(2003), noted the benefits of nature appear to be associated with children’s mental

health through the association of nature as a buffer against the impact of stressful life

events and global self-perception of psychological well-being. Taylor and Kuo (2006)

reviewed the literature around nature’s role in the healthy development of children and

suggest children’s interaction with green spaces has a positive influence on

concentration, impulse control, and delayed gratification as well as encouraging

children to be self-reliant in physically challenging environments. In a quantitative

study Ulset, Vitaro, Brendgen, Bekkhus, and Borge (2017) demonstrated interacting

with nature benefits cognitive development observed through achievement of higher

digit scores, and reduced inattention-hyperactivity symptoms. Nature has been shown

to boost academic learning, personal development and environmental stewardship

(Kuo, Barnes & Jordan, 2019). Play in nature engages the whole body, providing

increased opportunities to develop gross and fine motor skills, coordination, and

balance (Fjørtoft, 2004; Moore, 2014) and provides space and freedom for extended

peer negotiation (Aasen, Grindheim & Waters, 2009). The naturalistic outdoor

environment provides children with the opportunity to engage in problem solving

Page 22: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

14

(Fjørtoft, 2004), and conflict negotiation and resolutions (Bohling, Saarela, and Miller,

2010).

Although the current research supports the benefits of nature and outdoor play

for children, few empirical studies provide analysis of conflict occurrences in the

context of an outdoor nature-based preschool. Without empirical evidence of the

importance of navigating peer conflicts in the outdoor space, teachers, although aware

of the benefits of nature and outdoor spaces, may not replace indoor instruction with

outdoor child-initiated play opportunities.

Given the growing interest in nature-based preschools and the increase in the

number of outdoor nature-based programs in the U.S. it is important to understand

conflict occurrence in the context of child-initiated play in both the outdoor and indoor

environment of a nature-based preschool. The types and frequency of conflict

occurrences during indoor and outdoor free play in a nature-based preschool should be

investigated to establish occurrence rates, catalyst patterns, interventionist types, and

to foster exploration and conversation regarding the importance of outdoor child-

initiated play and child lead conflict management in nature-based preschools. It is

important to understand the context surrounding peer conflict occurrences and whether

the outdoor nature-based environment influences conflict occurrence and resolution or

no resolution. The outdoor environment may provide more opportunities for children

to engage in and resolve peer conflict scenarios. Establishing frequency and types of

conflict occurrences will provide insight for practitioners, professional development,

and pre-service teacher education.

Page 23: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

15

Defining Conflict

By definition a conflict arises when two individuals have incompatible goals or

behaviors (Hay, 1984). In research literature, conflicts are commonly defined as

events in which one child protests, retaliates, or resists the action or verbalization of

another child (Hay, 1984; Shantz, 1987). Conflicts can be verbal or social: Hay (1984)

defines social conflict as one individual’s actions causing a second individual to

object; Garvey (1984) defines verbal conflict, as an individual’s reaction to refuse,

deny, object, prohibit or disagree with the initial request by another individual.

Wheeler (2004, p.10) suggests, “Conflict is a disagreement between people, and it is

an opportunity for children to learn.” Conflict can be destructive (threats, coercion) or

constructive (mutual problem solving) both of which contribute to a child’s social,

emotional and cognitive development (Shantz, 1987; Wheeler, 2004).

Child-initiated play (free play) is one context in which preschool children

engage in peer conflict. Providing children with the opportunity to engage in, navigate,

and manage peer conflict is a key component for the formation and maintenance of

interpersonal relationships (Laursen, Hartup & Koples, 1996) and for children to

develop morally, socially and cognitively (Chen et al., 2001; Laursen, Finkelstein &

Betts, 2001; Shantz, 1987) through the development of negotiation skills, logical

thinking, perspective taking, relationship management, morality, and problem-solving

(Aasen, Gridheim & Waters, 2009; Laursen, et al., 2001; Malloy & McMurray, 1996;

Piaget, 1932; Wheeler, 2004).

An important distinction must be made between conflict and aggression;

aggressive behaviors may occur during conflict, but conflict in and of itself is not

aggression (Shantz, 1987, Wheeler, 2004). Conflict refers to incompatible behaviors

or goals while aggression suggests the intent to harm or injure another individual

Page 24: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

16

(Parke & Slaby, 1983; Shantz, 1987). Research indicates most conflict episodes do not

involve aggression (Shantz, 1987). Cordoni, Demuru, Ceccarelli & Palagi (2016) used

continuous video recordings and event sampling method to investigate aggressive

conflicts among 3- to 5- year old preschool children in the indoor environment. The

authors noted two levels of aggressive conflicts: low intensity, defined as aggressive

conflicts void of physical contact, and high intensity, defined as aggressive conflicts

with the presence of physical contact. Cordoni and colleagues found a higher

frequency of aggressive conflicts during free play compared to guided activities.

Conflict Categories, Constructs and Rates

Categories of Conflict. Conflict categories have been established through

empirical research. Chen, Fein, Killen and Tam (2001) organize child conflicts into

five categories (p.532):

• Physical harm (hitting, biting, punching),

• Psychological harm (name-calling, teasing),

• Distribution of resources (fairness issues such as violation of sharing or

turn taking, grabbing, taking),

• Play ideas (who will do what, how, when and where),

• Social-conventional issues such as the violation of class or school rules

about running indoors

The largest percentage of conflicts among toddlers and preschoolers involve

possession disputes (Brenner & Mueller 1982; Chen et al., 2001) referred to by Chen

et al., as distribution of resources. Ramsey (1986) conducted a naturalistic observation

study of twenty 3- to -4-year olds examining possession conflicts during indoor free

play. Ramsey used event sampling method to identify and examine how situational

Page 25: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

17

factors relate to possession-based conflicts. The study results suggest the types,

quality, and distribution of materials present in a setting significantly relate to the

occurrence of possession episodes. Additionally, the accessibility of space appeared to

significantly impact possession disputes; closed spaces or areas with single entrances

were more likely locations for possession-based conflicts. The natural outdoor

environment lends itself to endless resources (sticks, dirt, leaves, stones, flowers,

seeds) and wide open spaces which would indicate less possession conflicts in the

outdoor environment compared to the indoor.

The second largest conflict category identified is response regarding another

child’s actions or lack of actions such as refusal to adopt a suggested fantasy role

(Shantz, 1987). Chen et al., refer to this as conflict over play ideas. Given Bohling,

Saarela and Miller’s (2010) research which indicates children in nature-based

preschools play in relatively large groups of five or more, it would be expected

children within the group would disagree with a peer’s actions or suggestion more

often than a typical indoor classroom setting where children play in dyads and triads.

The majority of the studies examining conflict among preschoolers were

conducted in laboratory settings or typical indoor preschool classrooms. A few studies

included data collection in the indoor and outdoor environment. Malloy and

McMurray (1996) used a purposive sample of video data to investigate peer conflict

among typically developing children and children with disabilities in an integrated

preschool (mean age 4.42 years) in the indoor and outdoor setting, and determined

conflicts occurred most often during self-selection (free play) time.

A mixed methods study using an ethnographic approach by Stack and

Nikiforidou (2019) observed twelve children ages 3- to 4- years of age in the indoor

Page 26: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

18

and outdoor play environment of a private nursery school in England. The majority of

the observations were conducted in the indoor environment. The study focused on

possession disputes in play type (child- or adult-initiated play) among preschoolers,

and the role of the teacher in the dispute. The researchers concluded in the indoor

space there were significantly more disputes among children within adult-initiated

play and, in the outdoor space there were more disputes during child-initiated play.

Additionally, their findings suggest regardless of the play type the closer proximity of

teachers and children in the indoor space allows teachers more opportunity to

intervene during disputes. These findings support previous research indicating

teachers play a less involved role in the outdoor environment (Maynard et al., 2013a).

Conflict Constructs. Conflict episodes are broken down into five constructs:

the catalyst (what the conflict is about), the initial influence attempt, tactics and

strategies, oppositions, and outcomes or resolutions (Shantz, 1987). An initial catalyst

which triggers a conflict, whether a positive or negative social overture (Garvey,

1984), must be met by opposition (protest, retaliation or resistance) from another

individual (Shantz, 1987) in order to constitute a conflict occurrence. A simple

conflict sequence suggests catalyst event-opposition-compliance to opposition is

sufficient to define the occurrence of a conflict episode (Shantz, 1987). Maynard

(1985) suggests the catalyst event-opposition-compliance model is too simplistic.

Maynard acknowledges opposition is necessary but not sufficient; rather opposition to

the initial opposition must be present.

According to Shantz (1987) conflict resolution patterns differ with age with

coercion being the most common tactic used by children. Malloy and McMurray

(1996) noted that children resolved thirty-one percent of the conflicts by themselves

Page 27: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

19

most frequently when one child stopped insisting or protesting. A narrative

observation study conducted by Laursen and Hartup (1989) observed fifty-three

children between the ages of 3 years 4 months and 5 years 4 months during indoor free

play and concluded most conflicts were resolved by insistence resulting in a win-lose

outcome and child separation after the conflict episode. However, children who

interacted prior to the conflict catalyst were more likely to interact after the conflict

episode.

In a qualitative study, Killen and Turiel (1991) observed sixty-nine children

age 3- to 4- years during peer group sessions and school-time free play to examine

conflict resolution patterns (coding for child-generated, adult-generated, or no

resolution). Killen and Turiel determined more conflicts ended without defined

resolutions compared to child-generated resolutions. Adult intervention in the conflict

resolution resulted in ninety-one percent of conflict resolution outcomes ending as

adult-generated. The authors noted children used more negotiation skills in conflict

resolutions in the absence of the teacher. This is consistent with findings from Corsaro

and Rizzo (1990) comparing disputes in peer cultures of American and Italian nursery-

school children. Corsaro and Rizzo observed children were not always successful in

reaching mutual resolutions; however, the authors acknowledge disputes among

children as important elements in cognitive and communication development as well

as in the organization and construction of play frames. Additionally, resolved peer

conflict permits play to continue while unresolved conflict terminates or disrupts a

playful interaction (Garvey, 1984).

Conflict resolution tactics provide children the opportunity to practice and

acquire social skills such as negotiation, resource sharing, cooperation, emotional

Page 28: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

20

regulation and conflict management (Aasen, Grindheim & Waters, 2009; Wheeler,

2004). Aasen et al. note negotiation requires one to acknowledge another’s view point

suggesting conflict participation is an important context that affords children the

opportunity to develop and practice negotiation skills. Additionally, these skills have

been identified as necessary for the formation and maintenance of friendships

(Denham, et al., 2003). By engaging in conflict resolution children learn which

techniques and strategies are effective in resolving conflict, these skills are used later

as an adult when engaging in rational argumentation (Garvey, 1984). When a teacher

steps in and resolves the conflict, the children involved are removed from the

opportunity to develop negotiation and resolution skills.

