copyright by soo-hye han 2008

192
Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

Upload: others

Post on 01-Oct-2021

5 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

Copyright

by

Soo-Hye Han

2008

Page 2: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

The Dissertation Committee for Soo-Hye Han Certifies that this is the approved

version of the following dissertation:

The Untold Story:

Portrayals of Electoral Participation in Print News Coverage of

American Presidential Campaigns, 1948-2004

Committee:

Sharon E. Jarvis, Supervisor

Roderick P. Hart

Barry Brummett

Natalie (Talia) Jomini Stroud

Maxwell McCombs

Page 3: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

The Untold Story:

Portrayals of Electoral Participation in Print News Coverage of

American Presidential Campaigns, 1948-2004

by

Soo-Hye Han, B.A.; M.P.I.A.

Dissertation

Presented to the Faculty of the Graduate School of

The University of Texas at Austin

in Partial Fulfillment

of the Requirements

for the Degree of

Doctor of Philosophy

The University of Texas at Austin

August, 2008

Page 4: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

Dedication

To my parents, with love and appreciation

Page 5: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

v

Acknowledgements

This dissertation would not have been possible without the help and support of so

many individuals. First and foremost, I would like to thank the members of my

dissertation committee. I am very grateful to Dr. Maxwell McCombs for his wisdom and

encouragement over the years. Without his pioneering research three decades ago, this

dissertation could not have been conceived. Dr. Roderick P. Hart has been instrumental to

my scholarly development from the day I stepped foot on UT campus. He showed me

what it takes to be a good scholar through his impeccable example. I am indebted to Dr.

Talia Stroud for her invaluable suggestions and insights throughout this project. She

taught me how to be bold yet meticulous in my research. I appreciate Dr. Barry

Brummett for broadening my horizons both academically and in life. He showed me the

joy in exploring the world of the unfamiliar.

There are not enough words and space to express my appreciation and admiration

for my advisor, Dr. Sharon Jarvis. She has been my biggest supporter throughout my

graduate career and has always believed in me even when I couldn’t. Without her

constant support and guidance, I would not have been where I am today. Dr. Jarvis is my

Page 6: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

vi

hero, and I am eternally indebted to her confidence, optimism, generosity, and

mentorship.

In addition to my committee, I would like to thank several individuals who were

vital to the completion of this dissertation. I am grateful to Dr. Nicholas Valentino for his

valuable advice on experimental design, to Emo Rosas, Carla Wright, and Nicole Laster

for coding newspaper articles with me, to Becky LaVally for her keen journalistic advice,

and to Mary Dixson and my colleagues at the Strauss Institute for providing a warm,

friendly work environment.

My dear friends and dissertation buddies, Amanda Davis, Lisa Perks, and Jennifer

Asenas, deserve special thanks for reading my chapters, for listening to my hopes and

fears, and for cheering me up with inspirational mixed CDs, cupcakes and tea. Because of

these smart, fearless, truly compassionate women, I survived the toughest of my

dissertation days.

There is another set of extraordinary people who deserve special appreciation for

nurturing me and providing me a home away home during my many years at UT. I am

truly grateful to Margaret Surratt for her grounded wisdom, Jennifer Betancourt for her

contagious smile, Deanna Matthews for her boundless magic, and Susan Corbin for her

continuous counsel. Their warmth and kindness kept me physically, mentally and

emotionally healthy over the years.

I am also thankful for the support and friendship of John & Hilary Lithgow, Gabe

& Pam Levine, Tomoko Ikeda, Sae Oshima, and Stephanie Brown. These individuals

Page 7: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

vii

touched my life and made my life in Austin rich and nothing but a blast. I cherish their

friendship and will always remember the good times we spent together.

It simply would not have been possible for me to come this far without the love

and support of my family. My parents have been the best role models that I could ever

hope for and a source of my strength, courage, and pride. Aboji, Omoni, thank you for

providing me with endless love and possibilities. I am who I am today because of you. I

am also thankful to my two brothers for growing up fast so that I didn’t have to and to my

sister-in-law, niece and nephew for cheering me up with much-needed comic relief from

across the ocean.

Finally, I would like to express my sincere appreciation to Matthew Blomberg for

his support, patience, and unconditional love throughout this process. He found the best

in me even when things were difficult and gave me peace of mind when I needed it the

most. The size of his heart is bigger than Texas, and I feel immensely blessed to have him

beside me always and forever.

Page 8: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

viii

The Untold Story:

Portrayals of Electoral Participation in Print News Coverage of

American Presidential Campaigns, 1948-2004

Publication No._____________

Soo-Hye Han, Ph.D.

The University of Texas at Austin, 2008

Supervisor: Sharon E. Jarvis

This dissertation was inspired by two conflicting patterns: the United States is

very proud of its democratic tradition, yet has the second lowest voter turnout rate in the

world. In order to better understand America’s electoral hypocrisy and the decline in

voter turnout, this dissertation examined how Americans have been encouraged to think

about the vote, the voting process and their roles as voters through news media.

Specifically, this dissertation asked: (1) How have voters and voting been portrayed in

American newspapers from 1948-2004? (2) Have these portrayals changed over time?

And (3) what are some potential implications of these patterns for the electorate? To

answer these questions, an extensive content analysis and a set of experiments were

conducted.

In the content analysis, several electoral key terms pertaining to the electoral

participation (Vote, Voter, Voting, Election and Electorate—and their derivatives) were

Page 9: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

ix

located in the Campaign Mapping Project text-base and were subjected to quantitative

and qualitative coding techniques. Findings indicate that while (1) voters were

increasingly mentioned in print newspapers over time, (2) they were not featured

positively, (3) were cast in the shadow of elites, (4) were rarely reminded of democratic

responsibilities, and (5) were not connected to the past or each other in election print

news coverage. In addition to these dominant patterns, the data also revealed that voters

were more likely to be featured as (1) empowered agents in the democratic system

between 1948-1968, (2) subsumed under opinion polls and as pawns of elites between

1972-2000, and (3) faced with challenges in the electoral process in 2004.

Two on-line experiments (one with the general population and another with

college students) were conducted to test the effects of the empowered portrayal of voters

found in1948-1968. Results indicated that the empowered portrayal of voters increased

citizens’ participatory intentions and trust in news media (college students reported these

positive outcomes and more). These findings suggest that the way print news media cover

voters and electoral participation may have important socialization effects on citizens’

political attitudes as well as some important practical implications for the press and

journalists.

Page 10: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

x

Table of Contents

List of Tables .................................................................................................... xii

List of Figures .................................................................................................. xiii

Chapter 1. On the Study of American Electoral Participation............................... 1

Why Study Voting? .................................................................................... 3 Why Study Voting Today? ......................................................................... 7

Why Study Voting in the News?............................................................... 12

The Theory of Attribute Agenda-Setting................................................... 15

Overview of the Dissertation .................................................................... 17

Chapter 2. Methods ........................................................................................... 19

The Campaign Mapping Project................................................................ 20

Methodology ............................................................................................ 21 Conclusion ............................................................................................... 34

Chapter 3. Portrayals of American Electoral Participation (1948-2004) ............. 36

Voters Are Increasingly Mentioned in News Coverage ............................. 37

Voters Are Not Featured Positively........................................................... 42 Voters Are Cast in the Shadow of Elites ................................................... 46

Voters Are Rarely Reminded of Democratic Responsibilities ................... 50

Voters Are Not Connected to the Past or Each Other ................................ 54

Conclusion ............................................................................................... 61

Chapter 4. Portrayals of American Electoral Participation (1948-1968, 1972-2000

and 2004).................................................................................................. 63

News Coverage of Electoral Participation: 1948–1968.............................. 64

News Coverage of Electoral Participation: 1972–2000.............................. 74 News Coverage of Electoral Participation: 2004 ....................................... 80

Conclusion ............................................................................................... 88

Page 11: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

xi

Chapter 5. Effects of Portrayals in the News: General Population...................... 90

Hypotheses ............................................................................................... 92 Methodology ............................................................................................ 96

Results.....................................................................................................104

Discussion ...............................................................................................114

Chapter 6. Effects of Portrayals in the News: College Students.........................118 Political Disengagement of Youth............................................................119

Hypotheses ..............................................................................................124

Methodology ...........................................................................................125

Results.....................................................................................................129 Discussion ...............................................................................................140

Chapter 7. Conclusion ......................................................................................144

Contributions...........................................................................................146

Limitations ..............................................................................................153 Questions for Future Research .................................................................156

Conclusion ..............................................................................................158

References........................................................................................................160

Vita ................................................................................................................179

Page 12: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

xii

List of Tables

Table 2.1. Electoral Key Terms in Study .......................................................................24 Table 3.1. Electoral Key Terms by Context Variable .....................................................43 Table 3.2. Electoral Key Terms by Frame Variable .......................................................51 Table 3.3. Electoral Key Terms by Time Variable .........................................................55 Table 5.1. Effects of Empowered Voter Portrayal........................................................105 Table 5.2. Moderating Effects of Partisanship..............................................................110 Table 5.3. Moderating Effects of Political Knowledge.................................................112 Table 6.1. Effects of Empowered Voter Portrayal on Young Voters ............................130 Table 6.2. Moderating Effects of Partisanship among Young Voters ...........................136 Table 6.3. Moderating Effects of Political Knowledge among Young Voters...............138

Page 13: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

xiii

List of Figures

Figure 3.1. Voter Turnout in Presidential Elections in the United States, 1948-2004......39 Figure 3.2. Electoral Key Terms Appeared in Newspapers in the Campaign Mapping

Project (CMP), 1948-2004.....................................................................................39 Figure 3.3. “Voters,” “People,” and “Citizens” Appeared in Newspapers in the CMP,

1948-2004 .............................................................................................................41 Figure 3.4. Electoral Key Terms by Associations Variable ............................................47 Figure 3.5. Electoral Key Terms by Quality Variable ....................................................57 Figure 3.6. Subcategories in “Demographics” (in Quality Variable) ..............................60 Figure 4.1. “Party” Associations over Time...................................................................65 Figure 4.2. Mobilization Variable over Time.................................................................68 Figure 4.3. Goals and Rewards Variables over Time......................................................70 Figure 4.4. Behavior Variable over Time.......................................................................73 Figure 4.5. “Opinion Poll” Associations over Time .......................................................76 Figure 4.6. “Numbers” and “Demographic” (in Quality Variable) over Time.................79 Figure 4.7. Frame Variable over Time ...........................................................................81 Figure 4.8. “Government” Associations over Time........................................................82 Figure 4.9. Challenge Variable over Time .....................................................................84 Figure 4.10. Voting as a “Right” (in Assumptions Variable) over Time.........................87 Figure 5.1. Newspaper Article Used in the Experiment..................................................99

Page 14: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

1

Chapter 1. On the Study of American Electoral Participation

I believe it’s necessary for the Iraqi people to vote on January the 30th

because it provides an opportunity for people to participate in

democracy… It will give the Iraqi people a chance to become invested in

the future of that vital country.

– President George W. Bush, December 6th, 2004

Jill Davidson laughs when asked if she votes. “I don’t know where to go

register, and reason No. 2, I’m just lazy.”

– Christian Science Monitor, October 3rd, 2000

The United States prides itself on being a model of democracy in the modern

world. As seen in President George W. Bush’s remarks, it is not uncommon for the

Unites States to comment on the fundamental freedoms, civil rights and electoral

practices in other countries. At the same time, however, when it comes to actual electoral

participation, millions of Americans do not live up to the United States’ standards for

other countries. Notice the attitude of Jill Davidson, a 30-something massage therapist

and restaurant hostess in Miami. As her words reveal, the privilege of voting—the very

act that President Bush uses to help legitimize a war in Iraq—is something that can be

forsaken quite easily inside our borders. It is no small irony that the United States is so

proud of its democratic tradition and has the second lowest voter turnout rate in the

Page 15: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

2

world, a rate that has been steadily declining since the 1960s.1 How could all of these

things be true?

Political scholars have investigated America’s troubling state of electoral

participation and have offered a number of explanations. To date, though, they have left a

critical relationship unexplained: how Americans have been encouraged to think about

the vote, the voting process, and their roles as voters through the news media. In order to

better understand America’s electoral hypocrisy and the news media’s influence on the

political socialization process, this dissertation asks the following questions: (1) How

have voters and voting been portrayed in American newspapers from 1948 to 2004? (2)

Have these portrayals changed over time? (3) What are some potential implications of

these patterns for the electorate? To answer these questions, I will employ the content

analytic method to examine several keywords pertaining to the electoral system and

participation (Vote, Voter, Voting, Election, Electorate—and their derivatives2) in

selected newspapers from 1948 to 2004. I will also conduct a set of experiments to test

the effects of these portrayals on political attitudes among the nations’ youngest voters

(college students between 18 and 28 years-old) and older citizens (a national

representative sample of adults between 18 and 83 years-old). The remainder of this

chapter describes how and why this study is undertaken.

1 It should be noted that there are some exceptions to this downward trend: voter turnout spiked in 1992 and 2004 (U.S. Census). It should also be noted that McDonald and Popkin (2001) argue that the decline in voter turnout since the 1960s may be something of an “illusion” made by a methodological flaw. However, mounting evidence suggests otherwise. For the data supporting the decline in voter turnout since 1972, see Patterson (2002) and Wattenberg (2002). 2 I will examine the words voter, voters, vote, votes, voting, election, elections, electorate, and electorates.

Page 16: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

3

WHY STUDY VOTING? It is no accident that President Bush stresses the importance of voting in Iraq, a

country transitioning to a more democratic system. Democracy is the government of the

people, by the people, and for the people, and voting serves the most fundamental

function in building and sustaining democracy. Robert Dahl (1998) suggests that for a

country to be democratic, there are six minimal requirements to be met:

1. Elected officials—control over government decisions about policy is

constitutionally vested in officials elected by citizens;

2. Free, fair, and frequent elections—elected officials are chosen in frequent and

fairly conducted elections in which coercion is comparatively uncommon;

3. Freedom of expression—citizens have a right to express themselves without

danger of severe punishment on political matters broadly defined;

4. Alternative sources of information—citizens have a right to seek out

alternative and independent sources of information from other citizens,

experts, newspapers, magazines, books, telecommunication, and the like;

5. Associational autonomy—to achieve their various rights, citizens also have a

right to form relatively independent associations or organizations, including

independent political parties and interest groups; and

6. Inclusive citizenship—no adult permanently residing in the country and

subject to its laws can be denied the rights that are available to others and are

necessary to the five political institutions just listed (p. 85-86).

Page 17: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

4

As detailed in items one and two, here, voting is generally agreed upon as a foundation

for democracy.

While the centrality of voting has been generally agreed upon, it has been

fashionable for post-materialist scholars (Inglehart, 1997) and those following that

tradition (Dalton, 2007), to question and sometimes overlook the centrality of voting to a

democratic state. For this reason, it can be helpful to review, in some detail, seminal

arguments identifying voting as central to a democracy.

Besides the selection of leaders, voting serves several important functions that are

crucial to the health of democracy. Indeed, it is through voting that: the legitimacy of a

government is assessed; the workings of government are made effective and efficient; the

electors learn to become responsible citizens; and citizens protect their interests (Pomper,

1968).

First, voting provides legitimacy to government (Goodwin-Gill, 2006). According

to Lipset (1963), legitimacy entails “the capacity of the system to engender and maintain

the belief that the existing political institutions are the most appropriate ones for the

society” (p. 64). O’Donnell (2007) argues that “across most of the globe today, the

ultimate claim of a political regime to be legitimate—or at least acceptable—rests on the

kind of popular consent that purportedly finds expression in the act of free voting” (p. 6).

In liberal democratic states, legitimacy of government is achieved only when leaders are

elected by consent of the governed. According to John Locke, “The liberty of man in

society is to be under no other legislative power but that established by consent in the

commonwealth, nor under the dominion of any will, or restraint of any law, but what the

Page 18: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

5

legislature shall enact according to the trust put in it” (cited in Pomper, 1968, p. 25-26).

The moral principle of popular sovereignty is so deeply ingrained in the American psyche

that no other form of government than elective government can be considered legitimate.

Second, elections increase the power of government by providing elites authority

to govern. By selecting government officials via their ballots, citizens regard their

government as legitimate and their decisions as “morally binding” (Dahl, 1976, p. 60). A

government established by the will of the people gains power through its legitimacy, and

their decisions are upheld because of political equality embedded in electoral processes

(e.g., every citizen has a right to vote, one person one vote, etc.). Thus, elections increase

the power of government “without determining the specific actions of that government”

(Pomper, 1968, p. 26-27) and make the workings of government more effective and

efficient. Studies show that elections do indeed increase citizens’ support for and loyalty

to their political systems (Kornberg & Clarke, 1992).

Third, voting contributes to the personal development of the electors (Pateman,

1970; Pomper, 1968; Thompson, 1970). When citizens engage in electoral activities,

these individuals are called upon, theoretically at least, to consider not only their own

immediate interests, but also the public interests as a whole. Conceptually, this process

stimulates a sense of public responsibility in voters and broadens their perspectives. This

position is nicely articulated by John Stuart Mill who wrote that a citizen is “called upon,

when so engaged, to weigh interests not his own; to be guided in cases of conflicting

claims, by another rule than his private partialities . . . He is made to feel himself one of

Page 19: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

6

the public, and whatever is for their benefit to be for his benefit” (cited in Pomper, 1968,

p. 28).

Mill’s observations are evident in citizens’ voting patterns. Studies show that

voters engage in deliberative processes that put public interests before private concerns.

When making electoral decisions, voters are more likely to engage in “sociotropic

voting” than “pocketbook voting”—i.e., evaluating the economy as a whole rather than

their own personal financial situations (Miller & Shanks, 1996). Moreover, citizens

participate in electoral process out of their sense of civic duty (Riker & Ordeshook,

1968).

Finally, and most importantly, the ballot provides a check on power and

protection against tyranny. Macedo et al. (2005) argue “there is not now, and never will

be, a class of empathetic, non-self-interested elites who can be trusted to advance the

common good” (p. 12) so that public must be vigilant in holding elites accountable for

their actions. Writing in the eighteenth century, James Madison shared a similar concern

and suggested that elections are the only mechanism that protects public against tyranny.

In Federalist No. 52, Madison wrote, “it is particularly essential that the [representatives]

should have an immediate dependence on, and intimate sympathy with, the people.

Frequent elections are unquestionably the only policy by which the dependence and

sympathy can be effectually secured” (cited in Pomper, 1968, p. 30). Elections provide

opportunities for citizens to reward or punish their representatives with their votes, and

therefore, work to ensure that the interests of citizens are served.

Page 20: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

7

But this check on power, of course, is not automatic. For the mechanism of

dependence to work properly, citizens have to exercise their right to vote. As Pomper

(1968) suggests, “Unless the electorate is vigilant in defense of its interests, it may find

these interests neglected. Protection is an important indirect effect of elections, but it

demands attention on the part of the voters” (p. 32).

For these reasons, voting has been regarded by several political scientists as the

most fundamental aspect of democracy, and its functions are imperative for the health of

democracy. Voting provides legitimacy and stability to government, makes the workings

of government effective and efficient, educates citizens, and protects public interests.

Participation in electoral processes on the part of citizens is essential in ensuring a proper

functioning of democracy.

WHY STUDY VOTING TODAY?

Despite the centrality of voting in democracy, Americans have turned away from

electoral politics for quite some time. The percentage of voters casting presidential

ballots has largely declined since the 1960s, and the United States has the second lowest

voter turnout among other developed nations (Wattenberg, 2002).3 While almost two

thirds of eligible voters (62.8%) cast their ballots in 1960, barely half of the eligible

voters (51.2%) did so in 2000 (Wattenberg, 2002). In terms of young adults between 18

and 24, only one in three (36.1%) cast their vote in the 2000 presidential election, a drop

of fifteen percentage points since they first gained the right to vote in 1971 (U.S. Census,

3 See footnote 1.

Page 21: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

8

2002).4 While these downward trends were briefly suspended in the elections of 1992 and

2004 (and while the election of 2008 is anticipated to draw higher voter turnout), these

general patterns merit scholarly scrutiny.

Not only is the low level of voter turnout detrimental to the health of democracy,

it threatens the future of democracy as well. In the field of psychology, researchers

suggest that today’s behavior predicts future behavior (Freeman & Frazer, 1966). If

people are indeed creatures of habit, then, it is plausible that if one votes in an election,

s/he is more likely to vote in future elections. At the same time, if one does not

participate in electoral activities, s/he is less likely to participate in future elections. These

patterns have been documented by Gerber, Green, and Shachar (2003) in a large-scale

field experiment involving 25,200 registered voters. Based on their longitudinal study in

1998 and 1999, the authors found that those who voted in 1998 were more likely to vote

in the next election. They write:

The act of voting is self-reinforcing. When people abstain from voting, their

subsequent proclivity for voting declines; when they vote, they become more

likely to vote again. Voting and abstention, in other words, are habit forming.

Attitudes and the environment help explain whether voting habits take root, but

one’s pattern of behavior itself has an independent effect on subsequent conduct

(p. 540).

4 Note that the voter turnout among 18-24 year olds increased in 1992 (48.6%) and 2004 (46.7%) (Lopez, Kirby, & Sagoff, 2005).

Page 22: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

9

Another source of concern comes from a reciprocal causal relationship between

the act of voting and a sense of political efficacy (defined as a person’s belief in his/her

ability to influence the political process—internal efficacy—and a person’s belief in

government’s responsiveness—external efficacy). Studies show that not only does a

sense of efficacy influence the likelihood of voting, the act of voting itself also influences

political efficacy. Based on a three-way panel of the Survey Research Center’s 1972-

1974-1976 Election study, Finkel (1985) reveals that voting in national elections

positively affects one’s external efficacy and higher levels of external efficacy lead to

more electoral engagement in the future. On the flip side, then, today’s nonvoting

suggests lower levels of external efficacy, and therefore, less voting in the future.

Low levels of electoral participation among young adults are particularly

concerning considering the effects of political socialization during adolescence. Political

socialization is the process through which persons acquire political orientations (Dawson

& Prewitt, 1969; Easton & Dennis, 1969), are taught values, attitudes and other behaviors

(Hess & Torney, 1967) and learn the political norms deemed acceptable from a prior

generation (Sigel, 1965). The health of a political system arguably hinges on the political

socialization of its youth. Particularly, the period of adolescence is considered the most

impressionable years in political socialization process (Sears, 1975). Political learning

during the adolescent years profoundly influences later political attitudes and behaviors

(Hess & Torney, 1967; Greenstein, 1968; Sears, 1975). Several empirical studies reveal

the effects of preadult political socialization on adult political orientation, attitudes and

behaviors (Andolina, Jenkins, Zukin, & Keeter, 2003; Beck & Jennings, 1982; Conway

Page 23: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

10

& Damico, 2001; Verba & Nie, 1972; Verba, Schlozman, & Brady, 1995). All of these

researchers suggest that if today’s young adults are not voting, tomorrow’s democracy is

in danger.

Additionally, disengagement from electoral politics among today’s youth can

result in a missed opportunity in socialization for those young people. In general, young

adults possess less experience with and knowledge about political issues and processes

compared to adults, and political campaigns provide opportunity to close the gap

(Bennett, 2000; Sears & Valentino, 1997). Sears and Valentino (1997) write, “To

preadults, politics are usually of rather low visibility, with low ambient levels of exposure

to relevant communication” (p. 47), and therefore, events such as presidential elections

serve as catalysts for preadult socialization. Using a three-way panel study of Wisconsin

families conducted between 1980 and 1981, they show that preadults gain more

knowledge and crystallized attitudes toward parties and candidates during election

periods. Hence, if young adults are not engaged in electoral processes, they are more

likely to miss these socialization gains during election periods.

The decline in voter turnout in general, and among young adults in particular,

goes against several societal and legal changes that should have, in theory, increased

voter turnout since the 1950s in the United States (Wattenberg, 2002). For instance,

scholars have identified education as the most powerful predictor of political

participation (Campbell, Converse, Miller, & Stokes, 1960; Key, 1966; Wolfinger &

Rosenstone, 1980) Yet the rise in level of education since the 1960s did not bring about

an increase in voter turnout. Patterson (2002) reports, “In 1960, half of the adult

Page 24: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

11

population had not finished high school and fewer than 10 percent had graduated from

college. Today, 25 percent hold a college degree and another 25 percent have attended

college” (p. 5). With the rising levels of education, however, we have witnessed a

significant decline in voter turnout.

