crosscylinder accuracy

Upload: riezky-febriyanti

Post on 24-Feb-2018

222 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 7/25/2019 Crosscylinder Accuracy

    1/5

    Does the dynamic cross cylinder test measure

    the accommodative response accurately?

    Jaclyn A. Benzoni, O.D., M.S., Juanita D. Collier, O.D., M.S., Kimberley McHugh, O.D.,Mark Rosenfield, M.C.Optom., Ph.D., and Joan K. Portello, O.D., M.P.H., M.S.

    State University of New York State College of Optometry, New York, New York.

    KEYWORDSAccommodation;

    Near testing;

    Optometer;

    Refraction

    Abstract

    BACKGROUND: The dynamic cross cylinder (DCC) test is a standard clinical procedure used to assessthe

    accommodative response (AR) subjectively. However, because of potential problems arising from the

    ambiguous stimulus conditions, it is unclear whether this test provides an accurate measure of the AR.

    Theaim of this study was to compare clinical subjective findings with objective measurements of the AR.

    METHODS: Subjective findings to a 2.50-diopter (D) accommodative stimulus obtained with the DCC

    test (without fogging lenses) were compared with objective measurements of the AR obtained with a

    Grand Seiko WAM 5500 optometer (RyuSyo Industrial Co. Ltd., Kagawa, Japan) in 25 young subjects.

    As spherical lenseswere introduced to quantify thesubjective finding, objective measures of the AR were

    also recorded through these lenses.

    RESULTS: The mean AR recorded subjectively and objectively was 2.35 and 1.68 D, respectively

    (P , 0.0001). Of the 10 subjects who demonstrated a lead of accommodation subjectively, only 1 had

    a lead objectively. For the 8 subjects who showed a lag of accommodation subjectively, all had a lag ob-jectively. Introducing lenses produced a significant change in the mean AR.

    CONCLUSION: The subjective DCC test as performed here does not provide an accurate measurement

    of the AR to a near target in a young population. We recommend that alternative techniques such as

    using an objective, open-field optometer or Cross-Nott retinoscopy be adopted for determining the

    within-task AR.

    Optometry 2009;80:630-634

    The assessment of the accommodative response (AR) to

    a range of stimuli is an important part of the clinical

    optometric examination.

    1

    Patients symptoms frequently re-late to near vision activities, and inappropriate responses,

    whether under or over-accommodation relative to the object

    of regard are a frequent cause of asthenopia.2 Accordingly,

    it is essential for the clinician to determine the actual AR to

    the stimulus condition for which the patient is reporting

    difficulty. In some cases, a more complete examination of

    a range of responses may be appropriate, which can be

    achievedby plotting an accommodative stimulus-responsecurve.3,4 An example of such a plot is illustrated inFigure 1.

    Only for a single stimulus level (the so-called crossover

    point) are the accommodative stimulus and response equal.

    Whereas the plot illustrated in Figure 1 represents an

    average finding, patients presenting with symptoms relating

    to near vision activities may exhibit different results. For ex-

    ample, some patients show a tendency to overaccommodate

    for near targets2 rather than exhibiting the more typical lag

    of accommodation.3-6 A lead of accommodation can be a

    primary problem resulting from trauma, systemic pathology,

    Corresponding author: Mark Rosenfield, M.C.Optom., Ph.D, State

    University of New York State College of Optometry, 33 West 42nd Street,

    New York, New York 10036.

    E-mail:[email protected]

    1529-1839/09/$ -see front matter 2009 American Optometric Association. All rights reserved.doi:10.1016/j.optm.2009.07.012