The importance of conflict negotiation and resolution as a means for children

to develop social and emotional skills has been established; however, the majority of

the studies were conducted in a typical indoor classroom setting or on static

playgrounds.

Rates of Conflict. Killen and Turiel (1991) investigated the types of social

issues that produce conflicts among children 3- to 4- years of age in two settings,

semi-structured peer group and school-time free play. The authors observed 201

conflicts during forty-eight 15 minute semi-structured peer group sessions (indoors);

and 61 conflicts in nineteen 30 minute school-time free-play sessions (indoor and

outdoor). Conflicts were coded for the source of the conflict, outcome and resolution.

Actions that did not produce a conflict were not coded as a conflict. They noted the

mean number of conflicts during the 15-minute semi-structured peer group session

was 4.9. The mean number of conflicts in the 30 minute free play setting was 10.5.

Shantz (1987) reported findings from a summary study completed by Hay (1984)

Page 29: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

21

which pooled findings from 11 group observations (which used focal child sampling

in nursery school settings and laboratories) and concluded the median number of

conflicts per hour was approximately 8. In a pilot study, Han et al., (2019, March)

conducted 13 hours and 24 minutes of running narrative observations in a nature-

based preschool and noted the rate of conflict was 0.97 conflicts per hour. The

researchers also noted instances where initiated conflict behaviors were not met with

resistance. These exploratory findings are cause for further investigation into conflict

occurrences in the context of nature-based preschools.

It is important to investigate conflict occurrence in the natural setting in order

to include contextual factors and explore emergent themes. A research study needs to

explore conflict occurrences in the context of outdoor and indoor child-initiated play

in a nature-based preschool to add to the current literature regarding conflict among

preschool children.

Gaps in Current Literature

Although research studies have connected conflict episodes in the context of

child-initiated play to emergent social and emotional skills, fewer have done so in a

nature-based preschool environment. The majority of preschool child-initiated play

studies examining peer conflict are qualitative research conducted in indoor

classrooms with the goal to identify emergent themes. This qualitative study expands

this area by exploring the frequency and evolving nature of conflict occurrences

within child-initiated play that occur in the outdoor and indoor environment in a

nature-based preschool.

Without empirical research to support the importance of conflict occurrences

in the context of child-initiated play through social emotional and cognitive

Page 30: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

22

development, teachers may continue to abandon outdoor child-initiated play for

teacher directed activities.

Current Study

Empirical research affirms the importance of children’s participation in

conflict as significant to child development noting its importance in the formation and

function of peer relationship (Laursen, Hartup, & Koplas, 1996), moral development,

negotiation skills (Keller, 1984; Wheeler, 2004), and logical thinking (Aasen,

Gridheim & Waters, 2009). Given the importance of children’s participation in

conflict events it is necessary to understand the components and context of the conflict

events. Examining conflict categories in a nature-based preschool during outdoor and

indoor child-initiated play using the framework from Chen et al. adds to the current

literature because this type of research has not been done before.

This study used the definition of conflict by Shantz (1987) as incompatible

behaviors or goals between preschool children during child-initiated free play in a

nature-based preschool, and did not explicitly focus on differentiating between verbal

and social conflicts. The aim of this study was to examine the nature, direction and

resolution or non-resolution of conflict among preschoolers in a nature-based

preschool. The initial conflict catalyst, frequency, duration, interventionist types, and

resolution or non-resolution during both indoor and outdoor child-initiated play were

examined. Conflict resolution without a defined conclusion for this study followed the

protocol used by Laursen and Hartup (1989, p.286) “A conflict was considered

discontinued if 10 s passed after an interruption without resumption of the conflict.”

This study considered a conflict episode occurring “when child A does or says

something that influences child B, child B resists, and child A persists” (Shantz, 1987,

Page 31: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

23

p. 284); and drew from the definitions stated by Laursen and Hartup (1989, p.286)

which defined ‘influence’ as “any attempt by one child to change the behavior of

another” and defined ‘resistance’ as “opposition or refusal to comply.”

This exploratory study sought to understand conflict episodes in the indoor and

outdoor setting among children in a nature-based preschool in order to provide an

understanding of initial conflict catalyst, the evolving conflict scenario and resolution

or no resolution. The aim of this study is to add to the literature and understanding of

peer conflict in nature-based outdoor classrooms to provide insight into the benefits of

outdoor nature play and provide evidence of the importance of increasing children’s

outdoor play.

Research Questions

The outcome driven climate focused on academic goals and skill attainment

has diminished the amount of child-initiated play opportunities for preschool children.

Time spent outdoors has shifted to indoor teacher-instruction time. It can be argued

the outcome driven climate has lost sight of the whole child and the positive benefits

of child-initiated play in the outdoor environment; a context which allows children to

develop and refine their negotiation, communication, reasoning and perspective taking

skills through participation in conflict occurrence with their peers. According to

Shantz (1987), conflict among children should not be considered trivial, as it appears

to have a substantial affective meaning for those involved in the conflict. A study

investigating conflict in the context of child-initiated play in a nature-based preschool

would expand the current literature on the importance of conflict within the realm of

child-initiated play in nature.

This study seeks to answer the following three questions:

Page 32: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

24

1. Does the frequency of conflict episodes differ in indoor and outdoor settings

during child-initiated play in a nature-based preschool?

2. Do catalyst of conflict differ in indoor and outdoor settings during child-

initiated play in a nature-based preschool?

3. Does conflict resolution differ in indoor and outdoor child-initiated play in a

nature-based preschool?

Page 33: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

25

Chapter 3

METHOD

Present Study

This present qualitative descriptive study was informed by piloted research

conducted through running narrative and qualitative method of data collection (Han,

Pic, & Pollock, 2019 March). Naturalistic observation data via video recording were

collected from an outdoor classroom at a nature-based preschool affiliated with a

university laboratory school in the Mid-Atlantic Region.

The aim of this study was to investigate conflict occurrences in the context of

indoor and outdoor child-initiated play in a nature-based preschool, examining peer

conflict development among preschoolers noting the catalyst, frequency, duration,

interventionist type, and resolution or no resolution.

Setting

The Laboratory School operates an outdoor Nature Preschool classroom for

sixteen children ages older 3- to 4- years (fall enrollment ages). The Nature Preschool

operates Monday to Thursday from 9:15 AM to 1:00 PM and Friday 9:15 AM to Noon

(all weather types). The children are outdoors from 9:15 AM to 11:15 AM and again

from 12:00 PM to 1:00 PM. The outdoor setting includes a forested area, a meadow, a

creek, and a pond. The indoor setting is an Art room that doubles as a preschool

classroom in the morning and an elementary school Art room in the afternoon. Snack

Page 34: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

26

time operates as a center and is held outside when the outdoor sink is not closed due to

freezing temperatures. Indoor snack is limited to three children at a time.

The indoor space is 700 square feet and is set up as centers where children are

free to move about as long as the number of designated children for that center is not

exceeded. For example, the science center has two stations for two children at a time.

The centers change along with the lesson plan. Three separate tables with chairs allow

for various activities, such as writing, playdough, painting, table games, and

manipulatives. There is also a light table with magnetic colored tiles for building and

creating, an area rug designated for playing with trains, and blocks; an area rug for

reading books among pillows; and a dramatic play/science center. Children have

access to books, stuffed toys, blocks, puzzles, small manipulatives, dramatic play

props, and art supplies. Indoor layout is displayed in Figure 1.

The outdoor classroom space is approximately 5,000 square feet and provides

children with access to natural materials such as sticks, logs, stones, leaves, seeds,

sand, dirt, mud, tree stumps, water, and living creatures. In addition to nature

materials, child have access to a bird blind, stage, mud kitchen, wooden planks,

wooden pallet, rope swing, opaque double-sided painting board, and large wooden

spools to climb. There are indoor items in the outdoor space such as miniature fairies

with accessories, crayons, markers and colored pencils, plastic small animals, kitchen

utensils, hammer, microplanes, nails, creature collections containers, binoculars, and

butterfly wings costumes. Children are free to move about the outdoor classroom.

Rules govern the use of the rope swing and workbench tools and the number of

children allowed at the picnic table used for the daily art activity. Four children are

Page 35: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

27

allowed to work at the picnic table at a time, two at the workbench and one on the rope

swing. Outdoor layout is displayed in Figure 2.

Children are free to move about the indoor and outdoor space while abiding by

the rules for the number of children permitted at the center/location. There are more

rules governing the number of participants at centers in the indoor space compared to

the outdoor space.

Page 36: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

28

Figure 1 Indoor Setting Layout

Page 37: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

29

Figure 2 Outdoor Setting Layout

Page 38: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

30

Participants

All children enrolled in the outdoor nature-based classroom had consent to

participate in the study. There were sixteen children, average age of 49 months as of

10/04/2019, enrolled in the outdoor-nature based class. The age range was 36 months

to 60 months. Nine of the children were female. Three of the children included in this

study attended the outdoor nature-based preschool program the previous year. Seven

of the children were Dual Language Learners (DLL). None of the focal children had a

documented disability although during a member checking meeting it was discussed

that three of the children were being evaluated for the possibility. Fifteen of the

sixteen children were included in the final analysis for this study (female n=8). See

Table 1 for focal child characteristics.

Page 39: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

31

Table 1 Characteristics of Focal Children

The class was instructed by a lead teacher and an ECE pre-service teacher

completing her practicum. Additional class support was provided by university field

placement students from undergraduate ECE courses and a Laboratory School staff

member. The lead teacher has a BS in ECE and a MS in Literacy, and is a state

licensed teacher who has seventeen years of teaching experience teaching full day

Kindergarten, Pre-K, and Play & Grow. The lead teacher has her Naturalist

Certification. She is also an instructor for pre-service teachers at the University level

and has attended two Natural Start Conferences.

Pre-service teachers completing their practicum are placed in the nature-based

classroom for one semester. The student teacher assigned for the fall semester was

completing her first practicum. Prior to the start of the school year support staff was

Page 40: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

32

assigned to the classroom due to an injury sustained by the lead teacher. The injury

limited the physical abilities of the lead teacher thus necessitating the need for an

additional teacher. The support staff member completed a BS in ECE and a MS in

Human Development and Family Studies with an ECE concentration, and has eight

years of ECE classroom experience and two years of ECE administrative experience.

The Nature Preschool uses an emergent curriculum combining children’s

interests and needs, what is occurring in the nature environment, Teaching Strategies

GOLD indicators, and past experiences. The scope and sequence of the curriculum

themes change year to year dependent upon the children enrolled. The children are

indoors from 11:00 AM to12:30 PM (Noon on Fridays); the remaining time is spent

outdoors. The Laboratory School’s Nature Preschool is the only one in the

surrounding area making it the ideal context to study conflict events and resolution

among preschoolers during child-initiated play in a nature-based environment.

