distributive and procedural justice as predictors of js

Upload: coltun-victorita

Post on 26-Feb-2018

221 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 7/25/2019 Distributive and Procedural Justice as Predictors of Js

    1/13

    RESEARCH NOTES

    Academy

    o

    Management /ournai

    1992, Vol. 35, No. 3. 626- 637 .

    DISTRIBUTIVE AND PROCEDURAL JUSTICE AS

    PREDICTORS OF SATISFACTION WITH PERSONAL AND

    ORGANIZATIONAL OUTCOMES

    DEAN B. McFARLIN

    PAUL D. SWEENEY

    Marquette Universi ty

    Distributive justice was found to be a more important predictor of two

    personal outcomes, pay satisfaction and job satisfaction, than proce-

    dural justice, whereas the reverse w as true for two organizational out-

    comes organizational commitment and subordinate s evaluation of

    supervisor. However, procedural and distributive justice also inter-

    acted in predicting organizational outcomes. We discuss limitations of

    this study and directions for future research.

    Folger and Konovsky captured the key distinction regarding justice in

    work organizations, noting that distribu tive justice refers to the perce ived

    fairness of the amounts of compensation employees receive; procedural jus-

    tice refers to the perceived fairness of the means used to determine those

    am ou nts (1989: 115). However, few studies have examined how both dis-

    tribu tive and p roc ed ura l justice affect ou tcom es (e.g., Folger & Kon ovsky,

    1989; Greenberg, 1987a; Konovsky, Folger, & Crop anzan o, 1987). In add i-

    tion, most research has focused on legal rather than work-related issues. For

    example, Greenberg and Folger (1983) showed that defendants viewed trial

    verdicts (distributions) positively if they were seen as the result of fair pro-

    cedu res , an effect called the fair process effect (cf. M usante, Gilbert, &

    Thibaut, 1983). Folger and Konovsky pointed out that this research has

    suggested different predictive roles for procedural and distributive justice.

    In particular, studies have found distributive justice to predict satisfaction

    with specific, personal outcomes, like case verdicts, better than procedural

    justice. The reverse is true, however, when people make more general eval-

    uatio ns of, for instanc e, legal institutio ns or their rep resenta tives (Lind &

    Tyler, 1988).

    In fact, the few studies that have been done in organizational settings

    have tended to support the notion that the predictive roles of procedural and

    distributive justice depend, at least in part, on the nature of the outcome in

  • 7/25/2019 Distributive and Procedural Justice as Predictors of Js

    2/13

    1992 McFarlin and Sweeney

    7

    faction, and perceived conflict than distributive justice. Konovsky and col-

    leagues (1987] found that procedural justice predicted organizational com-

    mitment, but not pay satisfaction, whereas the reverse was true for distrib-

    utive justice. Similarly, Folger and Konovsky (1989) found that procedural

    justice accounted for more variance in organizational co mm itment an d trust

    in a supervisor than distributive justice, wbereas tbe reverse was true for

    satisfaction w itb a pay raise. i

    Ov erall, tbese re sults suggest tbat pr oce dur al justice may be la more

    important predictor tban distributive justice of outcomes related to evaluat-

    ing a company as an institution and its representatives, sucb as organiza-

    tional commitment and trust in supervisor. In contrast, distributive justice

    may be a more important predictor of personal outcomes, like satisfaction

    witb pay level, tban procedural justice.

    Interestingly, tbese results reflect wbat we migbt call a main effect ap-

    proacb to examining tbe predictive roles of distributive and procedural jus-

    tice in field research. Tbere are few studies of tbe interactive effects of tbe

    two types of justice. In fact, tbe existing researcb evidence about interactive

    effects com es from laboratory stu dies. For exam ple, in a recent investigation

    by Greenberg (1987a), subjects worked on a task in wb icb distributive justice

    (pay levels) and procedural justice (bow pay levels were determined) were

    manipulated. He found an interaction of tbe following form; subjects saw

    bigb p ay levels as fair regardless of proc edu res bu t saw low pay levels as fair

    only when fair procedures were used. Perceptions of unfair treatment were

    maximized wben pay was low and procedures were unfair. Similarly, in a

    laboratory study, Cropanzano and Folger (1989) found tbat resentnient was

    bigbest wben subjects perceived tbat unfair procedures prevented tbem

    from receiving big b rew ards for task perform ance. j

    Botb Greenberg (1987a) and Cropanzano and Folger (1989) pointed out,

    bowever, tbat researcb was needed to determine wbetber tbese laboratory

    interactions generalize to organizational settings. An attempt at suqb gener-

    alization was tbe focus of our researcb. Anotber key researcb question was

    wbetber existing tbeory allows for a precise prediction regarding tbe form

    sucb interactions may take. We used referent cognitions tbeoryj (Folger,

    1986) to derive just sucb a prediction.

