effect of maternal pre-pregnancy underweight and average...

9
1 SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2018) 8:12014 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-30514-6 www.nature.com/scientificreports Effect of maternal pre-pregnancy underweight and average gestational weight gain on physical growth and intellectual development of early school-aged children Chao Li 1 , Ni Zhu 2 , Lingxia Zeng 1 , Shaonong Dang 1 , Jing Zhou 1 , Leilei Pei 1 , Victoria Watson 3 , Tao Chen 3 , Duolao Wang 3 & Hong Yan 1,4,5 The aim of this study was to assess the effect of low maternal weight at pre-pregnancy and the average gestational weight gain on undernourished children and their intellectual development. From October 2012 to September 2013, we followed 1744 offspring of women who participated in a trial conducted from 2002 to 2006. Pregnant women recruited in the original trial could receive three prenatal health checks for free, at which maternal weight and height were measured. WISC-IV was used to estimate the intellectual development of children. Weight and height of both pregnant women and children were measured by trained anthropometrists using standard procedures. Having low maternal weight at pre- pregnancy was associated with an increased risk of undernutrition amongst children (underweight: OR = 2.02, 95%CI: 1.14–3.56, thinness: OR = 2.79, 95%CI: 1.50–5.17) and a decrease in verbal comprehension index (2.70 points, 95%CI: 4.95–0.44) of children. The effect of average gestational weight gain on occurrences of underweight children (OR = 0.08, 95%CI: 0.01–0.55) was also found. We identified the effect of maternal pre-pregnancy underweight on impairment of the separate intellectual domains (verbal comprehension index) and increasing occurrence of undernourished children. Average gestational weight gain was positively associated with a decreased prevalence of underweight children but not with the intellectual development of children in rural China. Maternal nutrition before and during pregnancy is recognized as being an important factor for offspring health 1 . Although a number of studies in developed countries focused on the association between high maternal pre-pregnancy BMI (overweight or obesity) and offspring obesity and intellectual development 24 , studies on pre-pregnancy underweight and associated outcomes are rarely studied. In many developing countries maternal underweight was more common than maternal overweight 5 . Recent research suggests that offspring of under- weight mothers may also have impaired intellectual development. Maternal underweight is a known risk factor for low birth weight which is predictor of adverse brain development 6,7 . In addition, evidence from the US cohort study showed low pre-pregnancy BMI was associated with increased risk of delayed intellectual development of 2-year-old children 8 . To the best of our knowledge, the effect of maternal underweight on offspring occurrence of stunting, thinness and underweight in childhood was rarely reported in developing countries. 1 Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, School of Public Health, Xi’an Jiaotong University Health Science Center, Xi’an, China. 2 Department of Health Information, Shaanxi Provincial Center for Disease Control and Prevention, Xi’an, China. 3 Department of Clinical Sciences, Liverpool School of Tropical Medicine, Pembroke Place, Liverpool, United Kingdom. 4 Nutrition and Food Safety Engineering Research Center of Shaanxi Province, Xi’an, China. 5 Key Laboratory of Environment and Genes Related to Diseases, Xi’an Jiaotong University, Xi’an, China. Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to H.Y. (email: [email protected]) Received: 14 September 2017 Accepted: 23 July 2018 Published: xx xx xxxx OPEN

Upload: others

Post on 04-Jul-2020

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Effect of maternal pre-pregnancy underweight and average ...archive.lstmed.ac.uk/9106/1/Sci_Report_Effect of... · world, but also the latest edition of Wechsler intelligence scale

1SCIENTIFIC RepoRts | (2018) 8:12014 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-30514-6

www.nature.com/scientificreports

Effect of maternal pre-pregnancy underweight and average gestational weight gain on physical growth and intellectual development of early school-aged childrenChao Li1, Ni Zhu2, Lingxia Zeng1, Shaonong Dang1, Jing Zhou1, Leilei Pei1, Victoria Watson3, Tao Chen3, Duolao Wang3 & Hong Yan1,4,5

The aim of this study was to assess the effect of low maternal weight at pre-pregnancy and the average gestational weight gain on undernourished children and their intellectual development. From October 2012 to September 2013, we followed 1744 offspring of women who participated in a trial conducted from 2002 to 2006. Pregnant women recruited in the original trial could receive three prenatal health checks for free, at which maternal weight and height were measured. WISC-IV was used to estimate the intellectual development of children. Weight and height of both pregnant women and children were measured by trained anthropometrists using standard procedures. Having low maternal weight at pre-pregnancy was associated with an increased risk of undernutrition amongst children (underweight: OR = 2.02, 95%CI: 1.14–3.56, thinness: OR = 2.79, 95%CI: 1.50–5.17) and a decrease in verbal comprehension index (−2.70 points, 95%CI: −4.95–0.44) of children. The effect of average gestational weight gain on occurrences of underweight children (OR = 0.08, 95%CI: 0.01–0.55) was also found. We identified the effect of maternal pre-pregnancy underweight on impairment of the separate intellectual domains (verbal comprehension index) and increasing occurrence of undernourished children. Average gestational weight gain was positively associated with a decreased prevalence of underweight children but not with the intellectual development of children in rural China.

Maternal nutrition before and during pregnancy is recognized as being an important factor for offspring health1. Although a number of studies in developed countries focused on the association between high maternal pre-pregnancy BMI (overweight or obesity) and offspring obesity and intellectual development2–4, studies on pre-pregnancy underweight and associated outcomes are rarely studied. In many developing countries maternal underweight was more common than maternal overweight5. Recent research suggests that offspring of under-weight mothers may also have impaired intellectual development. Maternal underweight is a known risk factor for low birth weight which is predictor of adverse brain development6,7. In addition, evidence from the US cohort study showed low pre-pregnancy BMI was associated with increased risk of delayed intellectual development of 2-year-old children8. To the best of our knowledge, the effect of maternal underweight on offspring occurrence of stunting, thinness and underweight in childhood was rarely reported in developing countries.

1Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, School of Public Health, Xi’an Jiaotong University Health Science Center, Xi’an, China. 2Department of Health Information, Shaanxi Provincial Center for Disease Control and Prevention, Xi’an, China. 3Department of Clinical Sciences, Liverpool School of Tropical Medicine, Pembroke Place, Liverpool, United Kingdom. 4Nutrition and Food Safety Engineering Research Center of Shaanxi Province, Xi’an, China. 5Key Laboratory of Environment and Genes Related to Diseases, Xi’an Jiaotong University, Xi’an, China. Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to H.Y. (email: [email protected])

Received: 14 September 2017

Accepted: 23 July 2018

Published: xx xx xxxx

OPEN

Page 2: Effect of maternal pre-pregnancy underweight and average ...archive.lstmed.ac.uk/9106/1/Sci_Report_Effect of... · world, but also the latest edition of Wechsler intelligence scale

www.nature.com/scientificreports/

2SCIENTIFIC RepoRts | (2018) 8:12014 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-30514-6

Although previous results showed that gestational weight gain is associated with an increased BMI of offspring in childhood, adolescence and early adulthood9–12, few studies reported the effect of gestational weight gain on the risk of undernutrition in childhood. The data examining the association between gestational weight gain and child intellectual development is limited13–15. Results are also inconsistent, with a large European cohort study reporting a decrease in offspring intelligence with increasing gestational weight gain13, whereas a null finding on the relation between gestational weight gain and offspring intelligence was reported in 2 studies14,15.

For childhood physical growth, many studies also determined the negative effect of malnutrition on their motor, cognitive and social-emotional development7,16,17. For childhood intellectual development, results from a series of population-based prospective cohort studies indicated that childhood intelligence quotient (IQ) is associated with further leadership success and school achievement and is inversely associated with several health outcomes ascertained in later life18–20. Therefore, understanding the effect of maternal BMI and gestational weight gain on offspring physical and intellectual development is vital in developing countries10. The aim of this study is to clarify the effect of low pre-pregnancy weight and gestational weight gain on physical and intellectual develop-ment at early school-aged children. The study field was rural areas of China and the type four villages, the poorest. Therefore, results from this study could provide important reference for improving physical development and intellectual development at early school-aged children in poor areas of developing countries.

MethodsStudy design and participants. The present follow-up study was conducted from October 2012 to September 2013. We followed the offspring of women who had participated in the large trial of prenatal micronu-trient supplementation and remained residents in the original study area. The baseline information was obtained from this trial, and the results of this trial are described elsewhere21–23. In summary, this was a double-blind, clus-ter-randomized, controlled trial of prenatal supplementation with three different combinations of micronutrients implemented from 2002 to 2006 that aimed to determine the effect of prenatal micronutrient supplementation on birth weight in two rural counties in Shaanxi Province of Northwest China. The enrolled pregnant women in the same village were randomly assigned to 3 supplementation groups (daily folic acid, folic acid plus iron, or mul-ti-micronutrients), and all pregnant women in the same village were allocated to the same treatment. To ensure geographic balance, villages were also stratified according to township and county. Finally, 5828 pregnant women from 531 villages enrolled, and there were 4604 single live births21–23.

In the present follow-up study, we excluded migrations because of the limitation of funding and it was not pos-sible to trace them. More importantly, the migrations were not representative of rural China. Households with eli-gible children were invited to participate in the local hospital or school in a standardized manner. Parental written informed consent and child assent were obtained. By September 2013, 96 children had died, and 1643 children had moved out of the study area with their family. Of the remaining 2865 children eligible for inclusion in present study, we followed 1744 of these eligible children (159 participates refused to take part or were not able to come after ≥3 reminders; 962 unknown location). Our previous published results reported participants and those who did not take part in the early school-aged follow-up study did not differ in any enrollment measure22. From this group, we also excluded women with missing information about important variables, i.e., pre-pregnancy BMI (n = 437), and average gestational weight gain (n = 581) (Fig. 1).

This follow-up study was designed to estimate the intellectual function differences among prenatal micronu-trients supplementation groups, a minimum of 426 children (142 in each group) was needed to detect 5 Full-Scale intelligence Quotient (FSIQ) points between groups with type I error and 80% power. Sample size in the present study (n = 1307) was large enough to detect 5 FSIQ points between maternal pre-pregnancy underweight and normal weight group. According to the results of previous studies, 5-IQ point difference was considered clinically significant because it is of the order of magnitude associated with IQ differences in children who were exposed to high lead concentrations or were fed breast milk rather than formula as infants24,25. The study was conducted in accordance with the declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the Xi’an Jiaotong University Health Science Center.

MeasurementsMaternal pre-pregnancy BMI and gestational weight gain. Maternal BMI during the first trimester is being used as a proxy for pre-pregnancy BMI As most women, especially in the undernourished populations, do not gain much weight in the first trimester (usually only a couple kilograms in US populations) and self-re-ported pre-pregnancy weights are often misreported. Findings from a Caucasian study indicate mean maternal weight did not change at all in the first trimester26. Pre-pregnancy BMI was categorized as underweight (BMI < 18.5), normal-weight (18.5 ≤ BMI < 25), overweight (25 ≤ BMI < 30), or obese (BMI ≥ 30)27. Pregnant women recruited in the original large trial could receive three prenatal health checks for free, at which maternal weight and height were measured at the clinic by trained maternal and child health (MCH) staff. In addition, pregnant women were interviewed to record their sociodemographic status such as educational levels, occupations of par-ents, and the number of older siblings and their menstrual, reproductive, medical, and family history. The total gestational weight gain was calculated from the weight measured in the third trimester (the last prenatal health check) and maternal weight measured in the first trimester (0–14 weeks of gestational age). Gestational age used to calculate weekly gestational weight gain was calculated from the weeks of gestation in the last prenatal health check in the third trimester minus weeks of gestation in the first trimester. Weekly gestational weight gain is calculated from total gestational weight gain divided by gestational age. Gestational age at birth was measured as completed days based on the first day of the last menstrual period.