Other structural improvements did not increase voter turnout, either. Legal

remedies such as the enactment of the Voting Rights Act of 1965, the 26th Amendment

to the United States Constitution in 1971, the National Voter Registration Act of 1993,

and the introduction of various registration flexibilities over the years did indeed increase

the number of registered voters, but have not increased the overall turnout rate (Patterson,

2002). In contrast, overall voter turnout kept declining over these years. Since the decline

of voter turnout contradicts both increases in education and legal efforts toward greater

enfranchisement and most of what has been known about voting behavior, it is

considered to be one of the biggest “puzzles” in American politics (Abramson & Aldrich,

1982; Brody, 1978).

To disentangle the puzzle of electoral participation, scholars have offered

numerous explanations. Some have pointed to a decline of political parties as

intermediaries between citizens and political life (Rosenstone & Hansen, 2003;

Wattenberg, 2002). Others have suggested that it might be a result of generational

replacement of voters and a decrease of social connectedness among citizens (Teixeira,

1992; Putnam 2000). Still others have pointed to a declining psychological involvement

in politics and belief in government responsiveness (Teixeira, 1992) while even others

suggest a sheer amount of electing (Wattenberg, 2002) and long campaigns (Patterson,

Page 25: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

12

2002). Scholars have pointed to mass media encouraging cynicism and making citizens

feel clever, informed, and busy (Hart 1999; Patterson, 1993). All of these factors seem to

have contributed to the decline of electoral participation over these years. However, these

scholars have left a critical relationship unexplained: how the news media are socializing

Americans as “voters.” Researchers have studied electoral participation from various

perspectives, but to date, no comprehensive project has specifically examined how

Americans have been encouraged to think about the vote, the voting process, and their

roles as voters by news media. In the following section, I will illustrate why it is

important to study news media’s influence on citizens as an agent of their political

socialization.

WHY STUDY VOTING IN THE NEWS? Political socialization is a complicated endeavor, and a variety of individuals and

institutions play a role in this process. People are politically socialized through their

families, schools, workplaces, churches, and mass media. These various agents have

different effects on the process, and there have been mixed results as to which agent has

the most important effects on how people develop their worldviews (Beck, 1977). Among

these socialization agents, the influence of parents, families, and schools on children’s

political socialization has been examined quite extensively (e.g., Beck & Jennings, 1975;

Feldman & Newcomb, 1969; Hess & Torney, 1967; Hyman, 1959; Jennings & Niemi,

1981; Litt, 1963; Merelman, 1980; Nie, Junn, & Stehlik-Barry, 1996; Parker & Buriel,

1997; Yates & Youniss, 1998; Zeigler, 1967).

Page 26: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

13

To date, however, the role of news media in political socialization has largely

been ignored (Buckingham, 1997; Calavita, 2003). Treating news media with secondary

importance risks harming our understanding of a dynamic political socialization process.

As Calavita (2003) argues:

News media are of subtle-but-fundamentally powerful ecological importance, not

just because news media engagement interrelates with, and takes place in the

context of, institutions and phenomena like the family, but because all aspects of

the larger culture and society – including family – are themselves shaped by mass

media (p. 23).

Considering the centrality of news media to American life and its effects on the political

socialization process, the cursory attention paid to the role of news media in political

socialization literature limits an understanding of how people come to have certain

political values, beliefs and behaviors from a macro perspective.

Furthermore, the ways in which news media are studied as a socialization agent

binds our understandings of its influence. Quite often, when the role of news media is

examined, the focus is on the acquisition of political knowledge and of partisan attitudes

(e.g., Atkin & Gantz, 1978; Chaffee, Ward, & Tipton, 1970; Dominick 1972; Drew &

Reeves, 1980; Hollander, 1971; Martinelli & Chaffee, 1995; Rubin, 1976). In the field of

communication, however, we know that the news media’s role as a socialization agent is

not limited to providing political information to the public. We know that news media

also provide the broader understandings of what it means to be an active participant in

democratic society.

Page 27: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

14

To date, this meaning-making function of news media has been largely

overlooked by researchers in the field of political socialization and mass media. British

scholar Buckingham (1997) iterates a need for examining the role of news media as a

meaning-making agent on individual’s political development. He contends:

Rather than attempting to measure the effectiveness of news in communicating

political information, we should be asking how it enables viewers to construct and

define their relationship with the public sphere. How do news programs position

viewers in relation to the social order –for example, in relation to the sources of

power in society? How do they enable viewers to conceive of the relations

between the personal and the political, and to establish connections to their own

direct experience? How, ultimately, do they define what it means to be a citizen?

(p. 353, italics added).

To sum, then, while political socialization has been largely studied in the field of

political science, a communication approach examining news media’s portrayals of

electoral participation will forge a new perspective in this important process. The

literatures on political socialization demonstrate the importance of news media in

citizens’ political learning process and demand further investigation of how news media

portray a democratic citizenry. Next, I will discuss why a specific theory of media

effects—attribute agenda-setting—is central to this study.

Page 28: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

15

THE THEORY OF ATTRIBUTE AGENDA-SETTING

Because of the crucial role that news media play during political campaigns,

media scholars have produced a great amount of literature that investigates the effects of

news media on citizens’ political opinions and attitudes (e.g., Cappella & Jamieson,

1997; Iyengar & Kinder, 1987; McCombs & Shaw, 1972; Patterson, 1993). Among them,

the theory of agenda-setting created by McCombs and Shaw (1972) has become one of

the most influential and extensively examined theories in the field of media effects

(Rogers, Dearing, & Bregman, 1993).

Inspired by Cohen’s (1963) observation that while the mass media may not be

successful much of the time in telling people what to think, “they are stunningly

successful in telling them what to think about” (p. 13), McCombs and Shaw (1972)

examined the transfer of issue salience from the news media to the public during the 1968

presidential election by comparing the news media’s issue salience and what newspaper

readers regarded as the most important problems in their minds. The study revealed that

the issues that were salient in the news became the issues that were salient among the

public. The study showed that the news media set the agenda for the public by

highlighting certain issues. A great number of additional studies followed suit supporting

the theory (Brosius & Kepplinger, 1990; Eaton Jr., 1989; Smith, 1987; Soroka,

2001;Takeshita, 1993; Weaver, 1996; Weaver, Graber, McCombs, & Eyal, 1981; Winter

& Eyal, 1981).

The continuous investigations of the agenda-setting function of news media have

resulted in much fine-tuning and elaboration of the original theory. One of the theories

Page 29: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

16

born out of the elaboration and of particular importance to this dissertation is attribute (or

second level) agenda-setting. While the first level agenda-setting hypothesized the

transfer of issue salience from news media to the public (McCombs & Shaw, 1972), the

attribute agenda-setting hypothesizes the transfer of attribute salience from news media

to the public (Kiousis, Bantimaroudis, & Ban, 1999). The core of the attribute agenda-

setting theory indicates that news media often tell us not just “what to think about,” but

“how to think about a topic in the news and sometimes even what opinion to hold about

that topic” (Kim & McCombs, 2007, p. 300).

A number of studies validate the effects of the attribute agenda-setting across the

globe (McCombs, 2004). For instance, McCombs, Llamas, Lopez-Escobar and Lennon

(1997) found that candidate attributes featured in news media were significantly

correlated with perceptions of candidate attributes among the public during the 1995

Spanish regional elections. Golan and Wanta (2001) revealed that voters’ evaluations of

candidates were significantly associated with attributes salient in three newspapers in

New Hampshire during the 2000 presidential primaries in the region. Kiousis et al.

(1999) empirically tested the effects of the attribute agenda-setting and concluded that

people’s perceptions of candidates’ traits reflect portrayals of those traits in newspapers.

Other studies have also found empirical evidence to support the effects of attribute

agenda-setting on candidates (Kim, Scheufele, & Shanahan, 2002; Kiousis, 2003;

McCombs, Lopez-Escobar, & Llamas, 2000) as well as issues (Chyi & McCombs, 2004;

Hester & Gibson, 2003; Kim et al., 2002).

Page 30: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

17

Much attention has been paid to the attributes of issues and candidates in news

reports and their effects on the public in the field of media effects, but no one to date has

examined the attributes of citizens (e.g., voters) or civic acts (e.g., electoral participation)

in newspapers and the effects of these portrayals on citizens’ political attitudes. In this

dissertation, I will ask fundamental questions to start a more robust scholarly

conversation about the ways in which news media encourage Americans to think of their

roles as voters and the effects of such coverage. Specifically, these key concerns are: (1)

How have voters and voting been portrayed in American newspapers from 1948 to 2004?

(2) Have these portrayals changed over time? and (3) What are some potential

implications of these patterns for the electorate?

OVERVIEW OF THE DISSERTATION

This dissertation investigates how print news has portrayed voters, voting and the

vote between 1948 and 2004 and the effects of these portrayals.

• Chapter Two describes the content analytic method employed in Chapters Three

and Four and provides a brief preview of experimental design used in Chapters

Five and Six.

• Chapter Three looks at an overall pattern of how voting and voters have been

portrayed over the past sixty years in six newspapers during presidential elections.

• Chapter Four examines changes in these portrayals over time.

• Chapter Five investigates the effects of these portrayals on political beliefs and

attitudes on a random sample of citizens (average age of 45). More specifically,

Page 31: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

18

the effects of portrayals of voters on people’s intention to vote, sense of civic

duty, political efficacy, perceived meaningfulness of election, information

seeking, and trust in news media are discussed.

• Chapter Six explores the effects of these portrayals on political beliefs and

attitudes among the nation’s youngest voters (average age of 20), and then,

compares the results among general populations and young adults.

• Chapter Seven reviews major findings from this dissertation, and discusses the

limitations and implications of the study.

The findings point to (1) an “untold” story about voters in the United States, (2) some

potential ways to re-craft this story, that may benefit voters and the press, and (3) many

questions for future research.

Page 32: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

19

Chapter 2. Methods

As discussed in Chapter One, political scientists have produced an impressive

amount of literature on electoral participation over the past fifty years, addressing how

individuals make electoral decisions and what contributes to the decline of voter turnout

over the years. In the field of mass communication, media scholars have investigated the

role of news media during elections, and found strong effects of news media in forming

citizens’ political beliefs and attitudes about political issues and candidates. Yet both

camps of scholars have left a critical question unexamined: how electoral participation

has been portrayed in news media and the effects of these portrayals.

This dissertation explores these concerns with two different methods: content

analysis and experiments. In order to track the portrayals of vote, voters, and voting in

news media, an extensive content analysis has been conducted. In order to test the effects

of specific portrayals on citizens’ political beliefs and attitudes, a set of experiments have

also been conducted. This chapter describes these methods and limitations. First, I will

review the Campaign Mapping Project (a dataset this study relies on). Second, the

content analytic methods employed in Chapters Three and Four are described in detail.

Finally, a set of experiments used in Chapters Five and Six are illustrated briefly.

Page 33: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

20

THE CAMPAIGN MAPPING PROJECT The Campaign Mapping Project is a multi-year research project directed by

Roderick P. Hart of the University of Texas and Kathleen Hall Jamieson of the

University of Pennsylvania. In this project, Hart and Jamieson collected the campaign

materials (the speeches, advertisements, debates, broadcast news stories, newspaper

accounts, and letters-to-the-editor) produced during presidential election campaigns in the

United States between 1948 and 2004.

Several book projects have emerged from this text-base. First, Hart’s Campaign

Talk (2000) analyzes the language of modern political campaigns using the texts

collected by the Campaign Mapping Project and a computer program called DICTION,

and provides rich understandings of political language and modern campaigns.

Another project that employs the dataset is Jarvis’s Talk of the Party (2005).

Jarvis conducts a “semantic genealogy” of political parties keying on six focal terms

(Democrat, Republican, Independent, Party, Liberal, and Conservative) and traces the

meanings of parties and partisanship in American political discourse over the past fifty

years. By employing an extensive content analysis on a broad range of elite discourse

(Presidential speeches, newspapers, Congressional debates, and civics textbooks), Jarvis

provides unique insights into how parties appear in discourse and how such appearances

influence people’s understandings and relationships of the parties.

Still another project that utilizes the dataset is conducted by Hart, Jarvis, Jennings,

and Smith-Howell (2005). In their book titled Political Keywords: Using Language That

Uses Us, the authors examine eight political keywords (Politics, President, Government,

Page 34: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

21

People, Media, Parties, Promises, and Consultants) employed during the presidential

campaigns. By looking closely into the language of political elites used during the

presidential campaign periods over the past fifty years, the authors show us how these

political keywords have changed over time tracing broader political and cultural

development. Combining three distinct but related scholarships—Raymond William’s

constructs (semantic genealogy), Kenneth Burke’s perspective (rhetorical approach), and

Harold Lasswell’s methodology (content analysis)—these studies meticulously examine

meanings attached to specific words and what functions they serve.

While this dissertation employs the same dataset as these researchers, a unique

contribution of this study is to bring together the keyword research tradition and the

theory of attribute agenda-setting. Specifically, like the keyword researchers, this study

focuses in on certain keywords (Vote, Voter, Voting, Election, and Electorate—and their

derivatives) appearing in the Campaign Mapping Project. This brings a longitudinal

approach and a more nuanced unit of analysis to the attribute agenda-setting approach. At

the same time, as in the field of attribute agenda-setting, this study attempts to

empirically verify the influence of the meanings attached to these keywords.

METHODOLOGY

Content Analysis

Content analysis is a research technique for the objective, systematic and

quantitative description of manifest content of communication (Berelson, 1971). As

discussed in the definition, the strengths of this method are that it is objective

Page 35: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

22

(researchers’ biases and idiosyncrasies are prevented through inter-coder reliability),

systematic (sample texts are selected through consistent and explicit rules), and

quantitative (analyses an accurate representation of a body of messages) (Wimmer &

Dominick, 1994). In addition, it is an unobtrusive method and effective in analyzing large

amounts of data (Krippendorff, 1980).

This method has high external validity (it is generalizable) and low internal

validity (cannot make claims of the effects of content on an audience). There is a good fit

between this method and prior research on these questions. Content analysis is a common

method in the tradition this project follows. For instance, Hart et al. (1991), Hart (2000),

Hart et al. (2005) and Jarvis (2005) all use content analytic method in their study as well

as McCombs et al. (2000), Hester and Gibson (2003), and Chyi and McCombs (2004).

Keywords. A set of keywords has been selected for this study. They are vote,

voter, voting, election, and electorate (and their derivatives, e.g. voters, votes, elections

and electorates). The voter and electorate are chosen to investigate the agent of electoral

participation; vote and voting are selected to examine the act of electoral participation;

election is chosen to explore the system of electoral participation.

Texts. While there are various media texts that can be analyzed, I chose to

examine newspaper articles since newspapers tend to be the dominant agenda-setter for

other news outlets (McCombs, 2004; Roberts & McCombs, 1994). For this study, six

newspapers were examined. The newspaper articles come from the New York Times,

Washington Post, Chicago Tribune, Los Angeles Times, Christian Science Monitor, and

Atlanta Constitution. The New York Times and Washington Post are selected as the elite

Page 36: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

23

media that set agenda for other news media outlet. Chicago Tribune, Los Angeles Times,

and Christian Science Monitor are also widely read newspapers and they add regional

viewpoints. The newspaper articles are gathered during presidential campaign periods

from 1948 to 2004. All the texts for this study come from the Campaign Mapping Project

and have been generously shared by Professors Roderick P. Hart and Kathleen Hall

Jamieson.

Using a Keyword-in-Context program, each keyword has identified with 20

words immediately before and after the keyword. These 41 word clusters serve as my

coding units (Hart et al., 2005; Jarvis, 2005). After locating every instance of each

keyword in the entire database, a stratified random sample was gathered. Originally, I

hoped to analyze a sample of 450 instances for each keyword (i.e., 30 instances of the

keyword x 15 campaign periods between 1948 and 2004) yielding a total of 2, 250 units

coded (i.e., 450 samples x 5 key words). However, one of the keywords, electorate, did

not appear often enough between 1948 and 1968. Therefore, a total of 2,138 terms were

collected. Table 2.1 displays the actual number of instances coded in my content analysis

(please note that a few quantitative statistics were ran on the raw appearance of all 37,491

terms as well; these data are specifically referred to as the “overall number of terms

appearing in the study” vs. “the data coded in my content analysis” throughout the

project).

Page 37: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

24

Table 2.1

Electoral Key Terms in Study

_________________________________________________________________ Key Terms n _________________________________________________________________ Voter(s) 450 Vote(s) 450 Voting 450 Election(s) 450 Electorate(s) 338 _________________________________________________________________ Total 2,138 _________________________________________________________________ Note. A stratified random sample of 2,138 out of 37,491 electoral key terms identified in the Campaign Mapping Project dataset were gathered and analyzed in this project.

Coding Processes. I worked with a small group of coders (two undergraduate and

one graduate student). The team was trained for one week on these variables. Following

the trends in the researches in the field, 10% of all texts were coded by two team

members. As the primary coder, I coded all texts in the sample. Inter-coder reliability

statistics show acceptable agreement across coding decisions.5

5 Inter-coder reliability score for each variable is as follows (Cohen’s Kappa): assumption, .90; mobilization agent, .83; association, .91; goals, .96; rewards, .88; role, .86; potency, .88; challenge, .77; context, .83; frame, .91; time, .87; behavior, .93; quality, .88.

Page 38: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

25

Coding Scheme. The coding scheme has two components. One of which has a

series of purely descriptive measures. These items are coded to keep track of the source,

year and grammatical usage of each keyword examined in this study.

1. Text # ________

2. Year: ________ 3. Keyword:

1. Voter(s) 2. Vote(s) 3. Voting 4. Election(s) 5. Electorate(s)

4. Grammar:

1. Noun 2. Verb 3. Adjective

Next, this study examined a series of measures guided by literatures on attribute

agenda-setting, political socialization and electoral participation. As discussed before, the

attribute agenda-setting literature suggests that attributes salient in the media become

salient in the public’s mind, and the political socialization literature points to the

significance of the meanings attached to democratic citizenry by news media. In order to

better understand citizens’ perception of their electoral participation and political

socialization process, then, it is important to ask what kinds of attributes are attached to

the voters and their electoral participation in the news media.

In attribute agenda-setting, attributes are defined as “those characteristics and

properties that fill out the picture of each object” (McCombs, 2004, p. 70) and are

Page 39: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

26

believed to have two parts: the affective and the substantive dimensions (McCombs et al.,

2000). The affective dimension deals with the tone of news coverage (e.g., whether

candidate attributes are described in positive, negative, or neutral terms) while the

substantive dimension looks at the attributes emphasized in news coverage (e.g., the

candidates’ ideology, qualifications and personality).

In this study, several variables have been created to tap these dimensions.

Specifically, the affective dimension of attribute agenda-setting is examined by the

context variable following the previous studies in attribute agenda-setting. The context

variable examines the tone of electoral participation. The attribute agenda-setting studies

show that whether candidates or issues are described in positive, negative, or neutral

terms in the news influences the public’s evaluation of them (Hester & Gibson, 2003;

McCombs et al., 2000). Following the coding scheme of attribute agenda-setting studies

and the keyword research, context is categorized as follows.

5. Context: overall tone of the article as linked to the voter, voting and vote(s). 1. Positive (“Your vote will move this country forward.”) 2. Negative (“Voting for the third party is complete waste of time.”) 3. Neutral (“You have an option to vote or not to vote.”)

Then, the rest of the variables listed here aim to unpack the substantive attributes

ascribed to electoral participation. These variables are: assumptions, mobilization agent,

association, goals, rewards, frame, role, potency, behavior, challenge, and quality.

The assumption variable explores the basic philosophy behind the act of voting as

discussed in news coverage. Interestingly, there is no agreement on the assumption of

voting in the academic literature. Some scholars argue that voting is a duty (Beekman,

Page 40: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

27

1891; Riker & Ordeshook, 1968), while others argue that it is a right (Mayo, 1959) or a

choice (Rational choice theorists, e.g., Downs, 1957; Fiorina, 1981; Popkin, 1991). With

this variable, I will investigate the assumptions of voting that are communicated to

citizens through news media. The variable is coded as follows:

6. Assumption: the assumption of (or philosophy behind) voting 0. None (“There is a controversy over electoral votes.”) 1. Voting as a right (“I urge you to exercise your right to vote.”) 2. Voting as a duty (“Voting is your sacred duty as a citizen.”) 3. Voting as a choice (“You may choose not to vote.”) The next variable taps into the core of political campaigns. Scholars have

proposed different mobilizing agents as having influence on voter turnout. These forces

include: political parties (Leighley, 2001; Rosenstone & Hansen, 1993; Wattenberg,

1998), candidates (Kernell, 1997), or other groups such as family members, the League

of Women Voters, and churches (Gerber & Green, 2000). While there is no agreement on

which mobilization agent has the biggest influence on voter turnout, many seem to agree

that it is the decrease in electoral mobilization that caused the decline in voter turnout for

the past sixty years (Rosenstone & Hansen, 1993). Rosenstone and Hansen (1993)

suggest, “political participation is the product of strategic interactions of citizens and

leaders. Few people spontaneously take an active part in public affairs. Rather, they

participate when politicians, political parties, interest groups, and activists persuade them

to get involved” (p. 228). I will investigate if voters are portrayed as mobilized in

newspapers, and if so, who is soliciting votes.

Page 41: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

28

7. Mobilization agent: an entity mobilizing voters or soliciting votes 0. None (“In 1996, 49 percent of eligible voters cast their ballot.”) 1. Party (“The Republican Party needs your vote.”) 2. Candidates/Politician (“Gore urges young adults to vote.”) 3. Friends/Family (“A voter said her vote choice was influenced by her family.”) 4. Citizens/Voters (“Local voters gathered to register new voters.”) 5. Other groups (interest group, church, etc.) (“The National Association for the

Advancement of Colored People has mobilized 4,000 voters in Florida.”) 6. Government (“The government has implemented several voter registration

reforms to increase the voter turnout.”) 7. Media (“Rock the vote campaign by MTV targets young voters.”) 8. Issue (“Abortion is getting much attention from conservative voters.”) 9. Mixed (“Both parties and their presidential hopefuls asked for their votes.”) 10. Unclear (“He said his vote was based on his instinct.”)

The next variable is similar to the mobilization agent, but does not include the

element of solicitation. Teixeira (1992) argues that while election law changes and

socioeconomic changes point to an increase in turnout, the decline in social and political

“connectedness” (p 23) is pulling the turnout down. During political campaign periods,

citizens interact with political parties, specific candidates, the media, and other groups.

The associations variable, then, will track those entities with which voters interact and

share space.

Page 42: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

29

8. Associations: a reference to some social entity with which the voters interact 0. None (“Today, voters decide.”) 1. Party (“53 percent of Indiana’s voters favor Republican party ticket.”) 2. Candidate/Politicians (“4 percent of likely voters said they support Nader.”) 3. Family/Friends (“She said she went to vote with her friend.”) 4. Citizens/Voters (“The first time voter was sitting next to an elderly voter.”) 5. Other groups (“Voters talk about the election with their church members.”) 6. Government (“The voters need to contact local County Board of Registrars.”) 7. Media (“Many voters say they follow news on television every day.”) 8. Issue (“The federal budget deficit is not the central concern of the voters.”) 9. Opinion Poll (“Recent opinion polls show that voters are turning out.”) 10. Expert/Consultant (“An expert in voting machine said voters are skeptical.”) 11. Mixed (“Voters looked at news reports and talked to friends to understand

candidates’ policy issues.”) 12. Other/Unclear (“Anonymous phone calls were made to voters.”)

The next set of variables will examine the goals and rewards of electoral

participation. The goals variable investigates if voters have a broader collective good in

mind or if they are self-interested when participating in the electoral process. Some

economic voting literatures advanced by political scientists suggest that the voters seek

private goals (Chong, Citrin, & Conley, 2001), while others suggest that voters assess

national issues divorced from their personal situation and therefore seek a greater

collective goal than their immediate self-interest (Kinder & Kiewiet, 1979). Similar to the

goals variable, the rewards variable taps into what is promised to citizens as a reward of

their participation and the scope of the reward.

9. Goals: the voter’s (voting’s, vote’s) goals as implied by the article 0. No goal mentioned (“Voting: more people can, but fewer will.”) 1. A broad (collective) goal (“Vote for liberty.”) 2. A narrow (private) goal (“Vote to protect your social security.”) 3. Mixed (“When you vote, vote for the future of yourself and your country.”) 4. Unclear (“What are the goals of voters?”)