    Briefly, according to referent cognitions tbeory, individuals evaluate

    tbeir work experiences by reflecting on w bat migbt bave be en un der dif-

    ferent circumstances and conditions (Folger, 1986). One way erriployees

    migbt accomplisb sucb a comparison would be to cognitively simulate bow

    tbeir current work situation migbt be different bad tbeir organization used

    fairer procedures. In fact, Cropanzano and Folger (1989) suggested' tbat ref-

    erent cognitions tbeory offers a potential conceptual framework for tbe in-

    teractive effects of distributive and procedural justice.

  • 7/25/2019 Distributive and Procedural Justice as Predictors of Js

    3/13

    628 Academy

    o

    Management Journal August

    contrast tbis situation to tbe more positive outcomes tbat tbey would bave

    obtained bad tbe decision maker used fair allocation procedures. On tbe

    otber band, referent cognitions tbeory predicts tbat wben people perceive

    procedures to be fair, resentment will be minimal, even when distributive

    justice is low. Unde r suc b cond itions, it wo uld be more difficult for em ploy-

    ees to envision alternative fairer procedures tbat could bave led to better

    outcomes.

    To test tbis intera ction pattern in a field setting, we su rveyed a group of

    bank employees. In addition to measuring procedural and distributive jus-

    tice, we assessed perceptions of two important personal outcomes, pay and

    job satisfaction, and two organizational outcomes: organizational commit-

    ment and subordinate's evaluation of supervisor. If Cropanzano and Folger's

    (1989) application of referent cognitions tbeory is correct, we would expect

    tbat tbe evaluation of tbese outcomes would be most negative wben botb

    distributive and procedural justice are low. But we would expect positive

    evaluations wben procedural justice is bigb, regardless of tbe level of dis-

    tributive justice.

    A more speculative prediction is tbat tbe interaction pattern referent

    cognitions tbeory suggests will be clearest wben tbe focus is on organiza-

    tional, as opposed to personal, outcomes. Because tbe former focus on tbe

    sources of tbe procedures tbat affect employees, sucb as supervisors and tbe

    employing organization as an institution, tbey provide a clearer target for

    retaliation tban outcomes of a more personal nature. In otber words, allow-

    ing employees to evaluate an organization as an institution gives tbem tbe

    opportunity to blame tbe parties responsible for creating tbe rules of tbe

    gametbe unfair procedurestbat have resulted in a lack of distributive

    justice. Referent cognitions tbeory suggests tbat employees' resentment is

    not only a function of low d istributive justice, but also of tbe opp ortu nity to

    blam e m isfor tune on tbe actions of otbers ratber tba n on tbeir ow n be-

    bavior (Cropanzano

    Folger, 1989).

    Finally, sbould tbe interactions we predicted fail to materialize, we

    determined tbat our focus would sbift to tbe interpretation of main effects.

    In view of prior researcb (e.g., Alexander

    Ruderman, 1987), we predicted

    tbat distributive and procedural justice would eacb produce strong main

    effects for a variety of outcomes. Given Folger and Konovsky's (1989) and

    Konovsky and colleagues' (1987) results, bowever, we expected procedural

    justice to produce a stronger main effect in predicting organizational out-

    comes tban distributive justice. We expected tbe reverse to be true witb

    personal outcomes.

    M THO S

  • 7/25/2019 Distributive and Procedural Justice as Predictors of Js

    4/13

    1992 McFarlin and Sweeney 629

    resulting in a response rate of 61 percent. Tbe respondents' mean age was

    33.8 years (s.d. = 12.1); tbeir m ean com pan y tenu re was 6.3 years (s.d. =

    5.8); and 74 percent were women. Tbese respondent demograpbics com-

    pared favorably witb company statistics for tbe work force as wboje, sug-

    gesting tbat problems resulting from response bias were unlikely to be a

    major tbreat. In tbe total work force, 68 percent of tbe employees were

    w om en, w itb an average age of 34.6 years and an average comp any te nur e of

    6.9 years.