Page 3: Effect of maternal pre-pregnancy underweight and average ...archive.lstmed.ac.uk/9106/1/Sci_Report_Effect of... · world, but also the latest edition of Wechsler intelligence scale

www.nature.com/scientificreports/

3SCIENTIFIC RepoRts | (2018) 8:12014 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-30514-6

Physical growth. The physical growth was estimated by weight and height of children in the present follow-up study. The anthropometric measurements of early school-aged children were measured using standard procedures by trained staff. Weight was recorded in standard school clothing (without shoes) using an elec-tronic scale (Tanita BC-420, Tanita Corporation, Tokyo, Japan) with precision to the nearest 10 g, and height (barefooted) was recorded using a calibrated stadiometer (Model SZG-210, Shanghai JWFU Medical Apparatus Factory, Shanghai, China) with precision to the nearest 0.1 cm. Weight-for-age z-scores, height-for-age z-scores (HAZ), and body mass index-for-age z-scores (BAZ) were derived from these observational data based upon 2007 ref.28. Stunting, thinness and underweight children were defined as HAZ, BAZ and WAZ of ≤−2, respectively.

Intellectual development. The fourth edition of Wechsler intelligence scale for children (WISC-IV) which can be applied to children aged 6 to 16 years is not only among the most widely used intelligence tests in the world, but also the latest edition of Wechsler intelligence scale for children. In the present study, WISC-IV which has been commercialized and standardized to be culturally appropriate in China, was used to evaluate the intel-lectual development of early school-aged children. Currently, the reliability and validity of these measures were evaluated and shown to be satisfactory29. There are 10 core subtests (Block Design, Similarities, Comprehension, Vocabulary, Picture Concepts, Digit Span, Letter–Number Sequence, Matrix Reasoning, Coding and Symbol Search) and 4 supplemental subtests (Picture Completion, Information, Cancellation and Arithmetic) included in WISC-IV. In addition, a FSIQ, which represents overall cognitive ability, and 4 other composite scores (verbal comprehension index VCI, working memory index WMI, processing speed index PSI, and perceptual reasoning index PRI) which represent different domains of cognitive function30 could been generated from WISC-IV. In more detail, VCI represents the ability in verbal reasoning and concept formation. WMI represents the ability to

Figure 1. Participant flow chart.

Page 4: Effect of maternal pre-pregnancy underweight and average ...archive.lstmed.ac.uk/9106/1/Sci_Report_Effect of... · world, but also the latest edition of Wechsler intelligence scale

www.nature.com/scientificreports/

4SCIENTIFIC RepoRts | (2018) 8:12014 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-30514-6

sustain attention, concentrate, and exert mental control, concentrate. PSI represents the ability in routine visual material without making errors or processing simple. PRI is designed to measure the ability to separate ground and figure in visual stimuli, and fluid reasoning in the perceptual domain31.

WISC-IV is a standardized test, with the same scoring criteria and method, and the same order of adminis-tered subsets. The intelligence test was done by five postgraduate students in psychology, and they were overseen and rigorously trained by LC who qualified to administer the WISC-IV and served as psychometrician. The five students were certified to collect data when they performed an accurate test administration and scoring. To ensure the children would not be interrupted, all the intelligence tests were conducted in the local hospital or pri-mary school where the hospital or school was asked to provide 5 quiet and separated rooms. After field work was completed each day, the answers of intelligence tests recorded by each student were reviewed by the psychometri-cian to ensure the scoring accuracy of each child.

Statistical analysis. All data were checked manually for completeness and double-entered for verification. Range, extremum and logical checks were conducted for accuracy. A 5% significance level was used for all sta-tistical tests, and testing was two-sided. The distributions of baseline information in different groups of maternal pre-pregnancy nutritional status were described by their means, standard deviations, and percentages. In addi-tion, the ownership of 16 different household facilities or assets were used to construct a household wealth index using principal components method to assign a weight for each assets32, and the household wealth index was categorized as tertiles indicating the poorest, middle and richest households.

In the present study, multilevel mixed-effects generalized linear models (township to level 3, village to level 2, and individual to level 1) were used to analyze the effect of maternal pre-pregnancy nutritional status and average gestational weight gain on intellectual development (WISC-IV tests scores) and malnutrition status (underweight, stunting and thinness) of early school-aged children, and the effects were reported in terms of the estimations of coefficients and 95% confidence intervals (CIs). As maternal overweight and obesity is associated with risk of several diseases in offspring, the main results were reported after excluding overweight and obesity participants. Previous studies identified age of children, sex of children, gestational weeks, economic status, pre-natal micronutrients supplementation, birthweight and other social backgrounds as detrimentally affecting cog-nitive development of children22,28,33,34. In addition, the majority of variables mentioned above were also reported as being associated with physical growth of children in previous studies35–37. Therefore, we considered the follow-ing variables as potential confounders in the multivariate adjusted analysis: age of children, age of parents, sex of children, gestational weeks at birth, type of micronutrient supplement, household wealth index, older siblings of children, birthweight, educational level of parents, father’s occupation of parents, child school level.

