discourse studies the story of ‘oh’: part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · in...

43
DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2 Animating transcript Douglas Macbeth Ohio State University, USA Jean Wong The College of New Jersey, USA DRAFT: 19 April 2016 Abstract In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual architecture is joined at the hip to a technical architecture of transcripts, sequence and turn productions. That the conceptual was to be found and demonstrated in the material detail of temporal productions was central to CA’s extraordinary innovations. As with CA, an Epistemic CA has the task of giving evidence of its conceptual order in actual materials, and thus animating the materials to show them. The task and relationship are emblematically reflexive: we shall find the expression ‘Oh’ indexing “changes of state” or “inapposite inquiries”, for example, as of the account-able animations of turn and sequence constructions. Our shared attachments to sequential analysis deliver the expectation that we shall see how Epistemic order is achieved on actual occasions, through actual materials, rendered as transcript. The discussion turns to how the EP engages and acquits this analytic expectation. Keywords: Epistemics, animating transcript, sequential analysis Corresponding author: Douglas Macbeth, Department of Educational Studies, 29 W. Woodruff Avenue, Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA. Email: [email protected]

Upload: others

Post on 02-Aug-2020

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

DISCOURSESTUDIES

Thestoryof‘Oh’:Part2Animatingtranscript

DouglasMacbethOhioStateUniversity,USA

JeanWongTheCollegeofNewJersey,USA

DRAFT:19April2016

AbstractInConversationAnalysisthroughSacks,Schegloff,Jeffersonandothers,theconceptualarchitectureisjoinedatthehiptoatechnicalarchitectureoftranscripts,sequenceandturnproductions.ThattheconceptualwastobefoundanddemonstratedinthematerialdetailoftemporalproductionswascentraltoCA’sextraordinaryinnovations.AswithCA,anEpistemicCAhasthetaskofgivingevidenceofitsconceptualorderinactualmaterials,andthusanimatingthematerialstoshowthem.Thetaskandrelationshipareemblematicallyreflexive:weshallfindtheexpression‘Oh’indexing“changesofstate”or“inappositeinquiries”,forexample,asoftheaccount-ableanimationsofturnandsequenceconstructions.OursharedattachmentstosequentialanalysisdelivertheexpectationthatweshallseehowEpistemicorderisachievedonactualoccasions,throughactualmaterials,renderedastranscript.ThediscussionturnstohowtheEPengagesandacquitsthisanalyticexpectation.

Keywords:Epistemics,animatingtranscript,sequentialanalysis

Correspondingauthor:

DouglasMacbeth,DepartmentofEducationalStudies,29W.WoodruffAvenue,OhioStateUniversity,Columbus,OH,USA.Email:[email protected]

Page 2: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

2

Introduction

Fromthearticlesofthisspecialissue,wenowhaveacollectionofEpistemic

conceptualizations,noticings,objects,andorganizationalthingsdrawnfromits

publications.Itisahybridcollectionofconversationanalyticthingsliketurns,positions

andsequences,andEpistemicthings,likechangesofstate,particlesthat“index”,and

tensionsaboutaccess,proprietyandauthoritytospeakofwhateverthepartiesmaybe

speakingof.PerhapscentraltotheEP’sdistinctiveprogrammaticidentityisthe

introductionofcontestedfieldsofgreaterandlesserepistemicagency(Heritage,2002,

passim),revealingasymmetricclaimsandendowmentsoperatinginthebackground,

shapingourwaysofspeaking(andhearing)byshapingourauthorizationstodoso.Noless

centralfortheEPistheseeminglyunavoidableintersectionsof‘knowledge’,‘experience’,

informationandtheirpossessions,andalsodifferentformsofknowledgeandexperience,

routinelyparsedasgreaterandlesserforms

AstheEPdevelopsinitsmorecontemporarypublications,naturalconversation

comestobeabout“monitoringepistemicstatus,”givingevidenceofimbalance,achieving

balance(seeLindwalletal.,2016),andthereby,inthecourseofthesepreoccupationsand

theiroperations,producingtheevidentorderoftalk–in–interaction(Heritage,2012a,b).

Asthepartiesactasepistemicclaimants,pressingand/ordeflectingauthority,accessand

ownership,often“indexed”by‘Oh’productionsandmodulatedbymorphosyntaxtoyield

theorderlyactionformationsandturnconstructionswethenfind,wecanexpectthatthese

orientations,tasksandtensionscanbe—andwillbe—revealedintheactionsand

constructionstheyshape.Oratleastthisistheexpectationofourattachmentsto

conversationanalysis,asitwasandhasbeenCA’sachievementtoweditssociologyof

Page 3: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

3

naturallanguageusetoactualexhibitsintheirconstitutivedetail.CAhasproducednot

onlydescriptionsofconversation’sorganizationsinmultipledomains,butdescriptionsof

socialactions—grammarsofaction—showninthetemporal–materialdetailoftranscript

onactualoccasions,anexpectationthatseemstoanchortheEpistemicProgramtomaterial

worldsandmaterialstudyaswell.

Casesintheirconstitutivedetail

Asawayofspeaking,EthnomethodologyandConversationAnalysis(EMCA)were

leveragedfromadeepdisputewiththenormativeanalyticculturesoftheirday(andtothis

day).Thereweremanydisputes,butthisonemayhavebeencentral:byconventional

academicwisdoms,‘everydaylife’—intheprofusionofitsexpressions,occasions,

unremarkableengagementsandrecurrence—wasadensequotidianmasklaidoverwhere

andhowtheenginesofsocialordermightactuallybefoundanddisclosed.Ifanything,

everydaylifewasamis-direction,itsorderlinesswashidden,andsocialsciencewasthe

corrective(seeLynchandWong,[2016]).AsSacks(1984)observed,theconsensusmodus

wasthatoverwhelminglytheproductsofeverydaylifewerethedross,andtheaimof

formalanalysiswastorecoverthe‘goldenchain’thatwouldshowitselfonlyhereandthere

tothecredentialeddevicesofformal–analyticreasoning(andglovedhands).

Tothesepresumptivereckonings,EMandCAproposedradicallyalternate

understandings:thateverydaylifewasalreadyinpossessionofthetermsandresources

forunderstandingitsrelentlesslyachievedorder.ThiswaspartoftheshockofCAfor

languagestudy.Ithadbeenassumedthatvernacularorderownedneitherprovenancenor

authorization.Itdidnotrundeeplyenough.Worse,werevernaculargrammarsadmitted,

Page 4: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

4

thedisciplinesmightconfrontanuncountablehorizonofgrammaticalformsand

competencies,outstrippingallbesteffortsatsystematicreduction.1EMCA’sunderstanding

ofvernacularworldssetstheselong–standingformal–analyticambitionsatseriousrisk,

andstilldoes.LikeCA’s“proofprocedure”whereinnextturnsareconsultedfor

understandingthesenseofpriorturns,ourinterests,descriptionsandmeasuresdonot

strayfarfromvernacularreckonings,orourtranscripts.Theaimisnottotakeourleaveof

thevernacularorder,atleastuntilweunderstandit.Andonceunderstood,theremaybe

noreasontotakeleaveofit.

Transcripthasthusbeenthe‘portal’forCAstudiesontoanunexaminedorderliness

runninginfullandpublicview.Itisthroughtranscriptthatsocialactionisrenderedas

concertedlyon–goingproductionsinfinelycraftedtemporalandsequentialdurations.

Theseproductionsarewhatholdourinterest,andthesenseofthephrase“animating

transcript”referstotheworkofhowweleveragetheseproductionaccounts,getting

transcripttospeakoftheobjects,descriptionsandorientationsweclaimforthem.2Often

enough,thetaskisnaturalisticallydescriptive,aswhennotinganoverlappingturn,a

complaint,orevenadisagreement.Routinely,wehearthemasanycompetentspeaker

wouldhearthem.Elsewhere,however,descriptionsareachievedbytheaccountsthat

organizeanddeliverthem.ForCA,andpresumablytheEP,transcriptisthegenerative,

constitutivefieldoffindings,andtheyincludeaccount–ablefindings,findingstiedtothe

accountsthatreflexivelyleveragethemintoview(asinaccountsof“doingindirection”

[Sacks,1992,v.2:101],therepairspace,everydiscussionof“possibles”[Schegloff,

2006:145],orevena“question”[Schegloff,1984:29]).

Page 5: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

5

TalkofEMreflexivityisseldomeasy,andwewanttopursueitwithasmall

collectionofmaterialsthatmaybegintogetatthiswork.Byturningtotreatmentsof

actualmaterials,wearereturningtotheexpectationwithinCAthatsequentialanalysiswill

treatactualcases“formally”and“informatively”,andfurtherthatwemightreturntothe

materialsthemselvestoassessanalyticdisputesordifferences(Sacks,1984).AsSchegloff

(1991)observedthe“testoftheadequacyofadescriptionofsomepractice[is]itscapacity

toyieldconvincinganalysisofsingularepisodesofconversation”(1991:153).Inpart,this

“test”isawaytoassessthecogencyofouraccountsandwhethertheyarerevealingof

whattheparticipantsaredoingandalso,andespecially,whether—andhow—ouraccounts

canhaveusefortheprofuse,unremarkableandyetconstitutivedetailregisteredinour

transcripts.

Implicittotheseproposalswasnotonlyapenetratingcritiqueofformalanalysis.It

wasalsotheproposalthatnaturallanguagestudycouldnotbedisengagedoruncoupled

fromthemasteryofnaturallanguage.Onecouldnotdothisworkandnotbeacompetent

overhearer(seeWongandOsher,2000).Forthisreasonweinvitethereadertoengagethe

discussionsthatfollowassequentialanalysts,ontheonehand,andalsoascompetent

over–hearers.Itisbybothcompetenciesthatwemayhavesomethinginterestingtosay

aboutordinarytalk,itsorderlyproductions,andtheirdescription.3

Thecorpus

ThematerialspresentedbelowappearintheearlypublicationsdiscussedinMacbethetal.