    Measures

    Predictors were measures of global distributive and procedural justice.

    Criterion variables w ere two m easures reflecting eva luations of tbe surveyed

    organization and its representatives and two measures of satisfaction witb

    personal outcomes. ;

    Distributive justice

    We used five of tbe six items from Price and Muel-

    ler's (1986) Distributive Justice Index. Tbese items ask workers to indicate

    tbe extent to wbicb tbey bave been fairly rewarded in view of tbeir respon-

    sibilities, experience, job stress, effort, and performance. Rewards in tbe

    index are defined broadly, witb money, praise, and recognition all being

    listed as exa m ples . An exam ple of tbe item format is as follows: How fair

    bas [company name] been in rewarding you wben you consider tbe amount

    of effort tbat you put into your w ork ? ( 1 = very unfair, 5 = very 'fair).

    Procedural justice

    We constructed a four-item procedural justice scale

    tbat use d tbe sam e basic format and five-point respo nse scale as tbeiDistrib-

    utive Justice Index. Respondents indicated tbe extent to wbicb tbe general

    proc edu res used to com m unicate performance feedback, determine, pay in-

    creases, and evaluate performance and prom otability were fair. '

    Organizational outcomes

    Organizational commitment was assessed

    using four items from Cook and Wall's (1980) commitment scale. Tbese

    items ask workers to express tbeir agreement or disagreement witb various

    statem ents (e.g., I feel myself to be a part of tbis com pany ), using five-point

    scales (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree). Tbe second organizational

    outcome, sub ordina te's e valuation of supervisor, was assessed w itb a single

    item: O verall, I tbink my boss is a poor performer (1 = strongly agree, 5 =

    strongly disagree). Several nationa l prob ability studies (e.g, Qu inn

    Staines,

    1979) bave previo usly used an essentially equiva lent item. j

    Personal outcomes

    We used tbree items to assess pay JeveJ satis/a ction .

    Two of tbe items used five-point agree-disagree formats; tbese were : Tb e

    pay for my job is exc ellent and I am satisfied w itb my current pay. Tbe

    tbird item asked resp ond ents bo w tbey felt about tbeir pay (7 = deligbted,

    1 = terrible ; cf. An drew s & W itbey, 1976). Since tbey bad different mea -

    surem ent scales, we standa rdized tbese tbree items before com bining tbem .

  • 7/25/2019 Distributive and Procedural Justice as Predictors of Js

    5/13

    630 Academy

    of

    Management Journa) August

    RESULTS

    Descriptive Statistics

    Table 1 prese nts m ean s, correlations, and reliability coefficients, w bere

    app licable, for all study variables. Tbe reliabilities for m ulti-item scales w ere

    generally very good, witb alpbas ranging from .75 to .92. One concern tbat

    migbt be raised, bowever, is tbe correlation between procedural and distrib-

    utive justice (.67). To belp allay tbis concern, we would make tbree points.

    First, prior researcb bas successfully sbown tbat tbese scales predict differ-

    ent dependent measures, suggesting tbat tbey are independent constructs.

    Second, prom inent researcbers bave claimed tbat altbougb tbey are separate

    constructs, in practice a sizable correlation sbould be found between tbem

    (e.g., Folger, 1987). For example, Folger cited an important study by Tyler

    (1984) in wbicb tbe correlation between distributive and procedural justice

    was

    77

    Tbird, we conducted an empirical analysis tbat sbeds some addi-

    tional ligbt on tbis issue.^

    Regression nalys is Results

    Separa te three-s tep, bierarcb ical regression analyses were performed for

    eacb outcome variable. At step 1, we entered five control variables: salary,

    job type, age, gender, and tenure. Staines, Pottick, and Fudge (1986) cau-

    tioned tbat tbese types of variables need to be controlled for, given tbeir

    general potential to inflate or suppress relations between otber variables.

    Salary and job typ e were of special co nce rn, given tbeir pote ntia l to affect tbe

    particular outcome variables used in tbis study.