To test the reliability of the analysis, two models were fitted with one model adding variables of social back-grounds (the age of children, age of parents, sex of children, educational level of parents, occupation of parents, household wealth index), and the other model including the variables of social backgrounds plus the variables of older siblings of children, child school level, gestational weeks at birth, weight at birth, type of prenatal micro-nutrient supplementation. When we estimated the effects of gestational weight gain, an additional variable of maternal BMI at pre-pregnancy was considered. Stata software 14.0 was used for all data analysis (Stata Corp LP, College Station, Texas 77845, USA).

ResultsTable 1 shows the baseline characteristics of children and their households in different groups of maternal pre-pregnancy nutritional status. The prevalence of maternal overnutrition (overweight or obesity) and those underweight was 2.5% (overweight: 32/1307; obesity: 1/1307) and 15.4% (201/1307) respectively. For the char-acteristics of children, the mean age was 8.78 (SD, 0.82) years; the majority of children (40.1%) were studied in schools of township level; mean FSIQ, VCI, WMI, PRI and PSI of investigated children were 89.58, 88.02, 91.36, 93.19 and 95.71 respectively, and WISC-IV tests scores of children were higher in the maternal non-underweight group, except for PRI and PSI. For nutritional status of children, the prevalence of those underweight (11.0%), stunting (6.0%) and thinness (12.4%) were higher in maternal underweight group compared with 6.1%, 3.5% and 5.5% in non-underweight group. Average gestational weight gain (0.36 kg/wk) was higher in the maternal underweight group than maternal non-underweight group (0.33 kg/wk).

Table 2 shows the associations between maternal pre-pregnancy nutritional status, average gestational weight gain and malnutrition status of early school-aged children. After adjusting for the confounders, the effect of low maternal weight on an underweight childhood (Model 1: OR = 1.99, 95%CI: 1.14–3.49; Model 2: OR = 1.95, 95%CI: 1.11–3.45) and thinness (Model 1: OR = 2.91, 95%CI: 1.62–5.23; Model 2: OR = 2.65, 95%CI: 1.43–4.92) remained significant. A negative association between average gestational weight gain and childhood underweight was found (OR = 0.09, 95%CI: 0.02–0.53) after controlling for the confounders (model 1). Results from model 2 suggested every 1 kg/wk increasing of average gestational weight gain, the odds of being underweight will decrease 0.08 (95% CI: 0.01–0.55) times.

Table 3 shows the effects of low maternal weight at pre-pregnancy on VCI scores. After adjusting for con-founders, we found the VCI score (Model 1: 2.75 points, 95%CI: −4.99–0.50; Model 2: 2.70 points, 95%CI: −4.95–0.44) was significantly lower in the maternal underweight group compared with the non-underweight group. However the effects of low maternal weight at pre-pregnancy was not found on FSIQ (Model 2: −0.76 points, 95%CI: −2.60–1.08), WMI (Model 2: −0.07 points, 95%CI: −1.85–1.71), PRI (Model 2: 0.84 points, 95%CI: −1.12–2.79) and PSI Model 2: 1.43 points, 95%CI: −0.47–3.34) scores.

Page 5: Effect of maternal pre-pregnancy underweight and average ...archive.lstmed.ac.uk/9106/1/Sci_Report_Effect of... · world, but also the latest edition of Wechsler intelligence scale

www.nature.com/scientificreports/

5SCIENTIFIC RepoRts | (2018) 8:12014 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-30514-6

Maternal pre-pregnancy BMI

TotalUnderweight Non-underweight

201 1106 1307

Child characteristics

Child age, mean ± SD, y 8.83 ± 0.81 8.77 ± 0.83 8.78 ± 0.82

School, n (%)

   Village 68(33.8) 380(34.4) 448(34.3)

   Township 66(32.9) 458(41.4) 524(40.1)

   County 67(33.3) 268(24.2) 335(25.6)

Birth weight, mean ± SD, kg 3.09 ± 0.44 3.22 ± 0.43 3.20 ± 0.43

Gestational age at birth, mean ± SD, wk 39.69 ± 1.53 39.84 ± 1.66 39.82 ± 1.64

FSIQ, mean ± SD 89.22 ± 14.87 89.65 ± 13.13 89.58 ± 13.41

VCI, mean ± SD 86.39 ± 15.60 88.31 ± 15.78 88.02 ± 15.76

WMI, mean ± SD 90.99 ± 13.06 91.43 ± 12.24 91.36 ± 12.36

PRI, mean ± SD 93.98 ± 14.30 93.05 ± 13.62 93.19 ± 13.73

PSI, mean ± SD 96.48 ± 14.81 95.57 ± 12.73 95.71 ± 13.06

Weight for age

   Underweight 22(11.0) 67(6.1) 89(6.8)

   Non-underweight 179(89.0) 1039(93.9) 1218(93.2)

Height for age

   Stunting 12(6.0) 39(3.5) 51(3.9)

   Non-stunting 189(94.0) 1066(96.5) 1255(96.1)

BMI for age

   Thinness 25(12.4) 61(5.5) 86(6.6)

   Non-thinness 176(87.6) 1043(94.5) 1219(93.4)

Women’s characteristics

Maternal age, mean ± SD, y 33.06 ± 4.20 34.14 ± 4.57 33.98 ± 4.53

Parity, n (%)

   0 146(72.6) 674(60.9) 820(62.7)

   1 50(24.9) 371(33.6) 421(32.2)

   ≥2 5(2.5) 61(5.5) 66(5.1)

Women’s education, n (%)

   Primary 68(33.8) 413(37.4) 481(36.8)

   Secondary 98(48.8) 564(51.0) 662(50.7)

   ≥High school 35(17.4) 128(11.6) 163(12.5)

Women’s occupation at enrollment, n (%)

   Farmer 122(61.6) 729(67.2) 851(66.4)

   Others 76(38.4) 355(32.8) 431(33.6)