Data Collection

In accordance with IRB requirements, informed parental consent was obtained

for collection of observation data (field notes, photographs, and video-record) and

demographic data for participants. Consent letters explaining the study and requesting

child participation were given to parents of enrolled children, by the researcher, at

morning drop off (Appendix A). The letters requested a parental response of either yes

or no to their child’s participation in the study. The researcher explained the study to

each parent/relative using the parental recruitment script (see Appendix B). The

researcher answered any questions the parents/relative had regarding the study. Two

relatives took the consent letters home to the child’s parent and relayed the

information. Parents either signed the consent letter after speaking to the researcher or

Page 41: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

33

returned the signed consent letter within one week. Parental consent for all enrolled

children (n=16) to participate in the study was received.

Each focal child was assigned a number, 001-016, to ensure confidentiality.

Observation order was determined by the focal child’s assigned number. All focal

children were present on their assigned observation day; thus, the observations were

conducted in numeric order. Demographic class data (child age, gender, ethnicity,

home language, cognitive and/or physical limitations) was obtained from the

Laboratory Preschool Director and Office Manager. The lead teacher provided weekly

lesson plans and informed the researchers about field trips, flipped day (indoors time

followed by outdoor time) due to extreme temperature, absent children, accreditation

visit, etc.

Researchers used naturalistic observation of focal children through video-

record in order to document children in their naturalistic environment engaging in

natural peer interactions. Each focal child was video-recorded for a 30-minute

observation periods during indoor and outdoor child-initiated play (free play) or until

free play session ceased. The focal child observation period began at the start of the

designated outdoor free play (approximately 9:45 AM) and again at the start of indoor

free play (approximately 11:15 AM). The focal child was followed at a reasonable

distance in order to record child-child, and teacher-child interactions but did not

interfere with the child’s free play. One focal child’s observation data were omitted

per the recommendation of the lead teacher after member checking process because it

was considered an unusual day for the child.

Prior to data collection the researcher collecting video data was introduced to

the children. The researcher briefly explained the study, showed the children the video

Page 42: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

34

camera, and answered the children’s questions (Appendix C). After the initial meeting

the researcher spent time with the class first without and then with the video camera in

order to increase the children’s comfort level with the researcher, and to test the

equipment selected for use indoor and outdoor. Video data were collected the first

week of November to the second week of December.

Video-record provides a detailed record of the conflict occurrences capturing

preceding interactions, conflict intervention development and resolution (Garvey,

1984; Shantz, 1987) and allows the researchers to view and review data in order to

provide an accurate assessment (Wheeler, 2004) of conflict events. Researchers did

not interact with focal child or other children unless someone initiated interaction

with the researcher accepting the ‘least adult’ role suggested by Mandell (1988).

Field Notes. The Field Notes Form modified from a pilot study (Han, Pic, &

Pollock, 2019 March) was used at the beginning of each observation session to note

the date, focal child identification number, class attendance, staffing numbers,

weather, and location (outdoor, indoor) (Appendix D). Field notes are used to “assist

in the maintenance of the research instrument” (Marshall, & Rossman, 2016, p.117).

During the collection of the video-record the researcher collected additional field notes

regarding environment information deemed important in order to obtain a complete

picture of the conflict occurrences, such as phrases or words spoken by the focal child

if concern was noted for poor audio quality. There is a train that runs near the outdoor

classroom which occasionally inhibited the microphone from recording verbal

exchanges.

Video Taping. The researcher used a video camera to record focal child

observations in order to record the entire conflict sequence from the catalyst to the

Page 43: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

35

resolution or no resolution. Additionally, videotaping captured both the preceding

interactions, which provided details regarding emotions and prior play interactions,

and the evolving conflict which aided in the determination of conflict interventionist

types and resolution. The researcher followed the focal child at a reasonable distance

as to avoid interfering with the focal child’s activities and peer interactions. The

researcher did not plan to interact with the children. Children’s body language,

expressions and verbal cues were observed to determine if the focal child was

comfortable being recorded. None of the focal children displayed signs of discomfort

with video recording nor inquired about the videotaping. Video-taping allowed

examination of the conflict event in detail noting the catalyst, conflict development

and resolution.

A total of 461 minutes and 52 seconds of indoor video data and 474 minutes

and 31 seconds of outdoor video data was collected. The indoor and outdoor video

data ranged from 20 minutes and 9 seconds to 29 minutes and 59 seconds per focal

child, and 24 minutes and 45 seconds to 29 minutes and 59 seconds per focal child

respectively. The average indoor video data were 28 minutes and 47 seconds per focal

child, and the average outdoor video data were 29 minutes and 38 seconds per focal

child.

Transcripts. Observation data collected through video-record were transcribed

verbatim by one researcher. Field notes provided verification of details when

applicable. Focal children were identified in the transcriptions according to their

identification number. Classmates interacting with the focal child were identified as

classmate 1, classmate 2, etc. according to the order of interaction with the focal child

in the setting (indoor/outdoor). University field placement students were identified as

Page 44: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

36

Student 1, Student 2, etc. in the order they interacted with the focal child. Lead

Teacher, Support Staff and Student Teacher were identified according to their title.

Sixteen indoor and sixteen outdoor transcripts were constructed. Focal child indoor

transcript length ranged from eight pages to twenty-eight pages with an average

transcript length of nineteen pages. The outdoor transcript length ranged from thirteen

pages to thirty-two pages with an average transcript length of twenty-two pages per

focal child. The transcripts were proofread, edited, and printed for analysis and

coding. Prior to coding the transcripts were reviewed to provide an overall picture of

the data.

Conflict Log. The Conflict Log (Appendix E). The Conflict Log was created

from information gathered by the researchers in a pilot study conducted at the Lab

School (Han, Pic, & Pollock, 2019) and informed by Chen et al., (2001) and Killen

and Turiel (1991). Two Conflict Observation Logs were completed, one for conflict

events in the indoor child-initiated play and one for outdoor. The Conflict Log was

used in the coding process to compile data on the frequency of conflict events per

focal child, location of conflict (e.g. light table), duration (determined by start and end

time of conflict event), catalyst, interventionist type, and resolution or no resolution .

Completed indoor and outdoor Conflict Logs were analyzed separately for emergent

themes and patterns, and were used to construct the evolution of the conflict

occurrence noting the exchange pattern, and resolution.

Conflict occurrences were counted per child (outdoor and indoor), and then

combined for a total class count (outdoor and indoor). Conflict events per child were

counted and compared to determine if one child drove the data to determine if specific

Page 45: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

37

child(ren) were driving the data results. It was determined no outliers needed to be

removed from the data.

Data Analysis/Coding

Event Sampling

Event sampling method was used with the transcription data to identify conflict

situations. The event sampling method is used to identify specific behaviors to serve

as the unit of analysis (Miller, 2018), and it is commonly used to discover its causes

and results (Wortham, 1995). Therefore, it is an appropriate method to discover the

conflict situation and its catalyst and resolution. For the purpose of this study, a

conflict was selected as the event and was defined as a social interaction in which an

emotional exchange occurs between two or more individuals that is characterized by

events in which one individual protests, resists, retaliates, or opposes the actions of

another individual, and the second individuals’ action is met with resistance or

acceptance (Chen, Fein, Killen, & Tam, 2001; Garvey, 1984; Hay, 1984; Shantz,

1987).

The indoor and outdoor transcripts were analyzed separately. The conflict

events were marked and tabulated. The identified conflict events were coded with the

focal child’s ID number and event number followed by ID for indoor and OD for

outdoor (e.g. 004_3ID indicates focal child four, third conflict event, indoor setting).

Focal child transcripts which contained identified conflict events were

separated from focal child transcripts that did not contain conflict events. Only the

transcripts which contained conflict events were used in further coding development

(See Figure 3).

Page 46: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

38

Figure 3 Conflict Identification and Transcript Coding Map

Page 47: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

39

Coding Development

After the conflict events were identified and transcripts were developed, an

initial coding process began with the pre-established categories identified by Chen et

al. (2001) and Killen and Turiel (1991). These included the initiating action (physical

harm, psychological harm, distribution of resources, play ideas, social-conventional

issues) and resolution or no resolution. While reading the transcripts, new codes

emerged. After rereading the transcripts and codes, and peer debriefing, three major

categories emerged related to the research questions: catalyst, interventionist type,

resolution outcome and then sub codes were identified and developed under these

three main categories (see Figure 4). The identified codes for catalyst, interventionist

type, and resolution or no resolution were used to construct the Code Book (Appendix

F). Conflict events identified in the transcripts were reviewed in detail to code each

event with the defined codes for catalyst, interventionist type, and resolution or no

resolution.

Page 48: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

40

Figure 4 Conflict Event Coding

Catalyst. Two new catalyst categories emerged from the data and were added

to the previously identified categories (Chen et. al, 2001; Killen & Turiel, 1991):

tickling/touching (non-receptive tickling or touching); and accusation (accusing

someone of a specific action). One new category description emerged and was added

Page 49: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

41

under social conventional-issues: number of center participants. The final codes for

the catalyst are identified and defined in Table 2. Transcripts were reviewed to

extrapolate details of the conflict catalyst in the outdoor and indoor environment. The

researcher coded the catalyst for the onset of noted conflict event: physical harm,

psychological harm, distribution of resources, play ideas, social conventional issues,

tickling/touching or accusation. The catalyst was determined by behaviors from child

A that met the criteria for initiating a conflict which was met with resistance, protest

or retaliation by child B, to which child A responded with insistence or acceptance.

Example of a tickling/touching identified as the catalyst:

002: (reaches over and touches the sparkles on the front of Classmate 4’s shirt)

Classmate 4: Don’t touch it. Stop it you’re tickling me.

002: (touches the sparkles on Classmate 4’s shirt again)

Classmate 4: Stop it. You’re tickling me.

002: (reaches over again and touches Classmate 4’s shirt).

Classmate 4: You’re tickling me. Stop it!

Page 50: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

42

Table 2 Catalyst Categories

Interventionist Types. The data revealed three distinct interventionist types

within the conflict event. The final codes for interventionist type are identified and

defined in Table 3. Interventionist type coding was used to form any combination of

the three distinct interventionist types involved in the resolution process (e.g. CI/PIN

indicates child(ren) interventionist and an uninvolved peer interventionist); ten

combination patterns emerged.