Page 43: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

30

10. Rewards: the benefit of voting as implied by the article 0. None (“Almost a half of eligible voters showed up on election day.”) 1. Private tangible benefits (“Vote for me, I will promise you a tax cut.”) 2. Public tangible benefits (“Your vote will help create safer roads.”) 3. Private intangible benefits (“A voter said she felt good about fulfilling her

civic duty.”) 4. Public intangible benefits (“Vote so that democracy will prevail.”) 5. Mixed (“Your vote will bring prosperity to your family and to this country.”) 6. Unclear (“I don’t know what my vote can do.”)

The next set of variables trace citizens’ political agency. The first variable under

this category is role and investigates what kind of job is assigned to the voters. Specific

attention focuses on if voters are portrayed as a part of solution, or a part of problem.

Another variable examining agency is potency. This variable looks at whether the voters

are portrayed as active participants or passive observers of political affairs. Then, the

challenge variable asks if the voters are engaging in some active enterprise, inquiring as

to if the act is hard or easy, and if voters featured in newspapers face obstacles while

engaging in electoral activities. These variables are coded as follows:

11. Role: the social or political job being performed by the voter(s), the act of voting, and the vote(s).

0. None (“Mr. Ryan is a Democrat who plans to vote for Mr. Gore.”) 1. Part of the solution (“Your vote will get this country move forward again.”) 2. Part of the problem (“Democracy is in decline because an estimated 100

million eligible voters don’t vote on Election Day.”) 3. As conflicted (“The vote for the candidate is a double-edged sword.”) 4. Unclear (“My vote does not matter.”)

12. Potency: a stated or implied task being projected on to voters

0. None (“There are a growing number of Latino voters.”) 1. As actor (“In California, voters have requested 3.2 million absentee ballots.”) 2. As recipient (“The candidate alienated young voters.”) 3. Balanced (“Clinton made an effort to get voters to reflect on the progress he

asserts has occurred during his watch.”)

Page 44: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

31

13. Challenge: a level of difficulty of engaging the act 0. Not mentioned (“As a citizen of this country, we have to vote.”) 1. Mentioned (“As initiatives and referenda proliferate, voters say that they need

to enroll in graduate school to understand all the issues.”)

The focus of election news has attracted much attention from political

communication scholars and has been criticized for its emphasis on political competition,

horse race and strategic concerns (Cappella & Jamieson, 1997; Fallows, 1997; Lawrence,

2000; Patterson, 1993). Researchers suggest that the media’s focus on game and

strategies as opposed to public policies and issues have created cynicism among the

American electorate. This variable looks at the frame of the news reports.

14. Frame: What is the frame of the article? 1. Issue (“Civil right is the only major issue average voter is concerned about.”) 2. Game (strategy, horserace) (“In a three-way race including Nader, Bush and

Kerry have 45 percent each among voters.”) 3. Mixed (“Candidates compete for the voters’ support on tax reform.”) 4. Identity/Other/N/A. (“Experts suggest that voter apathy is growing especially

among young adults.”)

Another concern is the rhetorical use of time in describing voters. Is electoral

participation talked about as a past, present, or future activity? Or, when electoral

participation is talked about in positive or negative ways, are journalists referring to the

past, present, or future activity? This variable will help to interpret other variables by

adding a temporal aspect to them.

15. Time: the voter’s moment in history as implied by the article 1. Past (“Many voters were frustrated by what happened in the last election.”) 2. Present (“Voters are concerned about our foreign policy in the Middle East.”) 3. Future (“Democratic voters will be working hard for the next election.”) 4. Across time (“Today, voters had a chance to reflect on these candidates’ votes

they cast four years ago.”)

Page 45: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

32

Next, the behavior variable tracks what, exactly, voters do. Rational choice

theorists argue that voters think rationally when engaging in the electoral process,

therefore, conceptualizing voters as a thinking body (Downs, 1957; Fiorina, 1981; Key,

1966; Nie, Verba, & Petrocik, 1976; Popkin, 1991). Other scholars claim that voters

engage in the electoral process psychologically. Marcus, Neuman, and MacKuen (2000)

suggest that emotions such as anxiety drive voters to certain vote choices. In this case,

voters are conceptualized as a feeling body. The behavior variable investigates how

American voters are conceptualized in news coverage.

16. Behavior (based on verb): how does the article conceptualize the voter as measured by the voter-linked verb?

0. None (“The survey of 1,000 voters was conducted last month.”) 1. Thinking (rational) body (“Voters questioned the candidate’s qualification.”) 2. Feeling (emotional) body (“Voters are frustrated with the current situation.”) 3. Acting body (“Voters engage in local debates.”) 4. Mixed (“Voters expressed resentment toward the candidate, but they said they

had to pick a lesser evil.”)

Finally, the qualities of the voters and electoral participation will be measured.

While the behavior variable is depends on verbs, this variable is based on adjectival

phrases (Hart, Smith-Howell, & Llewellyn, 1991). This variable assesses implicit and

explicit traits attached to voters and their electoral participation in newspaper articles.

Page 46: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

33

17. Quality (based on adjectival phrases): qualities of the voters, vote(s), and voting mentioned or implied by the article

0. None (“Voters cast their ballots.”) 1. Undecided (“Many undecided voters watched the debate.”) 2. Partisan (“Republican voters are better mobilized than Democratic voters.”) 3. Independent (“This election hinges on independent voters.”) 4. Demographic (“Gore made a special appeal to women voters.”) 5. Potential (registered, likely) (“Candidates wooed likely voters.”) 6. Affect (positive or negative) (“Apathetic voters have been a fixture in

American politics.”) 7. Intellectual (positive or negative) (“They are sophisticated voters.”) 8. Numbers (“Gore needs about 5,000 voters.”) 9. Candidate specific (“Perot voters rushing to the polls.”) 10. Mixed (“Young voters are considered apathetic, but they are quite

knowledgeable about issues like environment and education.”) 11. Other (“The polling place was packed with new voters.”)

To sum, an overall tone of the coverage was measured by context; electoral

philosophy was explored by assumption; political setting was investigated by

association, frame, time and challenge; political incentive was explored by mobilization

agent, goals, and rewards; and political agency was examined by potency and role as

well as behavior and quality that add richer understandings of what exactly voters do.

Data Analysis. In my analysis, I will use both quantitative and qualitative

evidence. Quantitative data will offer a big picture of the portrayals of electoral

participation by providing trends and patterns over fifty years as well as the news media’s

influence on citizens’ political socialization processes. Qualitative texts will provide

examples of these findings showing attributes allocated to these terms. Both data will

help better understand how Americans are socialized to think about their electoral

participation through news media.

Page 47: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

34

Experiments

While content analysis has high external validity, its limitation lies in internal

validity: it alone cannot provide a basis for the effects of content on an audience

(Wimmer & Dominick, 1994). On the other hand, experiments have a strong internal

validity as well-controlled experiments can illustrate clear causal relationships.

Therefore, in order to investigate the effects of the portrayals of electoral participation in

American newspapers (i.e., attribute agenda-setting effects), a set of experiments were

conducted.

Inspired by patterns in the content analysis, two experiments were conducted to

examine how the attributes attached to voters in newspapers influence citizens’ political

beliefs and attitudes: the first with a nationally representative sample of adults, the second

specifically, with young voters. In these experiments, participants read a manipulated

newspaper article which contained a paragraph featuring the attributes of voters found in

the content analysis and then were asked to complete a number of survey items taping

into citizens’ intention to vote, sense of civic duty, political efficacy, perception of

meaningfulness of elections, information seeking desires, and trust in news media. Two

experiments were identical in design except for the differences in samples. More detailed

descriptions of these experiments are discussed in Chapters Five and Six, respectively.

CONCLUSION This chapter described the methods employed in this dissertation. Drawing on

literatures on voting and following the tradition of keyword researchers and attribute

Page 48: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

35

agenda-setting scholars, an extensive coding scheme was devised to explore the meanings

attached to electoral participation. In order to examine the effects of newspaper portrayals

of electoral participation, two experiments were conducted.

Although the study does not provide answers to everything that needs to be

known about the news media and citizens’ electoral participation, by combining two

traditions and methods discussed here, I hope to shed light on how American newspapers

have portrayed voting, voters and the vote over time and circumstances as well as the

implications of such portrayals. The curious and troubling coexistence of America’s pride

as a model of democracy and her electoral realities demand deeper exploration. This

project hopes to take a first step in learning more on this topic.

Page 49: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

36

Chapter 3. Portrayals of American Electoral Participation (1948-2004)

This chapter presents the findings from a comprehensive content analysis and

discusses (1) how often the key terms pertaining to electoral participation—vote(s),

voter(s), voting, electorate(s) and election(s)—have appeared in American newspapers

over the past sixty years and (2) what kind of attributes have been most commonly

attached to voters and voting in American newspapers from 1948 to 2004. It is guided by

the following fundamental research questions:

RQ 1: How often have the key terms pertaining to electoral participation appeared

in print news between 1948 and 2004?

RQ 2: What are most commonly featured attributes of these electoral key terms?

Descriptive statistics were conducted on the 37,491 instances of key terms located

in the Campaign Mapping Project and qualitative analyses were conducted on a random

stratified sample of 2,138 of these terms. After these analyses, five broad themes emerged

in the data. These themes reveal that in news coverage between 1948 and 2004:

(1) Voters are increasingly mentioned over time;

(2) Voters and their electoral participation are not featured positively;

(3) Voters are cast in the shadow of elites;

(4) Voters are rarely reminded of democratic responsibilities; and

(5) Voters are not connected to the past or each other.

Page 50: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

37

This chapter describes these patterns, noting how even though voters are increasingly part

of news coverage, their roles in the democratic system are rarely embellished in news

coverage.

VOTERS ARE INCREASINGLY MENTIONED IN NEWS COVERAGE

A first step was to examine how often the key terms pertaining to electoral

participation have been featured in American newspapers between 1948 and 2004. Using

a Keyword-in-Context program, all instances of five key terms vote, voter, voting,

election and electorate (and their derivatives) in newspapers stored in the Campaign

Mapping Project dataset were identified (N = 37,491). Then, in order to measure the

frequencies of these terms per year, “word density” ratios were calculated by dividing the

number of instances of a specific keyword per year by the total number of words

appeared in newspapers per year and multiplying this figure by 100.6

Results reveal three notable patterns. First, while voter turnout has declined over

time, American newspapers have continually used the terms pertaining to electoral

participation in their coverage. Second, despite the decline in voter turnout, the number of

references to voters in newspapers has increased over time. Third, the number of

references to the electoral key terms has increased exponentially in 2004.

First, even though voter turnout in the United States has been on a gradual decline

from 1960 over the years (with slight reversals in 1992, 2000 and 2004, see Figure 3.1),

6 For example, the density ratio for the term voter in news coverage in 1948 was calculated as follows: total number of voter appeared in news coverage in 1948 / total number of words appeared in news coverage in 1948 x 100.

Page 51: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

38

no such decline appears in references to the key terms pertaining to electoral participation

in American newspapers. As Figure 3.2 shows, while the use of the term vote has

fluctuated over time, the term has been used steadily over the years. The term vote

increased in its use during the Civil Rights era of the 1960s—nearing 150% increase in

its use in eight years between 1960 (ratio = 0.19) and 1968 (ratio = 0.28)—and remained

relatively constant after the Civil Rights era with an exception of spikes in the years 1980

and 2004. The same can be said about the term election. References to election show an

ebb and flow over time closely following the pattern in the term vote, except for the Civil

Rights era. Unlike vote, the term election did not gain much prominence in the news

during the 1960s. Considering the explicit agendas of the Civil Rights era (one of them

being the right to vote), it is reasonable to find the increase in the number of vote

references but not in the number of election references in newspapers. The terms voting

and electorate are the least likely to appear in the news compared to the other terms

studied here, but the rate of usage over the years has remained constant.

Second, despite the decline in voter turnout, the number of references to voters in

newspapers has steadily increased over time. As Figure 3.2 shows, voters were

mentioned rather sparingly in newspapers coverage of presidential elections between

1948-1968. In 1972, however, the term gained considerable prominence in the news, and

at that time, voter (the actor) became more salient than vote (the act) itself. After 1972,

the term maintained its prominence in the news, outnumbering other key terms linked to

electoral participation. Indeed, the number of voter references has increased almost 600%

Page 52: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

39

from 1948 (word density = 0.07) to 2004 (0.41), and the voter has become the most

frequently used term among the other words examined here.

Figure 3.1. Voter Turnout in Presidential Elections in the United States, 1948-2004

45

50

55

60

65

1948

1952

1956

1960

1964

1968

1972

1976

1980

1984

1988

1992

1996

2000

2004

Year

Percentage

Voter Turnout

Source: The U.S. Census.

Figure 3.2. Electoral Key Terms Appeared in Newspapers in the Campaign Mapping Project (CMP), 1948-2004

Note: Word Density = (Number of Keyword / Number of Total Words) x 100

Page 53: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

40

One possible explanation for an increase in voter references since 1972 is the

expansion of the roles assigned to voters in the nomination process. After the McGovern-

Fraser Commission in 1968, the nomination process was opened to popular participation

for the first time, and voters began to occupy a larger role in selecting the party nominee.

These reforms made the input of the voters crucial in selecting the party candidate

resulting in “unequivocally the most democratic system this country has ever used to

nominate presidential candidates” (Cooper, 2001, p. 772). A second, possible,

explanation is that the 1972 presidential election was the first contest following the 26th

Amendment, granting 18-20 year olds the right to vote. Therefore, an increase in the

number of voter references might reflect the expansion of voting population.

The increased use of the term voters in news coverage since 1972 is a truly unique

phenomenon when compared to other related terms in the Campaign Mapping Project

dataset. Figure 3.3 illustrates that while voter references have increased over time,

references to more general masses, such as people and citizen(s), have remained constant

over the years. Notice how of these three words, the term citizen was least likely to

appear in the news while a more generic term people was used more often than the term

voter prior to 1972. Since 1972, however, the term voter became the most frequently used

word in election news coverage.

Page 54: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

41

Figure 3.3. “Voters,” “People,” and “Citizens” Appeared in Newspapers in the CMP, 1948-2004

Note: Word Density = (Number of Keyword / Number of Total Words) x 100

Finally, the coverage of the 2004 election shows an exponential increase in the

number of references to electoral participation in the news. The number of references to

the voter, voting and election reached all time high in 2004, and that of the vote reached

second highest level in 2004. Although I can only speculate why the rate of references

increased significantly in 2004, it may be the case that the controversial election of 2000

triggered the change in attention to voters and voting in the news. In the 2000 presidential

election, the butterfly ballot in Florida created much confusion among voters, and several

weeks of legal battles over vote counts created controversy over the election result and

the electoral process itself. The increased references to electoral key words in election

news coverage may reflect an increased attention to voting processes after the 2000

election. (Note: The qualitative nature of the data from 2004 will be discussed in Chapter

Four).

Page 55: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

42

In short, quantitative data indicate that the electoral key terms have been used

constantly in American newspapers, the number of references to voters has steadily

increased over time, and the usage of the key terms reached a record high in 2004. The

rest of the chapter discusses the dominant qualitative patterns that emerged from the

content analysis.

VOTERS ARE NOT FEATURED POSITIVELY

How have the terms central to the democratic process been described in the news

qualitatively? A first qualitative pattern reveals that voters and their electoral

participation are not overtly praised. The context variable, the one which measures the

tone of the coverage, suggests that the newspapers report electoral participation in a

mostly neutral tone, 85.1% (Table 3.1). The prevalence of the neutral tone is not

surprising considering the key words linked to electoral participation were often placed in

an identity category (e.g., “Texas has 32 electoral votes,” “30% of electorate in Illinois

are still making up their minds,” “Nixon voters cast their ballots,” etc).

As Table 3.1 displays, voters and their electoral participation were mentioned in a

positive tone only 1.2% of the time in news coverage. Although voters and electoral

participation are generally regarded as one of the most important and fundamental

elements of democratic society among the public, studies on attribute agenda-setting

suggest that when issues or groups are not discussed in a positive manner, the readers are

less likely to perceive them in a positive way (Hester & Gibson, 2003; McCombs et al.,

Page 56: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

43

2000). Here, the lack of positive tone in news coverage of voting and voters may pose a

concern regarding the political socialization of the news audience.7

Table 3.1

Electoral Key Terms by Context Variable

_______________________________________________________________________ Key terms Positive Negative Neutral _______________________________________________________________________ Vote(s) 0.2% 8.7% 91.1% Voter(s) 1.6 14.0 84.4 Voting 2.9 21.1 76.0 Election(s) 0.4 8.7 90.9 Electorate(s) 0.9 16.6 82.5 _______________________________________________________________________ Overall 1.2 13.7 85.1 _______________________________________________________________________

Furthermore, the context variable indicates that voters and their electoral

participation were far more likely to be cast in negative light than positive one. Table 3.1

also illustrates that the negative tone outnumbered positive tone by more than eleven to

one (13.7% vs. 1.2%). One of the ways in which voters were cast in negative light is

7 It is important to note that the news coverage of electoral keywords in 1992 and 2004 (two years that showed a spike in voter turnout) contained the highest level of “positive” context compared to other years examined here (1992 = 3.3%, 2004 = 3.3%). While these instances are modest in numbers, it is noteworthy that these two years that had higher voter turnout contained the highest level of positive coverage among all years.

Page 57: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

44

directly attaching negative attributes to voters. For instance, negative attributes (e.g.,

apathetic, cynical, disillusioned, pessimistic, etc.) were far more likely to be attached to

voters and electorates than positive ones (e.g. interested, thoughtful, open-minded,

informed, etc).

Here are a few textual examples where voters were described in a negative tone.

Notice how voters are encouraged to consider negative traits of the electorate and

candidates instead of positive ones:

The candidate told a story about a young supporter who warned him that he will

be disappointed by the electorate, that Americans are too cynical to accept his

moral message (Washington Post, November 7, 1972, p. A1).

Democratic presidential candidate Walter F. Mondale accused President Reagan

of trying to exploit the supposed selfishness and greed of young voters (Los

Angeles Times, September 26, 1984, p. A18).

The examination of verbs linked to voters and the electorate adds to this pattern.

The data show that when voters and electorates are featured as actors or recipients of a

certain act (examined by potency variable), they were more likely to be associated with

negative verbs (e.g., alienated, deflected, disenfranchised, destroyed, disturbed, exploited,

frightened, intimidated, misled, puzzled, set the clock back, troubled, etc.) than positive

ones (19.9% vs. 1.8%).8

8 The data are drawn from the cross-tabulation of context and potency and qualitative textual analysis of verbs linked to the terms voter(s) and electorate(s).

Page 58: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

45

The act of electoral participation is also connected to negativity (variable:

context). The following excerpts illustrate how the act of electoral participation is tied to

negativity such as losing Social Security, weakness, despair and disrespect:

The Vice President charged that Kennedy has been advising voters: “Don’t vote

for Mr. Nixon, because if you do, you’re going to lose your Social

Security”(Washington Post, November 2, 1960, p. A1).

Mr. Bush stressed the leadership theme, saying the voters faced a watershed

choice of following Mr. Reagan or going “back to weakness, despair and

disrespect” (New York Times, October 12, 1984, p. A1).

Although it is rare for journalists to feature negative attributes of voters and electoral

participation in their own words, by selecting certain parts of candidates’ campaign

rhetoric (that are often negative), they cast a hallmark of democracy in negative light.

Lance Bennett (1988) suggests, “reporters and editors search for events with dramatic

properties and then emphasize those properties in their reporting” so that the content of

the news is more dramatized than “any natural preeminence they may have in the

political scheme of things” (p. 35). Here, it appears that journalists’ tendency to seek

drama in news results in an emphasis on negativity in candidates’ campaign rhetoric.

Although candidates do make positive promise for the better future and cast voters in

more positive light in their candidate-centered campaign rhetoric (e.g., “I hope millions

of you will make history by voting for your future, voting for opportunity, voting for

leadership that trusts in you and the power of your dreams” from The Atlanta

Constitution, 1984, October 28, p. A19), these positive messages typically do not make it

Page 59: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

46

in print. Instead, their attempts to warn voters against their opponents are more likely to

be covered.

In short, these data show that voters have been more likely to be reminded of

negative attributes associated with themselves and their vote rather than a positive

outlook of their political engagement in the news over the years.

VOTERS ARE CAST IN THE SHADOW OF ELITES

A second qualitative pattern shows that voters have been sidelined in news

coverage in both subtle and obvious ways. The associations variable which assessed the

entities with which the terms—vote, voter, voting, election and electorate—interact

indicates that newspapers cast voters to the side in the democratic process by linking

electoral process to the candidates and politicians rather than citizens or issues. Figure 3.4

shows that, overall, the words in this study were most likely to be associated with

“candidates or politicians” (75.2%) followed by “party” (27.4%), “citizens or voters”

(23.1%), “opinion polls” (16.9%), “issue” (16.2%), “expert/consultants” (6.7%), “mass

media” (4.1%), “other group” (3.9%) and “government” (2.6%).

Page 60: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

47

Figure 3.4. Electoral Key Terms by Associations Variable

_______________________________________________________________________ All Key Terms (n = 2138) Vote & Voting (n = 900)

Voter & Electorate (n = 788) Election (n = 450)

_______________________________________________________________________

First, take the term election. This word was associated with “candidates or

politicians” 83.1% of the time while “citizens or voters” were linked to the term only

16.7% of the time. Consider some of the textual examples from Christian Science

Monitor in 1968 and The New York Times in 1992, both of which emphasize how

elections are about candidates, not voters.

Nixon people now are theorizing that Mr. Wallace would turn to their candidate

and give him the election. The further thought advanced aboard this plane is that

Page 61: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

48

Mr. Wallace would be able then to take considerable satisfaction in saying that he

had decided the presidency (Christian Science Monitor, September 14, 1968, p.

A1).

It is a stage of the election where the risks and the rewards are great for a

challenger like Mr. Clinton (New York Times, September 14, 1992, p. A1).

In the first excerpt, news coverage completely dismisses the role played by the voters in

the democratic process by casting a candidate, not the voters, as the agent who “gives”

and decides the highest office holder in the United States. In the second excerpt, the

“risks and rewards” of an election were discussed in terms of a candidate (Clinton or his

opponent) rather than the voters. As illustrated in these examples, the coverage of the

election centered around candidates leaving out voters, who are, in theory, at the center of

the democratic process. Indeed, the term election was directly coupled with a specific

candidate (e.g., “Truman’s election,” “Bush’s re-election”) 12.4% of the time.

Next, the act of electoral participation, vote and voting, and the actors, voters and

electorates, were also linked to “candidates or politicians” most of the time, 75% and

71%, respectively. As in the case of election, these terms were directly linked to

candidates (e.g., “vote for Ike,” “voting for Gore”), and were often cast as subordinate to

candidates (e.g., “The Nixon voters who chose Walker,” “Hispanic voters for the Bush

ticket”). It was far less common to see the voters and the electoral process connected to

ideas, groups or government outside of an association to a specific candidate. As

illustrated, there is a tendency to encourage newspaper readers to believe that the act of

Page 62: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

49

voting and the process of elections are most closely tied to candidates (at the expense of

issues, government or other concerns) and voters are sidelined in our concept of the

democratic process.

The candidate-centered coverage in the news during political campaign periods is

not a surprise to the scholars in the field of political communication. Bennett (1988), for

instance, contends that news media tend to personalize the coverage giving “preference

to the individual actors and human interest angles in events while downplaying

institutional and political considerations that establish the social contexts of those events”

(p. 26). While it may not be unexpected to see this candidate-centered coverage in the

news, it is still problematic that the key terms pertaining to democratic participation—

vote, voter, voting, election, and electorate—are all tucked under the personalization of

elite candidates rather than developed in discussions of citizens, issues or government.

When readers encounter this type of coverage, they may be prompted to think of

elections as an activity for politicians rather than citizens, which may lead to considering

themselves as an outsider to the democratic process. Furthermore, by downplaying

institutional context and real policy issues at hand, it makes it harder for citizens to grasp

how the political system really works and what is at stake for them (Bennett, 1988).

Kernell’s (1997) study on candidate-centered politics points to the potential

danger of this type of coverage as well. According to Kernell (1997), when citizens

develop relationships with individual leaders rather than institutions or political

processes, the political system turns into a volatile marketplace “whose currency of

exchange increasingly is public opinion” (p. 139) and produce more “volatile policy

Page 63: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

50

outcomes” (p. 223). Moreover, when individual leaders are cast as “the fount from which

the answers to the nation’s problems flow” (p. 45), public expectations of politicians

become unrealistically high. When these unrealistic expectations are not met, it could

lead to cynicism among the public. That is, when journalists associate elections and

electoral participation predominantly with candidates and politicians, it may contribute to

volatility and cynicism.