(2016),andalsosubsequentpublications.AsreadersoftheEPmayhavenoticed,manyof

Page 6: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

6

thetranscribedmaterialsappearmultipletimesacrosspublications,andalsomultiple

timeswithinsinglepublications.

ThefirstsequenceappearsatleastinHeritageandRaymond(2005)andinHeritage

(2011),andweareusingthenarrativefromthelaterpublication.Ittakesupassessments,

and“…casesinwhichonepartyevaluatessomestateofaffairstowhichtheotherhasno

accessatall...”(Heritage2011:160).Inthe“noaccess”wehaveafoundationalseparation

thatunderwritestheepistemicorganizationofinteractionasbetweenthosewhohave

access,knowledgeorinformation,andthosewhodonot.Asonemightthenexpect,a

recurrentprojectfortheEPistheparsingofkindsorwaysormeasuresofknowing.

LottieandEmma:firstassessments

Heritage(2011:160)introducesthefollowingsequence(hisexhibit3),bynotingthat

Emma’s(Emm)sisterLottie(Lot)hasreturnedfromanapparentlyexhilaratingtriptovisit

friendsinPalmSprings.Hermethodofrepresentingthehouseshestayedatcentersonits

inaccessibilitytohersister:

(1)

1 LOT: hhJeezizChris’yoush’dseethathouseE(h)mmayih’av

2 ↓noidea.h[hmhh

3 EMM: [Ibetit’sadrea:m.

Inthediscussionfollowingthetranscript,weread:

Patentlylackingtheresourcestoenterintoadirectappreciationofthehousebythe

verytermsofLottie’sassessment,EmmaalignswithLottie’sevaluationbymeansofa

subjunctiveexpressionofherlikelyevaluation,therebyachievingasimulacrumof

Page 7: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

7

agreement(HeritageandRaymond2005).(Heritage2011:160)

Thisisadenseaccountofathree–linetranscriptoftwoturns.Notefirsttheframing

ofwhatisinterestingaboutthesequence:itisanitemtoacollectionofassessmentsabout

matters“towhichtheotherpartyhasnoaccessatall…”(Heritage2011:160).Thereare

twopotentialdifficultieshere,beginningwithhowfirstassessmentsarerecipient

designed.Wedon’tnormallyofferknowingassessmentsofanoperaticperformance,for

example,tootherswhoknownothingaboutopera[unlessweareteachingthem].Both

Sacks(1992)andPomerantz(1984:61,63)remindusthatfirstassessmentsareproduced

forpersonswhocanperfectlywellofferasecond,andthusagreeornotinnextturn,and

wethinkthatisclearlysointhiscaseaswell.Intheparticularshere,wecanalsoask:is

Lottie’sfirstturnanassessmentcallingforasecond,oranenthusiasticreportofhertrip

experience?Byeitheraccountaresponseiscalledforinnextturn,andCA’sproof

procedurerecommendsthatweconsultnextturnforunderstandingitsprior.

Second,theexpression“youhavenoidea”figurescentrallyinthetreatmentof

Emma’s“access”.Butifoneexaminestheaudiorecordandtranscriptasitwaspreparedby

Jefferson,youwillfindmultipleusesoftheidiom“youhavenoidea”aspartofLottie’s

breathlessaccounting,andaverydifferentimpressionofEmma’s“access”.IntheJefferson

transcriptthetargetsequenceisfoundinlines81-84.Butinthepriortalktheydiscussthe

houseintermsweshallhearagain,alongwithitslocation—itsneighborhoodand

landmarks—alocationthatEmmaevidentlyknowswellenough(seeSchegloff,1972on

formulationsofplace):

(1a)

Page 8: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

8

[CABank/Jefferson/NB:IV:10:R/21swimnude.cha]4

43 Emm: =Oh:honeyIbetthehouseisbeautiful.hu:h?

44 Lot: °Oh:::GodEmma.°

45 (.)

46 Lot: °Jeeziz.°Ho:wlu:cky.h

47 (.)

48 Emm: Mm::::.

49 Lot: Yih'avnoideait'srightacrossthestreetfromthe:::El

50 Torrero.

51 Emm: Oh:::.

52 Lot: Ye:ah.

53 Emm: OhnotneartheIndianWe:lls.

54 (1.0)

55 Lot: °ihYe::ah:?°(0.2)It'sih-i-IndianWe:llsuhwellit'sa:ll

56 Indi⌈anWe⌈:lls'nP⌉a:lmDesertnowthey'vecha:nged=

57 Emm: ⌊·hhhh⌊Yeah.⌋

58 Lot: =ityihkn⌈owtuhP⌉a:lmDe:sert,

59 Emm: ⌊Yeah:.⌋

Page 9: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

9

Emmaknowstheseformulationsofplace,andastheirconversationcontinues,we

encounterthesequencewearediscussinganddiscoveradifferentsequence,and

specifically,averydifferentsecondturnbyEmma:

(1b)

79 Lot: [But]

80 Emm: [Ohisn’]thatwonder[ful]

81 Lot: ⌊hh⌋JeezizChriseshush'dseethathouse

82 E(h)mmayih'av↓noidea.h⌈hmhh

83–>Emm: ⌊Ibetit'sadrea:m.<Wihthe

84–> swimmingPOO:LENCLO:SED⌈HU:H?

85 [u-

86 Lot: Oh::::::::Kho:dwe·hhihhhuhhu↑Weswaminthen:ude·hh

87 Sundeenightu(h)ntilaba⌈httwouh'clo:ck.⌉

88 Emm: ⌊ehhhehhehhuhh⌋a:h

Ascanbeseen,Emma’sactualturnatlines83-84is:

Emm: Ibetit'sadrea:m.<WihtheswimmingPOO:LENCLO:SED ⌈HU:H

WecannottellhowEmmaknowsofthepoolfromthepriortalk.Andinsubsequenttalk

Lottiecorrectstheaccount:thepoolisn’tenclosed,it’s“outsidethebigglassdoors”,though

Lottietreatsthemis-descriptionasherown(lines529–534).Butwhatisquiteclearisthat

thetranscript,onceseen,ratherdeeplyqualifiestheaccountofwhatEmma‘knows’as“no

Page 10: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

10

access”,aformulationthatseemstobeoperatingfromakindof“sense–impression

empiricism”,wherebyifoneparty‘saw’somethingthattheotherpartydidn’tsee,theother

partywouldhave‘noaccess’andthefirstwouldhavesoleaccess.Butagain,thisdissolves

thepremiseofanassessmentsequence.Moreover,onemayknowofthingsnotseen‘this

time’(rushhourtraffic,drearyweather,magnificenthouses,etc.)wellenoughtojoina

knowingconversationaboutthem.Aninsistenceon‘senseimpressions’isnotquite

recognizableinaliteraturethathastakenkeeninterestinaccounts,tellings,andthe

account-abilityofordinaryworlds.Viatheaccountsofothers,weknowfarmoreofthe

world—anditswarrantedassessments—thanwhatwehave‘actuallyseen’.5

So,it’snotthatEmmawouldmistakethishouseforaboat,orthatshehasnever

heardLottie’sexpressionabout“havingnoidea”before(it’sfirstheardinline49-50).

LottieisnotspeakingnorisEmmahearingquitesoliterally.AndEmma'sreplyshowsshe

doesindeed‘haveanidea’,ofhowLottieisspeaking:afirstidiomaticexpressionreceivesa

next:justasshe‘hasnoidea’,thenit‘mustbedream’,forthemboth.Neitherisliterallyso,

andnotwithstandingsubjunctiveexpressions,alignmentsdomorethanmarkabsences.

Onelastpointontheissueof‘access’andthenotionthatEmma”…hasnoaccessat

all…”Briefly,inLottie’sfirstturnsheremarks:

1 hhJeezizChriseyoush’dseethathouseE(h)mmayih’av

2 |↓noidea.h[hmhh

Wewanttonotetheturnproduction“…youshouldseethathouse…,”andespeciallythe

phrase“thathouse”.Onconsideration,acompetentspeakerwillhearthatthisisnotafirst

topicmention.‘Thathouse’isanindexicalexpressionanddirectlyassumesthatthe

Page 11: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

11

recipientalreadyhasaccesstothemattersreferredto.IfEmmadidnot,shewouldhaveno

ideaofwhathousethathouseis.Butsheclearlydoes,andindeedthefulltranscriptshows

that‘thathouse’hasbeenatopicoftheirdiscussionforsometime.Oneneedn’tseeit,to

knowofit.

Sobyvariousmovesandframings,theEpistemictreatmentanimatesthetranscript

toleverageitsfindings.There’saformalityandstiffnesstotheaccount.There’snothingof

theevidentexcitementthepartiesshareinthetelling,andit’shardtofindevidenceoftheir

demonstrableorientations.Theturnsseemtobecharacterizedratherthananalyzedin

theirproductionfeatures.Further,tofinda“simulacrumofagreement”hereistowritea

metricof“kindsofagreements”andthendeclarewhatkindthisoneis.Itisanoverhearer’s

measureentirely.

Trixie:Epistemicassessmentsandoh–prefacedagreements

Thesecondsequencehastodowithaconversationbetweentwodogbreedersdiscussedin

Heritage(2002),HeritageandRaymond(2005)andHeritage(2012a).Arecurrenttheme

acrossmanyEPanalysesoftranscriptisoneof‘proprietaryrelations’,orhowonemay

‘own’thingslikeknowledge,experience,spouses,children,grandchildren,catsanddogs.