Average gestational weight gain, mean ± SD, kg/wk 0.36 ± 0.21 0.33 ± 0.16 0.33 ± 0.17

Others

Type of prenatal micronutrients supplementation, n (%)

   Folic acid 70(34.8) 363(32.8) 433(33.1)

   Iron/folic acid 71(35.3) 372(33.6) 443(33.9)

   Multimicronutrients 60(29.9) 371(33.6) 431(33.0)

Maternal age, mean ± SD, y 35.86 ± 4.36 36.67 ± 4.50 36.54 ± 4.49

Father’s education, n (%)

   Primary 32(16.0) 190(17.2) 222(17.0)

   Secondary 120(60.0) 685(62.1) 805(61.7)

   ≥High school 48(24.0) 229(20.7) 277(21.3)

Father’s occupation at enrollment, n (%)

   Farmer 55(27.8) 414(38.2) 469(36.6)

   Others 143(72.2) 671(61.8) 814(63.4)

Wealth index at enrollment, n (%)

   Poorest 58(28.9) 356(32.2) 414(31.7)

   Middle 66(32.9) 370(33.4) 436(33.3)

   Richest 77(38.3) 380(34.4) 457(35.0)

Table 1. Baseline characteristics of children and households by pre-pregnancy nutritional statusa. Abbreviation: FSIQ, Full-Scale Intelligence Quotient; PRI, Perceptual Reasoning Index; PSI, Processing Speed Index; VCI, Verbal Comprehension Index; WMI, Working Memory Index. aValues are n(%) or means ± SDs.

Page 6: Effect of maternal pre-pregnancy underweight and average ...archive.lstmed.ac.uk/9106/1/Sci_Report_Effect of... · world, but also the latest edition of Wechsler intelligence scale

www.nature.com/scientificreports/

6SCIENTIFIC RepoRts | (2018) 8:12014 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-30514-6

DiscussionIn this study we found a negative effect of low maternal weight at pre-pregnancy on nutritional status (under-weight and thinness) and verbal comprehension in early school-aged children. Average gestational weight gain was positively associated with a decrease in prevalence of underweight children but not with the intellectual development of children.

Results from many previous studies suggest a detrimental influence of low and high pre-pregnancy BMI and gestational weight gain on physical growth and intellectual development12,38,39. For physical growth, results from a systematic review showed that high pre-pregnancy BMI was associated with offspring overweight or obesity2. Another tow US studies reported that excessive gestational weight gain is significantly associated with the risk of being overweight in the offspring at 5 and 7 years of age10,11. To the best of our knowledge, the effect of mater-nal underweight on the risk of undernutrition in childhood was rarely reported. Another US study observed a U-shaped association between maternal gestational weight gain and child weight outcomes12. For intellectual development, results from a birth cohort study showed that low and very-high maternal pre-pregnancy BMI were associated with increased risk of delayed mental development among 2-year-old US children8. Another study conducted in the US reported that a detrimental influence of high gestational weight gain on intellectual development at 4 and 7 years of age14, but similar associations between low gestational weight gain and intellec-tual development of children was not found. Another longitudinal study reported a 0.02–0.07 point increase in children’s IQ with increasing trimester specific gestational weight gain13. Although in contrast to our results, this 0.02–0.07 increase in IQ is too small to have clinical implications, as previous studies reported that a 5 IQ point difference was considered clinically significant between intervention and control group24,25. Our findings are consistent with the remainder of studies as we also determine the negative effect of maternal underweight (low maternal pre-pregnancy BMI) on both physical growth and intellectual development of children, but we were unable to examine the effect of maternal obesity. Due to the target study fields being the poorest rural areas in China, overweight and obesity prevalence was low meaning the sample size was not large enough to analyze the association between gestational weight gain and risk of offspring obesity and being overweight. In addition, we were also unable to evaluate the effect of excessive or inadequate gestational weight gain due to it being calculated as the last weight at the third trimester of pregnancy (not at delivery) minus pre-pregnancy weight, therefore weight gain during the whole pregnancy could not be calculated.

The observed effect of maternal pre-pregnancy underweight on further development may be attributable to several mechanisms and potential biological mechanisms may include inadequate prenatal micronutrient status. Our previous results demonstrated the positive effect of prenatal micronutrients supplementation on increas-ing birthweight21, the mental development of 1-year old infants23. These results could provide indirect evidence supporting the possibility that prenatal micronutrients status may be the potential mechanism of these findings. Other hypothesized downstream pathways include gestational weight gain, birthweight, or the postnatal envi-ronment8,40. With regards to gestational weight gain, weight gains below the required amount for the products

Underweight of children Stunting of children Thinness of children

OR(95%CI) P OR(95%CI) P OR(95%CI) P

Unadjusted analysis

Maternal pre-pregnancy nutritional status

  Underweight 2.00(1.18,3.37) 0.010 1.82(0.90,3.67) 0.094 2.61(1.50,4.54) 0.001

  Normal 1.00 1.00 1.00

Average gestational weight gain, kg/wk 0.08(0.02,0.43) 0.003 0.50(0.07,3.43) 0.482 1.26(0.32,4.95) 0.744

Model 1b

Maternal pre-pregnancy nutritional status

  Underweight 1.99(1.14,3.49) 0.016 1.95(0.92,4.11) 0.080 2.91(1.62,5.23) <0.001

  Normal 1.00 1.00 1.00

Average gestational weight gain, kg/wk 0.09(0.02,0.53) 0.008 0.64(0.09,4.44) 0.652 1.65(0.43,6.35) 0.465

Model 2c

Maternal pre-pregnancy nutritional statusd

  Underweight 1.95(1.11,3.45) 0.021 1.16(0.53,2.57) 0.712 2.65(1.43,4.92) 0.002

  Normal 1.00 1.00 1.00

Average gestational weight gain, kg/wk 0.08(0.01,0.55) 0.010 0.87(0.14,5.35) 0.880 1.37(0.32,5.82) 0.670