    Optometry (2009) 80, 630-634

    mailto:[email protected]:[email protected]
  • 7/25/2019 Crosscylinder Accuracy

    2/5

    or the administration of either ocular or systemic pharmaco-

    logic agents, or secondarytoa vergence abnormality such as

    convergence insufficiency.1,7 Although the object of regard

    will remain clear as long as the accommodative error does

    not exceed the depth-of-focus of the eye, such a patient

    may still experience symptoms, possibly resulting from the

    effect of excessive accommodative convergence. An exces-

    sive lag or underaccommodation relative to the accommoda-tive stimulus may also produce asthenopia. This symptom is

    most typically found in early presbyopia, but may also be as-

    sociated with both systemic and ocular pathological condi-

    tions, pharmacologic therapy, neurologic abnormalities,

    and functional disorders such as basicexophoria, divergence

    excess, and accommodative infacility.7-9

    One of the most commonly used clinical procedures to

    assess the AR is the dynamic cross-cylinder (DCC) test.1

    Here, subjects view a pattern of intersecting horizontal

    and vertical lines through a cross cylinder (typically

    60.50 diopters [D]) to create mixed astigmatism, with

    the horizontal and vertical lines theoretically equidistantin front of and behind the retina. If the patient is accommo-

    dating exactly in the plane of the target, then after introduc-

    tion of the cross cylinder, the circle of least confusion

    (COLC) will lie on the retina, and the patient will report

    that both sets of lines (horizontal and vertical) appear

    equally clear (or more accurately, equally blurred). How-

    ever, if the patient has either a lag or lead of accommoda-

    tion, then the horizontal or vertical lines, respectively, will

    appear clearer. Spherical lenses can subsequently be intro-

    duced to move the COLC onto the retina, and the lens

    power required to make the 2 sets of lines appear equally

    clear provides a measure of the accommodative error.A number of different procedures have been advocated

    for performing the DCC test. In principle, the test may be

    carried out under monocular, binocular fused, or binocular

    unfused conditions.1 Although monocular testing should

    assess the blur-driven and proximally induced AR only, un-

    der binocular fused conditions, convergent accommodation

    will be added to the response. Binocular unfused testing

    uses vertical prisms to dissociate the patient, thereby elim-

    inating disparity-vergence. As might be expected, this

    results in a response very similar to the monocular condi-

    tion.10 Additionally, some techniques (e.g., the 14A and

    14B procedures advocated by the Optometric ExtensionProgram2,11) recommend that additional plus lens power

    should be added to the refractive correction so that the ver-

    tical lines appear clearer and then the plus power reduced

    until the horizontal and vertical lines are equally clear

    and dark. This is discussed later in this report.

    A number of significant problems with the DCC test

    make it of questionable value in prepresbyopic patients

    with active accommodation, irrespective of the specific

    technique being used. For example, it is assumed that a

    patient who has minimal accommodative error under

    naturalistic conditions will, during testing, produce an AR

    to the dioptrically conflicting, rectilinear target that liesexactly midway between the 2 foci (i.e., places the COLC

    on the retina). Little evidence supports this proposal. For

    example, Portello et al.12 used an infrared optometer to

    measure the AR in subjects with uncorrected astigmatism.

    They found that subjects generally exerted the minimum

    accommodation necessary to place the anterior focal line

    within the depth-of-focus of the eye. However, under

    none of the conditions tested was the COLC positioned

    on or close to the retina.

    The observation that the AR changes after the introduc-tion of lenses provides an additional difficulty with this test.

    If a lag of accommodation is observed, plus lenses are

    introduced to obtain the required endpoint. However, in a

    young patient with active accommodation, the introduction

    of additional plus power is likely to stimulate a reduction in

    the blur-driven AR, provided the subject is able to detect

    the change in accommodative stimulus in the presence of

    the uncorrected astigmatism. If the reduction in accommo-

    dation is equal to the magnitude of the plus lens, then the

    subjective response to the test will remain unchanged.13-16

    Accordingly, the current study compared subjective

    measurements of the AR obtained with the DCC procedurewith objective measurements taken using an open-field,

    infrared optometer to assess the validity of this clinical

    procedure.

    Methods

    The study was performed on 25 visually normal subjects

    having a mean age of 23.4 years (range, 20 to 30 years). All

    subjects were optometry students at the State University

    of New York State College of Optometry, and had best-

    corrected visual acuity of at least 6/6 (20/20) in each eye.None had any manifest ocular disease or strabismus. The

    Figure 1 Static accommodative stimulus-response curve for a normalsubject. 1 5 initial nonlinear region, 2 5 linear region, 3 5 transitional

    soft saturation region, 4 5 hard saturation presbyopic region. The diago-

    nal linerepresents the unit ratio (or 1:1) line. Figure redrawn with permis-sion from Ciuffreda and Kenyon4 (1983).