Thisownershipcanbecentraltoepistemicstatusandauthority.Intheparticularshere

(fromHeritage2002:204-205,exhibit11):

[T]wodogbreeders—NormanandIlene—havebeentalkingaboutthereadinessofone

ofNorman'syoungerdogstohaveafirstlitter.…Andatline9,Ilenementionsoneof

Norman'sotherdogs(Trixie),whoapparentlybeganbreedingatayoungage:

(2)[Heritage1:11:4]

Page 12: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

12

1Ile: Nowellshe'sstillabityoungthoughisn't[she<ahme]an:=

2Nor: [She::]

3Ile: =uh[:

4Nor: [Shewzayear:la:stwee:k.

5Ile: Ahyes.Ohwellanytimeno:w[then.]

6Nor: [Uh:::]:[m

7Ile: [Ye:s.=

8Nor: =Butshe[:'s()]

9Ile:—> [CuzTrixiestarted]soearly[didn'tsh[e,

10Nor:—> [°Oh::[ye:s.°=

11Ile: =°Ye:h°=

Thenarrativeresumes:

HereNorman'soh-prefacedagreement(line10),inconveyingtheindependenceofhis

assessmentfromIlene's,alsoalludestohisepistemicprioritywithrespecttothe

informationinquestion…Atthesamemoment,Ilene'stagquestion(line9)

downgradestheepistemicstrengthofwhatwouldotherwisebeaflatassertion.

Andfurther:

…theepistemicpriorityofthesecond,oh-prefacingspeakerisavailablefromthetopic

andcontextoftheinteractionandinexplicitlyindexedinthetalk.(Heritage,2002:205)

Thereareseveralthingstoteaseouthere.ThesequencebeginswithIlene

suggestingthatthepupinquestionis'stillabityoung'.Normancitesherageinline4,and

Ileneseemstoreceiveitassettlingthematter.Butshereturnswiththerecollectionabout

Page 13: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

13

Trixieinline9,andherre-completion,“didn'tshe”,seemslessaquestionoradowngrade,

thanacontinuationofherreminderaboutthecaseofTrixiewithacallforagreement.

Ileneisnot‘requestinginformation’;sheseemstobecallingforconfirmation,andshegets

it.HerturnseemstobethetouchoffforNormaninline10,inhissoftlyspoken'Oh::yes'.

ButnotethatitisinoverlapofIlene’sre-completion.Sothere-completion(‘didn’tshe’)is

nottheturncompletionthatNormanaddresses;headdressesIle’sreminderofline9,

wheresoftspeakingcanbeamarkofuncertainty,anddiscovery.

Andgiventhattheyarebothdogbreederswithevidentaccesstoasharedhistory,it

isdifficulttohearNormanconveyingeither‘epistemicpriority’or‘independence'inline

10,whether“inexplicitlyindexedinthetalk”,ornot(thesenseofthatphraseisnot

developed).Itisdifficulttoseehowthetranscriptdeliverstheaccounttheanalysis

suggests.

Instead,itseemsthatNormanhasbelatedlydiscoveredIlene'sfirstallusionto

‘troubles’producedinline1.IleneartfullypursuesitinherreferencetoTrixieinline9

(andhowNormanmaybeproceedingwithadecisionthatdidnotworkwellthelasttime).

Andthatheagreeswithitinline10,oncehediscoversit,thenreceivesIlene’ssoftlylatched

confirmationinline11ofwhatNormanhasnowfoundasthethingshehadbeenpointing

tofromthebeginning.Butthisisaverydifferentaccountofthesequence.Itattachesto

thedetailofthetranscriptratherthantoprioritiesorstatuses“inexplicitlyindexed”.It

treatstherecordinevidence.Ofthedifference,wethinkthatSchegloff’s(1991:153)

measureof“convincinganalysisofsingleepisodesofconversation”continuestobevery

useful.Moredirectly,

Page 14: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

14

Oneofthekeytasksofresearchersindevelopingclaimsforaphenomenonisnotto

sacrificethedetailedexaminationofsinglecasesonthealtarofbroadclaims—

especiallywhenthecasesaremeantasevidenceforthebroadclaim;oneofthekeytasks

ofreadersistoexaminethedetailedanalysisofsinglecasesasepisodeswiththeirown

reality,deservingoftheirownrigorousanalysiswithoutrespecttotheirbearingonthe

largerargumentforwhichtheyarebeingputforward.(Schegloff,2010:42,italicsin

original)

Janissick:inappositeinquiries

Thenextsequenceisabout‘Oh-prefacedresponsestoinquiries’andisplacedunderthe

heading“Questionsrenderedinappositebycontext.”Inthisinstance,

…therespondenttreatstheobjectofaninquiryasself-evidentbyvirtueofitsphysical

context,orofpersons'culturalorpersonalknowledge.Inexhibit15,Jancomestothe

phoneandgivesacough(1–>).Ivy'ssubsequentinference(2–>)getsanoh-prefaced

confirmation(3–>).(Heritage1998:301)

(3)(Heritage,1998:301-302,exhibit15)

[Rah:A:1(2):1]

1 Jan: 1–> khhhh-huhkhh-huhkhh.hhhHellothereI[vy..hhhh

2 Ivy: [Oh:dea:hme:.

3 Jan: khh=

4 Ivy: 2–> =A[h(r)youstill'vgoti:t.

5 Jan: [khh

Page 15: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

15

6 Jan: 3–> Ohyes.Iwzprettybady(h)estiday.[.hhhh-

7 Ivy: [Oh::dih-

8 Ivy: Yihnotinbedareyou,

ThenarrativeabovehasIvydisplayingherinferencethatJanisstillsickinline4,andJan

producingher‘Oh-prefaced’replyinline6tomarkitas‘inapposite’,asOh-prefacedreplies

insuchplacesaresaidtodo,becauseJan’sstateofhealthwasalreadyevidentinhercough-

interruptedgreeting.

WecannotefirstanequivocationinthedescriptionofIvy’sline4:itisframedasan

“inquiry”butthencharacterizedasan“inference”,andperhapsthereisgoodcauseforit.

Wearenotsureofthelogicalrelationsbetweeninquiriesandinferences,buttocomplicate

things,line4alsoappearstobeaconclusion,andanimmediateconclusionfromwithinits

localproductionenvironment.NotealsohowIvy’sfirstinference/conclusionisevidenced

inline2—her“Ohdearme”.Notefurtherhowtheproductionofline4,slightlysimplified,

seemstobe:

Ivy: Ahryoustill’vgoi:t.

Absenttheaudiorecord,wecan’tsettlethematter,butachangeinitsproduction—a

repair—seemstobeinplay.Itseemstobeginas‘Areyoustill…?’,andbecomes‘youstill’v

goi:t.’Thatis,whatmayhavebeenlaunchedasininquiry,iscompletedasaconclusion.

Andthereisgoodsenseforthehearingwhenweconsulttheproductionoftheturninits

course.

3Jan: khh=

Page 16: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

16

4Ivy: =A[h(r)youstill'vgoti:t.

5Jan: [khh

6Jan: Ohyes.Iwzprettybady(h)estiday.[.hhhh-

Line3showsacoughintheclear,towhichIvylatchesherline4with‘Ahr…’Butherturn

beginningisthenoverlappedwithasecondcough,andnowwehaveIvyspeakinginthe

contextofconsecutivecoughing,andinsofarascoughingevidencesillness,herturn

[seemingly]concludesasmuch.

Thepremiseofthediscussionof‘Oh-prefacedrepliestoinquiries’isthataninquiry

isinaptifitsanswerisalreadyinevidence,andwedoseemtohavethesecondpartofthat

formulationinplay.That‘youstill’vgotit’isindeedinevidence:thecoughingbracketsthe

turn’sinitiation.ButitisIvywhoisshowinganorientationto‘what’sevident’inhowher

turntakesthetrajectoryitdoes.Thisraisesfurtherquestions:isherconclusioninline4—

‘youstill’vgotit’—“inapposite”initsorientationtowhatisalreadyinevidence,andifso

wouldeverysuchconclusionfromwhatisinevidencenotbeinapposite?6AndifIvy’s

conclusionofline4isnotinappositeforthesereasons,theninwhatsenseisJanmarkingit

sowithher‘Oh-preface’inline6?7Isitthat‘inappositeness’isaunilateraljudgmentthat

fallstotherecipientwheneveracurrentturnremarksonaffairs‘inevidence’?Butof

course,thedomaininquestionisn’tjustanyremarkonaffairsinevidence.Therelevant

domainis“inquiries”,andthisreturnsustotheequivocationinthetreatmentofIvy’sturn

asan‘inquiry’,an‘inference’,ornowa‘conclusion’(ourterm).Andintheequivocation,

thereisthesuggestionofapotentiallyenormousorganizationaldomain—remarkson

thingsinevidence—forwhichtheepistemictreatmentoffersnoguidanceastowhere

Page 17: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

17

‘inquiriesaboutthingsinevidence’standwithrespecttothelargerdomain.Andifthatis

so,thenitwouldseemthediscussionofan‘Oh–prefaced’markingishugelyundeveloped;

thereissimplynodiscussionofwhatmattersinevidencearesensiblyfoundinapposite

whensightedasinquiries,inferencesorconclusions,andwhen,muchlessaconsideration

ofwhatotherworkan‘Oh-prefaced’responsetoaninquirymightdo.8

Inthisway,andforthissequence,wecanfindandaffirmanorientationto‘what’sin

evidence’.Butitisrelievedofanysenseofinappositeness.Thatsense,fortheEP,seemsto

relyentirelyontheassertionthat‘oh-prefacedreplies’aresomotivated;theassertion

seemstobedeterminativeofthefinding,yethasanuncertainrelationshiptothematerials.