Table 2. Association between maternal pre-pregnancy nutritional status, average gestational weight gain and malnutrition status of early school-aged childrena. Abbreviation: CI, Confidence interval. aMultilevel models were used to assess the association between pre-pregnancy nutritional status, average gestational weight gain and physical growth in early school aged children, with township to level 3, village to level 2, and individual to level 1 after excluding overweight and obesity participants. bModel 1 included the variables of county, the age of children, parental age, sex of children, educational level of parents, occupation of parents, household wealth index. cModel 2 included the variables of county, the age of children, parental age, sex of children, educational level of parents, occupation of parents, household wealth index, older siblings of children, child school level, gestational weeks at birth, weight at birth, type of prenatal micronutrient supplementation. dWhen estimate the effects of gestational weight gain, one more variable of maternal BMI at pre-pregnancy is added into the mode 2.

Page 7: Effect of maternal pre-pregnancy underweight and average ...archive.lstmed.ac.uk/9106/1/Sci_Report_Effect of... · world, but also the latest edition of Wechsler intelligence scale

www.nature.com/scientificreports/

7SCIENTIFIC RepoRts | (2018) 8:12014 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-30514-6

of conception have been hypothesized to be associated with delayed development via inadequate fetal growth or maternal ketosis40. Our previous results emphasis the importance of prenatal multi-micronutrients on increasing birthweight compared to the control group21, and the long-term effect of birthweight was also found on further intellectual development and physical growth7. In addition, postnatal nutritional factors, such as protein intake or breastfeeding, may also affect early child development41,42, although this pathway could be either biological or behavioral. Nonetheless, further investigations designed to understand the biological mechanisms of this unfa-vorable association are critically needed.

This present study has several strengths. Firstly, the information such as gestational weeks or birth weight was collected during the time of pregnancy or at birth, ensuring the accuracy and avoiding potential biases in using recalled information. Secondly, because of the nature of longitudinal data, we could reveal linkages between maternal weight in the first trimester, average gestational weight gain and physical growth of children. Finally, a well-known and standardized intelligence scale (WISC-IV) for use in various cultures was used to estimate intel-lectual development of children in the study. Our study also has several limitations. One limitation was our inabil-ity to follow all children in the original cohort. This limitation was mostly caused by rapid development of China, with mass migration out of the original area, making it impossible to trace the participants. After excluding death and migration, we compared the baseline characteristics between follow-up and lost to follow-up participants and we found there was no difference between the two groups in baseline characteristics (Supplementary Table). After excluding participants of migrations, the ability to generalize the population in other areas was limited, yet the data in the present study is still meaningful rare, and representative of rural China. Another limitation was that we could not determine the mechanisms by which low maternal weight may affect children’s physical and intellectual development. Although we discussed the possible mechanism, the present findings of effect on impairment of verbal comprehension index were rarely found, and further similar studies were needed to clarify the mechanisms of effect of maternal underweight at pre-pregnancy on childhood impairment of verbal ability. In addition, we did not include all possible confounders (such as parental intellectual function, diet of children and smoking) in our analysis, mainly because of small number of smokers among mothers and availability of dietary data. For parental intellectual function, a previous study reported it may affect childhood development via feeding practice or physical activity, and it is also reported as a strong factor of intellectual function of offspring43. Finally, due to the sample size not being large enough (prevalence of maternal overweight or obesity was only 2.45%, 33/1307), and missing data of maternal weight at delivery, we could not estimate the effect of maternal obesity, inadequate

Underweight of women Average gestational weight gaind

MD(95%CI) P Coef(95%CI) P

Unadjusted analysis

FSIQ −1.18(−3.12,0.75) 0.231 2.71(−1.77,7.18) 0.236

VCI −2.89(−5.16,−0.63) 0.012 5.22(−0.04,10.47) 0.052

WMI −0.72(−2.54,1.10) 0.439 1.18(−3.00,5.35) 0.581

PRI 0.18(−1.86,2.23) 0.859 0.15(−4.49,4.79) 0.950

PSI 0.47(−1.46,2.41) 0.632 1.99(−2.41,6.39) 0.375

Model 1b

FSIQ −1.11(−2.95,0.72) 0.235 2.27(−1.98,6.51) 0.238

VCI −3.08(−5.24,−0.92) 0.005 3.85(−1.14,8.84) 0.130

WMI −0.35(−2.12,1.43) 0.700 1.35(−2.68,5.39) 0.511

PRI 0.19(−1.78,2.17) 0.849 0.35(−4.11,4.82) 0.877

PSI 0.76(−1.14,2.67) 0.435 2.07(−2.19,6.33) 0.341

Model 2c

FSIQ −0.76(−2.60,1.08) 0.418 2.34(−1.94,6.62) 0.284

VCI −3.14(−5.30,−0.98) 0.004 4.24(−0.78,9.26) 0.098

WMI −0.07(−1.85,1.71) 0.937 1.14(−2.94,5.22) 0.585

PRI 0.84(−1.12,2.79) 0.402 0.05(−4.41,4.51) 0.983

PSI 1.43(−0.47,3.34) 0.140 2.31(−2.00,6.63) 0.294

Table 3. Association between pre-pregnancy nutritional status, average gestational weight gain and intellectual development of early school-aged childrena. Abbreviation: CI, Confidence interval. FSIQ, Full-scale Intelligence Quotient. MD, Mean Difference. PRI, Perceptual Reasoning Index. PSI, Processing Speed Index. VCI, Verbal Comprehension Index. WISC-IV, Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children Fourth Edition; WMI, Working Memory Index. aMultilevel models were used to assess the association between pre-pregnancy nutritional status, average gestational weight gain and intellectual development in early school aged children, with township to level 3, village to level 2, and individual to level 1 after excluding overweight and obesity participants. bModel 1 included the variables of county, the age of children, parental age, sex of children, educational level of parents, occupation of parents, household wealth index. cModel 2 included the variables of county, the age of children, parental age sex of children, educational level of parents, occupation of parents, household wealth index, older siblings of children, child school level, gestational weeks at birth, weight at birth, type of prenatal micronutrient supplementation. dWhen estimate the effects of gestational weight gain, one more variable of maternal BMI at pre-pregnancy is added into the mode 2.