To sum, voters who are at the center of the democratic process, theoretically at

least, have been put to the side in electoral news coverage, and the electoral process has

been cast as revolving around individual candidates over the years.

VOTERS ARE RARELY REMINDED OF DEMOCRATIC RESPONSIBILITIES A third qualitative pattern reveals that voters are often featured in horse race

coverage, and their electoral participation is reduced to a choice between elites rather

than a democratic duty or a right. The frame and assumptions variables uncover these

types of coverage.

The variable frame, which examines whether the terms vote, voter, voting,

election and electorate appeared in an “issue” or “game” frame, shows that all keywords

pertaining to electoral participation were more likely to appear in a game frame (39.0%)

than an issue frame (8.2%, see Table 3.2). When it comes to the term vote, the term was

used in the context of a game 57.3% of the time whereas it appeared in an issue frame

only 5.1% of the time. Similarly, the term election was used in a game frame nearly a half

of the time, 47.3%, while it was used in an issue frame only 6% of the time.

Page 64: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

51

Table 3.2

Electoral Key Terms by Frame Variable

_______________________________________________________________________ Key terms Game Issue Mixed Identity/Other _______________________________________________________________________ Vote(s) 57.3% 5.1% 3.1% 34.4% Voter(s) 34.9 7.3 8.2 49.6 Voting 26.2 12.9 6.0 54.9 Election(s) 47.3 6.0 4.4 42.2 Electorate(s) 25.7 10.4 5.9 58.0 _______________________________________________________________________ Overall 39.0 8.2 5.5 47.3 _______________________________________________________________________

The following excerpts illustrate how electoral key terms were featured in the

game frame coverage in newspapers between 1948 and 2004:

Mr. Nixon’s lead among registered voters now stands at 61% to 33%, with 1% for

other candidates and 5% undecided, as reported Sunday. The previous survey

showed Mr. Nixon leading 64% to 30%, with 6% undecided or voting for other

candidates (Los Angeles Times, October 6, 1972, p. A20)

Bush is also benefiting from two decades of Republican efforts to define the

electorate and plot strategies to win over voter blocs. A key aspect of Bush’s

game plan is to peel away traditional Democratic constituencies, with carefully

Page 65: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

52

targeted mailings and campaign symbols, all based on experience (The

Washington Post, October 4, 1988, p. A1).

Ohio’s 18 electoral votes have been safely in the Texas governor’s column for

some time…. Illinois and its 22 electoral votes should go to Gore, despite an

effort by Bush to steal the state (The Washington Post, November 5, 2000, p. A1).

In each instance above, the election is defined as a game of winning and losing between

elites. Meanwhile, voters and the vote are regarded as pawns of elites rather than

independent actors who are participating in democratic decision making process and

performing their democratic responsibilities.

The assumptions variable also shows a similar pattern. The assumptions variable,

which examines whether the vote is portrayed as a choice, a right or a duty, indicates that

these terms were predominantly featured as a choice (89.1%) rather than as a right (2.7%)

or a duty (0.6%) in news coverage between 1948 and 2004. That is, in the moments

where an assumption behind the vote was offered, the role of the voter is one who is

linked to candidates (not causes) and their action is narrowed to a choice between these

candidates (rather than a grander act involving democratic responsibilities).

Patterson (1993) argues that this type of coverage makes voters passive spectators

in the democratic process. Patterson (1993) states, “when voters encounter game-centered

stories, they behave more like spectators than participants in the election, responding, if

at all, to the status of the race, not to what the candidates represent” (p. 89). In his

analysis of election stories on the front page of the New York Times, Patterson found that

Page 66: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

53

the game schema outnumbered the policy schema consistently and increasingly since the

mid-1960s, reaching over 80% after the mid-1970s. While my data has a specific focus

on key terms of electoral participation and has yielded fewer “game” frame and more

“identity” frame statements than what Patterson found in his study of overall election

stories,9 the serious implications of this type of reporting remain the same. By both subtly

and explicitly putting the voters to the side, the newspapers prime the readers to act as

spectators in political process.

Cappella and Jamieson (1997) point to another possible implication of this type of

coverage. They argue that strategic and conflict coverage often found in game framing is

detrimental to people’s political understanding. Cappella and Jamieson (1997) write,

Strategy and conflict coverage activate the judgment operator that tallies evidence

about the self-interested nature of the political process and its players. Those

consuming a lot of news that is structured this way are as a result more cynical.

But it is not necessary to consume a lot of strategic news to see its effects on how

the actions of politicians are interpreted (p. 169).

Thus, by portraying electoral activities as a game, newspapers discount democratic

responsibilities involved in the electoral process and create a passive and cynical

citizenry. The results from the variables time and quality discussed in the following

section add to such concern.

9 This is partly because the term voting was often used as an adjective. Initially, I envisioned that the term voting would capture the action of casting a ballot. After coding for grammar, however, I found that this term was more likely to be used as an adjective (44.4%) than as a verb (37.3%). When the term was used as an adjective, the most frequently used phrase was “voting record” (18.5%) followed by “voting booth”

Page 67: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

54

VOTERS ARE NOT CONNECTED TO THE PAST OR EACH OTHER A fourth qualitative pattern shows that voters are not featured as a reflective or

collective body. Indeed, the time and quality variables illuminate that voters are not

connected to the past or to each other.

The time variable traces whether voters and their electoral participation were

placed in the “past,” “present,” “future” or “across time.” As Table 3.3 shows, the key

terms examined here were mostly placed in the “present” (86.7%). In terms of voters and

electorate, they were placed in the “present” over 90% of the time (91.8% and 94.1%,

respectively). While there is a general tendency in the newspapers to focus on the present

(Barnhurst & Mutz, 1997), the emphasis on the present is quite strong in my data. This is

because the coverage of elections and electoral participation has been centered around

specific candidates and placed in a game frame rather than an issue frame (as discussed

earlier), and the focus of the coverage became who is winning or losing now rather than

reflection to the past or direction to the future.

(9.5%), “voting age” (8.5%), “voting bloc” (6.0%), “voting rights” (6.0%), “voting pattern” (5.5%), “voting machine” (4.5%) and “voting group” (3.0%), and these words tended to appear in an identity condition.

Page 68: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

55

Table 3.3

Electoral Key Terms by Time Variable

_______________________________________________________________________ Key terms Past Present Future Across Time _______________________________________________________________________ Vote(s) 8.9% 84.4% 0.0% 6.7% Voter(s) 2.9 91.8 0.0 5.3 Voting 3.8 82.9 0.2 13.1 Election(s) 8.7 82.0 0.4 8.9 Electorate(s) 0.9 94.1 0.6 4.4 _______________________________________________________________________ Overall 5.2 86.7 0.2 7.9 _______________________________________________________________________

Iyengar’s (1991) study on thematic versus episodic framing points to a potential

implication of these presentistic types of news coverage. Iyengar suggests that while

thematic news coverage (which emphasizes broader contexts surrounding issues or

events) tends to promote a shared sense of responsibility and collective action, episodic

coverage (which focuses on specific instances or individuals) might deter the possibility

of imagining a shared responsibility. While Iyengar’s study focuses on social problems

rather than political campaigns and electoral participation, his insights are pertinent here.

By focusing on the present and placing individual candidates at the center of coverage

rather than discussing broader historical contexts surrounding issues and electoral

Page 69: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

56

participation, the newspapers discourage readers to see their place and responsibilities

beyond what is offered in the coverage.

The variable quality looks at characteristics attached to electoral participation.

The data here suggest that voters are not only fragmentized in time, but also in space. As

illustrated in Figure 3.5, voters and their electoral participation were most likely to be

treated as “numbers” (13.8%) followed by “demographics” (13.1%), “candidate specific”

(5.7%), “potential” (3.6%), “partisan” (2.2%), “undecided” (1.2%), “affect” (1.2%),

“independent” (0.9%) and “intellectual” (0.7%). Among all key terms examined here, the

vote was used most often as “numbers” (29.3% of the time—e.g., “Carter is behind by 45

votes”) followed by “demographics” (19.3%—e.g. “Jewish vote”). The voters were most

likely to be given an attribute of “demographics” (24.7%) followed by “numbers”

(16.9%) while electorate was described as “numbers” (26%) most frequently followed by

“demographics” (16.6%)

Page 70: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

57

Figure 3.5. Electoral Key Terms by Quality Variable10

_______________________________________________________________________ All Keywords (n = 2138) Vote (n = 450)

Voter (n = 450) Electorate (n = 338)

_______________________________________________________________________

The predominance of voters as “numbers” and “demographics” (as opposed to

emotional or intellectual beings) in news coverage is closely linked to the prevalence of

game framing discussed in the previous section. When journalists cover elections as a

10 The percentages of the quality variable are significantly lower for all keywords combined compared to the terms vote, voter, and electorate because the terms voting and election did not contain much adjectival phrases. As discussed in footnote 3, the term voting was often used as an adjective itself, therefore, it did not contain other adjectives. The term election was rarely featured with adjectives because of the nature of the word. When it is discussed with adjectives, however, it was mostly categorized as “candidate specific,” 12.4% (e.g., “Nixon’s election,” “Bush’s re-election,” etc). Because of the modest number of instances, individual results for the term voting and election are not displayed here.

Page 71: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

58

game, voters are often portrayed as targets of elites, transforming voters into mere

“numbers” and “demographics.”11

Consider the following excerpt from The Chicago Tribune (1976, September 12).

Here, Illinois voters are divided into several demographic subgroups—Catholics,

Protestants, young, old, blacks—and are reduced to numbers in a game frame:

The Tribune Poll indicates some problems for Carter among major groups of

Illinois voters. For instance, Catholic voters - who cast 30 per cent of Illinois’

votes - were found currently to prefer Ford to Carter by 50 to 37 per cent.

Traditionally seen as Democratic voters, Catholics also prefer Republican

Thompson to Howlett, but less strongly than they choose Republican Ford over

Carter. Voters who consider themselves Protestants - 55 per cent of the electorate

- reflect the statewide preference in choosing Ford over Carter by 44 to 39 per

cent. Carter makes up the seeming difference by taking a strong share of Illinois

voters who identify their religious preferences in other terms.

When voters are divided by age, Carter emerges as a narrow favorite of those

under 30; he evenly splits voters 30 to 49 years old with Ford; and loses those 50

and older to Ford by 11 points. Thompson is the favorite of voters in each of the

age groups - leading Howlett by 13 points among those under 30, by 31 points

among 30-to-49-year-old voters, by 23 points among those 50 and older. The only

11 A cross-tabulation of “numbers” and the context variable indicates that numeric qualities of vote, voter, and electorate most frequently appeared in a game frame (69.5%) followed by an “identity” condition (25.4%), an issue frame (2.5%), and a mixed category (2.5%). A cross-tabulation of “demographics” and the context variable indicates that demographic categorization of vote, voter, and electorate also appeared

Page 72: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

59

group of voters which disproportionately prefers Carter and Howlett over their

opponents are blacks. Carter wins an impressive 74 per cent of the blacks,

compared with Ford’s 10 per cent. Blacks favor Howlett to Thompson by 47 to 27

per cent (p. A1).

This type of coverage is complicated for the democratic process at least for two

reasons. First, as discussed earlier, by featuring voters in a game frame, newspapers turn

voters into passive spectators. Second, by presenting voters as divided pieces, this type of

coverage makes it hard for the readers to imagine a collective body of voters who strive

for a collective goal. As Figure 3.6 displays, voters and their vote were sliced and diced

into various subcategories such as gender (e.g., women voters), age (e.g., young voters,

voters between 30 to 49 years old, older voters, etc), race/ethnicity (e.g., African

American voters), religion (e.g., Catholic voters, anti-Catholic voters, Evangelical voters,

Protestant voters, etc), region (e.g., Chicago voters, suburban voters, rural area voters,

etc), and occupation/income (e.g., middle class blue-collar voters, voters with an annual

income of $12,500 to $35,000, etc).

most often in a game frame (56.5%) followed by an “identity” condition (33.7%), an issue frame (5.9%), and a mixed category (3.9%).

Page 73: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

60

Figure 3.6. Subcategories in “Demographics” (in Quality Variable)

_______________________________________________________________________ Vote (n = 60) Voter & Electorate (n = 197)

_______________________________________________________________________

A closer look at the demographic data points to another concern. Specifically,

specific demographic groups that were targeted by elites were more likely to be featured

in electoral news coverage than those that were left out of the elites’ mobilization efforts.

Consider the “race/ethnicity” subcategory. When these data are examined for the term

“voter,” African-American voters were most frequently featured in electoral news

coverage over the years (36.8%) followed by Jewish voters (26.3%), White voters

(15.8%), and Hispanic voters (2.6%). The following excerpts show how certain groups

(Jewish voters in the examples) are featured as “crucial” and “influential” while leaving

out others:

Carter campaign strategists believe that Jewish voters, while relatively few in

number compared with the overall electorate, will be crucial to Mr. Carter’s

chances of carrying certain key states in November (New York Times, September

9, 1980, p. A1).

Page 74: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

61

Nationally, the Jewish vote is small but influential, representing about 3 percent to

4 percent of the electorate and concentrated in a handful of states (Washington

Post, October 25, 2004, p. A1).

Practically, it may be the case that the certain groups of voters may be “crucial” or more

“influential” for candidates to win or lose their elections, and elites target their

mobilization efforts to specific kinds of people to maximize the likelihood of their

success rather than mobilizing all eligible voters (Rosenstone & Hansen, 1993).

However, these types of coverage not only encourage fragmentation of voters (which

makes it hard to imagine collective goals), but also promote uneven representations of

voters in the news, and possibly, at the polls.

To sum, the data suggest that voters are fragmented in time (mostly situated in the

present) and space (divided into pieces), and such coverage may make it hard for readers

to envision a collective citizenry, and potentially harm the equal representation in the

news and at the ballot box.

CONCLUSION Based on the aggregate data drawn from the content analysis of newspaper

coverage of presidential elections between 1948 and 2004, this chapter examined how

often the key terms pertaining to electoral participation have appeared in American

newspapers over the past sixty years and what kind of attributes have been most

commonly attached to voters and voting in American newspapers from 1948 to 2004.

Page 75: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

62

Quantitative results show that the terms pertaining to voters and their electoral

participation have been constantly featured in newspaper reporting, and the term voter

has been increasingly relied upon in newspaper reporting over the years. Qualitative

findings reveal, however, that voters and their electoral participation have been cast in

“thin” and not in detailed ways. Qualitative patterns suggest that over the past sixty years,

voters are not featured in a positive manner, are pushed aside in the democratic process

and are largely coupled with candidates rather than issues, government or other concerns,

are rarely reminded of their rights or duties involved in the democratic process, and are

featured as disconnected from the past and each other. I argue that these types of news

coverage make it hard for the citizens to imagine the power of their vote, its importance

in democratic governance and its connection to the legitimacy of the system.

While this chapter provides a broad overview of the portrayals of American

electoral politics, the next chapter looks underneath these aggregate patterns and

investigates trends over time in how voters and their electoral participation have been

featured in American newspapers between 1948 and 2004. Chapter Four illustrates how

the portrayals of voters and voting have changed over time, and discusses the potential

links to citizens’ political socialization processes.

Page 76: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

63

Chapter 4. Portrayals of American Electoral Participation

(1948-1968, 1972-2000 and 2004)

Chapter Three reported aggregate data on how key electoral terms—vote, voter,

voting, electorate, and election—have been used in newspaper coverage over the past

sixty years. This chapter looks underneath these patterns and examines if and how news

coverage of these terms has changed over time. The following research questions are

examined in this chapter:

RQ 1: Have portrayals of electoral key terms changed over time?

RQ 2: If so, how?

Based on an extensive content analysis of electoral key terms (n = 2,138), results

show that the portrayals of voters and their electoral participation have shifted over the

past sixty years. Three distinct patterns emerged in analyses of the data. Specifically,

voters have been portrayed as:

(1) Closely aligned with political parties and empowered (1948 to 1968);

(2) Subsumed under public opinion polls as pawns of elites (1972 to 2000); and

(3) Faced with challenges in the electoral process (2004).

The following chapter illustrates these patterns and addresses potential

implications of these portrayals on citizens’ political socialization process. I argue that

the coverage between 1948 and 1968 may hold cues that could empower voters

particularly in a way that was not witnessed in the other years examined.

Page 77: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

64

NEWS COVERAGE OF ELECTORAL PARTICIPATION: 1948–1968

Relative to the recent past, Americans were regarded to be politically active

between post-World War II and the height of the Civil Rights era. Studies report that

American people during this period expressed the highest levels of political efficacy since

World War II and more people were engaged in political activities than any other period

(American National Election Studies; Putnam, 2000; Schudson, 1998). Writing in the late

1950s, political scholar Robert Lane (1959) observed that “on the whole it seems clear

that more people, proportionate to the population (or even more men proportionate to the

male population), engage in more political activities today than has been true for at least

fifty years” (p. 93).

When the data in this project are examined over time, the findings suggest that

newspaper coverage of voters and their electoral participation between 1948-1968

reflects this robust political milieu. Results from the following variables—association,

mobilization agents, goals, rewards, role and behavior—show that voters were closely

aligned with political parties and featured as empowered participants in the democratic

process.

First, the association variable illustrates that voters and their electoral

participation were closely linked to political parties. Figure 4.1 shows that coverage

between 1948 and 1968 had the highest level of party references compared to later

periods (1948-1968 = 35.8%, 1972-2000 = 22.8%, 2004 = 20.7%—“party” association).

The figure also illustrates how the act (vote and voting), the actors (voter and electorate),

and the system of electoral participation (election) all follow a similar pattern.

Page 78: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

65

Figure 4.1. “Party” Associations over Time

_______________________________________________________________________ All Key Terms (n = 2138) Vote & Voting (n = 900)

Voter & Electorate (n = 788) Election (n = 450)

______________________________________________________________________

The following textual examples illustrate how closely voters’ electoral decisions

were tied to political parties between 1948-1968. For instance, one column from The

Christian Science Monitor (1956, October 12) depicted party labels as the center of what

motivates voters.

Laboring men and their wives were in a majority among voters who indicated

they expected to return to the straight Democratic ticket.… Very few of the

voters sampled gave Mr. Stevenson credit for their re-conversion. Most were

Page 79: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

66

returning because of what they felt the Democratic Party, rather than its

candidate, stood for. There was for instance, the janitor who said: “In my

opinion, Ike’s the bigger man, but I’m going to vote for the party of the

working man. I don’t think ‘Ike’ knows how much his Republican boys are

helping the rich and forgetting about us” (p. A1).

Notice how in this example voters are tied to “the straight Democratic ticket,” drawn to

“what the Democratic Party stood for,” voting for “the party of working man,” and

frustrated by the “Republican boys.”

The frequent use of party labels in the news reflects politicians’ reliance on these

labels in their campaign rhetoric as well. During these years, journalists quoted

candidates as claiming that political parties were important. Consider how the words of

candidates Harry Truman, John F. Kennedy and Richard Nixon, quoted in these columns

as they are, underscore the important connections between voters and parties in the news.

First, take Harry Truman’s words as paraphrased in The Chicago Tribune (1948,

September 7):

Hinting at a campaign strategy of ignoring the two rebel segments of the old

Democratic party, Mr. Truman said the American electorate has a choice

between the two major political parties. He accused the Republican party of

wanting to weaken labor’s position (p. A1).

Then, John F. Kennedy words in The Chicago Tribune (1960, November 5):

Backstopped by a howling audience of 25,000 persons, Sen. John F. Kennedy

last night pleaded with a nationwide television audience not to forget party

Page 80: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

67

labels in electing the next President on Tuesday… He said the voter’s decision

Tuesday should be not only between candidates, but between two parties.

Americans should choose between Nixon with the Republican party, and

Kennedy with the Democratic party, he said (p. A1).

And consider these statements by Richard Nixon quoted in The Los Angeles Times (1960,

September 25):

“Those who wrote the party platform in Los Angeles forfeited the right to ask

for millions of Democrats to vote for them in November,” Nixon said. “The

party of Galbraith, Schlesinger and Bowles is not the party of Jefferson,

Jackson and Wilson—and I don’t believe it is the party of Jackson, Miss.,

either” (p. A2).

As seen above, newspapers during this period were more likely to depict elections as a

choice between the Democratic Party versus the Republican Party and emphasize the

divisions between them compared to later periods. In doing so, the news reports during

this period portrayed electoral participation as something inseparable from political

parties.

Second, voters and their electoral participation were featured as highly mobilized

in news coverage between 1948-1968. As Figure 4.2 depicts, news coverage during this

period contained the highest level of mobilization agents (1948-1968 = 38.2%, 1972-

2000 = 29.2%, 2004 = 24.0%) indicating that citizens’ electoral participation was more

likely to be featured as solicited during this period compared to more recent periods.

Page 81: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

68

Figure 4.2. Mobilization Variable over Time

_______________________________________________________________________ All Key Terms (n = 2138) Vote & Voting (n = 900)

Voter & Electorate (n = 788) Election (n = 450)

_______________________________________________________________________

The majority of these mobilization efforts were linked to elites (78.5% of the

mobilization efforts featured between 1948-1968 was made by candidates and political

parties) while other entities (such as citizens and interest groups) were also described as

mobilizing forces (21.5%). The following excerpts illustrate how voter participation was

solicited during this period.

Page 82: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

69

The Los Angeles Times features mobilization efforts by a political campaign:

“We are going to directly to the voters asking for their volunteer participation…

This means that instead of having flashy headquarters on main streets, we will be

operating from store fronts, homes and offices, where the voters live” (October 8,

1968, p. A3).

The New York Times illustrates registration efforts by the unions:

The unions have undertaken the state registration drive, and $250,000 will

probably be spent in an effort to add 495,000 voters to the rolls (September 8,

1968, p. A81).

And, The Washington Post reports mobilization efforts by citizens:

Five Washington women will embark on a “midwest caravan-on-wheels” today,

to woo votes for the Democratic ticket of Johnson and Humphrey in four states”

(October 19, 1964, p. C2).

As seen, voters were featured as directly asked to participate in the electoral process,

wooed by other citizens for their participation, and money was spent on to register them.

These data are compelling because studies suggest that citizens tend to develop more

positive attitudes toward politics and participate more often when their involvement is

solicited (Leighley, 1995; Rosenstone & Hansen, 1993). Indeed, many scholars argue that

the decline of mobilization has contributed to the decline in voter turnout since the 1960s.

It is notable that newspapers discussed higher levels of solicitation in their coverage

during the height of political participation—years 1948-1968.

Page 83: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

70

Next, along with the mobilization variable, results from the goals and rewards

variables indicate that news coverage during this period provided larger incentives for

citizens to engage in electoral participation. These variables tracked if goals and rewards

of electoral participation were mentioned in the news, and if so, whether they were broad

(collective) or narrow (private) in nature. As Figure 4.3 indicates, this time period

featured the highest levels of these measures attached to electoral participation (goals

featured: 1948-1968 = 22.6%, 1972-2000 = 16.1%, 2004 = 7.3%; rewards featured 1948-

1968 = 10.7%, 1972-2000 = 8.2%, 2004 = 2.0%).

Furthermore, the stakes of electoral participation featured in news coverage were

higher than that of more recent years. As Figure 4.3 shows, voters and their participation

were linked to “broad goals” and “collective reward” roughly twice as much as more

recent years (“broad goal,” 1948-1968 = 7.5%, 1972-2000 = 3.8%, 2004 = 2.7%; “public

reward,” 1948-1968 = 7.1%, 1972-2000 = 4.6%, 2004 = 1.3%).

Figure 4.3. Goals and Rewards Variables over Time

_______________________________________________________________________ Goals Rewards

_______________________________________________________________________

Page 84: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

71

Consider how broad goals and public rewards are emphasized during this period in the

following excerpts from The Chicago Tribune and The Washington Post:

The President (Truman) expressed the hope that everyone that everyone who is

eligible to vote will do so tomorrow and “vote in the interest and welfare of the

free nations of the world, of the great country of which they are a part, and in their

own interest.” He said their interests are at stake because “it means prosperity at

home and peace in the world.” “If they vote their sentiments, as I think they are

going to vote, the country will be safe for another four years” (The Chicago

Tribune, November 4, 1952, p. A2).