WhileJan’ssecondturninline6iscertainly‘Oh-prefaced’,andwhilethediscussionwrites

amotivatedaccountforit,itisnotthenthecasethattheturnitselfgivesmuchevidencefor

theaccount.The‘Oh’herecouldwellbeanappreciationofIvy’sconclusionaboutJan’s

circumstances,aconclusionleveragedfromher[Ivy’s]closeattentiontotheirexchangein

itscircumstantialparticulars,andtheopeningitaffordsforJan’stellingthatfollows.

There’slotsofcoughing,andIvymissesnoneofit.

Assickpeopledo,weoftenanswerthephonewithevidencethatwe’resick.And

withsuchevidence,allinquiriesabout‘howareyou?’arevirtuallyquestionswithknown

answers.Thiskindofredundancyseemsfarfrominapposite.Instead,itgivesthecalled

theresourceswithwhichtoshowthemeasurenotofaninappositequestionorinference,

butofjusthowsicksheis,orhasbeen,asJandoesinthesameturn.Asanimpression,

commiseration,sympathyandcondolencesequencesareroutinelymadeofredundancy(as

aregreetings,closingsanddeliveriesofnews).Measuressurelyareatplayhere,measures

ofhowsickJanwasandisnow,butfortheseparties,theyarenotaboutinapposite

Page 18: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

18

inquiries.Andinthemeasure,wegainsomesenseforhowwell“informationredundancy”

isservingourunderstandingofactualcasesintheirturn–by–turnproduction.

Hyla’sboyfriend:Free–standing‘Ohs’andtheiropacity

Thefourthsequenceappearsinmultiplepublications,inHeritage,1984;2005;2012a,b;and

2013a,b,andinmultipleplaceswithinsinglepublications.Weareconjoiningtwoofthem

presentedin1984,andcollatingdiscussionsabout‘free-standingOhs’andtheiropacity.As

wasdiscussedinMacbethetal.(2016),therecipientofa‘free-standingOh’willawaitfurther

instructionorinvitationbeforetakinganextturnbecausetheopaqueexpressiondefeatsan

understandingofwhat’stobedonewithit,next.Inthissequence,Nancyistalkingtoher

friendHylaaboutHyla’snewromanticinterestinSanFrancisco.

(4)(Heritage1984:310,exhibit26)

[HG:II:25]

1 Nan: .hhhDzhe'av'izownapa:rt[mint?]

2 Hyl: [.hhh]Yeah,=

3 Nan: =Oh:,

4–> (1.0)

5 Nan: Howdidjugit'iznumber,

6 (.)

7 Hyl: I(h)(.)c(h)alledinfermation'nSanFr'ncissc(h)[uh.

8 Nan:–> [Oh::::.

Page 19: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

19

9 (.)

10 Nan:–> Verycleve:r…

(Heritage,1984:325)

Thenarrativetellsusthat“[Inline4],theinformativepartywithholdsfurtheron-topictalk

afteran‘oh’receiptuntilreceivingarequesttodoso…”

Inthiscontext,itmayfurtherbenotedthat,whereas"oh"mayproposeachangeof

stateinresponsetoaninforming,itisentirelyopaqueastothequalityorcharacterof

thechangeofstateproposedlyundergonebyitsproducer.Thusaninformant/"oh"

recipientmaywithholdfromfurthertalkwithaviewtopermitting/invitingthe"oh"

producertoelaboratewhatlaybehindtheproductionoftheparticle.(Heritage,1984:

325)

So,NancyisquizzingHylaabouthernewromanticinterest,andthenarrativetellsusthatit

isthe‘opacity’ofNancy’s“Ohs”inresponsetoHyla’sdisclosuresthatpressesthesequence

forward,asHylawithholdstopermitorinviteNancyto‘elaboratewhatliesbehindthe

particle’.Thus,whatchangeliesbehindtheparticle,initsopaqueandgenericsilence,

substantiallyshapesthesequencewefind.

Notefurtherthattheopacityandthewithholdingareexpressionsofuncertaintiesof

information,andthusuncertaintyorganizessequencedevelopment.Andnodoubt,there

aretimesandoccasionswherethisisso.Imagineinterrogations,whetherbypoliceof

suspects,orparentsoftheirchildren,orchildrenoftheirparents(seeHeritage,2012a,and

Terasaki,2004[1976]).Andinfact,wehavesomethingverymuchlikethathere,apractical

interrogation.Butitdoesn’tseemtobeanexpressionofahiddendynamicindexedonly

Page 20: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

20

hereandtherebyopaquetokens.Ratherthanexpressingsomethingthatis‘priorto’or

‘underneath’thegrammarsofconversation,whatHylaandNancyaredoinghereseems

insteadtobeamongtheirroutineachievements.

ThatHylaandNancyareengagedinapracticalandknowingcourseofinquiryis

evidencedwhenweconsultthelargersequence.Atranscriptofthissequencedatingtothe

1980sbeginswithaquestionfromHyla.9

(4a)(Schegloff1988:451-452,exhibit7)

[HG:22-23]]

01 Hyla: Y’knoww’tIdidlas’ni:[ght?

02 Nancy: [Wha:t,=

03 Hyla: =Didate:rriblethi::::[ng

04 Nancy: [YoucalledSi:m,

05 (04)

06 Hyla: No:,

07 (.)

08 Nancy: What,

09 (.)

10 Hyla: t’hhh[WellIhed–]

11 Nancy: [Youcalled]Richard,=

12 (): =hh–hh=

Page 21: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

21

13 Hyla: =(h)y(h)Yea(h)henIh(h)ungupw(h)un‘e

14 a(h)ns[wer

15 Nancy: [Oh:Hylawhy:::::

Thelastturnmayremindthereaderofthemodestpurchaseoftheinformational

interrogativeasacharacterizationofwhatworksuchturnscando.Butthelargerpointis

thatweseehowNancyandHylabecameengagedinaguessinggamecalled‘Doyouknow

whatIdidlastnight?’It’saperfectlyordinarything,andalovelycandidatefortalkabout

‘whoknowswhat’.AndindeeditunfoldsassequencesofNancypursuingwhatHylaknows

butisn’tsaying,yet[asinthegame’20questions’].Itshowsushowwemaywellfind

exchangesinwhichonepartyknowsthingstheotherdoesnot,andtheotherproceeds,

withthecooperationoftheknowingparty,tofindout.Andwheneveronefindssuchan

occasion,itwillbefoundastheoccasionedfeaturesof‘howwe’respeakingnow’.

WethereforeandcertainlyhaveinthissequencesomethinglikeadistributionofK+

andK–relations.Butdependingonwhetherwetreatthemasoccasionedrelationsor

formalstructures,wewillbeledtoverydifferentanimatingdescriptions.Intheparticulars

ofthesequence,asofHylaandNancyknowingperfectlywellhowtheyarespeaking,andas

itbecomesaseriesofNancy’spacedinquiries,eachnextanswertowhichreceivesher‘Oh’,

andthenadurationofherthinkingaboutit,andthenanextquestion,thereisnopuzzleor

mysteryoropacityasto“whatliesbehindtheproductionoftheparticle”.None.Thesense

ofthese‘Ohs’liesrightonthesurfaceofthesequenceswhoseproductionstheyjoinasan

emergingaccount.Wehavehereacollaborativecourseofinquiry.Thepartiesknowwho

knowswhat,asofthis‘languagegame’theyareplaying.TheplayofNancy’s‘Ohs’is

Page 22: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

22

evident,andwhatevertheymaybe‘indexing’ofanepistemicasymmetry,thepartieshave

beenknowingly,openlyspeakinginthosewaysfromthebeginning.Perhapsitisofthe

natureandachievementofsequentialanalysisandJeffersoniantranscriptsthattheyleave

littlespaceorneedforconjecturesofhiddenness.Therelentlesslyfamiliarworkof

commonunderstandingseemswrittenonthesurfacesofinteraction,andfortheparties

first.These‘Ohs’showustheparties’demonstrableorientations,andinthatwaywhat

theybringtoourattentionisfirstshowntothem.Formal–analyticaccountsarelate-

comerstothesepracticaltasksandachievements.

Discussion

ItisanunexpecteddimensionoftheCAcorpus,meaningthatperhapsnoonehadreasonto

imagineit40yearsago,thatintheduration,readersoftheliteraturewouldfindandnote

thereappearanceoftranscriptswehaveseenbefore.Samesequenceshavebeentreated

manytimestosameanddifferentissues,andweknowmanyofthembytheir‘handles’,like

“Twogirls”,or“Chickendinner”,or“Therapytalk”.Returningtothemislikerenewingan

acquaintance,andtheyieldisarichlyfamiliarcorpusfilledwithpracticaldemonstrations

ofwhattopics‘samematerials’cansustain,andalsoasensefortheemergenceoftopics,

interestsandconceptualdistinctionsacrossCA’sformativehistory.Theygiveevidenceofa

distinctivelydisciplinedformofinquiry.YetwhenweturntoEpistemicre–analysesof

thesesameandothermaterials,wecanfindaschism.

AcentralandrecurrentpuzzleforourreadingoftheEpistemicProgramishow

occasionedproductionfeaturesofturnandsequence,likethosewehavereviewedand

manyothers,arerenderedexpressionsofadurableformalstructureoperatinginthe

background.Transcriptisanimatedonbehalfofanomnipresentengineof“epistemic

Page 23: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

23

order”(Heritage,2008:309).Theserenderingsareprogrammaticachievements,genericin

theirtermsandoperations,andtreatedascausativeofwhatordermaybefoundonany

actualoccasion.ThiswouldseemtobethecentralaimandachievementoftheEP’s

animations,notwithstandingthatgeneralizationssuchastheseareroutinelycraftedatthe

expenseofactual,producedandconstitutivedetail,andwhatthatdetailmayshowus(cf.,

Sacks,1992,vol.2:430;Schegloff,2010).10

Ourinterestsfromtheoutsethavetakenupthecontinuitiesanddeparturesofthe

EpistemicProgramfromtheconceptualexcavationsofEMCA.Ontheonehand,thedebtis

unmistakable.Ontheother,departuresaretoo.Theseareofcourseconceptualmatters,

andasWinch(1958/1990)showsus,everytask,distinctionandpuzzleofsocialscience—

everythingtobegainedandlost—isaconceptualmatter.