Page 8: Effect of maternal pre-pregnancy underweight and average ...archive.lstmed.ac.uk/9106/1/Sci_Report_Effect of... · world, but also the latest edition of Wechsler intelligence scale

www.nature.com/scientificreports/

8SCIENTIFIC RepoRts | (2018) 8:12014 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-30514-6

or excessive gestational weight gain. Although there were several limitations in the present study, most similar studies were conducted in developed counties, and focus mainly on those who suffer from being overweight and obese, while largely neglecting research into malnutrition status in poor areas. Present findings can provide guid-ance for health professionals to further improve children’s physical growth and intellectual development. Similar studies implemented in developing countries were mainly cross-sectional studies, and longitudinal data from prospective cohort studies was rare.

In conclusion, we identified the effect of low maternal pre-pregnancy weight on malnutrition status (under-weight and thinness) and the verbal comprehension ability of children at school age. The positive effect of average gestational weight gain on decreasing occurrence of underweight children in poor areas of China was also iden-tified. Our findings reinforce the importance of encouraging women in poor areas to achieve healthy gestational weight gain and weight before pregnancy on further physical and intellectual development of children. Further research is required to explore possible biological mechanisms by which having a low maternal weight may affect children’s physical and intellectual development.

References 1. Ramakrishnan, U., Grant, F., Goldenberg, T., Zongrone, A. & Martorell, R. Effect of women’s nutrition before and during early

pregnancy on maternal and infant outcomes: a systematic review. Paediatric and perinatal epidemiology 26, 285–301 (2012). 2. Yu, Z. et al. Pre-pregnancy body mass index in relation to infant birth weight and offspring overweight/obesity: a systematic review

and meta-analysis. PloS one 8, e61627 (2013). 3. Brion, M. J. et al. Intrauterine effects of maternal prepregnancy overweight on child cognition and behavior in 2 cohorts. Pediatrics

127, e202–211 (2010). 4. Veena, S. R. et al. Association between maternal nutritional status in pregnancy and offspring cognitive function during childhood

and adolescence; a systematic review. BMC pregnancy and childbirth 16, 220 (2016). 5. Neggers, Y. H. The relationship between preterm birth and underweight in Asian women. Reproductive Toxicology 56, 170–174

(2015). 6. de Kieviet, J. F., Zoetebier, L., Van Elburg, R. M., Vermeulen, R. J. & Oosterlaan, J. Brain development of very preterm and very low-

birthweight children in childhood and adolescence: a meta‐analysis. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 54, 313–323 (2012). 7. Li, C. et al. Effect of prenatal and postnatal malnutrition on intellectual functioning in early school-aged children in rural western

China. Medicine 95, e4164 (2016). 8. Hinkle, S. N. et al. Associations between maternal prepregnancy body mass index and child neurodevelopment at 2 years of age.

International Journal of Obesity 36, 1312–1319 (2012). 9. Streuling, I. et al. Physical activity and gestational weight gain: a meta‐analysis of intervention trials. BJOG: An International Journal

of Obstetrics & Gynaecology 118, 278–284 (2011). 10. Hinkle, S. N. et al. Excess gestational weight gain is associated with child adiposity among mothers with normal and overweight

prepregnancy weight status. The Journal of nutrition 142, 1851–1858 (2012). 11. Wrotniak, B. H., Shults, J., Butts, S. & Stettler, N. Gestational weight gain and risk of overweight in the offspring at age 7 y in a

multicenter, multiethnic cohort study. The American journal of clinical nutrition 87, 1818–1824 (2008). 12. Oken, E., Rifas-Shiman, S. L., Field, A. E., Frazier, A. L. & Gillman, M. W. Maternal gestational weight gain and offspring weight in

adolescence. Obstetrics and gynecology 112, 999 (2008). 13. Gage, S. H., Lawlor, D. A., Tilling, K. & Fraser, A. Associations of maternal weight gain in pregnancy with offspring cognition in

childhood and adolescence: findings from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. American journal of epidemiology 177(5), 402–410 (2013).

14. Keim, S. A. & Pruitt, N. T. Gestational weight gain and child cognitive development. International journal of epidemiology 41, 414–422 (2012).

15. Tanda, R., Salsberry, P. J., Reagan, P. B. & Fang, M. Z. The impact of prepregnancy obesity on children’s cognitive test scores. Maternal and child health journal 17, 222–229 (2013).

16. Berkman, D. S., Lescano, A. G., Gilman, R. H., Lopez, S. L. & Black, M. M. Effects of stunting, diarrhoeal disease, and parasitic infection during infancy on cognition in late childhood: a follow-up study. The Lancet 359, 564–571 (2002).

17. Daniels, M. C. & Adair, L. S. Growth in young Filipino children predicts schooling trajectories through high school. The Journal of nutrition 134, 1439–1446 (2004).