“I think everybody ought to vote, and frankly, I think it enough that if a fellow felt

he had to vote against me I still would rather he voted than not vote at all...I still

believe that the neglect of the freedom of voting is the first step toward losing

your freedom” (The Washington Post, September 12, 1956, p. A1).

“I don’t think the average voter is particularly concerned about what I would call

a nitpicking type of issue. In other words, something of a local nature. The

average American I have run into is concerned about the type of government, the

type of country we are going to have. They are concerned about peace in the

world” (The Washington Post, October 13, 1964, p. A2).

By occasionally linking voting with grander principles and purposes such as “freedom”

and “peace in the world” as opposed to narrow goals and rewards such as a tax cut, news

coverage in this period offered the readers a larger purpose for engaging in the

Page 85: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

72

democratic process. Consequently, voters were cast as more powerful agents who are

entrusted with such larger tasks as “making the country safe” and ensuring “peace in the

world” than in later years examined.

The role variable underscores this pattern. As discussed in the previous chapter,

newspapers rarely portray voters and their electoral engagement in a positive way, let

alone as “a part of solution.” While it is critical to acknowledge that the numbers here are

modest, the data show that during this period voters and their electoral participation were

more likely to be featured as a “part of the solution” compared to later periods (1948-

1968 = 2.9%, 1972-2000 = 0.7%, 2004 = 1.3%).

Finally, the behavior variable reveals that voters were most likely to be featured

“in action” during this period. While the potency variable, which measures whether

voters are portrayed as actors or recipients of certain action, reports that voters have been

featured as actors at a constant level over the past sixty years (1948-1968 = 34.7%, 1972-

2000 = 35.6%, 2004 = 35.0%—“actor”), the behavior variable reveals a subtle distinction

in how these actors have been presented in the news. Figure 4.3 illustrates that when

voters and electorates were featured as actors in news coverage, they were depicted as

acting body in this period more frequently than later periods (1948-1968 = 19.8%, 1972-

2000 = 14.2%, 2004 = 15.7%). For instance, they were featured as “setting the issue,”

“giving state offices, senate seats, congressional majorities,” “participating in elections,”

and of course, “casting a vote.”

Page 86: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

73

Figure 4.4. Behavior Variable over Time12

As discussed above, the data presented thus far suggest that the coverage of

electoral participation during this period was more empowering compared to recent years.

While voters were featured less frequently (quantitatively) than later periods during this

time (see Chapter 3), when mentioned, their participation was solicited, they were

entrusted with broader purposes and influence, and they were depicted in action.

In short, voters were given more incentives to engage in the electoral process in

news reports and were featured as engaged in the democratic process. It is important to

keep in mind that news coverage of electoral campaigns during this period was not

necessarily an ideal form of reporting voters and their electoral participation (i.e., even

during this robust period, trends discussed in Chapter 3 were prevalent). Nevertheless, it

is worth noting that when voter turnout was at a relatively high level, the news reports of

electoral participation featured more empowered citizenry. It may well be that these

12 The data are based on the terms voter and electorate, n = 788.

Page 87: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

74

empowered representations of voters simply reflect the efficacious public of this period.

One wonders though, if the representation of efficacious voters in the news could create,

as well as reflect, efficacious voters.

NEWS COVERAGE OF ELECTORAL PARTICIPATION: 1972–2000 After the vibrant Civil Rights era of the 1960s, the high level of political

engagement during the 1960s gradually subsided and voter turnout steadily declined (see

Figure 3.1). As addressed in Chapter One, voter turnout began to decline when citizens

were given more opportunities to participate in the electoral process than ever before.

That is, even though the enactment of the Voting Rights Act in 1964 opened the door for

African Americans to participate in electoral politics and the 26th amendment to the

United States Constitution in 1971 gave young adults (over the age of 18) the right to

vote for the first time in American history (Wattenberg, 2002), turnout has not increased

since the 1960s.

The decline in voter turnout also coincides with an increase in the importance of

the role played by the news media in electoral process (Patterson, 1993). While electoral

politics revolved around political parties in earlier days (Wattenberg, 2002), the primary

system gradually took the institutional importance away from the parties in the candidate-

selection process (Polsby, 1983). As a result, both voters and candidates began to look to

the news media for information and support replacing the role played by political parties

(Kernell, 1997; Schudson, 1998).

Page 88: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

75

Interestingly, just as the role of the news media became essential in the electoral

process, portrayals of voters and their electoral participation took an unfortunate turn.

The variables mobilization, goals, rewards, and role indicate that news coverage of

voters and their electoral participation became “thinner” during this period, and

association and quality variables reveal that voters became subsumed under opinion polls

and increasingly cast as pawns of elites in electoral news coverage.

First, the empowered portrayals of voters in news coverage witnessed in 1948-

1968 began to dissipate during this period. As presented in the previous section, the level

of mobilization featured in the news dropped by 9 percentage points from 38.2% in 1948-

1968 to 29.2% in 1972-2000 (see Figure 4.2). Fewer direct incentives for electoral

participation were provided in news coverage (goals featured: 1948-1968 = 22.6%, 1972-

2000 = 16.1%; rewards featured 1948-1968 = 10.7%, 1972-2000 = 8.2%), and when they

were provided, the scope of them became narrower during this period (“broad goal,”

1948-1968 = 7.5%, 1972-2000 = 3.8%; “public reward,” 1948-1968 = 7.1%, 1972-2000

= 4.6%, see Figure 4.3). Moreover, the role of voters as “a part of solution” featured in

news coverage almost completely disappeared (1948-1968 = 2.9%, 1972-2000 = 0.7%).

Second, newspapers began to feature voters as subsumed under opinion polls. The

association variable indicates that while voters and their electoral participation were

closely tied to political parties in 1948-1968 (Figure 4.1), they became increasingly tied

to opinion polls during this period (Figure 4.5). The terms vote and voting were linked to

opinion polls more than twice as much as earlier period (1948-1968 = 6.4%, 1972-2000 =

16.7%, 2004 = 3.3%) and the rate quintupled for the term election during this period

Page 89: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

76

(1948-1968 = 2.2%, 1972-2000 = 12.5%, 2004 = 0%). When it comes to the terms voters

and electorate, the rate of association doubled, and they were linked to opinion polls over

one third of the time (1948-1968 = 17.7%, 1972-2000 = 34.0%, 2004 = 26.7%).

Figure 4.5. “Opinion Poll” Associations over Time

_______________________________________________________________________ All Key Terms (n = 2138) Vote & Voting (n = 900)

Voter & Electorate (n = 788) Election (n = 450)

_______________________________________________________________________

An increase in opinion poll coverage mirrors what was happening in the field of

journalism during this period. The 1970s saw the rise of precision journalism—“the

application of social and behavioral science research methods to the practice of

Page 90: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

77

journalism” (Meyer, 2002, p. 2)—and media polls proliferated in the 1980s (Meyer,

2002). Because opinion polls offer a number of characteristics that are newsworthy in

nature—timely, concrete, topical, reliable and considered objective (Crespi, 1980; Paletz

et al., 1980)—the press began to incorporate them profusely in their election coverage.

The rise of precision journalism during this period is also reflected in an increase

in references to experts in the news. Although the overall rate of references to experts in

electoral coverage are not as substantial as opinion polls or other entities (such as

candidates and political parties), the percentage of references to experts associated with

voters and their electoral participation in the news more than doubled since 1948-1968

(1948-1968 = 3.1%, 1972-2000 = 8.8%, 2004 = 9.3%).

While references to opinion polls and experts in electoral news coverage could be

beneficial to the democratic process (Meyer, 2002; Steele, 1995), the way journalists

incorporated them in electoral news coverage during this period did little to improve

citizens’ political understanding. A cross-tabulation of frame and association variables

reveals that when electoral key terms were linked to “opinion polls” between 1972-2000,

they were far more likely to be featured in a “game” frame (59.0%) than an “issue” frame

(6.6%), and experts were featured in “game” frame coverage seven times more than

“issue” frame coverage (34.0% vs. 4.7%). The following textual examples from The Los

Angeles Times and The New York Times illustrate how electoral key terms were featured

with opinion polls and experts in horserace coverage.

Mondale was close to Reagan in the polls only in Iowa, where a Des Moines

Register survey conducted over the four days immediately following the first

Page 91: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

78

presidential debate on Oct. 7 showed Reagan favored by 46% of registered voters

and Mondale by 41%. In Ohio, which has 23 electoral votes, Reagan held a 17-

point lead over Mondale in a poll by the University of Akron for the Akron

Beacon Journal. (The Los Angeles Times, October 30, 1984, p. A1).

Mr. Wirthlin predicted, based on his polls, that Mr. Reagan would get 59 percent

of the national popular vote, plus or minus two points (The New York Times,

November 6, 1984, p. A1).

This coverage links opinion polls and experts strictly to who was winning (Ronald

Reagan) and losing (Walter Mondale) instead of public policy issues or other reasons to

support these candidates. As a result, it does little to contribute in helping citizens make

informed and meaningful electoral decisions.

The quality variable underscores this point. Figure 4.6 illustrates that voters and

the electorate were more likely to be cast as “numbers” and “demographics” in this

period than other periods (“number,” 1948-1968 = 13.7%, 1972-2000 = 25.2%, 2004 =

15.0%; “demographics,” 1948-1968 = 18.5%, 1972-2000 = 22.7%, 2004 = 20.0%). The

increase in opinion polls combined with game framing reduced the role of voters into

mere numbers and striped their roles as active participants in the democratic process.

Page 92: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

79

Figure 4.6. “Numbers” and “Demographic” (in Quality Variable) over Time13

_______________________________________________________________________

_______________________________________________________________________

In short, while the roles of voters and the news media became ever more

important after 1968 and voters were featured more frequently than during the earlier

period (see Figure 3.2), voters during this period were not cast in a very meaningful way.

Compared to news coverage in 1948-1968, voters and their electoral participation were

less likely to be featured as mobilized, and their goals, rewards, and roles in the

democratic process diminished in the news coverage. Indeed, American voters were

increasingly linked to public opinion polls and featured as mere numbers, and ultimately,

pawns of elites. When the role of the news media became indispensable and its influence

became crucial in electoral process, the attributes of voters and their electoral

participation became thinner and the increased quantification of voters took the life out of

the empowered electorate featured in 1948-1968.

13 The data are based on the terms voter and electorate, n = 788. Variable: quality.

Page 93: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

80

NEWS COVERAGE OF ELECTORAL PARTICIPATION: 2004 Following the complicated presidential election in 2000, the references to

electoral key terms skyrocketed in 2004 (Figure 3.2). This year featured unique trends

qualitatively and quantitatively. As such, it is addressed independent of the other time

periods, here.

On one hand, some of the data for 2004 follow a trend of thinning treatments over

time (see Figures 4.2 and 4.3). Specifically, the mobilization variable indicates that

voters and their electoral participation were least likely to be featured as solicited in the

news in 2004 (Figure 4.2). Additionally, the goals and rewards variables also reveal that

purposes and benefits of electoral participation became negligible in the news coverage

of the 2004 election (Figure 4.3).

On the other hand, a closer examination of the variables frame, association,

challenge, and assumptions reveals that electoral news coverage in 2004 departs from

earlier years. These variables indicate that electoral news coverage after the controversial

election of 2000 shifted from horserace coverage to news reports that examined electoral

procedures, and began to feature voters as facing challenges in exercising their basic right

to engage in the democratic process.

First, the frame variable reveals that the way journalists portrayed elections in

2004 shifted away from horserace coverage. The results shown in Figure 4.7 indicate that

the use of “game” framing in 2004 dropped from 40.5% in 1972-2000 to 28.7% in 2004,

reaching all time low since 1948 (1948-1968 = 38.6%, 1972-2000 = 40.5%, 2004 =

28.7%). While all electoral key terms show a decline in a “game” framing in 2004, those

Page 94: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

81

pertaining to the act and the system of the electoral process show the most visible decline.

In terms of vote and voting, the rate of a “game” framing declined from 42.7% in 1972-

2000 to 31.7% in 2004. In terms of the system of electoral process (election), a “game”

frame dropped to less than a half of what was witnessed in 1972-2000 (50.0% vs. 23.3%).

Figure 4.7. Frame Variable over Time

_______________________________________________________________________ All Key Terms (n = 2138) Vote & Voting (n = 900)

Voter & Electorate (n = 788) Election (n = 450)

_______________________________________________________________________

The association variable points to a similar pattern. Figure 4.5 illustrates that the

spike in opinion poll references in electoral news coverage in 1972-2000 (22.8%)

Page 95: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

82

dropped by 10 percentage points in 2004 (12%). When it comes to the act (vote and

voting) and the system of electoral participation (election), references to opinion polls

became minuscule in 2004 (vote and voting, 20004 = 3.3%; election, 2004 = 0%, see

Figure 4.5). Furthermore, these terms became more closely linked to “government” in

2004. While the act (vote and voting) and the system of electoral participation (election)

were linked to “government” only 2-3% of the time in 1948-2000, the rate quintupled in

2004 (vote and voting, 2004 = 11.7%; election, 2004 = 10.0%, see Figure 4.8).

Figure 4.8. “Government” Associations over Time

_______________________________________________________________________ All Key Terms (n = 2138) Vote & Voting (n = 900)

Voter & Electorate (n = 788) Election (n = 450)

_______________________________________________________________________

Page 96: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

83

The challenge variable, which assesses obstacles engaging in electoral activities,

also illuminates how the nature of electoral news coverage changed in 2004 (Figure 4.9).

While newspapers rarely featured electoral participation as challenging in 1948-2000,

they began to present electoral participation in such a way in 2004 (1948-1968 = 3.9%,

1972-2000 = 1.8%, 2004 = 12%). The terms voters and the electorate were cast as facing

challenges in engaging in the electoral process in 2004 more often than any other periods

(1948-1968 = 5.6%, 1972-2000 = 1.3%, 2004 = 13.3%). The act of voting was featured

as a challenge to citizens (1948-1968 = 4.4%, 1972-2000 = 3.3%, 2004 = 10.0%), and the

term election was portrayed as challenged in the news (1948-1968 = 0.6%, 1972-2000 =

0%, 2004 = 13.3%) at this time, as well.

Page 97: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

84

Figure 4.9. Challenge Variable over Time

_______________________________________________________________________ All Key Terms (n = 2138) Vote & Voting (n = 900)

Voter & Electorate (n = 788) Election (n = 450)

_______________________________________________________________________

The following excerpts show how voters and their electoral participation were

reported as being challenged in 2004. Whether it was a power outage, redistricting, a

hurricane or the miscounting of votes, voters were faced with challenges in engaging in

the democratic process.

The Atlanta Constitution (October 16, 2004) features instances of registration

problems:

Page 98: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

85

Between 1.5 and 3 million Americans may not have been able to vote in 2000

because of registration problems, a U.S. Census survey estimated. The number of

voters turned away in Georgia in 2000 has never been determined. But Cox said

she knows of a situation in which some Atlanta University Center students were

denied their vote because a power outage had kept their names from being

properly entered into the computer database (p. D1).

The Los Angeles Times (October 19, 2004) reports problems with a provisional ballot:

In what activists have called a setback for low-income Floridians, the state

Supreme Court ruled Monday that people who cast a provisional ballot at the

wrong precinct here are not entitled to have their votes counted… Activists on

Monday said the ballot law unconstitutionally disenfranchised voters who did not

know their polling place. They argued that many people have new polling places

because of redistricting or might have been displaced by a hurricane (p. A14).

The Washington Post (October 16, 2004) touches on miscounting of votes:

After controversies about butterfly ballots and hanging chads in 2000, about three

in four Florida voters say they are very or fairly confident that the votes will be

counted accurately this time. But the quarter of the electorate that remains

skeptical has a dim view of what may happen on Election Day. Seven in 10

among those doubters believe that votes will be deliberately miscounted to help a

candidate, and by 9 to 1, they say any miscounting will favor Bush (p. A1).

As seen in these instances, in the wake of election 2000, journalists often cast voters as

facing challenges in executing their fundamental rights as citizens.

Page 99: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

86

The assumptions variable contributes to this point. Figure 4.10 shows that

electoral participation was more likely to be cast as a “right” in 2004 than in any of the

other periods (1948-1968 = 3.3%, 1972-2000 = 1.8%, 2004 = 6.0%). While the data are

somewhat modest, this pattern is particularly visible in the terms voter, electorate, and

election. The voters and electorates were featured as having a basic right to engage in the

electoral process more often than any other periods (1948-1968 = 1.6%, 1972-2000 =

0.2%, 2004 = 6.7%), and participating in elections was featured as a right as citizens

(1948-1968 = 0.6%, 1972-2000 = 0%, 2004 = 6.7%). It is intriguing that the news

coverage of the most recent election in 2004 portrayed voters as facing challenges in

executing their basic rights as citizens in the democratic process more so than during the

Civil Rights era in the 1960s.

Page 100: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

87

Figure 4.10. Voting as a “Right” (in Assumptions Variable) over Time

_______________________________________________________________________ All Key Terms (n = 2138) Vote & Voting (n = 900)

Voter & Electorate (n = 788) Election (n = 450)

_______________________________________________________________________

To sum, the news coverage of the 2004 election signaled a shift in how news

media portray voters and electoral participation. The horserace coverage gave way to

news reports that examined electoral procedures where voters’ basic rights were

challenged. Unlike the game oriented news coverage where dramatic tension is placed

between elites, this type of news coverage present a new form of drama in which

dramatic tension is placed between voters and the electoral process. While the departure

Page 101: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

88

from horserace coverage is a welcoming trend, implications of this new type of news

coverage remain to be seen.

CONCLUSION

This chapter examined how the electoral key terms have been portrayed in

American newspapers over time. Three distinct patterns emerged from content analysis:

voters were featured as closely linked to political parties and empowered in 1948-1968;

voters were subsumed under opinion polls and increasingly featured as pawns of elites in

1972-2000; and voters were featured as faced with challenges in the electoral process in

2004.

In a sense, these patterns mirror the political milieu of each time period. When

political participation rates and efficacy were comparatively higher, voters were featured

as empowered in electoral news coverage. When participation and efficacy began to

decline, portrayals of voters and their electoral participation in news coverage became

diluted and less meaningful. Following the controversial election of 2000, voters were

featured as facing challenges participating in the electoral process.

These patterns may suggest that news coverage of voters and electoral

participation simply reflect the political moment of each time period. Yet, it is also

possible that the way the news media portray voters have socialized citizens into seeing

their roles as the same way that was advanced in the news. Indeed, the theory of attribute

agenda-setting supports this possibility. Accordingly, the coverage of voters between

1948 and 1968 may hold cues to influence citizens’ political attitudes and engagement.

Page 102: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

89

The next two chapters will examine the effects of these news portrayals on citizens’

political beliefs and attitudes.

Page 103: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

90

Chapter 5. Effects of Portrayals in the News: General Population

Chapter Four revealed that the ways in which print news media featured electoral

key terms have changed over time. Specifically, journalists were more likely to feature

voters as empowered agents in a democratic system in earlier periods (the 1950s and

1960s) compared to more recent periods in which voters were treated as pawns of elites

and public opinion polls (the 1970s to the present). Intriguingly, these patterns coincide

with empirical trends: higher political engagement in the 1950s-1960s and a decline since

the 1970s. Building on the findings in the previous chapter, this chapter investigates the

effects of news framing on citizens’ political attitudes. More specifically, this chapter

tests the effects of the empowered portrayal found in the 1950s and 1960s on citizens’

participatory intentions, political efficacy, information seeking, and trust in news media.

The ways in which news media cover political campaigns have attracted much

attention and criticism from scholars concerned with the role of news media in

democracy (Bennett, 2001; Cappella & Jamieson, 1997; Fallows, 1996; Patterson, 1993).

Scholars suggest that horse race or strategic reporting has increasingly come to define

the national news media coverage of political campaigns, and it has exerted detrimental

effects on democracy and the electoral process. Patterson (1993) argues that news media

portray candidates as “deceptive, hypocritical, and manipulative” (p. 204) and by

reporting candidates and political campaigns this way, the news media have failed to

inform the public about the important issues in electoral process and, even worse,

contributed to general lack of faith in the political system. Cappella and Jamieson (1997)

Page 104: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

91

similarly argue that by focusing on motives behind candidates’ moves and rhetoric rather

than policy issues, the strategic coverage has created cynicism among voters. The authors

test these claims empirically, and conclude that strategic coverage produces political

distrust and cynicism among citizens. Valentino, Beckmann, and Buhr (2001) also reveal

the negative effects of strategic coverage especially among nonpartisans and less

sophisticated voters who are most vulnerable to such coverage and most likely to be

demobilized and alienated by such coverage.

Studies examining the negative effects of news framing during election periods

are abundant and the evidence of negative effects is well documented. However, it is rare

to find studies that examine the potentially positive effects of news framing during

election campaign periods. If news media have talked voters out of political engagement

(by covering elections as a game and voters as pawns of elites), is it possible for news

media to bring them back in (by following the patterns in earlier times and featuring

voters as empowered agents in a democratic system)? And, what are the journalistic and

democratic implications of featuring voters in a more empowered manner? This chapter

takes these questions seriously.

While no one study could test these matters definitively, I explore the possibility

that how voters were depicted in an earlier era (the 1950s to 1960s) might encourage

citizens to participate in electoral politics and create more positive attitudes toward

politics and news media. At a time of declining political engagement, it is important to

analyze what is contributing to the disturbing trend. At the same time, we should ask

what we can do about it and how.

Page 105: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

92

HYPOTHESES One of the most visible markers of democratic engagement is voter turnout rate.

National Election Studies (NES) data indicate that during the 1950s and the 1960s, (when

the voters were featured as empowered actors in print news coverage of the democratic

process), American citizens were voting at a higher rate than more recent years

(Wattenburg, 2002). While this pattern suggests a correlation between the news

portrayals of voters and voter turnout rate, there is no causal relationship examined

between these two phenomena. Therefore, Hypothesis 1 is set to test the causal

relationship between the two. Based on the data on voter turnout and my content analysis,

I predict that when voters are featured as empowered agents in news media, citizens’

intentions to vote will increase.

H1: The empowered voter portrayal will increase intention to vote.

Next, a sense of civic duty has been studied widely as a predictor of electoral

participation. Riker and Ordeshook’s (1968) calculus of voting suggests that a sense of

civic duty is an essential component of citizens’ likelihood of voting. When people feel a

sense of responsibility and obligation, they are more likely to participate in electoral

activities. Other studies demonstrate this relationship as well (Almond & Verba, 1963;

Gerber & Green, 2000; Verba & Nie, 1972). Therefore, I predict that when the news

media feature voters as empowered agents in the democratic process, citizens’ sense of

civic duty will increase.

H2: The empowered voter portrayal will increase sense of civic duty.

Page 106: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

93

Another important element pertaining to political engagement is citizens’ political

efficacy. Political efficacy is “the feeling that individual political action does have, or can

have, an impact upon the political process” (Campbell, Gurin, & MIller, 1954, p. 187).

Political efficacy has two components: internal and external efficacy (Converse, 1972;

Craig, 1979; Craig & Maggiotto, 1982). Internal efficacy represents “beliefs about the

impact a person may have on the political process as a result of their own skills and

confidence” that one can understand and influence political affairs, while external

efficacy represents “the impact a person believes they may have on the political process

as a consequence of institutions’ responsiveness” (Sullivan & Riedel, 2001, pp. 4353-4).

There is a large amount of literature that proposes a positive relationship between

citizens’ sense of political efficacy and electoral participation (Abramson & Aldrich,

1982; Almond & Verba, 1963; Campbell et al., 1960; Rosenstone & Hansen, 1993;

Verba & Nie, 1972).

However, there are few distinct characteristics that separate internal and external

efficacy. Scholars suggest that while internal efficacy is associated with individual’s

characteristics such as socioeconomic background and life stage (Abramson, 1983),

external efficacy is less susceptible to changes in individual’s characteristics, but more to

the changes in the political environment (Sullivan & Riedel, 2001). Accordingly, while

people’s internal efficacy has remained relatively high since the 1960s, external efficacy

has declined steadily over the years (Sullivan & Riedel, 2001). Based on these patterns, I

predict that the empowered voter portrayal will increase citizens’ external efficacy while

not influencing the level of internal efficacy.

Page 107: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

94

H3: The empowered voter portrayal will increase external political efficacy while

exerting no impact on the level of internal efficacy.