Wehaveaddressedseveralsuchconceptualdeparturesthroughtheearly

publicationsofwhathasbecomearecognizableliteratureandprogramofEpistemic

studiesofnaturalconversation.Wecouldvariouslysumthosedepartures,asin,for

example,adeparturefromthedemonstrableorientationsofthepartiesasboththeaimand

map–worksforunderstandingtheirsequentialproductions.Intheirstead,wefinda

preferenceforformalstructuresofgenericparticles,gradients,statusesandstructures

operatingpriortoanyactualparticularization.Or,anditisanalliedmove,adeparture

fromproductionaccountsoftemporal–sequentialorderinpreferenceformorenearly

ordinalaccountsofpositionalorder.Or,howHeritage(2012c)andLevinson(2013)write

critiquesofCA’s‘proofprocedure’,wherebyweconsultwhatafirstturnyieldsinnextturn,

forunderstandingwhatworkthefirstisdoing.Thesedescriptionsareofcourseintermsof

thevernacularreckoningsofnaturallanguage,aboutthingslikequestions,complaintsand

Page 24: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

24

answers.Andforthisreason,thereisforLevinsonahermeneutic“softunderbelly”verging

on“theoccult”(2013:105)thatorganizesthem(seeLindwalletal.,2016).Butthenext

turn‘proofprocedure’isnotreallyabout‘proving’(noriseveryclarifyinganalytic

descriptionaproof;seeLynchandWong,2016).Itisratherawayofmakinguseof

vernacularreckoningstoassessourprofessionalreckonings,byconsultinghowtheparties

assesstheirown.Andasnotedabove,EMCAdoesnottakeitsleaveofthevernacularand

itsgrammarsofaction.Schegloffhaswrittenclearrepliestothosewhoproposeweshould

(Schegloff,1987;1988a,b;1991;1996;2009;2010).ThosegrammarsareindeedEMCA’s

aimtounderstand.Nordoesitseemquitenewsworthytosuggestthatvernacularorderis

nomorethananoccultproduction.Theassessmentisthefamiliarself–appointmentof

formalanalysis;toit,everythingelseisanoccultproduction.

ButperhapsmoretellingfortheEP,havinglittleuseforCA’s“proofprocedure”,itis

innowayclearwhattheEPoffersinitsstead,thatis,whatanalyticdispositionor

instructionitofferswherebywealignourinquiriesanddescriptionswithwhattheparties

aredemonstrablydoing,asbotharesourceforourinquiriesandameasureforassessing

theadequacyofourdescriptions.IftheCA‘proofprocedure’istobesetaside,whatshall

doitsworkwithintheEpistemicProgram?

Thequestionseemsquiteopen.Andwecanseewhatmaybeakindofprocedurein

discussionsofactionformationwhereinsomeoneinitiatingtheactionof“requesting

information”,regardedbytheEPasthe“theultimateparadigmofanadjacency–pairfirst

action”(Heritage,2012a:3),returnstoitinthirdturn,toconfirmthatthatwasindeed

whatshewasdoinginfirstturn.Thus,inthefollowingsequence,the‘Ohreceipt’ofa

“first–person”informationalaccountworkstoconfirmthatindeed,thepriorreportofa

Page 25: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

25

“hearsayaccount”wasarequestforinformationand/orconfirmationfromthefirst–hand

knowersheisspeakingtonow.Admittedly,thesetupisnoteasy,buthopefullythe

materialsshowitclearly.

(5)(Heritage2012a:10,exhibit10)

[Rah:12:4:ST][arrowsareinoriginaltext]

1Jen:–> =[OkaythenIw]’zaskin=‘erenshesaysyer

2 –> workingtomorrowezwell.

3Ida: YesI’ms’posetobetihmorrowyes,

4Jen:–> O[h:::.

5Ida: [Yeh,

Thecommentaryobserves:

Jenny’sdeclarativelyframedutterancereferencesinformationthatisinherrecipient’s

epistemicdomainandistreatedasarequestforconfirmation(line3).Hereitcanagain

benotedthatJenny’schangeofstate(K−→K+)oh-receiptconfirmsbyimplicationthat

heroriginaldeclarativewasindeedaquestioninsearchofinformation.(2012a:10)

Thesuggestionofa‘proofprocedure’isthatforhavingproducedher‘Oh–receipt’inline4,

wehaveproof“byimplication”thatJen’sfirstturn“wasindeedaquestioninsearchof

information,”oralternativelyarequestforconfirmationoftheprioraccount.Wenowhave

groundsforidentifyingtheactionformedinJen’sfirstturn.Butifso,andbyeitheraccount

ofthefirstturn’saction—asearchforinformationorarequestforconfirmation—itwould

beacuriousprocedure.Therelevant‘proof’ofa‘questioninsearchof

Page 26: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

26

information/confirmation’inJen’sfirstturnwouldseemtobeIda’snextturninline3.This

isthe“nextturnprocedure”producedbyandfortheparties,suchthatifsomethingelse

werebeingsolicitedbyJeninfirstturn,themis-understandingcouldberevealed.

Butweseemtohavehereinsteadathird–turnprocedure.AfterIda’snextturn,Jen

confirmswhatherfirstwaswithher“Oh”receiptinline4.Wehavethen,itseems,a

‘secondproof’.ButwhatneedhasJenfora“confirmation”atall;it’sherquestion.Andif

theconfirmationwereforIda—aconfirmationinthirdturnofthehearingfirstevidenced

inhernext–turnreply—whatneedhasIda?Or,ifIdahasneedforaconfirmationofthe

hearingevidencedinhernextturn,whywouldthisthird–turnprocedurebeprovincialto

theactionformationof“questionsinsearchofinformation”?Thatis,whywouldnotevery

question,compliment,complaint,assessment,agreement,andthefullpanoplyofaction

formationsnotbenefitfromthird–turnconfirmationstoo?Butthen,absentevidenceofthe

partiesneedforthisprocedure,wouldnotamorequotidianunderstandingofthis

expressiononthisoccasion(“Oh”)saveusthedifficultiesthatfollow?11There’sno

evidencethatnaturalconversationcouldactuallyevergoonthisway,inaregimeofthird

turnconfirmations.Butperhaps,alternatively,thisisnotaprocedureforsecuringthe

understandingsoftheparties;thoseunderstandingareroutinelyinhandthroughtheon–

goingworkofnextturns.Perhapsitisinsteadaprocedurefordetailingtheanalytic

formationsoftheEpistemicProgram,aninterpretativetextualgrammar,ratherthanone

fortheproductionofunderstandinginsitu.

ForCA,itisfromtheparties’understandingsthattheanalysttakesthemeasureof

herown.Thoseunderstandingsarenottheonlyresource,buttheyareacentralresource.

Andintheabsenceoftheparties’local,demonstrableorientations,theEPmustthenhave

Page 27: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

27

someregularorstableprocedurefordiscipliningtheover–hearinganalystinhowweshall

heartheparticularsofparticles,theirplacements,indexing,markedness,upgrading,

inappositenessandtherest.Thequestionbecomes:whatprocedureisthis?Inaregister

bothseriousandplayful,wewanttosumourremarksontheseexhibitsandtheir

EpistemictreatmentsthroughSacks’earlyworkonmembershipcategorization,andhis

formulationofa“Hearer’sMaxim”:“iftwoormorecategoriesareusedtocategorizetwoor

moremembersofsomepopulation,andthosecategoriescanbeheardascategoriesfrom

thesamecollection,then:hearthemthatway”(Sacks,1992:221).

Borrowingontheexpression,wewanttosuggestthatwhentheEPturnstoactual

cases,wefindanexercisethatcanbecalledan“Over–Hearer’sMaxim”.Itrunsroughly:“if

theanalystcanhearanagonisticstruggle,ifshecanhearacontestofinformationalor

experientialpossessions,orredundancy,orepistemicauthority,orsubordination,hearit

thatway.”

Bothmaximsunderwriteinterpretativedegreesoffreedom.Forthe“hearer’s

maxim,”avernacularpracticeofhearingcategoricalreferences,therearenoparticular

rulestoobserve,beyondthecategoricalboundariesinplay.Themembershipcategories

mustbeheardandusedsensibly,asanycompetentmemberwouldhearthem.Therules

lieinagrammarofappositeusage.

Withrespecttoanover-hearer’smaxim,however,theredoseemtoberule–

resourcesattached,bothformalandpermissive,authorizinghearingsthatreckonthe

metricsofthingslikeaspeaker’sindependence,status,access,authority,subordination,

andthelike,andwehaveaparticularformulationinmindthatsuggeststheinterpretative

Page 28: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

28

degreesoffreedominvolved.WefinditinHeritage(2002),regardingOh–prefacedsecond

assessments:

Insum,oh–prefacinginthecontextofagreementsisamethodpersonsusetoindexthe

independenceoftheiraccessand/orjudgmentinrelationtothestateofaffairsunder

evaluation…Thus,thebasicclaimhereisthatoh–prefacing,inandofitself,indexes

epistemicindependence:anindependencethatmayormaynotbeelaboratedbyother

elementsoftheturnthatfollows.Theindexingisinexplicit,markedandoptional.

(2002:204)

Thereareatleasttwostrikingthingsaboutthisproposal.Thepassagetreats‘Oh-

prefacinginthecontextofagreements’asanactionthat“inandofitself,indexesepistemic

independence”.Therearemanythingsdescribedanddevelopedinsequentialanalysis.