Page 51: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

43

Table 3 Resolution Intervention Types

Participant Coding Description

Child CI child(ren) involved in conflict event

Peer PIN peer not involved in conflict

Teacher TI lead teacher, student teacher, support staff, university students

Emerged Patterns

Coding Description

CI child interventionsist

TI teacher interventionsist

PIN peer interventionsist

CI/TI child interventionsist /teacher interventionsist

CI/PIN child interventionsist /peer interventionsist

TI/CI teacher interventionsist /child interventionsist

CI/PIN/TI child interventionsist /peer interventionsist /teacher interventionsist

CI/TI/CI child interventionsist /teacher interventionsist /child interventionsist

CI/PIN/TI child interventionsist /peer interventionsist /teacher interventionsist

CI/TI/CI/TI child interventionsist /teacher interventionsist /child interventionsist

/teacher interventionsist

Resolution Outcome. Analysis of the conflict conclusion revealed three types

of resolution outcomes. The final codes for resolution or no resolution are identified

and defined in Table 4. Indoor and outdoor transcripts were analyzed to determine the

outcome of the conflict event. Each conflict event was coded as resolution (R), no

resolution (NR) (Chen et al., 2001; Killen & Turiel), or undetermined resolution

(UDR). For the purpose of this study a conflict was coded as R if agreement was

Page 52: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

44

reached through acceptance of the initial request or protest, compromise or

negotiation, or a return to play with deescalated emotion or the emotional state pre-

conflict event. Conflicts that ended due to distraction or interruption, or with high

emotion indicating nonacceptance, were coded as NR. Additionally, a conflict was

coded NR if no conflict resolution was defined, the conflict was considered ended if

10 seconds passed without resuming the conflict (Laursen & Hartup, 1989). Conflict

events without clear resolution or no resolution were coded as UDR.

Conflicts with the same focus that were divided into two intervals (e.g.

interruption or distraction) were coded as individual conflicts with defined ends if

more than 10 seconds elapsed between episodes.

Conflicts ending without a clearly defined resolution or no resolution were

reviewed again alongside the video record to note the details of the child(ren)’s body

language, emotional state, gestures, and facial expressions (e.g. crossed arms, pouty

lip, turning away).

Page 53: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

45

Table 4 Resolution Outcome

Resolution Coding Description Example

Resolution R agreement is

reached, there is

acceptance of the

initial request or

protest, or there

is a return to play

with deescalated

emotion

Classmate 2: You’re still making it blue! (pause) Now

may I have to that? (reaching for blue paint brush)

008: When I am finished.

Classmate 2: mmm… 008 is not letting me have a turn.

Student 1: Did you ask her for a turn?

Classmate 2: Yea (pause) mmmm-mmmm (whinny tone

bottom lip in a pout)

Student 1: Maybe when she’s done with the blue you can

finish using the blue.

Classmate 2: mmmm-mmmm, nnnnn (reaching for blue

paint container)

(008 is holds the blue paintbrush out)

008: Here. I’m giving you the blue paintbrush.

Classmate 2: Oh

No

Resolution

NR ends due to

distraction,

interruption, or

with high

emotion

indicating

nonacceptance

Classmate 2: Can you give me the dinosaur?

009: No. When I’m done.

Classmate 2: Okay (pause) I am waiting for my

tetradactyl. (pause) Are you done now? (classmate 2

attempts to take the dinosaur, 009 moves his arm with the

dinosaur away from classmate 2)

009: No

Classmate 2: May I borrow it now? May I have it?

009: See it’s flying

(009 and classmate 2 are distracted by classmates playing

Zingo)

Undetermined

Resolution

UDR

resolution cannot

be determined

Concluding exchange not in English. Facial expressions,

body gestures do not indicate resolution or no resolution.

Page 54: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

46

Credibility

The researcher used multiple processes to reduce subjectivity and establish

credibility and validity of data analysis. First, peer debriefing addressed potential bias

of the researcher and ensured the analyses were grounded in the data. Throughout the

research process the researcher conferred weekly with the study advisor regarding data

collection, coding and analysis. Member checking with the lead teacher during the

data collection and twice after the collection of data clarified questions, verified

information, and ensured the researcher captured accurate focal child data. Lastly, the

involvement of a non-involved peer coder maintained inter-rater reliability. A non-

involved peer researcher was trained on the definitions developed for coding, the

identification of a conflict event and the coding process. Miles and Huberman (1994)

suggest 80% agreement between coders on 95% of the codes is adequate agreement

among multiple coders to maintain inter-rater reliability. The non-involved peer

reviewed, analyzed and coded 20% of the transcribed data in order to identify conflict

events and apply the coding process to the identified conflict events. Coding

agreement between the two researchers in regard to identifying conflict events was

100%. The coding agreement between the two researchers in regard to coding each

conflict event for the five identified codes (location, catalyst, interventionist type,

participants, and resolution, no resolution outcome) was 93.8%. There were no

differences in agreement with regard to conflict location, involved participants, and

resolution, no resolution or undetermined resolution categories. The conflict events

coded differently were related to categorical agreement of the initial catalyst and

interventionist type. Differences in coding can be resolved through discussion in order

to reach consensus on categorical agreement (Bartholomew, Henderson, & Marcia,

Page 55: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

47

2002) therefore, the two researchers collectively reviewed and discussed the coding

disagreements. The discussion resulted in 100% agreement between coders.

Ethics

This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) at the

University of Delaware (Appendix G). This research was conducted in compliance

according to the standards set forth by the IRB. Parental consent and child assent were

obtained prior to the collection of data. Video data, identifying data and child

demographic information were kept confidential and maintained in a secure server

according to IRB requirements.

Page 56: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

48

Chapter 4

FINDINGS

The findings chapter is divided into three sections. In the first section analysis

was conducted to determine frequency of conflict events in the indoor and outdoor

setting for children as a collective group and per focal child. The second section noted

the observed catalysts which initiated conflict events and explored potential

differences between settings and focal children. The final section investigated emerged

patterns of intervention and the resolutions which followed the interventionist types.

Question 1. Frequency of Conflict Between Indoor and Outdoor Child-initiated

Play in a Nature-based Preschool

The first research question seeks to identify the frequency of conflict events in

both the indoor and outdoor setting in a nature-based preschool, and to investigate

whether conflicts emerged more in the indoor or the outdoor setting. Analyses were

conducted to compare the two settings, looking at overall conflict frequency, and

conflicts by adult presence, child gender and language status, and specific location in

the two settings. The analysis seeks to answer the question: To what extent does the

frequency of conflict differ among children during indoor and outdoor child-initiated

play in a nature-based preschool?

Table 5 presents the findings of research question 1.

Page 57: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

49

Table 5 Focal Child Conflict Events Indoor and Outdoor

Focal

Child

Gender

Dual Language

Learner (DLL)

Conflict Events More Conflict Events

Indoor Outdoor Indoor Outdoor

001 M N 3 1 +

002 F N 5 3 +

004 M Y 12 6 +

005 F Y 0 0

006 M N 2 0 +

007 M N 1 1

008 F Y 0 2 +

009 M N 2 0 +

010 F Y 4 2 +

011 M N 2 1 +

012 F Y 0 0

013 F N 0 3 +

014 F N 1 2 +

015 F Y 1 1

016 M Y 0 0

Total 33 22

Note. DLL Y=Yes; N=No.

A total of 33 indoor and 22 outdoor conflict events were identified. On average

there were 4.58 conflict events per hour in the indoor setting and 2.96 per hour in the

outdoor setting. This finding differs from the findings of Stack and Nikiforidou (2019)

which noted indoor and outdoor conflict were nearly equal. A teacher was present

during 39.39% of indoor and 63.64% of outdoor conflicts. The average indoor conflict

duration was 38 seconds with a duration range of 7 seconds to 1 minute and 57

Page 58: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

50

seconds. Outdoor conflict events lasted longer with an average duration of 50 seconds,

and a duration range of 18 seconds to 2 minutes and 31 seconds.

Three focal children did not engage in indoor or outdoor conflict events during

their observation period; twelve focal children participated in conflict events. Ten

focal children were involved in conflict events indoors, and ten were involved in

outdoor conflict events. Seven children were involved in more indoor conflict events

compared to outdoor; three children engaged in more outdoor conflicts; and five

children had equal indoor/outdoor rates. Of the focal children involved in conflict

event(s) four DLL children were involved in a total of 28 conflict events and eight

monolingual children were involved in a total of 27 conflict events. The rate of

conflict per child for DLL children is more than double the rate of conflicts per child

for monolingual children at a rate of 7 conflicts per child and 3.375 conflicts per child

respectively.

The frequency of indoor and outdoor conflicts varied by gender; males had

more conflicts in the indoor setting (n=22) while females had more conflicts in the

outdoor setting (n=13). Six of seven male focal children engaged in indoor conflicts

events while only four participated in outdoor conflicts. Four of eight female focal

children were involved in indoor conflict events and six of eight participated in

outdoor conflict events. Twenty-two of thirty-one total conflicts for males occurred

indoor (70.97%) and thirteen of twenty-four total conflicts for girls occurred outdoor

(54.17%).

Conflict events in the indoor space were concentrated at the light table (n=18)

followed by the middle activity table (n=6). Conflict events in the outdoor setting were

Page 59: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

51

spread out by location, the jumping stump (n=5) had the highest incidents of conflict

followed by the open play space (n=4), the stage (n=3) and the mud kitchen (n=3).

Question 2. Catalyst Differences Among Preschool Children During Child-

Initiated Play Indoor and Outdoor in a Nature-Based Preschool

The second question explored whether the frequency and types of catalysts that

initiated the conflict differ in the indoor and outdoor setting. Analyses were conducted

to compare the catalyst in the two settings. The second research question sought to

answer the question: How are the catalysts by which conflict emerges different among

preschool children during child-initiated play indoor and outdoor in a nature-based

preschool?

The findings demonstrated distribution of resources conflicts (75.75%) were

more prevalent in the indoor space as supported by previous research (Brenner &

Mueller 1982; Chen, et al., 2001) however, play idea conflicts (54.54%) were more

prevalent in the outdoor space (Table 6).

Indoor distribution of resources conflicts were generated by a limited supply of

magnetic light tiles, a red train engine, a one of a kind pillow, and an icing bag used

for decorating cookies. Four of nine outdoor distribution of resources catalysts were

generated by non-nature-based items (green rake, plastic turtle, and blue paint). Four

play idea conflicts in the indoor space revolved around building ideas and a request to

join a playframe. Play idea conflicts in the outdoor space focused on roles in play,

types of play in which to engage, and the intended use of an item during play.

Limitations placed on how many children could be present in a center generated three

instances of social-conventional issues in the indoor setting.

Page 60: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

52

Figure 5 Frequency of Catalyst that Initiates Conflict

There was a slight difference in distribution of resources conflicts by gender in

the indoor and outdoor setting. Five males and three females were involved in indoor

distribution of resource conflicts while one male and five females were involved in

outdoor distribution of resource conflicts. Play idea conflicts were almost the same for

males and females in the indoor and outdoor setting. One male and two females were

involved in indoor play idea conflicts, and four males and three females were involved

in outdoor play idea conflicts.

Page 61: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

53

Example 1. Play Idea (Outdoor). Play idea conflict occurs during play when

one child’s new idea or wish for the play scenario changes and conflicts with a peer’s

idea (Shantz, 1987). The catalyst is a conflict over a play idea between two classmates

and focal child 011. Classmates 7 and 8 are sitting on a plank in the obstacle area

playing with a leaf. Focal child 011 approaches the play frame and wants to play a

game of chase where he plays the role of a dinosaur. This conflict event lasted 46

seconds.