    Benzoni et al Clinical Research 631

  • 7/25/2019 Crosscylinder Accuracy

    3/5

    study followed the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki,

    and informed consent was obtained from all subjects after

    an explanation of the nature and possible consequences of

    the study. The protocol was approved by the Institutional

    Review Board at the SUNY State College of Optometry.

    All of the testing was completed in a single session.

    The DCC test was performed without the use of additional

    fogging lenses.1,17

    Cross-cylinders of60.50 D (minus axesvertical) were introduced before each eye. Subjects were re-

    quired to view a high-contrast, black on white, cross cylinder

    target, mounted on a near point rod at a viewing distance of

    40 cm. The target was viewed through the habitual refractive

    correction, which was mounted in a trial frame. Subjects

    were asked to indicate whether the horizontal or vertical lines

    appeared either clearer or darker. If a preference was indi-

    cated, then spherical lenses were introduced before both

    eyes until the 2 sets of rectilinear lines appeared equally

    clear. This lens value was taken as the accommodative error,

    i.e., the difference between the accommodative stimulus and

    response. If no equal response was found, then the endpointwas taken as the midpoint between the lenses that produced a

    change in subjective response. For example, if the subject re-

    ported that the horizontal lines were clearer initially, but the

    vertical lines appeared clearer through an additional10.25 D

    lens, this was recorded as a 0.12 D lag of accommodation. In

    accordance with the conventional clinical procedure, the lu-

    minance of the target was maintained at a low level (approx-

    imately 10 cd/m2). Additionally, the AR to the DCC target

    was measured objectively using a Grand Seiko WAM 5500

    infra-red optometer (RyuSyo Industrial Co. Ltd., Kagawa,

    Japan). This instrument has been described and evaluated

    previously.18-20

    Immediately after the subjective testing, ob-jective measurement of the AR was performed with the

    habitual refractive correction in place. If the subject indi-

    cated subsequently that one set of lines on the DCC target ap-

    peared clearer, then spherical lenses were introduced to

    achieve the position of subjective equality, and the AR was

    measured through each of these supplementary lenses usingthe infrared optometer. All objective data were recorded

    from the right eye only, and for each condition at least 10

    readings of the refractive state of the eye were taken, con-

    verted into spherical equivalents, and averaged. Because

    the instrument takes measurements at approximately 1- to

    2-second intervals, the time to assess the AR for each stimu-

    lus level was approximately 15 seconds. The objective mea-

    surements typically took 1 to 2 minutes to complete.

    However, subjects received a short break between measure-

    ments while the lenses were being changed.

    The same experimental set up was used for both the

    objective and subjective measurements. Subjects wore atrial frame and lenses with their distance refractive correc-

    tion, and additional lenses, when indicated by the subjec-

    tive responses, were added to the trial frame as necessary.

    For all testing, subjects viewed the nearpoint card mounted

    on the infrared optometer, with their head against the

    forehead rest and chin placed on the chinrest. The exper-

    imental setup is shown in Figure 2.

    Results

    The mean AR recorded subjectively and objectively was2.35 D (SD 5 0.60) and 1.68 D (SD 5 0.49), respectively.

    A paired t test indicated that this difference was significant

    (t 5 7.13; df5 24; P , 0.0001). Additionally, the differ-

    ence between the subjective and objective findings was cal-

    culated for each individual, and the 95% limits of

    agreement, calculated as 1.96 multiplied by the standard

    deviation of the differences21 was 60.95 D. These differ-

    ences are illustrated inFigure 3.

    ARs for each individual are shown inTable 1. Linear re-

    gression analysis indicated a significant correlation between

    the subjective and objective findings (r25 0.39;P 5 0.001),

    with a regression line described by the equation: objectiveAR5 (0.51x subjective AR)1 0.47. Ten subjects exhibited

    Figure 2 Photograph of the experimental setup. Subjects wore boththeir habitual refractive correction and the 60.50 D cross cylinders in a

    trial frame, and viewed a rectilinear target (not visible here as it was on

    the side of the nearpoint card nearest the subject) at a viewing distance

    of 40 cm. The subject was positioned at the autorefractor throughout,

    which allowed objective measurements to be recorded in addition to the

    subjective responses.