Manyofthemarenaturallanguageobjectsthatanycompetentspeakermightrecognize,

thingslikequestionsandanswers,requestsandrefusals.Therearealsolatches,hitches,

positionsandtechnicalnoticings,andsurmisesmadeofstillotherproductionfeatures.But

“indexingepistemicindependence”seemsanentirelydifferentkindofnoticing,andthe

difficultyisnotwith‘independence’,but“indexing”.Itseemstorequirean‘elsewhere’for

therecognizabilityoftheaction,giventhatit“mayormaynotbeelaboratedbyother

elementsoftheturnthatfollows”,andespeciallysoifthoseelementsincludethe

demonstrableorientationsoftheparties.“Indexing”seemstostandwideofthem.Inthis

way,itisafurtivething,andaswemayormaynotfinditsevidenceintheon-goingturn

andsequenceproduction,itnowseemsthatitisindexingthatis“opaque”andperhaps

“occult”,removedfromactualsequentialorganizationsandavailableonlysometimes,to

certaineyes.‘Indexingindependence’seemstoformulateanactionwithinananalytically

Page 29: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

29

constructedvocabularyofmotives,andforeverysuchconclusiononecanfairlyask:has

thisbeenshown,andcanitbeshownorfoundasamovewithinthesequentialgrammarof

naturalconversation?12

Second,thereisaremarkablestringofadjectivesthatconcludesthispassage:that

theindexingworkofanOh-prefaceis“….inexplicit,marked,andoptional.”Thereseemto

betworeadingsthatwecanhaveforit:oneisthattheseareadjectivesforthe“indexing”

workof‘Oh’,andthatthatwork—‘Oh’swork’—canbe“…inexplicit,marked,andoptional.”

Thisseemstobetheaim,althoughwethenhavethetaskofreconcilingthe“inexplicit”,the

“marked”,andthe“optional”.Thisisadiversecollectionofcategories[“markedness”isa

productionfeature;whatare“inexplicit”and“optional”?],andevenifwepermitthem,how,

onanyactualoccasion,shallweknowwhichitis,orwas,thistime?Thequestionleadstoa

secondreading.

Thesecondreadingistoseethestringofadjectivesasresourcesfordevelopinga

professional–analyticdiscourse,meaningthatiftheworkof“indexing”showsitselfinthese

ways—again,as“…inexplicit,marked,andoptional”—thenshouldthereaderortheparties

totheoccasionfailtoseeorgiveevidenceofit,itisbecausethesearemattersthatareboth

“inexplicit”and“optional,”andonlysometimes‘marked’;itmightbedoneandnotheard

(inexplicit),ornotdoneatall(optional).(Perhapsanalystsandpartiesalikecansee

indexingthatis‘marked’.)Thisarmamentofadjectivespermitstheanalysttofindindexing

workwherenooneelsecan.Andthatisanextraordinaryresourceandprivilegeforthe

tasksof“animatingtranscript”,asitisanextraordinaryretreatfromthepremisethatthe

orderoftalk-in-interactionisthroughoutanorderlinessavailabletothepartiessoengaged,

asoftheircompetencetoitsproductions.TheEPwouldseemtohaveitthattheanalyst

Page 30: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

30

canseewhatthepartiescannot,andinthiswecanfindareturntothefamiliardisposition

ofsocialscienceasaformal–analyticexercise(Garfinkel,2002,passim).

Conclusion

BeyondtheseveralremarksinHeritage(1984a)and(1998),thecentralroleofinformation

transferasthetrafficofinteractionisnotmuchdevelopedforanothertenyears.Itisthen

formulateddirectlyinHeritage2012a,2012b,thoughasargumentwithoutbenefitof

materials(seeLynchandWong,2016):

[H]owdoutterancesfunctionasrequestsforinformation?Howarerequestsfor

informationasaspecificformofsocialactionbuiltandmadeactionableassuch?Thisis

notanidlequestion.Requestsforinformationaretheultimateparadigmofan

adjacency–pairfirstaction(Schegloff,2007;StiversandRossano,2010)thatmake

responseactionableandaccountablewithoutdelayacrossmanylanguages....(Heritage,

2012a:3)

Andinthesamespecialissue:

Theideathat“information”isakeyelementincommunication,motivatingand

warrantingcontributionstotalk,ishardlyanewone.Itisastapleofcommunication

theoriesfromShannonandWeaver(1949)onward,ofawidevarietyoffunctional

linguistictheoriesfocusingonthegiven–newdistinction…,andmany,comparatively

diverse,pragmatictheories…dealingwithsentenceconstructionandinterpretation….

Ingeneral,however,conversationanalysis(CA)stoodasidefromthesetrends,despite

clearevidencethatacknowledgingnewinformationasnewandtherebyenactingthe

updatingofcommongroundisthefirstorderofbusinesstransactedbymany“sequence

Page 31: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

31

closingthirds”(Schegloff,2007)andrelatedacknowledgments.(Heritage,2012b:31,

selectedcitationsomitted)

Perhaps.Yetsomehow,itseemscompletelyunlikelythatSacksetal.wereun-awareofthe

flourishingtradeininformationstudiesreportedabove.It’sentirelypossiblethatthey

“stoodaside”forgoodreason,thoughwhatreasonstheywerearenowheretakenupinthe

EP’sdiscussion.

Schegloffhaswrittenmorefrequentlyinrecentyearsonthefirsttasksofmaking

senseofsinglecasesintheirconstitutivedetailandorganizations.Thispassageisn’tquite

sorecent:

Amongthemostrobusttraditionalanchorsfortheanalysisoflanguagebeyondthelevel

ofsyntaxareorientationstoinformationandtruth.Thispositionneedstobe

reconsidered….Especially(butnotexclusively)inconversation,talkisconstructedand

isattendedbyitsrecipientsfortheactionoractionsitmaybedoing.Evenifwe

consideronlydeclarative-typeutterances…theinformativenessortruthofan

utteranceis,byitself,nowarrantorgroundsforhavinguttereditorforhavinguttered

itataparticularjunctureinanoccasion.Thereisvirtuallyalwaysanissue(for

participantsand,accordingly,forprofessionalanalysts)ofwhatisgettingdonebyits

productioninsomeparticularhere-and-now.(Schegloff,1995:187)

Aprogrammaticcritiqueisquiteclear.WewanttoextenditthroughapassagefromSacks.

WeofferitonbehalfofthetasksandconceptualcommitmentsthatmarkwhatSacks

(1984)referredtoas“ethnomethodology–conversationanalysis”.Thosetaskslievery

Page 32: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

32

closetotheachievementsofcommonunderstandingandthusworlds–in–common,andthe

challengeofpursuingtheirproductiondescriptionsonanynextoccasion.

Thepassageisfoundinhislectureon“conveyingInformation”(1992),andneedsto

bereadcarefullytogethisspokenphrasingjustright:

(P)eoplesupposethatwhatwe'vebeentalkingaboutallalong,youknowinthewayI

toldittoyou,andIsupposethatinproducinganynextthingIsay.Andwithout

thinkingaboutit,theworkIdoistofindforanyitemyousay—nomatterhowgrosslyit

misunderstandswhatIsay—howwellitunderstandswhatIsay.(Sacks,1992,v.2,part

III:lect.2:184,emphasisadded.)

Hisremarksareatalkingformulationoflongandthoughtfulobservationsabout

ordinarythingsandoccasions,andourquestionis:HastheEpistemicProgramneedoruse

forobservationslikethese?Doesit,canit,takeinterestinthepraxiologiesSacksis

referringto?Saiddifferently,aretheachievementsthatSacksisspeakingof,intheirtaken

forgrantedvernaculargrammarsandorganizations,expressionsof‘information

possessionsandtransfers’?Andifnot,howdoestheEpistemicProgramstandtoCA’s

long–standingtaskandprogramofun-packingtheachievementsofcommon

understandingonanyactualoccasion,whilenottakingleaveoftheiroccasion?

DrewobservesmorethanoncethattheEpistemicproposalisa“radical”one,and

continues,“Ifinditdifficultfullytoconceptualizeorexpresssuccinctlyhowitisthat

Heritage’sepistemicengineissoradicalandprofoundaproposal”(2012:63).Wethinkhe

isquiteright.Thequestionthenbecomes:Whatkindofradicalinnovationisthis?We

wanttorespectfullysuggestthatitleveragesaturnawayfromthecorpusofGarfinkel,

Page 33: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

33

Sacks,Schegloff,Jefferson,theircolleaguesandstudents,aturnawayfromtheanalysisof

occasionedproductionsandtheplayoftemporalityandsequenceinthereflexive

constructionofsocialaction.

Saiddifferently,alongsideCA’sregardforthingsliketheparties’“demonstrable

orientations,”or“proceduralconsequentiality,”or“problemsofrelevance”,allasof“cases

intheirconstitutivedetail”(seeSchegloff,1991),perhapstheemblematicheuristicofCA,a

questionintendedtodirectusbacktothecontingentproductionsweencounterinactual

materials,has,bythelightsoftheEpistemicTurn,beenansweredinadvance.“Whythat

now?”isthequestion.TheEPrecommendsepistemicstatusorstanceor“balance”asthe

answer,ineverycase.Butaheuristicsoansweredinadvancenolongerhasheuristic

powers.

TheEpistemicturnmaywellhaveitsrewards.Thepromiseofagrandanalytic

amalgamisattractive,andthepromiseofanover-archinganalyticconsensusonourtasks,

butfirst,onourquestions,issomethingsocialsciencehaspursuedmorethanonce.The

pathseemsfamiliarinthatway,wherebystipulationsarerequiredinadvanceinorderto

underwrite—intheparticularshere—aprevailingstructureofinvidious,agonistic

relationsasthedriverofconversationitself,itsturns,sequences,andcontingent

productions.AsDrewobserves,wewouldthenfindanorderlinessthatis“constant,

omnipresent,andomnirelevant”(2012:64).NeitherSacksnorSchegloff,norGarfinkel

beforethem,hadusefor,orfaithorinterestin,suchlandscapes.