    As Table 2 sbows, age significantly predicted pay level satisfaction, job

    satisfaction, and organizational commitment. Consistent witb earlier re-

    searcb findings (Weaver, 1980), our finding was tbat older workers te nd ed to

    bave bigber commitment and satisfaction tban younger workers. Job type

    also significantly predicted job satisfaction, evaluation of supervisor, and

    organizational commitment, witb managers being more positive tban cleri-

    cal employees.

    At step 2, distributiv e and proce dural justice we re entered . Botb types of

    justice were significant predic tors of eacb outcome variable. Con sistent w itb

    Folger and K onovsky s (1989) conten tions, bow ever, d istributive justice

    ^

    Podsakoff and Organ (1986) reviewed techniques that address method variance, including

    a procedure called the single factor approach. The logic of this approach is that if method

    variance ac coun ts for the relations betw een two or more variables, a factor an alysis should yield

    a single global (method) factor. Since researchers have generally viewed this approach as weak

    but as possibly prov iding some useful information, w e combined it with a stronger confirmatory

    factor a naly sis, u sing LISREL VI (Joreskog

    Sorbom, 1985), which tests models that increase in

  • 7/25/2019 Distributive and Procedural Justice as Predictors of Js

    6/13

    992

    Mc arlin and Sweeney

    6 3

    I

    (8

    tn

    u

    I

    'ft

    (S

    o

    O5

    CO

    o

    CO

    t H

    f H

    o

    o

    1

    2

    o

    o

    CO

    CO

    i n

    1

    CM

    1

    CO

    o

    i n

    r H

    CM

    1

    CM

    o

    o

    i n

    CO

    1

    i n

    i n

    CO

    CO

    i n

    o

    o

    o

    o

    o

    CO

    J"

    CO

    e

    00

    00

    o

    1

    i n

    o

    O l

    o

    CM

    CO

    CM

    o

    o

    o

    B

    c

    "cr

    CO

    CM

    i n

    CM

    O

    o

    1

    r H

    r H

    O l

    i n

    i n

    tv

    CO

    ;o

    r H

    C

    t

    o

    s

    a

    B

    O

    cd

    3

    te

    s

    e

    n

    CO

    t v

    r H

    CO

    q

    t n

    q

    C31

    O

    1

    r-*

    i n

    1

    oa

    r H

    t v

    i n

    CO

    so

    v

    i n

    tv

    CM

    CO

    O l

    i n

    CO

    r H

    r H

    CM

    CM

    CM

    r H

    1

    r H

    r H

    r H

    r H

    CO

    CO

    CO

    CO

    "B

    1

    [

    J

    B

    U

    O

    z

    iq

    CO

    i n

    CO

    CO

    i n

    q

    CM

    1

    CO

    o

    CO

    o

    CO

    CO

    CO

    r H

    o:

    v .

    Z

    a

    t

    9

    t v

    CO

    CJl

    " :

    o

    CO

    t v

    CO

    CO

    i n

    q

    o

    CO

    o

    1

    r H

    o

    t v

    o

    o

    c:i

    u

    s

    c

    B

    i

    e

    l

    3

    B

    o

    i

    i2

    B

    i

    c

    01

    o

    B

    cd

    B

    o

    5

    II

    CM

    T3

    B

    cd

    B

    cd

    2 .

    "3 01

    oi -o

    in 01

    o 00

    V r?