011: RAWH! RAAAWH! (011 makes dinosaur roars to classmate 8

and 7)

Classmate 8: I don’t like that. (looks at researcher)

(classmate 7 reaches to get a leaf from classmate 8’s right hand.

Classmate 8 gives classmate 7 the leaf)

(011 stands very close to classmate 8 and 7 and looks around while

classmates 8 and 7 sit on the plank of the obstacle course)

Classmate 7: We’re taken a break here. (classmate 7 looks up at the

researcher after speaking. 011 completely ignores classmate 7 and

looks up and around standing about 2 inches from classmate 7 who is

seated on a plank)

Classmate 8: Go (classmate 8 swings her leg in a kicking motion

towards 011)

(classmate 8 raises arm and points towards 011)

Classmate 8: Not going!

(011 responds by leaning over to where classmate 8 and looking her in

the eyes at a close range)

Classmate 8: Ahh (classmate 8 leans backwards to move away) Ahhh.

Hey. Stop it!

(support staff is helping another classmate walk on the obstacle course

and is standing about 3 feet from 011)

Support Staff: What are you doing 011?

Classmate 8: Stop it.

(011 pulls on his pocket)

Classmate 7: Stop it. Can’t you see classmate 8 doesn’t like it.

(011 is standing close to classmate 8 while looking up and around

ignoring the support staff and classmate 7)

Classmate 8: Leave the space here.

(011 walks away)

Page 62: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

54

Example 2. Distribution of Resources (Indoors). Conflicts over distribution of

resources occurs during child’s play when one child desires an object in another

child’s possession (Shantz 1987). The catalyst for this conflict is over a pretend icing

bag used to decorate gingerbread cookies. The conflict occurs in the dramatic play

kitchen. The focal child, classmates 4 and 6, and the lead teacher are present along

with the focal child. The conflict duration is 27 seconds.

(classmate 4 holds a pretend icing tube in her right hand and pretends

she is applying icing to her cookie. 011 leans back and watches

classmate 4)

Lead Teacher: I love the way you’re putting icing on him.

(classmate 6 leans forward and reaches with her left hand)

Classmate 6: Can I try icing?

(011 reaches with his right hand towards the icing bag)

011: I I I

(011 grabs a hold of the icing with both hands and tries to pull it from

classmate 4’s hands)

Lead Teacher: It’s just for pretend. I think classmate 4’s (lead teacher

places her hands on the icing bag and on 011’s right hand) using it

right now.

(011 continues to try and take the icing bag from classmate 4)

011: Can I use it? Can I have it?

Classmate 4: No

Classmate 6: Please may I have a turn?

(classmate 4 is holding the icing bag in her right hand and 011 has

both his hands on the icing bag. The lead teacher places her pinkie

finger between the icing bag and 011’s right hand)

Lead Teacher: Hear her? Make make your cookie then (011 releases

his grasp on the icing bag with his right hand but continues to hold it

with his left hand) come over to do it.

(011 lets go of the icing bag and classmate 4 takes a step away from

the table)

Classmate 6: Please may I have a turn with the cream?

Classmate 4: Okay. (classmate 4 hands the icing bag across the table to

classmate 6)

Page 63: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

55

011: I I I I want (011reaches for the icing bag) I want to have a turn

with the…(interrupted)

Lead Teacher: You don’t have cookie. (classmate 6 pulls the icing bag

down to her right side. 011 let’s go) What are you going to decorate

(pause) use as a cookie to decorate?

(011 reaches in to the bowl of dough and takes out two handfuls. 011

places the dough on the table and begins trying to flatten the dough.)

Question 3: Conflict Resolution Among Children in the Context of Child-

Initiated Play in a Nature-Based Preschool

The final research question evaluated the emerged interventionist types, child,

peer, or teacher, present during the conflict process and the resulting outcome:

resolution, no resolution or undetermined. Analysis of the three types of resolutions

were conducted looking for patterns in the indoor and outdoor setting. Examining

interventionist types may help predict if one interventionist type is more successful in

achieving conflict resolution. The analysis sought to answer the question: How are

indoor and outdoor conflict events resolved among children in the context of child-

initiated play in a nature-based preschool?

Eight resolution interventionist types emerged; five interventionist types were

documented in the indoor setting and all eight were present in the outdoor setting. This

finding suggests the outdoor setting may provide richer and more complex conflict

negotiation and resolution participation and may provide insight into the duration

differences between indoor and outdoor conflict events. Figure 2 displays the emerged

interventionist type by setting.

Page 64: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

56

Table 6 Interventionist Type by Setting

Indoor Interventionist Types Outdoor Interventionist Types

CI child interventionist CI child interventionist

TI teacher interventionist TI teacher interventionist

CI/TI child interventionist/teacher

interventionist

CI/TI child interventionist/teacher

interventionist

CI/PIN child interventionist/peer

interventionist

CI/PIN child interventionist/peer

interventionist

CI/TI/CI child interventionist/teacher

interventionist/child

interventionist

CI/TI/CI child interventionist/teacher

interventionist/child

interventionist

TI/CI teacher interventionist/child

interventionist

CI/PIN/TI child interventionist/peer

interventionist/teacher

interventionist

CI/TI/CI/TI child interventionist/teacher

interventionist/child

interventionist/teacher

interventionist

Focal child patterns of child interventionist types were consistent between the

indoor and the outdoor settings indicating one child did not generate the pattern

results. Of the three child interventionist types employed in the indoor setting, the

prominent interventionist type was child interventionists. Eight focal children were

Page 65: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

57

involved in the child interventionist type, two children were involved in child

interventionist/teacher interventionist/child interventionist type, and one child was

involved in child interventionist/peer interventionist type. In the outdoor setting of the

four child interventionist types, six focal children were involved in child

interventionist type, four children were involved in child interventionist/teacher

interventionist/child interventionist type, one in child interventionist/peer

interventionist and one is teacher interventionist/child interventionist.

The frequency of indoor and outdoor resolution by interventionist type is

displayed in Table 7. Due to the low number of peer interventionist type the child and

peer interventionists types were combined into one group for analysis.

Table 7 Frequency of Indoor and Outdoor Resolution by Interventionist Type

Interventionist

Type

Indoor Interventionist Resolution Outdoor Interventionist Resolution

Child/Peer N (%) Teacher N (%) Child/Peer N (%) Teacher N (%)

TI 4 (18.2)

CI 13 (59.1) 5(38.5)

CI/PIN 1 (4.5) 1(7.7)

CI/TI 1 (4.5) 3 (23.1)

TI/CI 1 (7.7)

CI/TI/CI 3 (13.6) 2 (15.4)

CI/PIN/TI 1 (7.7)

Total (%) 17 (77.2) 5 (22.7) 9 (69.3) 4 (30.8)

Page 66: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

58

A collective analysis of resolved conflicts revealed indoor conflicts (n=33)

were resolved 66.66% of the time. Outdoor conflicts (n=22) resulted in a lower

resolution percentage (59.09%). The most successful interventionist type for reaching

a resolution for both the indoor and the outdoor setting were child interventionist (CI).

Example 3. Child Interventionist (Outdoor). The conflict between focal child

013 and classmate 6 is an example of an unresolved outdoor conflict. The conflict

occurred at the fairy garden in the outdoor play space and began when classmate 6

expressed desire for the plastic turtle the focal child was using. The conflict did not

have a defined conclusion due to a distraction. The conflict duration was 50 seconds.

013: Quick. I know you who you are. Come on hop on my turtle.

(013 extends her arms out for classmate 6 to hold her fairy on the

turtle’s back)

Classmate 6: I’ll take the turtle. (classmate 6 tries to take the turtle

from 013)

013: No. (013 pulls her arms back away from classmate 6)

Classmate 6: Let’s switch. (classmate 6 holds out her piece to offer for

the switch)

013: No. (013 backs away from classmate 6)

Classmate 6: Heyyyy that’s not nice (spoken in a whinny voice)

(classmate 6 turns away from 013)

Classmate 6: That’s not nice.

(classmate 6 turns slightly back towards 013. Classmate 6 stands with

her arms crossed)

Classmate 6: I want the turtle.

013: But can this be both our turtle our pet?

(classmate 6 takes the turtle from 013 and the turtle falls to the ground,

classmate 6 bends down to pick up the turtle)

013: Can this be both our pets? Classmate 6?

Classmate 6: No because I want it.

(classmate 6 stands holding the turtle, 013 starts to walk towards

classmate 6 and sees something on the ground)

(013 bends down to look more closely at the object which is a small

Styrofoam pumpkin ornament)

013: Huh? Ah look! What’s that? (013 looks up at classmate 6)

Page 67: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

59

Classmate 6: I don’t know. (classmate 6 walks over to see what 013

found on the ground)

013: Ooooo hmm (013 picks up the object) A pumpkin? What?

(both 013 and classmate 6 smile and giggle)

(the conflict over the turtle is interrupted and a discussion begins about

the little pumpkin being eaten by a squirrel)

013: Maybe this get eat by a squirrel! (the word squirrel is said loudly

and in a dramatic tone)

Of the total indoor conflicts resolved by child/peer interventionist types, 14

(82.4%) were resolved without the involvement of a teacher, and 3 (17.6%)

resolutions involved a teacher. Six (66.7%) outdoor conflicts were resolved by

child/peer interventionists types without the involvement of a teacher and 3 (33.3%)

involved teacher presence. During indoor conflict resolutions, child interventionist

type occurred as the last interventionist type 2.3 times more than teacher

interventionist type. In the outdoor setting child interventionist type occurred last 2.6

times more than teacher intervention type.

Example 4. Child Interventionist-Peer Interventionist Type (Indoor). The

conflict event began when the focal child 002 entered the play frame of classmates 1

and 4, and requested to help classmate 1 build her tower. The focal child was standing

at the light table across from classmate 1 across and with classmate 4 to her left.

Classmates 1 and 4 are building with magnetic light tiles. Focal child 002 tried to

negotiate her way into the play frame. The conflict concluded with classmate 4

offering focal child 002 a solution which she accepted. The event lasted 33 seconds.

002: Hey, can I help you build?

Classmate 1: I don’t need help.

(002 does not accept classmate 1 doesn’t want help and places a

magnetic tile on classmate 1’s structure and then glances up to see if

classmate 1 is watching her. 002 watches classmate 1’s face for a

moment, then attempts to place a second light tile on the structure. 002

Page 68: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

60

glances up a second time at classmate 1 and tries to place another tile

on classmate 1’s structure)

002: That’s ok.

(002 continues to try and place tiles while watching classmate 1’s eyes

in between placing tiles. When 002 notices classmate 1 watching her

place a tile she attempts to hand classmate 2 the magnetic tile)

002: Here.

Classmate 1: Mine doesn’t need those. (002 pulls her arm back from

trying to place the small

triangle light tile on classmate 1’s structure) It just needs these.

Classmate 4: You can help me build my little houses. I mean my little

blocks.

002: Okay.