Epistemicsthuswritesinadvanceouranalyticinterestsandourconclusionsaswell.

Astableofconcepts,suchas‘status’,indexing,authorityandaccess,informationandstates,

Page 34: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

34

theirchangesandgradients,andotherformalandextraconversationalthingsarein

principleavailableatanynextglance.Whatwouldthenberequiredtoclaimtheir

evidenceseasilybecomesaweakenedversionof‘evidence’.(SeeDrew’ssimilarconcerns.)

TheEpistemicturnstrikesusasaradicalinnovationindeedonthecorpusworksof

Garfinkel,Sacks,Schegloff,Jefferson,theircolleaguesandstudents,andthusgroundsfora

verycarefulandcautionaryreading.Thequestionswehaveraisedareintendedasuseful

measuresfortakingthemeasureofthecontinuitiesanddeparturesoftheEP’s

extraordinaryanalyticinnovationsfromthecorpusstudiesofnaturallanguageuse

leveragedbyConversationAnalysis.

Notes

1Inthisfashion,Garfinkel(1967:31)pointsto“[n]otamethodofunderstanding,but

immenselyvariousmethodsofunderstanding[as]theprofessionalsociologist'sproperand

hithertounstudiedandcriticalphenomena.”

2“Animatingtranscript”isadelicatephrase;itcaninviteahearingofpuppeteering,or

writingaccountsasonelikes,imposingthem,oralternativelythatourmaterialsarejust

waitingto‘speak’,withourassistance.Itcanthusbeheardasacritique,andthistoocanbe

afairaccountofit.Buttheintendedhearingisthattalk-in-interactionisafontofsocial

actionwhoseanalysisaimstofindanddescribeitsgrammarsofactionandwhatthey

Page 35: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

35

achieveintheirproduceddetail.Inthislight,thephraseispointingtothedisciplinedwork

ofwritingproductionaccountsthatarefaithfultotheoccasion’sevidentdetail,andthus

revealthesociologyofwhatthepartiesaredoingtogether.Howananalyticprogramgoes

aboutdoingthisishowitgoesaboutanimatingtranscript.

3Theinstructivetiebetween“describing”asanordinarysocialpractice,and“description”

asaprofessionaltaskandtopicofmethodforsocialsciencehaslongbeenatopicforEMCA

(cf.,Garfinkel,1967,passim;Sacks,1963,1972;Schegloff,1987,1988b).Sameworldsare

inplay.CitingWeber,Schegloffspeaksof

theindefiniteextendabilityofdescriptionsofsocialobjectsofinquiry…thesetofways

ofdescribinganysetting,anyactor,anyaction,etc.isindefinitelyexpandable.Literalor

exhaustivedescriptionsarenot,then,availablesolutionstotheproblemsofsocial

inquiry(1988b:2)

Thus,whenHeritage(2012c:80)findsCA’s“proofprocedure”limitingbecause“nextturn

willnotalwaysbeasourceofunequivocalvalidation,”anoddexemptionunderwritesthe

dissatisfaction.Notunlessnaturalconversationitselfproceedsby“unequivocalvalidations”

canitsdescriptiveanalysisfindthesame.Onthecentralplaceof“contingency”inthe

organizationandorganizationalachievementsofnaturalconversation,seeSchegloff

(1996).Asheobserves,“[c]ontingency–interactionalcontingency–isnotablemishonthe

smoothsurfaceofdiscourse,oroftalk-in-interactionmoregenerally.Itisendemictoit.Itis

itsglory.Itiswhatallowstalk-in-interactiontheflexibilityandtherobustnesstoserveas

anenablingmechanismfortheinstitutionsofsociallife(1996:22).Thepremiseof

“unequivocalvalidation”thusseemstomis-sighttheorganizationitwouldmeasure.

Page 36: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

36

4ThetranscriptwepresentherereducessomeofthedetailandsymbolsusedbyJefferson

inheroriginaltranscript.Theaudiorecordisacontinuous55-minutephonecallbetween

thetwosisters.

5Thepremiseisfamiliarinlaw,wherethedistinctionbetweendirectobservationand

hearsayisusedforeyewitnesstestimony,butthereisanexemptionforqualifiedexperts

whoareallowedtotestifytowhattheyhavelearnedfromothersinthefield.Whatwehave

learnedfromothersmaybemostofwhatweknow,epistemicallyspeaking.

6Schegloff(2007)discussesapreferencefornoticingsoverannouncementsandremarks

thatnoticingsaretypicallydoneearlyinaninteraction.Ivy’sline4(“Ah(r)youstill’vgot

i:t”)isjustsuchanoticing,producedintheimmediatelocalenvironmentofJan’scoughing,

andherfirstnoticingisregisteredintheveryopeningoftheconversation,“Oh:dea:hme:.”.

TheseobservationssupportanargumentthatIvy’sline4isnotonlyapposite,butexpected.

7Itisnotclearwhether“inapposite”hereisamember’smeasure,i.e.,thattherecipient

(Jan)isactuallymarkingthequestionthatway,orisanoverhearinganalyst’sdisengaged

measure.JanseemspleasedtoreceiveIvy’sattentionandconclusion,andaffirmsit.Ifso,

thenperhaps‘inappositeness’issomethingforthedisengagedanalysttofindandmark.

Whilethiswouldbeanunremarkableprivilegeinotherformsoflanguagestudy,itwould

bearemarkableoneforCA.

Page 37: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

37

8JeanWongreportedthefollowing“fieldnote”inherkitchen,asheradultsonisstanding

attherefrigerator,freezerdooropen,lookinginside:

[JW:FN]

Son: Doyouhaveanyicecubes?

Mom: Idon’tknow,you’relookinginthefreezer.

Wewishtodulynotethecomedyoftheirexchange.Herewehaveaninquiryaboutmatters

directly‘inevidence’totheinquirer,and/but‘owned’bytherecipient.Agreatmanythings

maybemadeofsuchinquiries.Indeed,‘inappositeness’isdemonstrablyinevidenceinthis

one,withoutbenefitof‘Oh’prefacing.Ifso,thenboththecategoriesof‘thingsinevidence’

and‘thingsinapposite’maywellextendbeyondtherangeof‘Oh-prefaced’replies,meaning

thatwehardlyneedtheprefacetorecognizethem.Wecertainlymightfindan‘Ohpreface’

heretoo(‘Oh,Idon’tknow…’),butwewillfinditasafeatureofanoccasionedproduction,

ratherthantheproductofanengine.

9Schegloff(1988:451-453)treatsthissamesequenceundertherubricof“guessingbad

news”.

10Borrowingthephrase“atallpoints”fromSacks(1984),butthenfittingittoaGoffmanian

register,Heritageassertsthatthe“managementofsolidaryfacerelationshipisan

obligationofspeakersjustasitisofrecipients,andatallpointsofinteraction”(2008:312).

Sacks(1984:22),ofcourse,spokeof“orderatallpoints”(andnotethatSchegloff[2005]

hashisownreadingofit).Butthenconsider,byeachassuranceof“order”ontheonehand,

andnow“solidaryfacerelations,”ontheother,whatwoulditmeantofindthese

Page 38: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

38

organizations,at‘allpoints’?Whatwouldcountasafinding?Measuredtotheregular

productionoftherepairspace,forexample,anorderlinesswithoutneedofauthority,

subordination,orinvidiousaccess,whatwouldadescriptionof‘managingfacerelations’

looklike?Or,measuredtothepartiessustainedorientationtotheprojectablecompletion

ofaturnunderway–availablefordescriptioninthemultipleevidencesofoverlaps,

simultaneousstarts,collaborativecompletions,etc.–howdoesoneshow‘managingface

relations’–Goffman’sengine–intemporal-sequentialdetail?ItwasmorethanGoffman

coulddo(seeSchegloff,1988).AndSacksetal.werenotspeakingofdrivers,butof

grammars,the“modelofroutinelyobservable,closelyorderedsocialactivities”(1984:25).

Thedescriptiveregistersareentirelydifferent,andthedifferencemayaccountfortheonly

occasionalandoftenpuzzlinginterestintheconstitutivedetailoftranscriptsshownin

epistemictreatmentsofconversationalorder,aswehaveseeninourexhibits.WhiletheEP

assuresusoftheplayofcausativesat‘allpoints’,whetheras‘facerelations’or‘epistemic

status’,itisanassuranceoperatingpriortoanyactualsequentialproduction.Forthis

reason,perhaps,theproductionsthemselves–thetranscripts–arehardpressedtoshow

them.

11InHeritage(2005)wefindthissamesequencewhereit’ssaid,“Ida’sresponsesimply

confirmswhatJennyreports,yetJennystillacknowledgesthatconfirmationwith‘oh’,

indicatingachangeinherstateofinformation”(2005:192).Onecanwellimaginea‘change

ofstate’givenJen’sstretched“Oh:::”inline4.Buttheremaybemoreatplayherethan

‘simplyconfirming’aninformationalexchange.GivenIda’sturn-initialandturn-closing

“yes”inline3,andthequalification“s’posetobe”inthemiddleofherturn,andthenJen’s

Page 39: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

39

“Oh:::”innextturn,wemayhavethegroundsforanapologyin-the-making.Inthatcase,

Ida’sline3(andher“Yeh”inline5)maynotbesosimple,andinwaysthat‘confirming

information’willnotaccountfor.