For citizens to engage in their electoral activities, they have to perceive that

elections are meaningful and that who gets elected to public office matters. Since the

1970s, however, the degree to which citizens feel connected to a political process has

declined among the public (Putnam, 2000). If the strategic coverage of political

campaigns contributed to general decline in meaning that citizens find in the electoral

process (Valentino et al., 2001), it could be hypothesized that citizens’ perceptions of the

meaningfulness of elections will increase when voters are featured as empowered agents

engaging in the meaningful process of electoral politics.

H4: The empowered voter portrayal will increase meaningfulness of elections.

A healthy democratic system also rests on the notion that citizens seek the

political information necessary to make informed decisions. During political campaign

periods, the news media provides enormous opportunities for citizens to learn about

issues and think about politics. Over the years, however, the strategic campaign coverage

and negativity in news reporting have turned citizens off, and political interest among the

public has declined. Meanwhile, a recent study shows that interests in political

information can be stimulated by a subtle shift in campaign discourse. Kam (2007)

demonstrates that when citizens are reminded of their civic duty in campaign discourse,

they are more likely to devote increased cognitive effort in thinking about politics and

seeking more information about politics in an open-minded way. By instilling in voters a

Page 108: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

95

sense of agency in news coverage, I predict that the empowered voter portrayal will

stimulate information seeking desires among the public.

H5: The empowered voter portrayal will increase information-seeking desires.

Another disconcerting trend over the past several decades has been a drastic drop

in trust in news media among the public (Cappella & Jamieson, 1997; Moy & Pfau, 2001;

Tsfati & Peri, 2006). According to survey data, the percentage of people who express a

“great deal of confidence” in the press dropped from around 30 percent in early 1970s to

only 12 percent in 2000 (Cook, Gronke, & Rattliff, 2000). Scholars have linked declining

levels of trust in news media and other political institutions with the strategic frame in

news media (Cappella & Jamieson, 1997; Patterson, 1993). Based on the content analysis

of newspapers and public opinion polls over the past three decades, Kiousis (2002)

empirically shows that the trust in news media is negatively correlated with the amount of

cynical coverage of the president. That is, distrust against the press among the public may

be self-inflicted. By covering politics in a cynical and negative manner, the press not only

creates distrust against politicians, but also against itself among the public. At the same

time, it poses a promise that it might be possible for the press to regain trust from the

public by reforming the ways in which journalists cover political campaigns and voters.

Kiousis (2002) offers insight to this pattern by pointing to congruity theory. He

states that congruity theory suggests, “people strive to maintain equilibrium between

attitude objects and sources making assertions about those objects.” (p. 561). Based on

congruity theory, then, if news coverage of voters could create positive attitudes toward

electoral politics, citizens may try to maintain equilibrium between their positive attitude

Page 109: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

96

toward electoral politics (attitude object) and the press (the source of the message) by

changing their attitude toward the press in a positive way. Therefore, I predict that when

the voters are featured as empowered agents in democratic system rather than as pawns of

elites in a game framing, citizens will be more likely to trust the news media.

H6: The empowered voter portrayal will increase trust in news media.

Finally, I consider the possibilities that the effects of these portrayals will vary

across individuals. Political and media scholars suggest that political knowledge and

political attachment moderate framing effects (Price & Zaller, 1993; Zaller, 1992). Some

argue that the politically sophisticated tend to be inoculated against attitude change

because of already established opinions (McGuire, 1964). However, the empirical results

of these moderators are inconsistent. For instance, Valentino et al. (2001) found that

those with low levels of political knowledge are more likely to be influenced by media

frames while Nelson, Clawson, and Oxley (1997) found the opposite result. Therefore,

given the less definitive nature of these findings, I will explore the moderating effects of

political knowledge and partisanship without making a priori theoretical predictions.

RQ 1: Do political knowledge and partisanship either enhance or diminish

the effects of the empowered voter portrayal?

METHODOLOGY To test these hypotheses, an on-line experiment, an increasingly common method

in communication research, was conducted. I used a post-test only between-subjects

Page 110: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

97

experimental design with random assignment to one of two conditions: the empowered

voter condition and a control condition.

Procedure The experiments took place online two weeks before Super Tuesday (a 24 state

primary date on February 5, 2008). Each participant completed a questionnaire regarding

media habits and political engagement and then was randomly assigned to one of two

conditions: the empowered voter condition or control condition. In the empowered voter

condition, participants read a newspaper article containing a manipulation paragraph; in

the control condition, participants read the same article without a manipulation paragraph

(detailed in Materials section, below). After the participants read their article, they were

asked a series of questions pertaining to political beliefs and attitudes.

Participants Participants were recruited through Polimetrix, a leading firm in online opinion

measurement in political science. Two hundred and twenty two adults participated in the

study. Fifty percent of participants were female, 77 percent identified as white or

Caucasian, 11 percent identified as Black or African American, 9 percent as Hispanic,

and 3 percent as other. The average age of the participants was 45 years old (SD = 15.9,

Range = 18 to 83). Sixty one percent of the participants had beyond high school

education. Since chi-square tests indicated no significant differences between the

experimental conditions, these demographic variables were not included as covariates.

Page 111: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

98

Materials The baseline story was culled from real articles addressing the 2008 election. The

empowered voter portrayal was created to mirror the patterns found in newspaper articles

in the 1950s-1960s and portrayed voters as being mobilized by the candidates and as

important actors in electoral process (Figure 5.1). To create realistic newspaper articles, I

worked with a journalist with over 20 years of print journalism experience in covering

politics and elections to craft an empowered voter portrayal. To reflect the characteristics

of newspaper articles during this period, party labels were inserted as well. The control

article consisted of three paragraphs that were identical to the empowered voter portrayal

but without a manipulation paragraph featuring voters as empowered agents in electoral

process and party labels. This ensured that any differences found between the control

group and other conditions were due to the manipulation of portrayal inserted in the

empowered voter condition and not simply due to the exposure to election related

coverage.

Page 112: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

99

Figure 5.1. Newspaper Article Used in the Experiment

National News Washington (AP)—There is life in politics after New Hampshire and Iowa. Presidential hopefuls who survived those two early presidential contests in January will face a daunting challenge spanning 21 states and four time zones: the Feb. 5 primaries. Political observers say they expect Feb. 5 voting to winnow the field even further—perhaps to two or three major candidates. "It may not decide who gets the nomination, but Super Duper Tuesday will probably take a huge chunk of the field out," said Joe Patterson, Washington-based political analyst. While candidates navigate hurdles posed by the primaries, major party hopefuls are asking American voters to perform their own important role in the 2008 election. “We need every vote to move this country forward,” one Republican candidate said. A Democratic candidate opined, “If voters act in the interests and welfare of the country and the world, they will make a historic difference in this election.” (the empowered voter portrayal, not italicized in the actual experiment). It is hard to predict how things will turn out in this election. But, one thing is certain. As laid out by the United States Constitution, the individual who receives a majority of votes for president in the Electoral College in November will be the 44th president of the United States, and will be sworn in to the office early 2009. Note: The newspaper article used in the empowered voter condition is displayed here. The article for the control condition did not contain the italicized paragraph.

Measures

Dependent variables. Intention to vote was measured by the following question:

“Looking forward to the 2008 election, do you expect to vote in the general election?

Please indicate how likely you think it is that you will vote in the general election in

2008, where ‘0’ means you definitely will not vote and ‘10’ means you definitely will

vote.” The self-reported intention to vote is considered to be susceptible to the social

desirability bias—i.e., participants tend to over-report their intention to vote simply

because it is more socially acceptable (Belli, Traugot, Young, & McGonagle, 1999).

Page 113: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

100

However, since the experimental conditions were randomly assigned, it is safe to assume

that any social desirability bias we might find was also randomly assigned. Therefore,

any differences we find between conditions should not be attributed to the social

desirability effect.

Civic duty was measured by the extent to which participants agreed with the

following statement: “People like me have a duty to vote in elections.” The response

options ranged from “strongly disagree,” “somewhat disagree,” “neither disagree nor

agree,” “somewhat agree,” to “strongly agree.”

Political efficacy was measured in two dimensions. Internal efficacy was

measured by the extent to which the participants agreed with the following five

statements (Cronbach’s alpha = .757): (a) “I consider myself well-qualified to participate

in elections”; (b) “I feel that I have a pretty good understanding of the important political

issue facing our country”; (c) “I feel that I could do as good a job in public office as most

other people”; (d) “I think that I am as well-informed about the election as most people”;

(e) “Sometimes elections seem so complicated that a person like me can’t really

understand what’s going on” (reverse-coded). The response options ranged from “1 =

strongly disagree” to “5 = strongly agree.”

External efficacy was measured by the extent to which the participants agreed

with the following three items (Cronbach’s alpha = .671): (a) “People like me don’t have

any say about who gets to be president” (reverse-coded); (b) “I don’t think public

officials care much what people like me think” (reverse-coded); and (c) “Candidates for

Page 114: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

101

office are only interested in people’s votes, not in their opinions” (reverse-coded). The

response options ranged from “1 = strongly disagree” to “5 = strongly agree.”

The perceived meaningfulness of elections consisted of four questions

(Cronbach’s alpha = .647). The participants were asked to state the extent to which they

agreed with the following statements: (a) “It makes a difference who gets elected”; (b)

“The policies of government will always be the same regardless of which party is in

power” (reverse-coded); (c) “Candidates all say the same things these days” (reverse-

coded); (d) “There are important differences between the political parties in America.”

The response options ranged from “1 = strongly disagree” to “5 = strongly agree.”

Desire for information seeking was measured by the following question: “Are you

interested in getting more information on the topic discussed in the article?” The response

options consisted of “not at all interested,” “not so interested” “somewhat interested” and

“very much interested.”

Media trust was measured by the following six items (Cronbach’s alpha = .914):

(a) “How much of the time do you think you can trust newspapers to report the news

fairly?” (“none of the time,” “only some of the time,” “most of the time,” “just about

always”); (b) “Thinking about the newspapers you are most familiar with, please indicate

whether you think they are fair, (c) tell the whole story, (d) accurate, (e) can be trusted”

(“not at all,” “not so much,” “not sure,” “somewhat,” “very much”); (f) “How much

confidence would you say you have in the people now running the newspapers?” (“none

at all,” “not much,” “some,” “a great deal”). For multiple item measurements, all scales

Page 115: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

102

were added and converted to 0 to 1, with higher scores representing higher levels of each

dependent variable.

Independent variables. Participants were asked whether they identified as

Democrats, Republicans, Independents or other. Thirty four percent of participants

identified themselves as Democrats, 32 percent as Republicans, 21 percent as

Independents, and 12 percent other. Those who identified as either Democrats or

Republicans were classified as partisans (66 percent).

Political knowledge was measured by the following five items (Cronbach’s alpha

= .645): (a) “What job or political office is now held by Dick Cheney?”; (b) “Do you

happen to know which party currently has a majority in the U.S. House of

Representatives?”; (c) “Whose responsibility is it to determine if a law is constitutional or

not?”; (d) “How much of a majority is required for the U.S. Senate and House to override

a presidential veto?”; (e) “Which one of the parties would you say is more conservative

than the other at the national level?” Because participants were asked to respond to these

knowledge items via on-line, there was a concern about respondents using search engines

to look for correct answers while taking the survey. In order to prevent potential

“cheating” on these knowledge items, a 20-second time limit was set to answer each

question, and participants were notified of the time limit. On average, the participants

answered 3.7 out of 5 questions correctly (SD = 1.3).

Page 116: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

103

Covariates.14 Political interest was measured using the following question: “Some

people seem to follow what’s going on in government and public affairs most of the time,

whether there’s an election going on or not. Others aren’t that interested. How often

would you say you follow what’s going on in government and public affairs?” The

response options included “hardly at all” (3.6%), “only now and then” (5.0%), “some of

the time” (31.1%), “most of the time” (60.4%). The average score was 3.48 (SD = .75). A

t-test was conducted to see if there were any differences between the empowered voter

portrayal and the control condition. The results indicated some differences between the

experimental conditions. Specifically, those who were in the empowered voter condition

showed a slightly higher level of political interest (mean = 3.57) compared to the control

group (mean = 3.39). Therefore, political interest was included as a covariate.

Political participation was measured using the following question: “Here is a list

of things some people do about government and politics. Have you happened to have

done any of these things in the past?” Participants were asked to select from the

following five political activities that they have done in the past: (a) Tried to persuade

someone to vote for a specific candidate or party; (b) Wore a campaign button, put a

campaign sticker on your car, or placed a sign in your window or in front of your house;

(c) Attended political meetings, rallies, speeches, dinners or things like that to support a

particular candidate; (d) Worked for a political party or candidate as a volunteer or paid

14 While most experiments ignore other influences on dependent variable (mainly because random assignments to treatment and control group typically ensure researchers’ ability to estimate the effects of experiments without bias), some scholars argue for the inclusion of covariates. For instance, Franklin (1991) suggests that by including variance in experiments, “greater precision can be achieved for a fixed cost, or equal precision reached for a lower price” (p. 3).

Page 117: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

104

staff; or (e) Gave money to a political party or an individual candidate for public office.

The political participation item ranged from “none of the above” (21.1%), “one of the

above” (31.1%), “two of the above” (16.7%), “three of the above” (10.8%), “four of the

above” (10.4%), and “all of the above” (9.9%). The average score was 1.88 activities (SD

= 1.6). As in political interest item, a t-test was conducted to see if there were any

differences between conditions. The results suggested no difference. Therefore, political

participation was not included as a covariate.

Previous voting behavior was measured using the following question: “Have you

ever voted in local, state or national elections?” Eighty-nine percent answered yes. A chi-

square test indicated some differences between the conditions. Specifically, the

empowered voter condition had a higher percentage of participants who have voted

before (93.7%) than the control group (84.6%). Therefore, previous voting behavior was

added as a covariate.

RESULTS Table 5.1 represents the summary of findings drawn from an analysis of

covariance with experimental condition, political interest, and previous voting behaviors

as covariates.

Page 118: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

105

Table 5.1

Effects of Empowered Voter Portrayal

Control Empowered Voter Dependent variable: (N=110) (N=112)

Intent to vote .867 .910**

(.018) (.018)

Civic duty .941 .917

(.014) (.014)

Internal efficacy .811 .826

(.012) (.012)

External efficacy .502 .548**

(.019) (.019)

Meaningfulness of elections .697 .709

(.014) (.014)

Information seeking .654 .647

(.021) (.021)

Media trust .552 .598**

(.018) (.018)

Note: Table entries are estimated means with standard errors in parentheses drawn from an analysis of covariance with condition, political interest, and previous voting behavior. All scales run from zero to one, with higher values representing higher scale in dependent variables. **p < .05, one-tailed.

Page 119: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

106

Intention to vote. As expected, social desirability bias was detected in regard to

participants’ intention to vote. As table 5.1 suggests, overall intention to vote was very

high. The control group that was not exposed to the empowered portrayal scored .867 on

the scale of 0 to 1 in terms of their participatory intentions. However, the results indicate

that the empowered portrayal had a positive impact on citizens’ intention to vote

(Hypothesis 1). Those who were exposed to the empowered voter portrayal scored even

higher on their likelihood of voting, .91. The difference between the control group and

the treatment group is statistically significant (F = 2.768, p = .049).

Civic duty. The results of citizens’ civic duty also indicate a very high sense of

duty among both the control group and those in the empowered voter condition.

Interestingly, those in the control group scored higher on civic duty item, .941, compared

to the treatment group, .917. The difference, however, was not statistically significant (F

= 1.544, p = .108).

While my hypothesis was not supported (Hypothesis 2), it is possible that the

results might have something to do with the sample of this study. The sample of this

experiment is approaching middle-age (average = 45 years old, SD = 15.9), and it is

entirely possible that these citizens have already established their sense of civic duty as

citizens over their lifespan. Therefore, a one-time exposure to the empowered voter

portrayal in the experiment may not alter their already established sense of civic duty. On

the other hand, younger adults who are in the process of learning the roles as citizens

might be more susceptible to the news portrayal of voters and electoral participation in

acquiring a sense of duty. Indeed, while overall feeling of sense of duty remained strong

Page 120: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

107

over the years regardless of voter turnout and news media coverage of voters and

electoral politics, recent studies suggest that it is young adults, in particular, that are

losing their sense of obligation toward politics (Conover & Searing, 2000; Doppelt &

Shearer, 2001; National Association of Secretaries of State, 1999). The effect of the

empowered portrayal on young adults will be discussed in the next chapter.

Political efficacy. As expected, the overall level of internal efficacy was

considerably higher than the level of external efficacy. For instance, within the control

group, the level of internal efficacy was about .30 higher than external efficacy (.811 vs.

.502). These data suggest that despite declines in voter turnout and political trust,

citizens’ confidence and beliefs about their impact on the political process remain steady.

In terms of hypotheses, I predicted that those who are exposed to the empowered voter

portrayal would have a higher level of external political efficacy and similar level of

internal efficacy (Hypothesis 3). The results show that the hypothesis was supported.

Those who were exposed to the empowered voter portrayal scored higher on external

efficacy (.548) than the control group (.502), and the difference was statistically

significant (F = 2.964, p = .043). Since external efficacy and political trust are highly

correlated and are considered broad indicators of support for political system (Sullivan &

Riedel, 2001), this finding is particularly important in a time of increased cynicism and

alienation. The results for internal efficacy show a slight increase among those who read

the empowered voter portrayal article (.811 vs. .826), but as hypothesized, the difference

was not statistically significant (F = .818, p = .367).

Page 121: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

108

Meaningfulness of elections. Does the empowered voter portrayal increase the

perceived meaningfulness of elections (Hypothesis 4)? The results illustrate that it does

not. Those in the control condition and the empowered voter condition expressed similar

level of perceived meaningfulness of elections (.697 vs. .709, F = .329, p = .284).

Information seeking. I also tested the effects of the empowered voter portrayal on

citizens’ interest in seeking information about politics (Hypothesis 5). The results

indicate that there was no significant difference in people’s information seeking desires

between the control condition and the empowered voter condition (.654 vs. .647, F =

.052, p = .41). This finding is contrary to Kam’s (2007) findings on the effects of civic

duty appeal and citizens’ information seeking behaviors. Kam (2007) found that when

people are reminded of their civic duty (to learn about issues), they were more likely to

seek information on candidates’ issues stances. I suspect that the differences in results

might rest on the scope of these studies. While Kam tested participants’ tendency to seek

information on candidate’s policy stance explicitly listed in her manipulation article, this

study looked at interest in seeking information on the topic of election in general (“Are

you interested in getting more information on the topic discussed in the article?”) Also,

news articles used in this study did not contain any information on candidates’ policy

issue stances. Thus, it might be the case that participants in this study did not feel a need

to seek more information on what they might already know.

Media trust. Next, I explored the possibility that news media themselves can

benefit from featuring voters as important players in political system. Based on the level

of media trust in the 1950s and the 1960s and congruity theory, I predicted that the

Page 122: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

109

empowered voter portrayal would have a positive impact on citizens’ trust in news media

(Hypothesis 6). As expected, along with external efficacy, overall trust in news media

appeared to be relatively low compared to other variables. However, those who were

exposed to the empowered voter portrayal exhibited a higher level of media trust

compared to the control group, and the difference was statistically significant (.598 vs.

.552, F = 3.254, p = .036). This finding is crucial at a time of prevalent media distrust

among the public. It is hopeful that the press can regain the trust of citizens that they have

lost for quite some time by implementing subtle changes in their political campaign

coverage. Regaining the legitimacy of the press is essential for the overall health of

democracy, but for a practical reason for the journalists as well. Several studies have

revealed that trust in news media is associated with news exposure (Kiousis, 2001;

Rimmer & Weaver, 1987), and the decline of trust in news media is considered a possible

explanation for the decline in news exposure (Gans, 2003). Therefore, journalists might

reap the benefit of increased trust and news consumption by bringing the voters back to

political arena and to the news.

Moderating effects of partisanship and political knowledge. As seen in Table 5.2

and 5.3, none of the interaction terms were significant (RQ 1).15 This finding suggests

that the effects of the empowered voter portrayal did not vary across individuals.

15 Note that there were main effects of partisanship on external efficacy and meaningfulness of elections as well as main effects of knowledge on intentions to vote and internal efficacy. The effects of partisanship on external efficacy are to be expected as scholars suggest some positive correlation between the two (Teixeira, 1987), and the effects on meaningfulness of elections may be caused by strong party cues in items measuring this variable. The main effects of knowledge on intentions to vote and internal efficacy are expected as other survey research has found the same relationship (Delli Carpini & Keeter, 1996).

Page 123: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

110

Table 5.2

Moderating Effects of Partisanship

Control Empowered Voter Dependent variable (N=110) (N=112)

Intent to vote

Partisan .882 .915 Condition x Partisan (n=147) (.022) (.021) F = .455

Nonpartisan .834 .903 p = .501 (n=75) (.031) (.031)

Civic duty Partisan .953 .930

(.017) (.017) F = .000 Nonpartisan .916 .892 p = .999

(.024) (.024) Internal efficacy

Partisan .803 .821 (.014) (.014) F = .004

Nonpartisan .822 .841 p = .950 (.02) (.021)

External efficacy Partisan .533 .564

(.023) (.023) F = .898 Nonpartisan .441 .524 p = .344

(.032) (.032) Meaningfulness of elections

Partisan .724 .741 (.018) (.017) F = .069

Nonpartisan .643 .649 p = .793 (.024) (.024)

Information seeking Partisan .642 .664

(.026) (.026) F = 1.908 Nonpartisan .677 .612 p = .169

(.036) (.037)

Page 124: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

111

Control Empowered Voter

Dependent variable (N=110) (N=112) Media trust

Partisan .568 .605 (.023) (.022) F = .238

Nonpartisan .522 .586 p = .626 (.032) (.032)

Note: Table entries are estimated means with standard errors in parentheses, drawn from an analysis of covariance with condition, partisanship, political knowledge, and interactions for condition x partisanship and condition x political knowledge with covariates political interest and previous voting behavior. All scales run from zero to one, with higher values representing higher scale in dependent variables.

Page 125: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

112

Table 5.3

Moderating Effects of Political Knowledge

Control Empowered Voter Dependent variable (N=110) (N=112)

Intent to vote

0-1 answered correctly .771 .731 (n=15) (.070) (.072)

2 answered correctly .883 .914 Condition x Knowledge (n=23) (.057) (.053) F = .000

3 answered correctly .767 .933 p = .988 (n=38) (.043) (.043)

4 answered correctly .894 .917 (n=75) (.030) (.033)

5 answered correctly .878 .928 (n=71) (.033) (.031)

Civic duty 0-1 .910 1.00

(.053) (.055) 2 .952 .889 (.044) (.041) F = 1.474

3 .900 .932 p = .226 (.033) (.033)

4 .933 .871 (.023) (.025)

5 .959 .923 (.026) (.024)

Internal efficacy 0-1 .718 .717

(.045) (.046) 2 .758 .751 (.036) (.036) F = .139

3 .844 .825 p = .709 (.027) (.028)

4 .781 .855 (.019) (.021)

5 .868 .861 (.021) (.020)

Page 126: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

113

Control Empowered Voter Dependent variable (N=110) (N=112) External efficacy

0-1 .416 .584 (.073) (.075)

2 .447 .593 (.059) (.056) F = 1.531

3 .437 .430 p = .217 (.045) (.045)

4 .490 .555 (.032) (.034)

5 .543 .566 (.035) (.033)

Meaningfulness of elections 0-1 .684 .741

(.055) (.057) 2 .684 .668 (.045) (.042) F = .187

3 .626 .682 p = .666 (.036) (.034)

4 .713 .702 (.024) (.026)

5 .680 .693 (.027) (.025)

Information seeking 0-1 .621 .755

(.083) (.086) 2 .678 .621 (.068) (.064) F = 1.567

3 .680 .628 p = .212 (.051) (.051)

4 .633 .689 (.036) (.039)

5 .682 .580 (.040) (.037)

Page 127: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

114

Control Empowered Voter

Dependent variable (N=110) (N=112) Media trust

0-1 .602 .587 (.071) (.073)

2 .577 .747 (.061) (.054) F = .586

3 .519 .581 p = .445 (.044) (.044)

4 .510 .546 (.031) (.033)

5 .582 .597 (.034) (.032)

Note: Table entries are estimated means with standard errors in parentheses, drawn from an analysis of covariance with condition, partisanship, political knowledge, and interactions for condition x partisanship and condition x political knowledge with covariates political interest and previous voting behavior. All scales run from zero to one, with higher values representing higher scale in dependent variables.