18. Wechsler, D. The measurement and appraisal of adult intelligence. 4th ed. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins (1958). 19. Batty, G. D., Mortensen, E. L., Nybo Andersen, A. M. & Osler, M. Childhood intelligence in relation to adult coronary heart disease

and stroke risk: evidence from a Danish birth cohort study. Paediatric and perinatal epidemiology 19, 452–459 (2005). 20. Hart, C. L. et al. Childhood IQ and cardiovascular disease in adulthood: prospective observational study linking the Scottish Mental

Survey 1932 and the Midspan studies. Social science & medicine 59, 2131–2138 (2004). 21. Zeng, L. et al. Impact of micronutrient supplementation during pregnancy on birth weight, duration of gestation, and perinatal

mortality in rural western China: double blind cluster randomised controlled trial. BMJ 337, a2001 (2008). 22. Li, C. et al. Prenatal micronutrient supplementation is not associated with intellectual development of young school-aged children.

The Journal of nutrition 145, 1844–1849 (2015). 23. Li, Q. et al. Effects of maternal multimicronutrient supplementation on the mental development of infants in rural western China:

follow-up evaluation of a double-blind, randomized, controlled trial. Pediatrics 123, e685–e692 (2009). 24. Anderson, J. W., Johnstone, B. M. & Remley, D. T. Breast-feeding and cognitive development: a meta-analysis. The American journal

of clinical nutrition 70, 525–535 (1999). 25. Carpenter, D. O. Effects of metals on the nervous system of humans and animals. International journal of occupational medicine and

environmental health 14, 209–218 (2001). 26. Fattah, C. et al. Maternal weight and body composition in the first trimester of pregnancy. Acta obstetricia et gynecologica

Scandinavica 89, 952–955 (2010). 27. World Health Organization. Obesity: preventing and managing the global epidemic. World Health Organization (2000). 28. Onis, M. D. et al. Development of a WHO growth reference for school-aged children and adolescents. Bulletin of the World health

Organization 85, 660–667 (2007). 29. Chen, H., Keith, T. Z., Weiss, L., Zhu, J. & Li, Y. Testing for multigroup invariance of second-order WISC-IV structure across China,

Hong Kong, Macau, and Taiwan. Personality and Individual Differences 49, 677–682 (2010). 30. Wechsler, D. Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. 4th ed. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation (2003). 31. Weiss, L. G., Saklofske, D. H., Prifitera, A. & Holdnack, J. A. WISC-IV advanced clinical interpretation. Academic Press (2006). 32. Filmer, D. & Pritchett, L. H. Estimating wealth effects without expenditure data-or tears: an application to educational enrollments

in states of India. Demography 38, 115–132 (2001). 33. Li, C. et al. Sex differences in the intellectual functioning of early school-aged children in rural China. BMC public health 16, 288

(2016).

Page 9: Effect of maternal pre-pregnancy underweight and average ...archive.lstmed.ac.uk/9106/1/Sci_Report_Effect of... · world, but also the latest edition of Wechsler intelligence scale

www.nature.com/scientificreports/

9SCIENTIFIC RepoRts | (2018) 8:12014 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-30514-6

34. Christian, P. et al. Prenatal micronutrient supplementation and intellectual and motor function in early school-aged children in Nepal. JAMA 304, 2716–2723 (2010).

35. Zhou, J. et al. Maternal Prenatal Nutrition and Birth Outcomes on Malnutrition among 7-to 10-Year-Old Children: A 10-Year Follow-Up. The Journal of pediatrics 178, 40–46 (2016).

36. Adair, L. S. et al. Associations of linear growth and relative weight gain during early life with adult health and human capital in countries of low and middle income: findings from five birth cohort studies. The Lancet 382, 525–534 (2013).

37. Igel, U., Romppel, M., Baar, J., Brähler, E. & Grande, G. Association between parental socio-economic status and childhood weight status and the role of urbanicity. Public health 139, 209–211 (2016).

38. Neggers, Y. H., Goldenberg, R. L., Ramey, S. L. & Cliver, S. P. Maternal prepregnancy body mass index and psychomotor development in children. Acta obstetricia et gynecologica Scandinavica 82, 235–240 (2003).

39. Keim, S. A. & Pruitt, N. T. Gestational weight gain and child cognitive development. International journal of epidemiology 41, 414–422 (2012).

40. Institute of Medicine and National Research Council. Weight Gain During Pregnancy: Reexamining the Guidelines. The National Acadamines Press: Washington DC (2009).

41. Kramer, M. S. et al. Breastfeeding and infant growth: biology or bias? Pediatrics 110, 343–347 (2002). 42. Ong, K. K., Emmett, P. M., Noble, S., Ness, A. & Dunger, D. B. Dietary energy intake at the age of 4 months predicts postnatal weight

gain and childhood body mass index. Pediatrics 117, e503–e508 (2006). 43. Deary, I. J., Strand, S., Smith, P. & Femandes, C. Intelligence and educational achievement. Intelligence35, 13-21 (2007).

AcknowledgementsHealth Departments of each project county, the local health bureaus, and education bureaus for their cooperation and organization of the field data collection; and the staff of Xi’an Jiaotong University for their participation in the field data collection. All the work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (No: 81230016), China Postdoctoral Science Foundation (No: 2016M592804) and New Century Excellent Talents in University grant (No: NCET-11–0417).

Author ContributionsChao Li: Dr. Li conceptualized and designed the research, drafted the initial manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. Ni Zhu, Lingxia Zeng, Shaonong Dang, Jing Zhou, Leilei Pei, Tao Chen, Victoria Watson: Carried out the initial data analyses, reviewed and revised the manuscript. Duolao Wang and Hong Yan: The principal investigator, designed the data collection instruments, and coordinated and supervised data collection at the study sites, critically reviewed the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. All authors approved the final manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Additional InformationSupplementary information accompanies this paper at https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-30514-6.Competing Interests: The authors declare no competing interests.Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or

format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Cre-ative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not per-mitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. © The Author(s) 2018