12Thesedifficultiesarejoinedtothefirstphraseofthepassage:Ohprefacingisamethodof

indexingindependence,anddoesso“inandofitself”.Butagain,whatwoulditmeanto

showtheworkofanexpression,“inandofitself”?Havewenoneedforrecipients,next

turns,orsequentialenvironments?Theproposalseemstobeleveragedonastriking

departurefromthesystematicsofturn-and-sequenceconstruction,whereinthesenseofan

expressionissequentiallyembedded,tiedtoaproductionhistory,andmadesenseof.‘In

andofitself’wouldseemtorundirectlyagainstthisgrain,andinthesewaysandothers,

theEPseemstoturnawayfromcentralandidentifyingpremisesofsequentialproduction,

contingency,andlocalanalysis.

FundingThisresearchreceivednospecificgrantfromanyfundingagencyinthepublic,commercial

ornot-for-profitsectors.

References

DrewP(2012).Whatdrivessequences?ResearchonLanguageandSocialInteraction45(1):

61-68.GarfinkelH(1967)StudiesinEthnomethodology.EnglewoodCliffs,NJ:PrenticeHall.

Page 40: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

40

GarfinkelH(2002)Ethnomethodology’sProgram:WorkingoutDurkheim’sAphorism.

Lanham,MD:Rowman&Littlefield.HeritageJ(1984)Achange-of-statetokenandaspectsofitssequentialplacement.In:

AtkinsonJMandHeritageJ(eds)StructuresofSocialAction:StudiesinConversationAnalysis.Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress,pp.299–345.

HeritageJ(1998)Oh-prefacedresponsestoinquiry.LanguageinSociety27(3):291-334HeritageJ(2002)Oh-prefacedresponsestoassessments.In:FordC,FoxBandThompsonS

(eds)TheLanguageofTurnandSequence.Oxford:OxfordUniversityPress,pp.196–224.

HeritageJ(2005)Cognitionindiscourse.In:teMolderHandPotterJ(eds)Conversation

andCognition.Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress,pp.184-202.HeritageJ(2008)Conversationanalysisassocialtheory.In:TurnerB(ed)TheNew

BlackwellCompaniontoSocialTheory.Oxford:Wiley-Blackwell,pp.300-320.HeritageJ(2011)Territoriesofknowledge,territoriesofexperience.In:StiversT,Mondada

LandSteensig(eds)TheMoralityofKnowledgeinConversation.Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress,pp.159-183.

HeritageJ(2012a)Epistemicsinaction:Actionformationandterritoriesofknowledge.

ResearchonLanguageandSocialInteraction45(1):1-29.HeritageJ(2012b)Theepistemicengine:Sequenceorganizationandterritoriesof

knowledge.ResearchonLanguageandSocialInteraction45(1):30-52.HeritageJ(2012c)Beyondandbehindthewords:Somereactionstomycommentators.

ResearchonLanguageandSocialInteraction45(1):76-81.HeritageJ(2013a)Actionformationanditsepistemic(andother)backgrounds.Discourse

Studies15(5):551-578.HeritageJ(2013b)Epistemicsinconversation.In:SidnellJandStiversT(eds)Handbookof

ConversationAnalysis.Cambridge:Blackwell,pp.370-394.HeritageJandRaymondG(2005)Thetermsofagreement:Indexingepistemicauthority

andsubordinationinassessmentsequences.SocialPsychologyQuarterly68(1):15–38.

LevinsonS(2013)Actionformationandascription.InSidnellJandStiversT(eds)

HandbookofConversationAnalysis.Oxford:Blackwell,pp.103-130.

Page 41: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

41

LindwallO,LymerGandIvarssonJ(2016)Epistemicstatusandtherecognizabilityof

socialactions.DiscourseStudies(thisissue).LynchMandWongJ(2016)Revertingtoahiddeninteractionalorder:Epistemics,

informationism,andconversationanalysis.DiscourseStudies(thisissue).MacbethM,WongJandLynchM(2016)Thestoryof‘Oh’: Indexingstructure,animating

transcript,Part1.DiscourseStudies(thisissue).MoermanMandSacksH(1988)On"understanding"intheanalysisofnatural

conversation.InMoermanM(ed.)Talkingculture:Ethnographyandconversationanalysis.Philadelphia,PA:UniversityofPennsylvaniaPress,pp.180-186.

PomerantzAM(1984)Agreeinganddisagreeingwithassessments:Somefeaturesof

preferred/dispreferredturnshapes.In:AtkinsonJMandHeritageJ(eds)StructuresofSocialAction.Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress,pp.57–101.

RaymondGandHeritageJ(2006)Theepistemicsofsocialrelations:Owning

grandchildren.LanguageinSociety35:677–705.SacksH(1963)Sociologicaldescription.BerkeleyJournalofSociology8:1-16.SacksH(1972)Aninitialinvestigationoftheusabilityofconversationaldatafordoing

sociology.InSudnowD(ed)StudiesinSocialInteraction.NewYork:FreePress,pp.31-74.

SacksH(1984)Notesonmethodology.In:AtkinsonJMandHeritageJ(eds)Structuresof

SocialAction.Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress,pp.2-27.SacksH(1992)LecturesonConversation,Vol.I&II.Oxford:Blackwell.SacksH,SchegloffEAandJeffersonG(1974)Asimplestsystematicsfortheorganizationof

turn-takingforconversation.Language50:696–735.SchegloffEA(1972)Notesonaconversationalpractice:Formulatingplace.In:SudnowD

(ed)StudiesinSocialInteraction.NewYork:FreePress,pp.75-119.SchegloffEA(1984)Onsomequestionsandambiguitiesinconversation.In:AtkinsonJM

andHeritageJ(eds)StructuresofSocialAction.Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress,pp.28–52.

SchegloffEA(1987)Betweenmicroandmacro:Contextsandotherconnections.In

AlexanderJ,GiesenB,MünchRandSmelserN(eds)TheMicro-MacroLink.Berkeley:UniversityofCaliforniaPress,pp.207-234.

Page 42: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

42

SchegloffEA(1988a)Goffmanandtheanalysisofconversation.In:DrewPandWoottonA

(eds)ErvingGoffman:ExploringtheInteractionOrder.Oxford:Polity,pp.89-135.SchegloffEA(1988b)DescriptionsinthesocialsciencesI:Talk–in–interaction.IPrA

PapersinPragmatics2(1-2):1–24.SchegloffEA(1991)Conversationanalysisandsociallysharedcognition.InResnickLB,

LevineJMandTeasleySD(eds)PerspectivesonSociallySharedCognition.Washington,D.C.:AmericanPsychologicalAssociation,pp.150-171.

SchegloffEA(1995)DiscourseasaninteractionalachievementIII:Theomnirelevanceof

action.ResearchonLanguageandSocialInteraction28(3):185-211.SchegloffEA(1996)ConfirmingAllusions:TowardanEmpiricalAccountofAction

Author(s):AmericanJournalofSociology102(1):pp.161-216SchegloffEA(2005)Whistlinginthedark:Notesfromtheothersideofliminality.Texas LinguisticForum48:17-30.SchegloffEA(2006)Onpossibles.DiscourseStudies8(1):141-157.SchegloffEA(2007)SequenceOrganizationinInteraction:APrimerinConversationAnalysis,

Vol.1.Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress.SchegloffEA(2009)OneperspectiveonConversationAnalysis:ComparativePerspectives.

In:SidnellJ(ed),ConversationAnalysis:ComparativePerspectives.Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress,pp.357-406.

SchegloffEA(2010)CommentaryonStiversandRossano:“Mobilizingresponse”.Research

onLanguageandSocialInteraction43(1):38-48.ShannonCandWeaverW(1949)Themathematicaltheoryofcommunication.Urbana,IL:

UniversityofIllinoisPress.

StiversTandRossanoF(2010)Mobilizingresponse.ResearchonLanguageandSocial

Interaction43(1):3-31.

TerasakiAK(2004[1976])Pre-announcementsequencesinconversation.In:LernerG

(ed),ConversationAnalysis:StudiesfromtheFirstGeneration.Amsterdam:John

Page 43: DISCOURSE STUDIES The story of ‘Oh’: Part 2radicalethno.org/documents/macbethandwong.pdf · In Conversation Analysis through Sacks, Schegloff, Jefferson and others, the conceptual

43

Benjamins,pp.171–223[SocialScienceWorkingPapers,No.99,UniversityofCaliforniaIrvine,1976].

WinchP(1990)TheIdeaofaSocialScienceanditsRelationtoPhilosophy,2ndedition.

London:Routledge&KeganPaul.WongJandOlsherD(2000)Reflectionsonconversationanalysisandnonnativespeaker

talk:AninterviewwithEmanuelASchegloff.IssuesinAppliedLinguistics11(1):110–128.

Authors’Biographies

DouglasMacbethisAssociateProfessorintheDepartmentofEducationalStudiesatOhio

StateUniversity.HisresearchpursuesEMCAstudiesofclassroomorderandinstructionas

grammarsofaction.Theaimistowriteanalternatepraxeologyofinstructionasitis

playedoutinfinedurationsofmaterialdetail,andtoaddresstheconceptualconfusions

thatcontinuetohauntdiscussionsof‘teachingandlearning’.

JeanWongisAssociateProfessorintheDepartmentofSpecialEducation,Languageand

LiteracyatTheCollegeofNewJersey(USA).Sheusesconversationanalysisforexamining

interactionalcompetence,particularlyinmultilingualsettings.Herworkappearsinedited

volumesandinjournals,includingAppliedLinguistics,ELTJournal,InternationalReviewof

AppliedLinguistics,IssuesinAppliedLinguistics,PragmaticsandLanguageLearningand

ResearchonLanguageandSocialInteraction.SheistheauthorofConversationAnalysisand

SecondLanguagePedagogy(2010,withHansunWaring),whichbridgesconnections

betweenCAandconcernsinsecond/foreignlanguageeducation.