DISCUSSION

This experiment shows that the ways in which journalists portray voters had a

significant impact on citizens’ participatory intentions and political attitudes. When

voters were cast as efficacious and as an important part of the electoral system (as found

in the 1950s and the 1960s), citizens’ intentions to vote and external efficacy increased.

Based on the level of political engagement in the 1950s and the 1960s, the results point to

the possibility that the ways in which news media portrayed electoral participation may

have contributed to the robust political engagement during these periods. Moreover, the

positive portrayals of voters had a spillover effect on news media themselves. When

citizens encountered empowered portrayals of themselves in the news, they were more

likely to trust the news media. These findings provide optimism to scholars, pundits and

Page 128: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

115

media organizations about what we can do to encourage citizens to be more engaged in

elections and to be less cynical about political process and news media.

There are some limitations to these findings, however. One limitation is that while

I found significant effects of news portrayals on participatory intentions and political

attitudes in the present time, this study does not validate the causal relationship between

news coverage and citizens’ political participation and attitudes in the 1950s and the

1960s. There is simply no way to test the actual effects of the empowered voter portrayals

on people who were living and consuming news back in the 1950s and the 1960s. Still, it

is intriguing to find that the news portrayal found in earlier times propelled citizens to be

more efficacious and engaging as well as more trusting of the press.

Another limitation is that these findings are confined to short-term effects,

therefore, long-term effects of the empowered voter portrayal are unknown. In order to

measure the long-term effects of the empowered voter portrayal on citizens’ participatory

intentions and political attitudes as well as trust in news media, a longitudinal study is

necessary. The longitudinal study will also help better understand the generalizability of

the results. Because this study was conducted during the early primary election cycle in

2008, it should be examined that whether or not these findings will continue to hold in

other contexts.

Finally, there is an important question to be asked in terms of political

socialization via news media. This study tested the effects of news portrayal found in the

1950s and the 1960s on political attitudes among citizens with an average age of 45 (SD

= 15.9). However, as discussed in Chapter One, the biggest concern for the future of

Page 129: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

116

democracy rests on the youngest generation of voters. The period of adolescence is the

most impressionable one in the political socialization process (Sears, 1975), and it is

when individuals form their political identities, attitudes and behaviors (Hess & Torney,

1967; Greenstein, 1968; Sears, 1975). Because of the close link between preadult

political socialization and adult political orientation, attitudes and behaviors suggested by

socialization scholars (Andolina et al., 2003; Beck & Jennings, 1982; Conway &

Damico, 2001; Verba & Nie, 1972; Verba et al., 1995), it is essential to examine if the

results found among older cohorts will still hold when tested on younger voters nation’s

youngest voters.

The replication of this experiment using young adult sample is particularly

important since there is mounting evidence to indicate that it is the younger generation of

voters that has become particularly alienated from the political process (Delli Carpini,

2000). Compared to their parents’ generation, today’s young people are less likely to be

interested in public affairs, to be knowledgeable about political process or issues, to feel

the obligation associated with citizenship, to participate politically through voting or by

making their views known, or to trust their fellow citizens (Bennett, 2001; Delli Carpini,

2000; Graber, 2000; Lopez et al., 2006; Macedo et al., 2005; Mindich, 2005; Sherr &

Staples, 2004; Zukin, Keeter, Andolina, Jenkins, & Delli Carpini, 2006).

It is worth noting that the younger generation of voters were raised in the news

media environment where their roles as voters were presented predominantly in terms of

opinion polls and as pawns of elites rather than in the news environment where voters are

featured as empowered agents in a democratic system (see Chapter Four). The next

Page 130: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

117

chapter will examine potential socialization effects of news media on young adults’

political beliefs and attitudes and see if it is possible to motivate today’s young adults to

engage in electoral politics via empowered portrayals of voters in the news media.

Page 131: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

118

Chapter 6. Effects of Portrayals in the News: College Students

Chapter Five examined how portraying voters as efficacious and as important

agents in the democratic process in news coverage affects citizens’ political attitudes. The

experiment revealed that when voters are portrayed as empowered agents in election

coverage, citizens’ intention to vote, external efficacy, and trust in news media increased

(as might have been the case in the 1950s and 1960s). These results suggest that ways in

which print news media cover voters may have important socialization effects on

citizens’ participatory intentions and their political attitudes.

This chapter examines if similar effects can be found among nations’ youngest

voters. Today’s young adults have often been criticized for their lack of interest,

knowledge, and participation in the political arena. Particularly in the field of electoral

politics, young adults fall behind their older counterparts considerably. While there are

many possible reasons why today’s youth are staying away from electoral politics, the

patterns found in my content analysis offer an important observation on the political

socialization of today’s youth: this cohort was not raised on the “empowered voter”

portrayal in print coverage of elections as their grandparents and, to a smaller extent,

their parents were.

It is entirely possible that older generations were raised in a political moment in

which voters were featured as efficacious and as central to the system. It is also possible

that these attributes in news stories influenced how those generation of voters regarded

their roles in the polity. Similarly, current news depictions of voters as pawns in elite

Page 132: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

119

political strategy and public opinion polls may have possibly influenced how young

people have been socialized to view their roles as citizens. While no one experiment

could test these matters definitively, this chapter explores if how voters were depicted in

an earlier era are more likely to encourage young people to see their attitudes and

behaviors as central to political life.

POLITICAL DISENGAGEMENT OF YOUTH

Over the past four decades, Americans of all ages have turned away from politics,

but the rate of political disengagement among young adults has been beyond comparison

to any other age groups. In studies conducted prior to 2007, researchers suggest that

today’s young adults lack the knowledge, skills and values to inherit and sustain

democratic life. It should be noted that some of the pessimistic trends became tempered

with time (e.g., a spike in voter turnout among 18-24 year olds in 2004), and scholars

publishing after 2007 began to posit more optimistic views on political engagement of

today’s young adults (e.g., Bennett, 2008; Dalton, 2007). Nonetheless, an overall decline

in political engagement among today’s youth relative to their older counterparts (at the

same age) begs serious investigation.

For instance, today’s young adults are less likely to feel sense of duty as citizens

or trust their fellow citizens compared to other generations (Delli Carpini, 2000).

According to the Pew Research Center (1992, 2007), when asked if they agree with the

statement, “It’s my duty as a citizen to always vote,” only 40% of young adults (18-25

years-old) agreed while 80% of those over fifty agreed. Another study suggests that

Page 133: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

120

today’s young people are significantly less likely to believe that most people can be

trusted, try to be helpful, or are fair compared to the youth cohort of the 1970s (Rahn &

Transue, 1998).

In the realm of electoral politics, the 2004 election and the current 2008 primary

season not-withstanding, voter turnout rate among young adults has declined over the

years.16 In 1972 when 18 year-olds first gained their right to vote, more than a half of

young adults between 18 to 24 year-olds turned out, but only one-third of young people

did in 2000 (U.S. Census, 2002). Another study suggests that today’s youth are less likely

to get involved with other forms of electoral process as well. For instance, the percentage

of adolescents who indicate that they “could see themselves working on a campaign”

declined by half from the mid 1970s to the last few years (Macedo et al., 2005, p. 29).

This downward trend in electoral politics up until 2004 coincides with a decline in

political interest among young adults. While 51% of Americans over 50 say they follow

politics “most of the time,” only 19% of 18-29 year-olds do (Zukin, 1997). Moreover,

whereas 59% of college students in the 1960s indicated that it is very important to keep

up with public affairs, 27% of college students did in 1997 (Sax, Astin, Korn, &

Mahoney, 1997).

Despite the fact that today’s young adults are more educated and have easier

access to political information than any other young cohort in the past, today’s young

people are less knowledgeable about the substance or process of politics as well (Delli

Carpini, 2000). When asked basic factual items such as “which party controls the House

Page 134: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

121

of Representatives,” today’s young adults were 20 percentage points less likely than older

cohorts to answer correctly, whereas there was virtually no knowledge gap between

young people and older generations in the 1940s and 1950s (Delli Carpini & Keeter,

1996).

The same pattern can be observed in news consumption among young adults

(Levine, 2007). While nearly a quarter of young adults (18-25 years-old) followed the

news consistently in 1960, only 5.1% of young people do in 2000 (NES cited in Levine,

2007, p. 80). In terms of newspapers, the news media analysis in this dissertation, today’s

young adults are less likely than their parents were (at the same age) to read the

newspaper—a rate that is less than half of what it was for young adults in 1965 (Zukin,

1997). Because political interest and knowledge often serve as prerequisites for political

engagement, these data posit a grave concern for the future of democracy (Putnam,

2000).

The Causes of Disengagement

Why are today’s youth disengaged from politics? It is tempting to blame today’s

young adults for their lack of interest, knowledge, and participation in the political arena,

but researchers suggest that the causes of these patterns lie in external factors. Delli

Carpini (2000) argues that in order to engage in public life, one has to have the

“motivation, opportunity, and ability to do so” (p. 343), and the lack of each of these

factors has contributed to the current disengagement among young adults. He explains

16 See footnote 4.

Page 135: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

122

that the motivation for young people to engage in public life has been diminished over the

past three decades after the systematic devaluing of the public sector since the Watergate

scandal in the 1970s; the meaningful opportunities to engage in public life have been

deprived by public, formal institutions (such as government, political parties, news

media, and schools) that ignore young adults and issues that are important to them; and,

consequently, young people are not adequately equipped with the ability to engage in

public life. These socialization processes unfavorable to foster engaged citizenry

combined with their relative lack of experience in public life pushed today’s young adults

from politics. Delli Carpini (2000) states,

In short, while older Americans have the ability to put the current anti-politics

environment in perspective, drawing on experiences of effective public-sector

policy, of respected public-sector leaders, and of meaningful collective action, for

Americans under the age of 30, the current environment is all they know. Never

having experienced a period in which their own participation has effected

meaningful change on an issue that mattered to them, and raised in an

environment that regularly tells them such action is unlikely to succeed, it is

hardly surprising that they are disinclined to participate in public life. Young

Americas are not disengaged because they are satisfied with the current state of

affairs, because they are apathetic, or because they do not care about their fellow

citizens. Rather, they are disengaged because they are alienated from the

institutions and processes of civic life and lack of the motivation, opportunity, and

ability to overcome this alienation (Italics original, p. 345).

Page 136: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

123

Buckingham (1999) concurs with this point of view. He argues that young adults are

“actively excluded from the domain of politics, and from dominant forms of political

discourse,” and their disengagement from politics is “merely a rational response to their

own powerlessness” (p. 172).

If disengagement of today’s youth is a result of their powerlessness in the political

arena and the political discourse that surrounds them, would it be possible to reverse the

course by injecting a different form of political discourse that features voters as

efficacious and important actors in a democratic process (found in the 1950s and the

1960s) to encourage today’s young adults to engage in politics? Can positive portrayals

of voters and electoral process increase young adults’ motivation to participate in

electoral activities? And, ultimately, can the empowered news coverage create

empowered electorate out of today’s youth? Because the adolescent years are considered

the most impressionable years in the political socialization process, and political learning

during the period of adolescence profoundly impacts later political attitudes and

behaviors (Hess & Torney, 1967; Greenstein, 1968; Sears, 1975), these questions merit

our attention.

Furthermore, these questions are quite timely. At the time of writing, we are

witnessing a major deviation from the norm in terms of electoral campaigns and news

media coverage. Although political parties, candidates, and news media have long

ignored young adults, in the year 2008 they have reached out to young voters placing an

unprecedented spotlight on their engagement and the important roles they play in the

presidential election (and young voters are turning out to vote in unprecedented

Page 137: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

124

numbers). Young voters are featured on the cover of Time magazine (February 11, 2008)

“Why Young Voters Care Again: And Why Their Vote Matters” and numerous other

articles across the country highlight youth engagement in the 2008 elections (e.g., “How

Youth Rule An Election” Forbes.com, March 26, 2008; “Millennials Are About to Give

American Politics an Extreme Makeover” The Huffington Post, February 7, 2008;

“Young Obama Backers Twist Parents’ Arms,” The New York Times, April 8, 2008;

“Youth Vote Surge in Austin and Across the Country” The Austin American Statesman,

February 26, 2008). While this study does not offer a definitive answer to the impact of

these news coverage that focus on young voters, it takes a first step in examining the

impacts of putting voters at the center stage of electoral process in news media on young

adults.

HYPOTHESES This chapter tests seven hypotheses that are identical to the ones examined in the

previous chapter.

H1: The empowered voter portrayal will increase intention to vote.

H2: The empowered voter portrayal will increase sense of civic duty.

H3: The empowered voter portrayal will increase external political efficacy while

exerting no impact on the level of internal efficacy.

H4: The empowered voter portrayal will increase meaningfulness of elections.

H5: The empowered voter portrayal will increase information-seeking desires.

H6: The empowered voter portrayal will increase trust in news media.

Page 138: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

125

RQ 1: Do political knowledge and partisanship either enhance or diminish

the effects of the empowered voter portrayal?

While the rationale behind these hypotheses are similar to those explained in the

previous chapter, it is particularly important to reexamine these hypotheses with young

adults for two reasons. First, considering the acute decline in political engagement among

youth, it is vital to test these hypotheses with youth and explore the ways to engage them

in politics. Second, because young adults are at the most impressionable stage in political

socialization process, socialization gains (such as hypothesized below) may have the

long-term effects. A close link between political socialization during youth and adult

political attitudes and behaviors suggested by socialization literature points to the long-

term implications of these hypothesized effects.

I also speculate that these hypotheses are more likely to be supported among

youth compared to older citizens. Unlike older citizens (who tend to have relatively

crystallized attitudes about civic duty and electoral engagement), young adults (who lack

political socialization experiences) are still in the process of learning their roles and

responsibilities as citizens (Greenstein, 1968). Therefore, compared to older citizens,

young adults may be more likely to be influenced by positive news coverage of voters.

METHODOLOGY To test these hypotheses, an on-line experiment was conducted. I used a post-test

only between-subjects experimental design with random assignment to one of two

conditions: the empowered voter condition and a control condition.

Page 139: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

126

Procedure The experiments took place online between November and December 2007. Each

participant completed a questionnaire regarding media habits and political engagement

and then was randomly assigned to one of two conditions: the empowered voter condition

or control condition. As in the experiment employed in the previous chapter, in the

empowered voter condition, participants read an article which contained a paragraph

where voters were featured as efficacious and mobilized by elites (italicized in Figure

5.1), and in control condition, participants read the same article without the manipulation

paragraph. After the participants read the article, they were asked a series of questions

pertaining to political beliefs and attitudes.

Participants Participants were recruited through a department participant pool at a large

Southwestern university using flyers and e-mail messages. Only those who were between

18 and 29 years old and eligible to vote in the United States were allowed to participate

in the study. One hundred and twelve college students participated in the study. Sixty one

percent of participants were female, 54 percent identified as white or Caucasian, 16

percent identified as Hispanic, 20 percent as Asian or Pacific Islander, and 4 percent as

Black or African American. The average age of the participants was 20 years old (SD =

1.9). The use of college sophomores in experiments often raises questions concerning

generalizability of results (Sears, 1986). However, the focus of this study is to examine

the framing effects on the nation’s youngest voters. Therefore, the use of an

Page 140: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

127

undergraduate sample is appropriate for this study. Chi-square tests indicated no

significant differences for these demographic dimensions across the experimental

conditions.

Measures Dependent variables. All of the dependent variables—intention to vote, sense of

civic duty, internal efficacy, external efficacy, perceived meaningfulness of elections,

information seeking desires, trust in news media—were measured by the items identified

in Chapter Five. Reliability scores for multi-item measurements in this sample were as

follows: internal efficacy (Cronbach’s alpha = .825), external efficacy (Cronbach’s alpha

= .577), perceived meaningfulness of elections (Cronbach’s alpha = .551), and trust in

news media (Cronbach’s alpha = .869).

Independent variables. Participants were asked whether they identified as

Democrats, Republicans, Independents or other. Twenty four percent of participants

identified themselves as Democrats, 26 percent as Republicans, 37 percent as

Independents, and 13 percent other. Those who identified as either Democrats or

Republicans were classified as partisans (50 percent).

Political knowledge was measured by the five factual items mentioned in the

previous chapter (Cronbach’s alpha = .483). On average, the participants answered 4 out

of 5 questions correctly (SD = 1.1). The high average score with low reliability score on

Page 141: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

128

this variable raise some concerns about interpreting the results.17 I will discuss this point

in the results section.

Covariates. Political interest was measured using the same question in the

previous chapter: “Some people seem to follow what’s going on in government and

public affairs most of the time, whether there’s an election going on or not. Others aren’t

that interested. How often would you say you follow what’s going on in government and

public affairs?” The response options included “hardly at all” (6.2%), “only now and

then” (23.2%), “some of the time” (37.5%), “most of the time” (33.0%). The average

score was 2.97 (SD = .90). A t-test was conducted to see if there were any differences

between the empowered voter portrayal and the control condition. The result did not

indicate differences between the experimental conditions. Therefore, political interest

was not included as a covariate.

Political participation was also measured using the identical question as in the

previous chapter: if respondents have done any of these things in the past: (a) Tried to

persuade someone to vote for a specific candidate or party; (b) Wore a campaign button,

put a campaign sticker on your car, or placed a sign in your window or in front of your

house; (c) Attended political meetings, rallies, speeches, dinners or things like that to

support a particular candidate; (d) Worked for a political party or candidate as a volunteer

or paid staff; or (e) Gave money to a political party or an individual candidate for public

office. The political participation item ranged from “none of the above” (24.1%), “one of

17 Unlike the online survey conducted via Polimetrix (see Chapter 5), the online survey tool I used to conduct this experiment (Survey Monkey) did not provide a tool to set a time limit on these knowledge

Page 142: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

129

the above” (32.1%), “two of the above” (16.1%), “three of the above” (13.4%), “four of

the above” (12.5%), and “all of the above” (1.8%). The average score was 1.63 activities

(SD = 1.4). As with the political interest item, a t-test was conducted to see if there were

any differences between conditions. The result suggested no significant differences.

Therefore, political participation was not included as a covariate.

Previous voting behavior was measured using the following question: “Have you

ever voted in local, state or national elections?” Forty three percent answered yes. A chi-

square test indicated no difference between the conditions. Therefore, previous voting

behavior was not included as a covariate.

RESULTS Table 6.1 represents the summary of findings drawn from an analysis of variance

with condition as a fixed factor.

items. Therefore, there is a concern that some of the respondents might have searched for correct answers online while taking the survey. This point will be revisited in the results section.

Page 143: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

130

Table 6.1

Effects of Empowered Voter Portrayal on Young Voters

Control Empowered Voter Dependent variable: (N=51) (N=61)

Intent to vote .798 .882**

(.038) (.029)

Civic duty .800 .882**

(.029) (.026)

Internal efficacy .704 .718

(.026) (.024)

External efficacy .614 .671**

(.023) (.021)

Meaningfulness of elections .693 .751***

(.018) (.016)

Information seeking .603 .689**

(.030) (.028)

Media trust .646 .706**

(.021) (.019)

Note: Table entries are estimated means with standard errors in parentheses drawn from an analysis of variance with condition as a fixed factor. All scales run from zero to one, with higher values representing higher scale in dependent variables. This analysis was re-run including political interests and previous voting behaviors as covariates (see Chapter Five). When this was done, all of the results remained the same. **p < .05, ***p < .01, one-tailed.

Intention to vote. As seen among the older sample, a social desirability bias was

detected among young adults as well. An overall level of intention to vote is quite high

among young adults. The control group scored .798 on the scale of 0 to 1 in terms of their

Page 144: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

131

intention to vote (Table 6.1). It is worth noting that the level of participatory intentions

among young adults is lower than that of the older sample revealed in the previous

chapter (.798 vs. .867 = the control group score among the older sample, see Table 5.1).

This mirrors an empirical reality—lower voter turnout among young adults. However, as

in the case of older cohorts, the empowered portrayal had a positive impact on young

adults’ intention to vote. While those who were in the control group scored .798 on their

likelihood of voting, those who were exposed to the empowered voter portrayal scored

.882, and the difference between the control group and the treatment group is statistically

significant (F = 3.698, p = .028). Hypothesis 1 is supported.

Civic duty. The results of citizens’ civic duty indicate a similar pattern found in

intention to vote. While the young people who were in the control group showed a

relatively high level of sense of civic duty (.800), it is much lower than what was found

among the older sample (.941). What is important, however, is that although the

empowered voter portrayal did not have a positive impact on citizens’ sense of civic duty

among the older sample, it had a statistically significant impact among young adults.

Those who read the empowered voter portrayal scored .882 on their sense of civic duty

while the control group scored .800, and the difference was statistically significant (F =

4.472, p = .018). In the previous chapter, it was noted that the older citizens’ sense of

civic duty might not be easily influenced by external stimuli such as news media

coverage of political campaigns. The results among young adults indicate otherwise.

When newspapers emphasize voters’ duty in a democratic process, young people who are

in the process of establishing their roles as citizens gained stronger sense of civic duty.

Page 145: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

132

Because of the importance of socialization process during adolescent years, this finding is

critical.

Political efficacy. Compared to the older sample, young adults showed a lower

level of internal political efficacy. Between control groups, the level of internal efficacy

in the older sample was .811 while it was .704 among young adults. This is expected

based on young adults’ lack of experience in politics. Generally speaking, young adults

are less likely to believe that they have competence to understand and participate in

politics compared to older citizens, but their lack of confidence tends to decrease over

one’s life cycle (Abramson, 1983). The results show that young adults who read the

empowered voter portrayal expressed a slight increase in internal efficacy (.704 vs. .718),

but as hypothesized, the difference between the empowered voter group and the control

group was not statistically significant (F = .155, p = .347).

On the other hand, comparing the control groups among young adults and the

older sample, young adults showed a higher level of external efficacy than the older

sample (.614 vs. .502). This reflects a pattern suggested by MacManus (1996) that young

voters are slightly more trusting of government responsiveness. More importantly, the

results show that young adults who were exposed to the empowered voter portrayal

scored higher on external efficacy (.671) than the control group (.614), and the difference

was statistically significant (F = 3.282, p = .036). Hypothesis 3 was supported.

Meaningfulness of elections. Unlike other variables, the control groups for both

older and younger citizens expressed a similar level of perceived meaningfulness of

elections (.697 vs. .693). However, a difference can be found in the effect of news

Page 146: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

133

portrayal. While the empowered voter portrayal did not increase the perceived

meaningfulness of elections among the older sample, it did exert a significantly positive

impact among young adults (Hypothesis 4). Those in the empowered voter condition

expressed a higher level of perceived meaningfulness of elections than those in the

control condition (.751 vs. .693, F = 5.801, p = .009).

Information seeking. Compared to the older sample, young adults expressed a

lower level of interest in seeking political information (.654 vs. .603). Intriguingly,

however, the pattern was reversed after exposed to the empowered voter portrayal. The

results among young adults showed a significant increase in information seeking desires

among those who read the empowered voter portrayal article (.603 vs. .689, F = 4.385, p

= .019). When comparing the treatment groups between the older sample and young

adults, young adults showed a higher level of interest in seeking more information than

the older sample (.689 vs. .647). Hypothesis 5 was supported.

Media trust. Finally, contrary to the conventional wisdom that today’s young

adults are cynical, the data suggest that young adults had a higher level of trust in news

media than the older sample (.646 vs. .552). Further, those who were exposed to the

empowered voter portrayal exhibited a higher level of trust in news media compared to

the control group, and the difference was statistically significant (.706 vs. .646, F =

4.318, p = .02). Based on the findings on information seeking and media trust, featuring

voters as empowered agents in the democratic process can influence young adults’ often

lamented indifference toward political news and cynicism. Hypothesis 6 was supported.

Page 147: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

134

Moderating effects of partisanship and political knowledge. Table 6.2 and 6.3

indicate that none of the interaction terms were significant (RQ 1).18 As in the case of the

older sample, these results suggest that there is no difference between partisans and

nonpartisans and between those with a high level of political knowledge and a low level

of political knowledge in terms of the effects of the empowered voter portrayal. Based on

the results from both the older citizens and young adults, it is reasonable to conclude that

the effects of the empowered voter portrayal are not moderated by partisanship or

political sophistication.