Classmate 4: It’s easy (classmate 4 holds up his cube to show 001)

Seventy percent (n=14) of indoor conflict events that involved only child/peer

interventionist types [CI (n=18), CI/PIN (n=2)] were resolved by children. One indoor

conflict resolution could not be determined and therefore was not included in the

resolution analysis. Outdoor conflicts that involved only child/peer interventionist

types [CI (n=10) CI/PIN (n=1)] were resolved by children 54.5% (n=6) of the time.

These findings are higher than child resolution percentages found by Chen et al.

(2001) for children 3- to 4-years old.

Example 5. Child Interventionist (Indoor). Focal child 006 was playing Zingo

at the center activity table with classmates 2 and 7. Focal child 006 was seated at the

end of the table with classmate 2 on his right. Classmate 7 was seated to the right of

classmate 2. The conflict began when the focal child took the game tile away from

classmate 2. The conflict was resolved by the involved children. The conflict duration

was 19 seconds.

Classmate 2: Ouh. I have a bug. (classmate 2 holds the bug tile and

looks for the bug picture on her Zingo card)

Page 69: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

61

006: I have a bug! (006 reaches over and takes the bug tile out of

classmate 2’s hands)

Classmate 2: Oh. (classmate 2 sounds surprised)

(classmate 7 takes the dispenser to begin his turn)

Classmate 2: Hey! (pause) (classmate 2 looks at her card) Hey! I had

that. I have a bug!

006: Where? (006 continues to hold the bug tiles and looks towards

classmate 2’s card)

Classmate 2: Right there. (classmate 2 points to the picture of a bug on

her game card) That’s it!

(006 looks at classmate 2’s card)

006: Oh. (006 takes the bug piece off his card and places it on

classmate 2’s card)

Indoor conflicts resolved with teacher interventionist (n=5) as the last

interventionist type ended in resolution 50% of the time. Indoor conflicts with

child/peer interventionist as the last interventionist type ended in resolution 73.9% of

the time. Outdoor conflicts resolved with teacher interventionist type were resolved

66.8% of the time. Child/peer interventionist as the last interventionist type were

resolved 56.3% of the time in the outdoor setting.

Example 6. Child Interventionist-Teacher Interventionist Type (Outdoor). The

conflict began when classmate 4 noticed focal child 014 dragging a wooden plank

across the open play space. Focal child 014 verbally attempted to get classmate 4 off

of the end of the plank. When the strategy was unsuccessful the focal child solicited

the help of the student teacher. The conflict was resolved when classmate 4 accepted

the student teacher’s explanation and gets off the plank. The conflict lasted 2 minutes

and 32 seconds.

Classmate 4: Bouncy!

(classmate 4 straddles the plank and sits on the end of the plank that is

dragging along the ground)

Page 70: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

62

014: No ba. No Classmate 4! No! (pause) (014 looks around scanning

the open play space)

(classmate 4 continues to sit on the end of the plank and bounce)

014: No Classmate 4. Get off! Raa-Ehhh (014 tries to hold up her end

of the plank while classmate 4 sits on the other end more towards the

middle and bounces. 014 looks up and sees the student teacher walking

towards her)

014: Ah Miss Student Teacher he keeps going on here when I do this.

He keeps bumping on…(interrupted by the student teacher addressing

classmate 4)

Student Teacher: Classmate 4 what’s going on? (student teacher goes

over and takes classmate 4’s hand) Classmate 4 what’s going on?

014: Just get him off.

Classmate 4: Rrrrrrr (classmate 4 leans back to be dead weight for the

student teacher)

Student Teacher: Classmate 4 I need you to get off (student teacher

let’s go of classmate 4’s hand and steps back) because…

Classmate 4: I’m

Student Teacher: ….you could hurt

Classmate 4: Don’t wanna get off.

Student Teacher: I understand but if 014 is trying to move that you

could hurt her.

(classmate 4 gets off the plank)

Student Teacher: Thank you

Analysis of conflict resolution in which the only interventionist type was

teacher interventionist revealed 50% (n=4) of indoor and 0% (n=0) of outdoor

conflicts were resolved. Interventionist type data are found in Appendix H.

Page 71: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

63

Chapter 5

DISCUSSION

Previous research has examined preschoolers’ peer conflict events, how they

present in a nature-based preschool has not been examined. The findings of this study

begin to fill this gap. This study examined and described conflict events among

preschool children in an outdoor nature-based preschool. In this study an exploration

of conflict catalysts, interventionist types and resolutions was undertaken. The results

provide a deeper understanding of indoor and outdoor conflict event frequencies,

catalysts which propel the conflict, participant involvement as interventionist, and

patterns of resolution or no resolution in the context of a nature-based preschool.

Results from this current study are explored and discussed using Vygotsky’s belief in

the importance of play as a context for child development (Vygotsky,1966, 1978) and

Bronfenbrenner’s (1977, 1979, 1986) bioecological systems theory which states a

child’s environment must be considered in the child’s developmental processes

through a bidirectional interconnection between the child and their surroundings.

Frequency of Indoor and Outdoor Conflict

The first research question focused on the frequency of indoor and outdoor

conflict occurrence. The findings in this study demonstrate preschoolers peer conflict

is present during free play in both the indoor and outdoor environment. The presence

of conflict in the outdoor space during free play suggests learning and development

are occurring in this context. This finding supports both outdoor play as a viable

Page 72: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

64

context for social and cognitive development drawing on Vygotsky’s belief in the

importance of play in child development and previous research that identified conflicts

as naturally occurring, positive opportunities for children to develop social and

cognitive skills (Chen et al., 2001, Shantz, 1987). The results of this study demonstrate

the healthy conflict present in the outdoor setting provides children with the

opportunity to develop problem solving, turn taking, perspective taking, negotiation,

conflict resolution skills, and friendship building.

Based on piloted research (Han, Pic, & Pollock, 2019), the overall rate of

conflict in the indoor setting was expected to be higher than the outdoor setting. This

study supports the initial findings of the piloted research noting the rate of indoor

conflict was higher than the outdoor rate. The lower outdoor conflict rate may provide

children with additional time to engage in outdoor free play and allow more

opportunities for deep play, learning, and development due to a reduction in the

amount of time spent on conflict management.

For the focal children in this classroom, this study revealed a higher number of

children were involved in more conflict events in the indoor setting (n=7) compared to

the outdoor setting (n=3). This difference may be due to a variety of indoor factors

such as limited resources (e.g. magnetic light tiles), available space, non-natural

lighting, noise level, and restrictions governing the number of center participants.

Initial results from this study suggests gender may play a role in conflict

frequency by setting. Males were found to engaged in more conflict events in the

indoor setting while females engaged in more conflict events in the outdoor setting.

The gender findings for the indoor space are consistent with previous studies in which

boys were found to have more conflicts than girls (Malloy & McMcurray, 1996). This

Page 73: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

65

was not the case for the outdoor setting; girls engaged in more conflict events

compared to indoor. Due to the small sample size the indoor and outdoor gender

difference may be due to chance and needs further exploration.

It was expected that indoor conflicts would reveal more teacher presence than

outdoor conflicts given the differences in square footage between the indoor and

outdoor setting, the openness of the outdoor environment, and the expected proximity

of teachers to children. The opposite results emerged in this study. This finding may

be due to the difference in field placement student’s presence. Field placement

students were present for the duration of the outdoor sampling time but were present

for less than half of the indoor sampling time, increasing the ratio of teacher-to-child

indoors. On average the teacher-child ratio in the indoor setting was 1:4 while the

outdoor setting ratio was 1:3. These ratios are lower than the recommended ratios

from NAEYC (1:10) (2018), and the Washington State Department of Children,

Youth, and Families (1:6) (n.d.) for a group size of sixteen children ages 30 months-6

years. Washington State is the only state which issues licenses for the operation of an

outdoor nature-based preschool; the previously mentioned ratio applies to outdoor

preschools. Although the square footage of the outdoor setting is larger than the

indoor, the increase in the number of teachers allowed for teacher presence in the

majority of the outdoor area where children engaged in activities. The teacher-to-child

ratio may have impacted the frequency of teacher presence in outdoor conflicts.

Catalysts Which Initiate Conflict

The second research question explored the catalysts by which conflict is

initiated. Previous research has identified conflicts over possession and use of objects

(distribution of resources) as the primary catalyst for conflict among preschool

Page 74: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

66

children (Shantz, 1987). The findings from this study found distribution of resources

conflicts were most prominent in the indoor setting but not in the outdoor setting. This

finding does not support previous research on disputes over possessions during indoor

and outdoor play which concluded possession-based disputes were nearly equal (Stack

& Nikiforidou, 2019) in both settings. Play idea disputes emerged as the most

prevalent catalyst in the outdoor space. The outdoor setting catalyst findings of this

study may be due to the nature of the outdoor setting which provides more

opportunities for deep engaged play, more play choices, materials, and the

construction and reorganization of the play space (Bohling et al., 2010; Fjørtoft,

2004).

Interventionist Types and Resolutions

According to Shantz (1987) children generally resolve peer conflicts on their

own without teacher intervention while other studies found teacher intervention as the

most prevalent resolution type (Malloy & McMurray, 1996). The final research

question investigated interventionist types and successful resolution interventionist

patterns to explore whether children were the primary interventionists in peer conflict

events. In this study the simple child interventionist (CI) resolution pattern was

demonstrated as the most frequently employed pattern in both settings. The findings

demonstrate children were able to engage in conflict resolution and resolve conflict

without adult intervention in both the indoor and outdoor setting.

Overall, the findings of this study demonstrated child-initiated play in the

indoor and outdoor setting provided children with the opportunity to engage in peer

conflict, intervention, and resolutions, providing children social and cognitive

development opportunities. This is consistent with Bronfenbrenner’s belief that the

Page 75: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

67

child’s environment and those with whom a child interacts influence the child’s

development. Allowing children to be a part of the negotiation process affords them

the ability to learn to take the perspectives of peers, merge the peers’ perspective with

an understanding of their own needs and desires in order to achieve higher levels of

thinking, and to build and maintain relationships (Church, Mashford-Scott &

Cohrssen, 2018; Wheeler, 2004) . Additionally, conflict negotiation supports problem-

solving and enables children to discover and practice workable compromises (Church,

et al., 2018). All of which have been identified as skills necessary for the formation

and maintenance of friendships (Denham, et al., 2003). Rizzo (1992) suggests

conflicts do not have to reach resolution to produce benefits, the act of engaging in

conflicts with peers allows children the opportunity to develop their relationship with

one another. It must be acknowledged that teachers play a key role in conflict

management to prevent conflicts from escalating to a critical level.

Limitations

The small sample size and the collection of data at one location are major

limitations to this study. The sample size and one study site may not provide a

complete representation of children’s peer interactions as each play session has a new

agenda where peers, age, context, and physical and emotional states change (Cordoni

et al., 2016). Findings from this study may be different than other nature-based

preschools due to factors such as children enrolled, teacher-student ratio, the space

utilized by this program, and this program’s lead teacher’s qualifications, certification

and experience; therefore finding are limited to this study and are not generalizable.