    c

    CO

    u

    g

    cn

    03

    tH

    CO

    CD

    O

    CO

    x

    03

    0 0

    CO

    C

    CO

    B

    II

    CM

    -a

    a

    cd

    L

    cn 01

    B a

    o >>

  • 7/25/2019 Distributive and Procedural Justice as Predictors of Js

    7/13

    6

    Academy ofManagement Journal

    TABLE 2

    Results of Hierarchical Regression Analyses

    August

    Independent

    Variables

    Step 1

    Tenure

    Gender

    Age

    Salary

    Job type

    AR^

    Step 2

    Procedural justice

    Distributive justice

    AR2

    Step 3

    AR^ for interaction

    of procedural and

    distributive justice

    F

    f

    Personal

    Pay Level

    Satisfaction

    .02

    - . 0 2

    .16***

    .0 0

    .0 8

    .04***

    .14***

    .52***

    .37***

    .0 0

    56.29***

    8 63

    Outcomes

    Job

    Satisfaction

    - . 0 3

    .02

    .31***

    .07

    .15***

    .12***

    .18***

    .30***

    .18***

    .0 0

    32.68***

    8 6 4

    Organizational Outcomes

    Subordinate s

    Evaluation

    of Supervisor

    .0 0

    .0 5

    - . 0 2

    - . 0 5

    .12**

    .03**

    .23***

    .15**

    .11***

    .01*

    14.74***

    8 626

    Organizational

    Commitment

    - . 0 2

    - . 0 3

    .23***

    .0 8

    .10*

    .08***

    .34***

    .23***

    .26***

    .01**

    42.19***

    8,631

    * p < .05

    ** p < .01

    *** p < .001

    tended to be a more important predictor of personal outcomes (pay and job

    satisfaction) than procedural justice. In addition, procedural justice was a

    more important predictor of both organizational outcomes than distributive

    justice (see Table 2).^

    In step 3, we entered th e interaction of distributive and proc edu ral

    justice. As Table 2 shows, the interaction terms for both evaluation of su-

    pervisor and organizational commitment were significant. When plotted (cf.

    Cohen Cohen, 1983), both outcomes yielded the same interaction pattern.

    ^ Following a reviewe r's suggestion, we condu cted our regression analyses again, en tering

    distributive and procedural justice on separate steps to more closely examine their relative

    predictive power. The results of tbese analyses were consistent with those given in Table 2;

    procedural justice was a stronger predictor of organizational outcomes tban distributive justice,

    with the reverse being true for personal outcomes. Specifically, witb procedural justice con-

    trolled, distributive justice accounted for an additional 5 percent of tbe variance in job satis-

    faction and 14 percent of tbe variance in pay level satisfaction; wben tbe order of entry was

  • 7/25/2019 Distributive and Procedural Justice as Predictors of Js

    8/13

    1992

    McFarlin and Sweeney

    6 33

    FIGURE I

    Interactive EffectsofD istributiveandProcedural Justiceon

    Organizational ommitment

    Organizational

    Commitment

    19.0 -

    18.5 -

    18.0

    -

    17.5

    -

    16.5

    -

    16.0

    -

    15.5

    15.0

    -

    14.5

    Higb procedural justice

    Low procedural justice

    Low

    Higb

    Distributive Justice

    To illustrate this pattern,wepresent theinteraction fororganizationalcom-

    mitment in Figure 1, which shows that although employees who felt that

    proced ures w ere fair tende d tohave higher levelsof organizational commit-

    ment than thosewho felt procedures were unfair, thisgap wasm uch larger

    when distributive justice was low. Specifically, when procedural justice was

    low, organizational commitment varied considerably

    as a

    function

    of dis-

    tributive justice. When procedural justice washigh, how ever, organizational

    commitment varied little

    as a

    function

    of

    distributive justice. Again,

    an

    identical interaction pattern emerged for evaluation of supervisor.^

    DIS USSION I

    Distributive

    and

    Procedural Justice

    as

    Predictors

    Our finding that both distributive and procedural justiceareimportant

    predictors of work outcomes underscores recent admonitions for organiza-

    ^

    A

    reviewer raised

    a

    concern about

    tbe

    reliability

    of our

    single-item supervisor evaluation

    measure.

    To

    address this issue,

    we

    corrected

    tbe

    correlation matrix

    for

    measurement error

    and

    conducted

    the

    analysis again using

    the

    corrected matrix. Wbere possible, used alpha

  • 7/25/2019 Distributive and Procedural Justice as Predictors of Js

    9/13

    634 Academy o Management Journal August

    tional researchers to examine both types of justice (Greenberg, 1987a,b), We

    also found, as expected, that distributive justice tended to be a stronger

    predictor of personal outcomes than procedural justice, whereas the reverse

    was true for organizational outco m es. The fairness of a firm s pro ced ure s

    may have a greater impact on organizational commitment than the fairness

    of personal outcomes that workers receive, perhaps because procedures de-

    fine th e org aniza tion s cap acity to treat employee s fairly. Th us , if they see

    procedures as fair, employees may view the organization positively, even if

    they are currently dissatisfied with such personal outcomes as a low pay

    raise. In contrast, a pay cut may c ripp le an emp loyee financially, and a larger

    paycheck will buy more, regardless of whether procedures were fair or not

    (cf. Folger

    Konovsky, 1989; Konovsky et al, 1987). Our findings are con-

    gruent with research in the legal arena that suggests that distributive and

    proce dural justice have different predictive roles depen ding on whe ther th e

    outcome in question is personal or reflects more general evaluations of legal

    institutions or their representatives (Lind Tyler, 1988).