    -1

    -0.5

    0

    0.5

    1

    1.5

    2

    0 2 4Objective AR (D)

    SubjAR

    ObjAR

    (D)

    1 3

    Figure 3 Difference between the subjective AR determined using theDCC procedure and the objective AR measured with an infrared optometer

    as a function of the objective response. The horizontal dashed line indi-

    cates the mean difference, and the area between the 2 solid horizontal

    lines represents the 95% limits of agreement between the 2 values.

    632 Optometry, Vol 80, No 11, November 2009

  • 7/25/2019 Crosscylinder Accuracy

    4/5

    a lead of accommodation (AR . 2.50 D) based on the sub-

    jective findings. However, only one of these subjects demon-

    strated a lead of accommodation objectively. Of the 8

    subjects who showed a lag of accommodation (AR , 2.50

    D) subjectively, all also demonstrated a lag objectively.

    For the 8 subjects with a lag of accommodation based on

    the subjective response, plus lenses were introduced to

    achieve subjective equality, i.e., so that the 2 sets of lines

    appeared equally clear. Objective measurements using the

    infrared optometer verified that the lenses produced a

    decline in the AR, as shown in Figure 4. Friedmans non-

    parametric test indicated that the decline in AR after the in-troduction of the plus lenses was significant (c2 5 16.05;

    P 5 0.000).

    Discussion

    The findings of the current study clearly indicate that the

    subjective DCC test using the methodology adopted here

    does not provide veridical measurements of the AR in young

    subjects with active accommodation. The mean results

    obtained with this test are significantly higher than those

    obtained with an objective infrared optometer. In addition,the range of differences between the 2 measurements of AR

    exceeded 2 D, while the DCC test overestimated the number

    of subjects having a lead of accommodation.

    In comparing the findings of the subjective and objectivetechniques for assessing the accommodative response, dif-

    ferences between the procedures should be noted. For

    example, with the DCC technique adopted here, a single

    measurement was taken. Had either a bracketing technique

    been used, i.e., going beyond the first neutral response and

    then changing lenses in the opposite direction, or a high

    neutral procedure, whereby additional plus power is added

    initially, and then reduced until an equal response is

    obtained, then different (and possibly superior) findings

    might have been found. In addition, 10 readings of the

    accommodative state were obtained with the infrared op-

    tometer over approximately 15 seconds and subsequentlyaveraged. Accordingly, depending on the number of lens

    changes required, the objective determination typically took

    1 to 2 minutes to complete. In contrast, the single subjective

    assessment generally took only 20 to 30 seconds. Further, it

    is unclear whether subjects can be trained to respond better

    with the DCC, i.e., to become more sensitive to the

    differences in clarity of the rectilinear lines. However, our

    personal experience when teaching this procedure to op-

    tometry students is that they quickly become capable of

    achieving any response, i.e., altering their AR so that either

    the horizontal lines appear clearer, the vertical lines appear

    clearer, or both sets of lines appear equal. Finally, to ourknowledge, no studies have been performed to determine the

    sensitivity of the DCC, i.e., its ability to identify accurately

    those individuals with accommodative anomalies.

    It is clear that the subjective procedure frequently

    overestimates the AR in young subjects. One possible

    explanation for this difference is that subjects are volun-

    tarily changing their accommodation to the target. It is

    presumed at the outset that a subject who is accommodating

    accurately on the target (AR 5 2.50 D) will maintain the

    COLC on the retina, with the horizontal and vertical lines

    remaining equally blurred. However, subjects could in-

    crease their AR, thereby making the vertical lines appearclearer, or alternatively reduce the AR to make the

    0

    0.5

    1

    1.5

    2

    0 0.25 0.5 0.75

    Lens power (D)

    ObjectiveAR

    (D)

    Figure 4 Mean AR measured objectively through plus lenses using theinfrared optometer in 8 subjects. These subjects all exhibited a lag of ac-

    commodation when tested subjectively with the DCC test (i.e., horizontal

    lines appeared clearer) through their habitual refractive correction (0 D).