This sample focused on typically developing children and did not include

children with documented disabilities. Malloy and McMurray (1996) noted children

Page 76: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

68

with disabilities were more likely to ignore another child’s strategy, and tend to use

conflict strategies not used by typically developing children.

The number of University field placement students present was not expected.

In addition to their presence, they were learning how to support children in the

management of conflict. They were practicing providing guidance and scaffolding for

children to learn conflict resolution strategies. This field placement assignment may

explain the high percentage of teacher involved conflicts in the outdoor space, and the

interventionist patterns in which a teacher was involved but was not the final

interventionist in the conflict event.

Another possible limitation is focal child changes in behavior due to the

presence of a researcher. To address the potential of researcher presence affecting

participant behaviors the researcher spent time with the class prior to data collection,

however there exists the possibility that a child’s behavior changed due to the presence

of the researcher.

Conclusion and Implications

This study adds to the literature and understanding of peer conflict in outdoor

nature-based programs and provides an example for investigating conflict in nature-

based preschools. Analysis of the data emerged two conflict catalyst categories not

previously identified. The exploration of the children and teachers’ role within the

resolution process emerged interventionist patterns consistent in both the indoor and

the outdoor.

In 2014 Dennis and colleagues noted a lack of research demonstrating the

effectiveness of outdoor nature-based programs to produce behavior, learning and

developmental outcomes. Despite the limitations value can be gained from this

Page 77: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

69

research though the findings demonstrating the outdoor setting provides opportunity

for children to participate in and resolve conflict participation thus providing children

the opportunity to practice perspective taking, communication, negotiation, and self-

regulation skills.

Additionally, this study provides insight into the benefits of outdoor nature

play and provides evidence supporting the importance of increasing children’s outdoor

free play time as a means for developing social-emotional and cognitive skills.

A broader study across multiple nature-based site locations that gathers data at

multiple time points would provide a richer understanding of conflict within nature-

based preschools. This study provides a foundation for assessing children’s roles as

participants in the conflict resolution process which could be expounded upon and

used to inform teacher practices. The identified intervention types should be further

explored to add a deeper understanding of conflict management. A larger study should

map out the verbal strategies used by children, peers, and teachers during the

interventionist role to help explain how and when children use resolution tactics to

resolve peer conflict. A deeper exploration of possible connections between employed

strategies and outcomes as well as further analysis to identify strategies used by

teacher in the TI/CI and CI/TI/CI patterns may provide insight into how children use

modeled information to manage in future peer conflict events.

Lastly, dual language learners in this classroom were involved in conflicts

at higher rates than monolingual children. DLL children were more likely to be

involved in a conflict in the outdoor environment. This may be due to DLL children’s

lack of nature vocabulary to express their needs or desires in the outdoor environment.

To provide insight to the results of this study, a larger in-depth study should explore

Page 78: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

70

conflict differences among DLL and monolingual children in a nature-based preschool

assessing language and communication skills.

Page 79: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

71

REFERENCES

Aasen, W., Grindheim, L., & Waters, J. (2009). The outdoor environment as a site for

children's participation, meaning-making and democratic learning: Examples

from Norwegian kindergartens. Education 3-13, 37(1), 5-13. doi: 10.1080/

Aras, S. (2016). Free play in early childhood education: A phenomenological study.

Early Childhood Development and Care, 186(7), 1173-1184. doi:

10.1080/03004430.2015.1083558

Bassok, D., Latham, S., & Rorem, A. (2016). Is kindergarten the new first grade?

AERA Open, 2(1), 233285841561635. doi:10.1177/2332858415616358

Berkhout, L., Bakkers, H., Hoekman, J., & Goorhuis-Brouwer, S. M. (2013).

Observing free play in classrooms with an instrument based on video

analysis. Early Child Development and Care, 183(1), 125-136.

doi:10.1080/03004430.2012.658385

Bodrova, E., Germeroth, C., & Leong, D. J. (2013). Play and self-regulation lessons

from Vygotsky. American Journal of Play 6(1), 111-123.

Page 80: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

72

Bohling, V., Saarela, C., & Miller, D.L. (2010). This never would have happened

indoors: Supporting preschool-age children’s learning in a Nature Explore

classroom in Minnesota. White Paper. Retrieved from

https://dimensionsfoundation.org/wpcontent/uploads/2016/07/skillsforestlkmn

_10.pdf

Brenner, J., & Mueller, E. (1982). Shared meaning in boy toddlers' peer

relations. Child Development, 53(2), 380-391. doi:10.1111/j.1467-

8624.1982.tb01327.x

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of human development.

American Psychologist, 32, 513-531.

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by

nature and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a context for human

development: Research perspectives. Developmental Psychology, 22(6), 723-

742. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.22.6.723

Brown, S., & Vaughan, C. (2009). Play : How it shapes the brain, opens the

imagination, and invigorates the soul. New York: Avery.

Page 81: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

73

Chen, D., Fein, G., Killen, M., & Tam, H. (2001). Peer conflicts of preschool children:

Issues, resolution, incidence, and age-related patterns. Early Education &

Development, 12(4), 523-544. doi:10.1207/s15566935eed1204_3

Church, A., Mashford-Scott, A., & Cohrssen. (2018). Supporting children to resolve

disputes. Journal of Early Childhood Research, 16(1) 92-103.

Cordoni, G., Demuru, E., Ceccarelli, E., & Palagi, E. (2016). Play, aggressive conflict

and reconciliation in pre-school children: What matters? Behaviour, 153(9-11),

1075-1102. doi:10.1163/1568539X-00003397

Corsaro, W. A. & Rizzo, T. A. (1990). Disputes in the peer culture of American and

Italian nursery school children. In A. D. Grimshaw (Ed.), Conflict Talk (pp.

22-66). Great Britain: Cambridge University Press.

Craft, A., McConnon, L., & Paige-Smith, A. (2012). Child-initiated play and

professional creativity: Enabling four-year-olds’ possibility thinking. Thinking

Skills and Creativity, 7(1), 48-61. doi:10.1016/j.tsc.2011.11.005

Davies, R., & Hamilton, P., (2018) Assessing learning in the early years’ outdoor

classroom: Examining challenges in practice, Education 3-13, 46(1), 117-129.

doi: 10.1080/03004279.2016.1194448

Page 82: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

74

Denham, S., Blair, K., DeMulder, E., Levitas, J., Sawyer, K., Auerbach-Major, S., &

Queenan, P. (2003) Preschool emotional competence: Pathway to social

competence? Child Development 74(1), 238-256. doi: 10.1111/1467-

8624.00533

Dennis, S., Wells, A., & Bishop, C. (2014). A post-occupancy study of nature-based

outdoor classrooms in early childhood education. Children Youth and

Environments, 24(2), 35-52. doi:10.7721/chilyoutenvi.24.2.0035

Fjørtoft, I. (2004). Landscape as playscape: The effects of natural environments on

children's play and motor development. Children Youth and

Environments, 14(2), 21-44. Retrieved

from https://www.jstor.org/stable/10.7721/chilyoutenvi.14.2.0021

Frost, J. L., Worthman, S., & Reifel, S. (2012). Play and Child Development 4th

Edition. Boston, MA: Pearson.

Garvey, C. (1984). Children’s talk. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Gray, P. (2011). The decline of play and the rise of psychopathology in children and

adolescents. American Journal of Play, 3(4) 443-463.

Green, M. & Piel, J. (2010). Theories of human development: A comparative approach

(2nd ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Page 83: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

75

Han, M., Pic, A., & Pollock, K. (2019, March) The effects of Nature-Preschool on

children’s language, conflict resolution, and child-initiated learning. Paper

presented at The Association for the Study of Play & International Play

Association U.S.A. Harrisonburg, VA

Hay, D. F. (1984). Social conflict in early childhood. In G. Whitehurst (Ed.) Annals of

child development (Vol. 1, pp. 1-44). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press Inc.

Johnson, J. E., Christie, J. F., & Wardle, F. (2005). Play, development, and early

education. Boston: Pearson.

Keller, M. (1984). Resolving conflicts in friendship: The development of moral

understanding in everyday life. In W. Kurtines & J. Gewirtz (Eds.). Morality,

Moral Behavior, and Moral Development (pp. 140-158).New York: A Wiley-

Interscience Publication.

Killen, M., & Turiel, E. (1991). Conflict resolution in preschool social

interactions. Early Education and Development,2(3), 240-255.

Kuo, M., Barnes, M., & Jordan, C. (2019). Do experiences with nature promote

learning? Converging evidence of a cause-and-effect relationship. Frontiers in

Psychology, 10. Retrieved from

https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00305/full#h12

Page 84: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

76

Laursen, B., Finkelstein, B., & Betts, N. (2001). A developmental meta-analysis of

peer conflict resolution. Developmental Review, 21(4), 423-49.

Laursen, B., & Hartup, W. (1989). The dynamics of preschool children's

conflicts. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 35(3), 281-297.

Laursen, B., Hartup, W., & Koplas. (1996). Understanding peer conflict. Merrill-

Palmer Quarterly, 42(2), Invitational Issue: Conflicts in families and between

children: Advances in theory and research, 76-102.

Louv, R. (2008). Last child in the woods: Saving our children from nature-deficit

disorder. Chapel Hill, N.C: Algonquin Books of Chapel Hill.

Malloy, H., & McMurray, P. (1996). Conflict strategies and resolutions: Peer conflict

in an integrated early childhood classroom. Early Childhood Research

Quarterly, 11(2), 185- 206.

Mandall, N. (1988). The least-adult role in studying children. Journal of

Contemporary Ethnography, 16(4), 433-467.

Marshall, C., & Rossman, G. B. (2016). Designing Qualitative Research. Los

Angeles, CA: SAGE.

Maynard, D. (1985). How children start arguments. Language in Society, 14(1), 1-29.

Page 85: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

77

Maynard, T., Waters, J., & Clement, J., (2013a). Moving outdoors: Further

explorations of ‘child-initiated’ learning in the outdoor environment.

Education 3-13, 41(3), 282-299. doi:10.1080/03004279.2011.578750

Maynard, T., Waters, J., & Clement, J. (2013b). Child-initiated learning, the outdoor

environment and the ‘underachieving’ child. Early Years, 33(3), 212-225.

doi:10.1080/09575146.2013.771152

Miles, M. B., & Huberman, A. M. (1994). Qualitative data analysis: An expanded

sourcebook (Second Edi). London: Sage Publications.

Miller, S., (2018). Developmental Research Methods. Los Angeles, CA: SAGE.