    However, we also found that distributive and procedural justice had

    significant interactive effects on our organ izational outco m es, su bo rdin ate s

    evaluation of supervisor and organizational commitment. Consistent with

    Crop anzan o an d Folger s (1989) app lication of referent cogn itions theo ry,

    these interactions revealed that the combination of unfair procedures and

    low distributive justice produced the lowest ratings. In contrast, fair proce-

    dures produced high commitment and supervisor evaluations, regardless of

    the level of distributive justice. Advocates of referent cognitions theory ar-

    gued that, under conditions of procedural fairness, employees will find it

    difficult to envision that more positive alternative outcomes could have

    occurred.

    Referent cognitions theory may also help explain why we found inter-

    action effects for organizational, but not personal, outcomes. According to

    the theory, employee resentment requires not only low distributive justice,

    but also the ability to identify other people and the procedures that they use

    as the source of poor outcomes (Cropanzano

    Folger, 1989). Thus, organ-

    izational outcom es m ay represent a clear target for blame, whereas personal

    outcomes do not. By definition, organizational outcomes make salient the

    institutional sources of the procedures that affect employees.

    Limitations and irections for Future Research

    One of the most basic limitations of the present study is its exclusive

    reliance on cross-sectional, self-report data. This reliance precludes us from

    making strong causal statemen ts about our results. The use of a longitud inal

    design wo uld improve the ab ility to make causal stateme nts. In add ition, o ur

  • 7/25/2019 Distributive and Procedural Justice as Predictors of Js

    10/13

    1992 McFarJin an d Sweeney 635

    approach raises the issue of common method variance. Although we can at

    least partially allay concerns about method variance in our particular case

    (see the Results section), we wo uld also encourage future justice resea rchers

    to take steps to avoid method variance problems in field settings', by, for

    instance, collecting mu ltiple measures. [

    Concern might also arise about the small magnitude of our interaction

    effects. Since interaction terms are not independent of main effects in re-

    gression analyses, Cohen and Cohen (1983) recommended that researchers

    test simple main effect models before entertaining more complicated inter-

    active models. This procedure tends to underestimate the amount; of vari-

    ance for which interaction terms truly account. Furthermore, the literature

    on regression ana lysis prov ides several cau tions about interpreting t;he theo-

    retical or practical significance of interaction terms on the basis ofjthe pro-

    portion s of variance expla ined (cf. Cham poux Peters, 1987; Ped hazu r,

    1982; Stone Hollenbeck, 1984). Apparently, a key issue is not how much

    variance is explained, but whether the increment is statistically significant,

    indicating that the relationship in question is being moderated. However, to

    understand the actual nature of an interaction effect, researchers should plot

    separate regression equations for different values of the moderator^ variable

    and exam ine the resulting pa ttern for its theoretical or practical significance

    (Stone Hollenbeck, 1984). This is the approach we took in this research.

    Still, we would suggest that researchers be sensitive to the limitations of

    moderated regression analysis and follow the recommendations that have

    been made to minimize them.

    Finally, a basic direction for future research is to determine whether

    personal and organizational outcomes other than those examined here and

    in previous research will yield similar predictive patterns for distributive

    and procedural justice. More research is also needed to explain why distrib-

    utive and procedural justice may differentially affect personal and organiza-

    tional outcomes. To address this issue may require a better understanding of

    w

    pe rsonal an d organizational outco me s differ. A s Cropan zano a nd Folger

    (1989) suggested, referent cognitions theory may provide a useful guiding

    framework for such efforts.

    R E F E R E N E S I

    Alexander, S., Ruderman, M. 1987. The role of procedural and distributive justice in organ-

    izational bebavior. Social Justice Research 1: 177-198 .

    And rews, F. M.,

    W ithey, S. B. 1976.Social indicators of well-being: Ame ricans] perception

    of life quality. New York: Plenum Press.

    Ben tler, P., Bone tt, D. G. 1980. Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of

    covariance structures. Psychological Rulletin 103: 588-606.