    Accordingly, supplementary plus lenses were introduced to produce an

    equal subjective response and the AR measured objectively though these

    lenses. It is apparent that the AR changed significantly when the lens

    power was modified. Error bars indicate 1 SEM.

    Table 1 Subjective and objective measurements of the AR

    for each of the 25 subjects tested

    Subjective AR,

    [D]

    Objective AR,

    [D] (SD)

    Difference

    (subjective-objective)

    2.75 2.20 (0.87) 0.55

    3.00 1.74 (0.13) 1.26

    3.25 2.06 (0.33) 1.192.62 2.33 (0.12) 0.29

    3.00 1.82 (0.35) 1.18

    3.25 2.89 (0.20) 0.36

    2.50 1.90 (0.19) 0.60

    2.50 1.71 (0.24) 0.79

    1.75 1.11 (0.27) 0.64

    2.25 1.98 (0.30) 0.27

    2.75 1.23 (0.28) 1.52

    2.62 1.62 (0.73) 1.00

    2.62 1.33 (0.24) 1.29

    1.75 1.13 (0.16) 0.62

    2.50 1.67 (0.18) 0.83

    1.75 0.43 (0.57) 1.321.25 1.09 (0.17) 0.16

    1.75 1.93 (0.25) 20.18

    1.75 1.36 (0.15) 0.39

    2.50 1.88 (0.17) 0.62

    2.50 1.72 (0.47) 0.78

    2.50 1.56 (0.12) 0.94

    2.50 1.78 (0.14) 0.72

    2.50 1.85 (0.24) 0.65

    0.75 1.30 (0.19) 20.55

    Mean 2.35 1.66 0.69

    SD 0.60 0.49 0.48

    Benzoni et al Clinical Research 633

  • 7/25/2019 Crosscylinder Accuracy

    5/5

    horizontal lines clear. Interestingly, Bannon and Walsh22

    stated that for the recognition of letters, the vertical strokes

    are most important, so that a patient may prefer to keep the

    vertical strokes clear. If a patient habitually accommodates

    to make the vertical portion of the target clear, then this

    would explain the overaccommodation seen with the sub-

    jective test. Certainly, use of a rectilinear target is not con-

    ducive to maintaining the COLC on the retina. It has alsobeen proposed that fogging lenses (e.g., an additional

    11.00 sphere) be introduced over the distance refractive

    correction so that both the horizontal and vertical focal

    lines lie in front of the retina.17 This is also the high neu-

    tral procedure advocated by the Optometric Extension

    Program and others.2,11,17 The termfoggingis inappropriate

    here, because it refers to making a patient myopic when de-

    termining the distance refractive error so as to minimize the

    AR. During the DCC test, introduction of an additional

    11.00 sphere will reduce the accommodative stimulus

    from 2.50 D to 1.50 D but not make the patient myopic

    or blur their visual acuity.The introduction of any lens during the test procedure

    will modify the accommodative stimulus and therefore alter

    the resulting AR. If the goal is to determine the response for

    a particular stimulus level (e.g., 2.50 D), then that stimulus

    must be maintained throughout the test. Once plus lenses

    are introduced, if the patient reduces the AR by an amount

    equal to the magnitude of the lens power, then the

    subjective response will remain unchanged. Under binoc-

    ular test conditions, a change in subjective response may be

    obtained only when the patient is no longer able to reduce

    accommodation while maintaining accurate vergence on

    the target, i.e., exert negative relative accommodation.1

    Therefore, the DCC procedure is not a direct quantification

    of the AR for a 2.50 D stimulus, but rather uses subjective

    responses to determine the lens that yields zero accommo-

    dative error.

    Accordingly, the DCC test as performed here does not

    provide an accurate measure of the AR under normal

    viewing conditions. When assessing the AR on a young

    patient, an ideal test should use a naturalistic stimulus

    placed at the patients habitual working distance to simulate

    normal near-work conditions. Additionally, it should avoid

    the use of supplementary lenses over the refractive correc-

    tion because they will alter the accommodative stimulus(and resulting AR) from the habitual state. The use of either

    an objective, open-fieldoptometer (if available) or Cross-

    Nott retinoscopy,1,23-25 whereby the point conjugate with

    the retina during active accommodation is determined by

    altering the retinoscopy working distance seem to be the

    optimal procedures to determine the within-task AR.