Moore, R. (2014). Nature Play & Learning Places. Creating and managing places

where children engage with nature. Raleigh, NC: Natural Learning Initiative

and Reston VA: National Wildlife Federation. Retrieved from

https://conservationtools.org/library_items/1320-Nature-Play-Learning-Places-

Creating-and-Managing-Places-Where-Children-Engage-with-Nature

National Association for the Education of Young Children (NAEYC). (2018). Staff-

to-child ratio and class size. Retrieved from

https://www.naeyc.org/sites/default/files/globally-

shared/downloads/PDFs/accreditation/early-learning/staff_child_ratio.pdf

Page 86: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

78

Owens, R.E. (1996) Language Development: An Introduction. Needham Heights,

MA: Allyn & Bacon

Parke, R. D. & Slaby, R. G. (1983). The development of aggression. In E. M.

Hetherington (Ed.), P. H. Mussen (Series Ed.), Handbook of child psychology:

Vol. 4, Socialization, personality, and social development, 4th ed. (pp. 547-

641). New York: Wiley.

Pelletier, J. (2011). Supporting early language and literacy. What works? Research in

Practice 37. Retrieved from

http://www.edu.gov.on.ca/eng/literacynumeracy/inspire/research/WW_Early_

Language.pdf

Piaget, J. (1932). Moral judgement of the child. New York: Free Press. Retrieved from

https://archive.org/details/moraljudgmentoft005613mbp/page/n19

Ramsey, P. (1986) Possession disputes in preschool classrooms. Child Study Journal,

16(3), 173-181.

Rizzo, T. (1992) The role of conflict in children’s friendship development. New

Directions for Child Development, 58, 93-111.

Rosa, E. M. & Tudge, J. (2013). Urie Bronfenbrenner’s theory of human development:

Its evolution from ecology to bioecology. Journal of Family Theory & Review,

5, 243-258.

Page 87: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

79

Shantz, C. U. (1987). Conflict between children. Children Development, 58, 283-305.

Stack, J., & Nikiforidou, Z. (2019). Preschoolers’ possession-based disputes indoor

and outdoor play, Early Child Development and Care, 1-14. doi:

10.1080/03004430.2019.1649667

Stephenson, A. (2003). Physical risk-taking: Dangerous or endangered? Early Years,

23(1), 35-43. doi:10.1080/0957514032000045573

Sutterby, J. A., & Frost, J. (2006). Creating play environments for early childhood:

Indoors and out. In B. Spodek & O. N. Saracho (Eds.), Handbook of research

on the education of young children (2nd ed.). 305-321. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence

Erlbaum Associates.

Taylor, A. F., & Kuo, F. E. (2006). Is contact with nature important for healthy child

development? State of the evidence. In C. Spencer & M. Blades (Eds.),

Children and their environment: Learning using and designing spaces, 124-

140. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Tovey, H. (2007). Playing outdoors: Spaces and places, risk and challenge. Berkshire,

England: Open University Press.

Page 88: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

80

Ulset, V., Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., Bekkhus, M., & Borge, A. I. H. (2017). Time

spent outdoors during preschool: links with children’s cognitive and behavioral

development. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 52, 69–80. doi:

0.1016/j.jenvp.2017.05.007

Vygotsky, L. S. (1966). Play and its role in the mental development of the child.

Translated by N. Versov and M.Barrs. International Research in Early

Childhood Education, 7(2) 3-25. Retrieved from

https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/EJ1138861.pdf

Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Interactions between learning and development. In L. S.

Vygotsky & M.Cole. Mind in Society. (pp. 79-91). Retrieved from

doi:10.12691/education-5-1-4

Washington State Department of Children, Youth, and Families. (ND) Outdoor

Preschool Pilot Standards-“The Touchstone Standard’, p. 74. Retrieved from

https://www.dcyf.wa.gov/sites/default/files/pdf/Outdoor_Preschool_Pilot_Stan

dards.pdf

Wells, N. M., & Evans, G. W. (2003). Nearby nature: A buffer of life stress among

rural children. Environment and Behavior, 35(3), 311-330.

doi:10.1177/0013916503035003001

Wheeler, E. J. (2004). Conflict resolution in early childhood: Helping children

understand and resolve conflicts. Upper Saddle River, N.J: Pearson Education.

Page 89: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

81

Wood, E. A. (2014). Free choice and free play in early childhood education: Troubling

the discourse. International Journal of Early Years Education, 22, 4-18.

doi:10.1080/09669760.2013.830562

Wortham, S. C. (1995). Measurement and evaluation in early childhood education.

Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Page 90: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

82

Appendix A

PARENTAL CONSENT LETTERS

Page 91: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

83

Page 92: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

84

Page 93: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

85

Page 94: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

86

Page 95: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

87

Page 96: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

88

Appendix B

PARENTAL SCRIPT

Verbal Recruitment Script-Parental

“Good morning/Good afternoon, my name is Annette Pic and I am a master’s student

at the University of Delaware in Human Development and Family Sciences. I am

interested in understanding peer interactions in outdoor nature-based education.

Nature-based education appears to create unique experiences for children, however

there is little research on peer interactions in nature-based preschools. To help fill this

gap I will be conducting a research study in the Lab School’s Outdoor Nature-based

classroom for the next three months. I would like to request the participation of your

child. This parental consent form details the information regarding the study and

requests your permission to observe and video-record your child during their outdoor

and their indoor free play time. Your child will not be identified by name at any point

in the study or in the concluding thesis or conference presentation. Thank you for your

consideration in allowing your child to participate.”

Page 97: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

89

Appendix C

CHILD SCRIPT

Script used for speaking to the children in group setting:

“Hi. My name is (researcher’s name) and I am here to learn about how children play.

Today I am here to watch you play. In a few days I will be using my camera (hold up

camera) to record the playing you do with your friends.”

On the day of the focal child’s observation:

If a child asks what we are doing, we will reply, “We are videoing to learn about how

you play with your friends.”

Page 98: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

90

Appendix D

FIELD NOTES FORM

Conflict Observation Field Notes Outdoor/Indoor

Focal Child: __________________________

Date: ________________________________

Class Attendance: ______________________

Weather: _____________________________

Conflict

Observed

Time Location Notes

Page 99: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

91

Appendix E

CONFLICT LOG INDOOR/OUTDOOR

Page 100: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

92

Page 101: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

93

Appendix F

CODE BOOK

Peer conflict occurrences and resolutions in a nature-based preschool

Definitions:

Conflict- a social interaction in which an emotional exchange occurs between two or

more individuals that is characterized by events in which one individual protests,

resists, retaliates, or opposes the actions of another individual, and the second

individuals’ action is met with resistance or acceptance (Chen, Fein, Killen, & Tam,

2001; Garvey, 1984; Hay, 1984; Shantz, 1987)

Interventionist Type- participants who employ strategies during the conflict event that

either continue the conflict or attempt to resolve the conflict

Resolution- agreement is reached, there is acceptance of the initial request or protest,

or there is a return to play with deescalated emotion

No Resolution- conflict that ends due to distraction, interruption, or with high emotion

indicating nonacceptance will be considered unresolved after 10 seconds elapses

without the continuation of the conflict

Undetermined Resolution- conflict ending cannot be determined by verbal exchange,

gestures or body language, and does not fit the either the definition for resolution or no

resolution

General Information

ID=Indoor

OD=Outdoor

001-016 focal child identification number

Classmate 1-15- refers to children in the class with the focal child

Lead Teacher- full time teacher

Support Staff- staff member assigned to the classroom

Student Teacher- university student in the practicum placement

Student 1, 2, 3, etc.- university student in fieldwork placement

Student Worker- university student providing classroom support

Page 102: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

94

Locations: coded in orange

Indoor Conflict Locations

SC science center

MAT middle activity table

LT light table

CWT carpet area with trains

DPK dramatic play kitchen

CBS carpet near bookshelf

ST snack table

Outdoor Conflict Locations

OPS open play space

WB workbench

S stage

JS jumping stump

RS rope swing

PT picnic table

OC obstacle course

MK mud kitchen

ST snack table

Conflict Catalyst: coded in green (Chen et al., 2001; Killen & Turiel, 1991)

PH Physical harm (hitting, biting, punching, kicking)

PSH Psychological harm (name-calling, teasing)

DR Distribution of resources (fairness issues such as violation of sharing, turn

taking, grabbing, taking)

PI Play ideas (who will do what, how, when and where, use of space)

SCI Social-conventional issues (violation of class or school rule about running

indoors, how to take turns, or cleaning up time and procedure, number of

center participants)

T Tickling/touching (non-receptive tickling or touching)

A Accusation (accusing someone of a specific action)

Interventionist Type: coded in purple

CI child interventionist (child(ren) involved in conflict event)

TI teacher interventionist (lead teacher, student teacher, support staff,

university students)

PIN peer interventionist (peer not involved in conflict)

CI/TI child interventionist-teacher interventionist

CI/PIN child interventionist-peer interventionist

TI/CI teacher interventionist- child interventionist

CI/PIN /TI child interventionist-peer interventionist-teacher interventionist

Page 103: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

95

CI/TI/CI child interventionist-teacher interventionist-child interventionist

CI/TI/CI/TI child involvement-teacher interventionist

Resolution Outcome: coded in blue

R resolution (agreement reached, acceptance of request or protest, return to play

with deescalated emotions)

NR no resolution (distraction, interruption, high emotion indicating nonacceptance)

UDR undetermined resolution (not possible to determine if a resolution was reached)

Participants: coded in pink

f1c1 focal child and 1 classmate

f1c2 focal child and 2 classmates

f1c1a1 focal child, 1 classmate, adult

f1c2a1 focal child, 2 classmates, adult

Page 104: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

96

Appendix G

IRB FORMS

Page 105: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

97

Page 106: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

98

Page 107: EXPLORING CONFLICT OCCURRENCES IN A NATURE-BASED …

99

Appendix H

INTERVENTIONIST TYPE DATA

Indoor Intervention Outcome Result.2 Count

CI or

PIN R

TI R

CI or

PIN NR

TI NR

CI UDR

Outdoor Intervention Outcome Result.2 Count

CI or

PIN R

TI R

CI or

PIN NR

TI NR

TI-R 4 0 4 0 0 0 TI-NR 1 0 0 0 1

TI-NR 4 0 0 0 4 0 CI-R 5 5 0 0 0

CI-R 13 13 0 0 0 0 CI-NR 5 0 0 5 0

CI-NR 5 0 0 5 0 0 CI/PIN-R 1 1 0 0 0

CI-UNR 1 0 0 0 0 1 CI/TI-R 3 0 3 0 0

CI/PIN-R 1 1 0 0 0 0 TI/CI-R 1 1 0 0 0

CI/TI-R 1 0 1 0 0 0 CI/TI/CI-R 2 2 0 0 0

CI/TI-NR 1 0 0 0 1 0 CI/TI/CI-NR 2 0 0 2 0

CI/TI/CI-R 3 3 0 0 0 0 CI/PIN/TI-R 1 0 1 0 0

33 17 5 5 5 1

CI/TI/CI/TI-NR 1 0 0 0 1

22 9 4 7 2