  • 7/25/2019 Distributive and Procedural Justice as Predictors of Js

    11/13

    ^36

    Academy of

    Managem ent Journal August

    Cohen, J.,

    Gohen, P. 1983.Applied mu ltiple regression/correlation analysis for the beha v-

    ioral sciences (2d ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Gook, J.,

    Wall, T. 1980. New work attitude measures of trust, organizational commitment and

    personal need nonfulfillment.

    Joumal of Occupational Psychology

    53 : 39-5 2 .

    Gropan zano, R., Folger, R. 1989. Referent cognitions and task decision autono my : Beyond

    equity theory.Joumal of Applied Psychology 74: 293-299.

    Folger, R. 1986. Rethinking equity tbeory: A referent cognitions m odel. In. H. W.Bierhoff R. L.

    Gohen,

    J. Greenberg (Eds.),Justice in social relations: 145-162. New York: Plenum.

    Folger, R. 1987. Distributive and procedural justice in tbe workplace. Social Justice Research

    1: 143-159.

    Folger, R.,

    Konovsky, M. A. 1989. Effects of proc edural and distributive justice on reactions

    to pay raise decisions.Academy of Managem ent Journal 32: 115-130 .

    Greenberg, J. 1987a. Reactions to procedural injustice in payment distributions: Do the means

    justify the ends? Joumol of Applied Psychology 72: 5 5 - 7 1 .

    Greenberg, J. 1987b. A taxonomy of organizational justice theories.Academy of Managemen t

    Review 12: 9-22.

    Green berg, J., Folger, R. 1983. Procedural justice, participation, and the fair process effect in

    groups and organizations. In P. Paulus (Ed.), Rasic group processes: 235-256. New York:

    Springer-Verlag.

    Joreskog, K. G., Sorbom, D. 1985.LISR LVI:Analysis of linear structural relationships by

    the method of maximum likelihood. M ooresv ille, IN: Scientific Software.

    Konovsky, M. A., Folger, R.,

    Gropanzano, R. 1987. Relative effects of procedural and distrib-

    utive justice on employee attitudes.

    Represen tative Research in Social Psychology

    17 :

    Lind , E. A., Tyler, T. R. 1988. The social psychology of procedural justice. New York:

    Plenum.

    Musante, L., Gilbert, M. A.,

    Tbibaut, J. 1983. Tbe effects of control on perceived fairness of

    procedures and outcomes.Journal of Experim ental Social Psychology 19: 223-23 8.

    Pedbazer, E. J. 1982. Multiple regression in behavioral research. New York: Holt, Rinebart,

    Winston.

    Podsakoff P. M., Organ, D. W. 1986. Self-reports in organizational research: Problems and

    prospects

    Joumal of Management

    12:

    531 544.

    Price , J. L., M ueller, G. W. 1986. Handbook of organizational measurement. Marshfield,

    MA: Pitman.

    Qu inn, R. P., Staines, G. L. 1979. The 1977 quality of employm ent survey. Ann Arbor:

    Institute for Social Researcb, University of Michigan.

    Sta ines , G. L., Pottick, K. J.,

    Fudge, D. A. 1986. Wives' employment and husb and s' attitudes

    toward work and life.Journal of Applied Psycho logy 71: 118-12 8.

    Stone, E. F.,

    Hollenbeck, J. R. 1984. Some issues associated w ith the use of m oderated regres-

    sion. O rganizational Rehavior and Human Performance 34: 195-21 3.

    Tucker, L. R., Lewis, G. 1973. The reliability coefficient for maximum likelihood factor anal-

    ysis.

    Psychometrika

    38: 1 10.

  • 7/25/2019 Distributive and Procedural Justice as Predictors of Js

    12/13

    1992

    McFarlin and Sweeney 7

    Dean B McFarlin is an associate professor of management in the College of Business

    Administration at Marquette University. His research interests include job satisfaction,

    participative management, justice in organizations, and cross-cultural differences in

    organizational behavior.

    Paul D Sweeney

    is an associate professor of management in the College of Business

    Administration at Marquette University. His research interests include employee per-

    ceptions of fairness, control, and satisfaction as well as cross-cultural differences in

    organizational behavior.

  • 7/25/2019 Distributive and Procedural Justice as Predictors of Js

    13/13