    References

    1. Rosenfield M. Accommodation. In: Zadnik K, ed. The ocular examina-

    tion. Measurement and findings. Philadelphia: Saunders; 1997:87-121.

    2. Birnbaum MH.Optometric management of nearpoint vision disorders.

    Boston: Butterworth-Heinemann; 1993:53-71.

    3. Morgan MW. Accommodation and its relationship to convergence.Am

    J Optom Arch Am Acad Optom 1944;21:183-95.

    4. Ciuffreda KJ, Kenyon RV. Accommodative vergence and accommoda-tion in normals, amblyopes and strabismics. In: Schor CM,

    Ciuffreda KJ, eds. Vergence eye movements: basic and clinical as-

    pects. Boston: Butterworths; 1983:101-73.

    5. Heath GG. Components of accommodation. Am J Optom Arch Am

    Acad Optom 1956;33:569-79.

    6. Charman WN. The accommodative resting point and refractive error.

    Ophthalmic Optician 1982;21:469-73.

    7. Scheiman M, Wick B. Clinical management of binocular vision. Phil-

    adelphia, Lippincott; 1994.

    8. Hofstetter HW. Factors involved in low amplitude cases.Am J Optom

    Arch Am Acad Optom 1942;19:279-89.

    9. Ciuffreda KJ. Accommodation and its anomalies. In: Charman WN

    ed.Vision and visual dysfunction. Vol 1. Visual optics and instrumen-

    tation. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press, 1991:231-79.10. Ong J, Schuchert J. Dissociated versus monocular cross-cylinder

    method. Am J Optom Arch Am Acad Optom 1972;49:762-4.

    11. Manas L. Visual analysis, 3rd ed. Chicago: Professional Press; 1965:

    155-6.

    12. Portello JK, Hong SE, Rosenfield M. Accommodation to astigmatic

    stimuli.Optom Vis Sci (suppl) 2001;78:91.

    13. Fry GA. Significance of fused cross cylinder test. Optometric Weekly

    1940;31:16-9.

    14. Goodson RA, Afanador AJ. The accommodative response to the near

    point crossed cylinder test. Optometric Weekly 1974;65:1138-40.

    15. Rosenfield M, Carrel M. Effect of near-vision addition lenses on the

    accuracy of the accommodative response. Optometry 2001;72:19-24.

    16. Haynes HM. Clinical observations with dynamic retinoscopy. Opto-

    metric Weekly 1960;51:2243-6, 2306-9.

    17. Grosvenor T. Primary care optometry, 4th ed. Boston: Butterworth-Heinemann, 2002:284-5

    18. Chat SWS, Edwards MH. Clinical evaluation of the Shin-Nippon

    SRW-5000 autorefractor in children. Ophthal Physiol Opt 2001;21:

    87-100.

    19. Mallen EAH, Wolffsohn JS, Gilmartin B, et al. Clinical evaluation of

    the Shin-Nippon SRW-5000 autorefractor in adults. Ophthal Physiol

    Opt2001;21:101-7.

    20. Wolffsohn JS, Gilmartin B, Mallen EAH, et al. Continuous recording

    of accommodation and pupil size using the Shin-Nippon SRW-5000

    autorefractor.Ophthal Physiol Opt2001;21:108-13.

    21. Bland JM, Altman DG. Statistical methods for assessing agreement

    between two methods of clinical assessment. Lancet 1986;1-8476:

    307-10.

    22. Bannon RE, Walsh R. On astigmatism. Part IIId

    Subjective tests.Am JOptom Arch Am Acad Optom 1945;22:210-8.

    23. Cross AJ. Dynamic skiametry in theory and practice. New York: AJ

    Cross Optical Co, 1911.

    24. Nott IS. Dynamic skiametry, accommodation and convergence.Am J

    Phys Opt1925;6:490-503.

    25. Nott IS. Dynamic skiametry. Accommodative convergence and fusion

    convergence.Am J Phys Opt1926;7:366-74.

    634 Optometry, Vol 80, No 11, November 2009