However, these results should be interpreted with caution. As discussed earlier,

there is a concern about the accuracy of respondents’ political knowledge among young

adults. The reliability score for political knowledge items was relatively low among

young adults (Cronbach’s alpha = .483) compared to the older sample (Cronbach’s alpha

= .645), and the average score for political knowledge items was quite high—on average,

young adults answered 4 out of 5 questions correctly (SD = 1.1) whereas the older sample

scored an average of 3.7 out of 5 (SD = 1.3) on the same questions. Although my sample

comes from college students that tend to have a relatively high level of political

sophistication, the combination of low reliability score and unusually high level of

political knowledge posits a concern. As noted earlier (footnote 1), unlike the online

18 Note that there were main effects of partisanship on intentions to vote and civic duty as well as main effects of knowledge on civic duty and internal efficacy. The effects of partisanship on intentions to vote and the effects of knowledge on internal efficacy are to be expected as scholars suggest positive correlations between the two (Delli Carpini & Keeter, 1996; Maisel & Buckley, 2004). However, main effects of partisanship and knowledge on civic duty are unique to this sample and not much is known about the effects of partisanship and knowledge on civic duty among this particular age group. In order to fully understand these patterns, further studies are necessary.

Page 148: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

135

survey conducted via Polimetrix (see Chapter 5), the online survey tool I used to conduct

this experiment did not provide a tool to set a time limit on these knowledge items.

Therefore, it is possible that some of the respondents have searched for correct answers

online while taking the survey.

Page 149: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

136

Table 6.2

Moderating Effects of Partisanship among Young Voters

Control Empowered Voter Dependent variable (N=51) (N=61)

Intent to vote

Partisan .869 .929 Condition x Partisan (n=56) (.046) (.040) F = .062

Nonpartisan .745 .825 p = .804 (n=56) (.043) (.042)

Civic duty Partisan .862 .914

(.041) (.035) F = .151 Nonpartisan .756 .837 p = .699

(.038) (.037) Internal efficacy

Partisan .705 .716 (.037) (.032) F = .053

Nonpartisan .714 .710 p = .819 (.034) (.033)

External efficacy Partisan .607 .694

(.034) (.029) F = .777 Nonpartisan .615 .646 p = .380

(.032) (.031) Meaningfulness of elections

Partisan .722 .767 (.026) (.022) F = .193

Nonpartisan .667 .732 p = .662 (.024) (.024)

Information seeking Partisan .558 .679

(.045) (.038) F = .555 Nonpartisan .639 .697 p = .458

(.042) (.040)

Page 150: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

137

Control Empowered Voter Dependent variable (N=51) (N=61)

Media trust

Partisan .627 .713 (.032) (.027) F = .801

Nonpartisan .663 .696 p = .373 (.030) (.029)

Note: Table entries are estimated means with standard errors in parentheses, drawn from an analysis of covariance with condition, partisanship, political knowledge, and interactions for condition x partisanship and condition x political knowledge. All scales run from zero to one, with higher values representing higher scale in dependent variables.

Page 151: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

138

Table 6.3

Moderating Effects of Political Knowledge among Young Voters

Control Empowered Voter Dependent variable (N=51) (N=61)

Intent to vote

0-1 answered correctly .515 .448 (n=4) (.128) (.222)

2 answered correctly .656 .817 Condition x Knowledge (n=5) (.159) (.128) F = .334

3 answered correctly .819 .974 p = .564 (n=18) (.070) (.078)

4 answered correctly .868 .844 (n=28) (.066) (.054)

5 answered correctly .811 .893 (n=57) (.045) (.039)

Civic duty 0-1 .450 .765

(.113) (.196) 2 .751 .812 (.140) (.113) F = .025

3 .810 .841 p = .875 (.062) (.069)

4 .886 .830 (.059) (.047)

5 .810 .927 (.039) (.035)

Internal efficacy 0-1 .520 .603

(.105) (.183) 2 .700 .626 (.131) (.105) F = .005

3 .636 .691 p = .945 (.058) (.064)

4 .716 .647 (.055) (.044)

5 .749 .775 (.037) (.032)

Page 152: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

139

Control Empowered Voter

Dependent variable (N=51) (N=61)

External efficacy 0-1 .689 .643

(.098) (.170) 2 .732 .741 (.122) (.098) F = .833

3 .634 .678 p = .364 (.054) (.060)

4 .618 .639 (.051) (.041)

5 .586 .678 (.034) (.030)

Meaningfulness of elections 0-1 .639 .629

(.073) (.127) 2 .807 .857 (.091) (.073) F = .159

3 .699 .707 p = .691 (.040) (.045)

4 .706 .742 (.038) (.031)

5 .686 .758 (.025) (.022)

Information seeking 0-1 .598 .765

(.125) (.218) 2 .581 .412 (.156) (.125) F = .553

3 .659 .754 p = .459 (.069) (.077)

4 .618 .718 (.065) (.053)

5 .571 .682 (.044) (.038)

Page 153: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

140

Control Empowered Voter

Dependent variable (N=51) (N=61)

Media trust 0-1 .528 .820

(.089) (.154) 2 .792 .596 (.110) (.089) F = .145

3 .670 .736 p = .705 (.049) (.054)

4 .660 .681 (.046) (.037)

5 .632 .719 (.031) (.027)

Note: Table entries are estimated means with standard errors in parentheses, drawn from an analysis of covariance with condition, partisanship, political knowledge, and interactions for condition x partisanship and condition x political knowledge. All scales run from zero to one, with higher values representing higher scale in dependent variables.

DISCUSSION

This experiment illustrates that how journalists feature voters in election

coverage has important implications for young adults’ political attitudes and for the news

media. When young adults are exposed to news coverage featuring voters as empowered

agents in electoral process, their participatory intentions, sense of civic duty, external

efficacy, perceived meaningfulness of elections, information seeking desires, and trust in

news media increased. These findings show that while today’s youth have not been

brought up in the news environment in which their roles as voters are discussed in an

empowering way, a subtle change in news coverage might encourage today’s young

adults to get involved in electoral politics.

Page 154: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

141

Moreover, comparisons between the older sample and young adults suggest that

the positive gains of the news coverage are larger among young adults. While there were

no gains in older citizens’ sense of civic duty, perceived meaningfulness of elections, and

information seeking desires, young adults gained a higher level of civic duty, perception

that elections are meaningful, and information seeking desires after reading the

empowered voter portrayal article.

Of course, some of these effects may be attributed to the nature of the college

student sample. Sears (1986) suggests that college students are systematically different

from general populations or other late adolescents in general since they have “unusually

adept cognitive skills” (p. 521) and a tendency to easily comply with authority. However,

it is also the case that (1) the fact that the older sample gained in the areas of intention to

vote, external efficacy, and trust in news media as well indicate that at least in these

areas, the gains are generalizable, and (2) the areas that showed significant increase only

among young adults—sense of civic duty, perceived meaningfulness of elections, and

information seeking desires—are the areas that young adults in general have a

considerable deficiency when considered in light of older citizens because of their lack of

positive political socialization experiences. Therefore, it is reasonable to expect that older

populations are less likely to gain in these areas because they may have already

established their sense of civic duty and perception that elections are meaningful, and

possess certain level of political information. Meanwhile, young adults who are in the

process of learning their roles as citizens are more likely to be influenced by these

Page 155: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

142

socialization experiences in establishing their sense of civic duty, finding meanings of

elections, and gaining more information.

Whether or not these results are generalizable to other adolescents is an empirical

question. Studies show that there are some differences between college youth and non-

college (or “working”) youth in terms of political experiences (Jarvis, Montoya, &

Mulvoy, 2005). For instance, Jarvis et al. (2005) suggest, “young workers report lower

levels of political socialization and interest as well as fewer civic skills, group

memberships and mobilization opportunities than college students” (p. 2), and are less

likely to participate in political activities. Thus, positive effects of the empowered voter

portrayal found among the college sample provide a good reason to replicate it with other

populations of young adults.

This study offers important implications for the news media as well. When young

adults were exposed to the empowered portrayals of voters in the news, not only were

they more likely to be engaged in elections, but they became more trusting of news

media. Scholars have established links between negative news coverage to higher levels

of cynicism and lower levels of trust in political institutions and in the news media

themselves (Cappella & Jamieson, 1997). The results shown here and the previous

chapter suggest that these patterns may also work in a more positive direction. Just as

negative coverage of political institutions and politicians in the news media can produce

negative feelings toward the messenger, it may be possible for the news media to produce

positive feelings toward themselves and regain trust by adjusting the ways in which

journalists portray voters.

Page 156: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

143

While it might be easier to blame young adults for their lack of interest in news

and an acute drop in news consumption, perhaps, it is time to stop pointing fingers at

young adults and start looking into what kinds of stories are told about the meanings of

electoral participation and the roles of voters in the news media. Mindich (2005) suggests

that “despite their disengagement with news, young people are as thoughtful and

passionate and self-reflective as they have ever been, ready to interact with news if we

just provide the right conditions for them to do so” (p. x). One of the right conditions

may simply be putting the voters back into where they belong—the center of democratic

process. The data here, at least, suggest potential benefits of doing so for the youth,

citizens, news media, and the future of democracy.

Page 157: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

144

Chapter 7. Conclusion

A 2006 survey conducted by the National Opinion Research Center found that

Americans are the “proudest people in the world” (Smith & Kim, 2006). Indeed, after

interviewing citizens of 34 countries, their data reveal that Americans reported the

highest levels of “national pride” and “pride in their democratic state” (Smith & Kim,

2006). At the same time, however, millions of Americans do not participate in the most

fundamental act that preserves their democracy: voting.

In order to better understand America’s electoral hypocrisy and the decline in

voter turnout, this study has examined how Americans have been encouraged to think

about the vote, the voting process and their roles as voters through the news media. To

review, I conducted a comprehensive content analysis of a random stratified sample of

2,138 of electoral key terms—Vote, Voter, Voting, Election and Electorate—in selected

newspapers from 1948 to 2004. I also conducted two experiments to explore the effects

of these portrayals on political attitudes among older adults (n = 222) and young adults (n

= 112).

Results from the content analysis revealed that while voters were increasingly

mentioned in the news, voters and their electoral participation have not been portrayed in

meaningful ways. Chapter Three presented how, overall, voters have not been featured in

a positive manner, have been sidelined in the democratic process, have rarely been

reminded of democratic responsibilities, and have not been connected to each other or the

political past or future in news coverage. Chapter Four looked underneath these dominant

Page 158: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

145

portrayals for potential shifts in coverage over time. The data presented there show how

voters have been (1) featured as empowered and closely linked to political parties in

1948-1968; (2) subsumed under opinion polls and increasingly featured as pawns of elites

in 1972-2000; and (3) featured as faced with challenges in engaging in the electoral

process in 2004.

The effects of the empowered news portrayals of voters found in the 1950s and

1960s were examined with two experiments: initially with the general population, then

with young adults. These experiments revealed that the empowered portrayal of voters

had a positive impact on citizens’ political attitudes. The first experiment revealed that

when exposed to the empowered voter portrayal, adults’ participatory intentions, external

efficacy and trust in news media increased (Chapter Five). The positive effects of the

empowered portrayal of voters in the news were greater among college students. In

addition to the effects witness among the general population (an increase in participatory

intentions, external efficacy and trust in news media), young adults’ sense of civic duty,

perceived meaningfulness of elections and information seeking desire increased as well

when they were exposed to the empowered voter portrayal in the news (Chapter Six). In

both experiments, these effects were not moderated by partisanship or political

knowledge.

There are two major findings of this project. A first finding concerns the

relationship between portrayals of voters and historical trends in political engagement.

My data suggest that voters were more likely to be featured as empowered agents in the

democratic process in the period when political engagement and voter turnout were

Page 159: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

146

relatively high (1948-1968). During the time of decline in electoral participation, voters

became increasingly featured as pawns of elites (1972-2000). These correlations suggest

that portrayals of voters in news coverage may play an important role in forming citizens’

political attitudes and behaviors. Results from experiments conducted in this dissertation

underscore this possibility. While it is hard to establish the causal relationship between

news portrayals of voters and political engagement during these political moments (e.g.,

was the empowered portrayal of voters leading to more political engagement in the 1950s

and 1960s, or the other way around?), it is still noteworthy that these patterns were found

together.

A second key finding concerns news narratives and trust in news media. My data

from Chapters Five and Six suggest that portrayals of voters in the news can positively

influence citizen’s opinions toward the press. Experiments showed that when newspapers

featured voters as efficacious and as empowered agents in the democratic system, not

only were citizens’ attitudes toward politics and electoral participation positively

affected, but their trust in the news media also increased significantly. These findings

suggest that a subtle change in electoral news coverage may help the press regain

citizens’ trust and, possibly, their readership.

CONTRIBUTIONS This dissertation has a number of contributions to the study of electoral news

coverage, political socialization, attribute agenda-setting and the profession of

journalism.

Page 160: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

147

First, this dissertation adds to the study of electoral news coverage. Previous

research on political news coverage has focused largely on political elites and has

identified a set of troubling patterns in news coverage. To review, Bennett (1988)

suggests that news is personalized, dramatized, fragmentized and normalized.

Patterson (1993) suggests that the press is in the news business (not the democracy

business) and that the journalists’ frame of viewing and interpreting political events in

terms of a game schema limits citizens’ ability to think outside of winning and losing

candidates. Cappella and Jamieson (1997) observe that journalists tend to report

candidates’ motives behind their actions and rhetoric and these types of strategic

coverage produce political distrust and cynicism among citizens.

While previous research has focused largely on coverage of political issues and

elites, this study has focused on coverage of voters (non-elites) and locates new cause for

concern. Intriguingly, the expectations of these scholars hold true in coverage of voting;

curiously, though, the patterns take on a unique cast when the focus is on citizens rather

than political elites. For instance,

• while Bennett’s work might suggest that coverage of voting would be

personalized, this study shows that the subject of personalization in electoral

news coverage is candidates, not voters. Voters are often used to develop the

personalization of candidates and are rarely the subject of intense or detailed

personalization themselves in electoral news coverage;

• while Patterson’s work might suggest that a game schema surrounding the

coverage of voting would include voters as players in the game, this study

Page 161: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

148

finds that game frames explicitly leave voters on the sidelines in news

coverage by featuring them as simply numbers and demographics devoid of

political agency; and

• while Cappella and Jamieson’s work might suggest that the coverage of voting

would examine the motives of electoral participation, this study details how

voters—especially after the 1960s—are rarely given credit for having any type

of incentives or goals whatsoever.

To sum, the expectations of prior research do inform the patterns described here.

Interestingly, however, they take on a different cast when the focus is on citizens rather

than political elites.

Because prior research has focused so heavily on elites, the recommending force

of prior works implies that voter turnout has decreased because candidates are portrayed

in unfavorable ways. This dissertation adds an important element to that argument. When

looking to media coverage as a possible reason for decreased turnout, the press’

reluctance to remind voters to think about their connection to ideas and policy stances, to

each other, to a larger set of intuitions, or to the ideal of democracy (the sort that appears

in President Bush’s speech that opened this dissertation) may also shape turnout patterns.

Admittedly, the qualitative patterns documented in my content analytic data are subtle.

They are also, however, quite stable. It is entirely possible that the persistent lack of

attention to democratic roles of voters in print news may have desensitized citizens to

important role(s) in electoral politics.

Page 162: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

149

Second, this study contributes to the study of political socialization by providing

broader understandings of news media as a powerful socializing force in citizens’

political development. To review, the role of news media in political socialization has

largely been overlooked (Buckingham, 1997; Calavita, 2003). When the role of news

media is examined, the focus of studies has been largely limited to the acquisition of

political knowledge and of partisan attitudes (e.g., Atkin & Gantz, 1978; Chaffee, Ward,

& Tipton, 1970; Dominick, 1972; Drew & Reeves, 1980; Hollander, 1971; Martinelli &

Chaffee, 1995; Rubin, 1976). This project departs from the traditional approach and

investigates how news media define what it means to be a citizen in a democratic state

and the effects of these portrayals.

There are three key findings in this dissertation that add to the study of political

socialization. First, this project suggests that different generations of citizens may have

been socialized to view their roles as voters differently via news media. Results from the

content analysis reveal that there have been shifts in news coverage of voters over the

past sixty years. Therefore, citizens who were raised in the 1950s and the 1960s may

have been encouraged to think of their roles as voters as central to the democratic system,

and those who were raised in after the 1970s to view their roles as voters as pawns of

elites.

Next, this project illustrates that the way news media portray the roles of citizens

may have important effects on citizens’ political socialization. This study illustrates that

shifts in news coverage of voters are related to shifts in political energy among citizens:

voters were featured as more efficacious and empowered during the time of robust

Page 163: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

150

electoral engagement (1948-1968) compared to later periods when voters were subsumed

under opinion polls and featured as pawns of elites in the news (1972-2000). While it is

impossible to assess the effects of news coverage of voters during the 1950s and the

1960s with today’s citizens, correlations between news portrayals of voters and political

engagement along with results from two experiments suggest that portrayals of voters in

news coverage can play an important role in forming citizens’ political attitudes and

behaviors.

Furthermore, this study indicates that socialization gains from positive portrayals

of citizens in the news may be contingent on age. While older adults showed some

positive gains in their political attitudes after reading the empowering news portrayals

witnessed in 1948-1968, larger gains were observed among young adults—an increase in

civic duty, meaningfulness of elections and information seeking desires in addition to

those found in older adults (an increase in participatory intentions, external efficacy and

trust in news media). These patterns suggest that more empowered news coverage could

be particularly beneficial to young adults who are at the prime of learning their roles and

responsibilities as citizens.

Third, this dissertation contributes to the expansion of attribute agenda-setting

theory. To review, the theory of attribute agenda-setting suggests that portrayals of an

object in the news media influence people’s attitudes toward the object. A number of

studies have examined the attributes of issues and candidates in news reports and their

effects on citizens’ political attitudes. This study adds to the theory by introducing an

analysis of attributes of citizens (voters) and civic acts (electoral participation) and their

Page 164: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

151

effects on citizens’ political attitudes. Also, a more extensive coding scheme and a more

nuanced unit of analysis were used than in most projects examining attribute agenda-

setting. Results obtained from a longitudinal analysis of attributes and their effects on

citizens’ political attitudes suggest a great potential in the application of this theory to

political socialization.

In addition, while the original theory of attribute agenda-setting only suggests the

primary effect of attribute agenda-setting (i.e., attributes of an object salient in news

media influence people’s attitudes toward the object), this project indicates the secondary

effect of attribute agenda-setting (i.e., attributes of an object salient in news media

influence people’s attitudes toward the new media). On the secondary effects of attribute

agenda-setting, Kiousis (2002) states that while “second-level [attribute] agenda-setting

theory postulates that news attention to certain aspects of politics affect public opinion of

politics (primary object),” it is possible that “press emphasis on certain aspects of politics

might also impact public perceptions of the press (secondary object)” (p. 561). Results

from experiments support these patterns. When citizens encountered news coverage

featuring voters as empowered agents in the democratic process, not only were their

political attitudes influenced positively, but also their trust in news media increased.

While there is a need to replicate this study in a different setting in order to affirm the

relationship between the attributes of the object featured in the news and perceptions of

the news media, the results shown here demonstrate a strong link between the two and

provide strong bases for further investigation.

Page 165: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

152

Finally, this study offers practical implications for the press and journalists. To

review, the press and journalists have suffered severe declines in trust among the public

over the past several decades. Survey data show that the percentage of people who

express a “great deal of confidence” in the press dropped from around 30 percent in early

1970s to only 12 percent in 2000 (Cook, Gronke, & Rattliff, 2000). While other

governmental institutions (such as the Executive branch, Congress, etc.) have also

experienced similar declines in trust over the past three decades, the decline in trust in the

news media has been most severe among other political institutions (Gronke & Cook,

2007).

Although there are numerous possible explanations as to why such decline has

occurred,19 many suggest that the way news media cover politics is partly to be blamed

(Cappella & Jamieson 1997; Kiousis, 2002; Patterson, 1993). For instance, Cappella and

Jamieson (1996) suggest that a strategic framing of news media creates cynicism and

distrust against the news media. Cappella and Jamieson (1996) state:

The way in which the news media frame political events stimulates cynicism.

When reporters persistently focus on self-interest as the motivation for political

decisions, they may be helping create the mistrust that feeds their own reporting

and ultimately feeds back on the institution they represent (p. 84).

19 Gronke and Cook (2007) list some of the most often cited explanations for the decline in news media trust, “the ‘blaming-the-messenger’ phenomenon, a spillover effect of the growing cynicism of the public, public disaffection from more negative ‘gotcha’ journalism, increasing attacks by politicians on the news media” (p. 262). In addition, they observe, “confidence in the press has fallen in part because those groups that formerly constituted a core of support for the press (Democrats, liberals, partisans in opposition to the party in power) have shrunk considerably over the last three decades” (p. 275).

Page 166: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

153

This study follows these researchers’ footsteps, but by focusing on the positive

rather than the negative effects of electoral news coverage, this study shows that the press

may be able to rebuild trust by implementing subtle changes in political campaign

coverage. Results from experiments illustrate that featuring voters as empowered and

efficacious in electoral news coverage can increase citizens’ trust in news media.

Besides an obvious benefit of gaining the legitimacy of the press, this finding is

critical for journalists because trust in news media is linked to news exposure (Gans,

2003; Kiousis, 2001; Rimmer & Weaver, 1987). Along with the decline in trust in news

media, news consumption has declined over the years, and the downward trend has been

particularly acute among youth (Peiser, 2000; Patterson, 2007). A survey conducted on

1,298 citizens (613 young adults, 685 older adults) reports that while 35% of those who

are over 30 years old read a newspaper every day, only 16% of those who are between

18-30 years old do (Patterson, 2007). Because the future of the press depends on news

consumption of today’s youth, it is imperative for the press to find a way to regain their

trust. This study suggests that journalists may be able to gain some public trust and,

possibly, their readership, by bringing voters back to the center stage in electoral news

coverage. Future research should examine these patterns in a more detailed and grounded

way.

LIMITATIONS While there are several major contributions in this dissertation, this study is not

without limitations. The first set of limitations concern the content analysis. For one,

Page 167: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

154

findings from this content analysis are limited to the datasets and the categories discussed

in Chapter Two. Therefore, other potentially important attributes of electoral participation

featured in American newspapers may have been overlooked in this study. Another

limitation is that because only 41 word clusters serve as my coding units (20 words

immediately before and after the keywords), I may have forgone some of the contexts

surrounding the electoral key terms.

Second, there are limitations concerning the measurement used in the

experiments. Because my experiments solely relied on self-reports, responses to

behavioral questions such as previous voting behaviors and participatory intentions

cannot be validated. In addition, the accuracy of political knowledge measurement used

may be in question. As noted in Chapter Six, this variable had a low reliability score

(below 0.48) and unusually high average score (on average, four out of five questions

were answered correctly) among college students. It is suggested that measuring political

knowledge (or sophistication) is tricky business (Luskin, 1987), but the unusually high

level of political knowledge in this sample signals some irregularities. I suspect that

because the experiments were conducted online and there was no time-limit imposed on

answering these questions (unlike in the experiment with older adults), some of the

participants may have looked up their answers online while taking the survey. Future

studies should be mindful of these potential pitfalls.

Third, while experiments were conducted with two different populations, the

limited external validity of the experiment using a college student sample should be

acknowledged. While it is widely common to use college sophomores in experiments,

Page 168: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

155

college students are considered to have unique characteristics that could lead to an

overestimation of effects (Sears, 1986). Therefore, although larger effects were found

among young adults compared to the general population, these effects may be simply

because of the nature of college student sample, and not necessarily generalizable to non-

college youth. In order to more accurately assess the effects of the empowered voter

portrayal on young voters, a replication of the experiment with non-college youth is

necessary.

Fourth, another limitation concerning the generalizability of the experiments

comes from the context of the experiment. Since my experiments were conducted in the

2007-2008 election cycle, results may be unique to this election cycle. We do not know if

the results gained from the experiments in this study will continue to hold in other

election cycles. To remedy this, a replication of the experiment during a different election

cycle is necessary.

Fifth, it should also be recognized that the effects observed in the experiments

may be short lived. While results from experiments demonstrate that the empowered

voter portrayal had positive impacts on citizens’ political attitudes and their trust in news

media, we do not know if these effects can be sustained for a long time. In order to

measure the long-term effects of the empowered voter portrayal, a longitudinal study

should be conducted.

Sixth, it should be noted that this study does not demonstrate the causal

relationship between newspaper portrayals of voters and political engagement during the

1950s and the 1960s. Although the experiments attempted to test the effects of portrayals

Page 169: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

156

of voters in newspapers during the 1950s and the 1960s, there is no simple way of

empirically testing the actual effects of the empowered voter portrayal on those who were

living at that time and exposed to these news coverage in the 1950s and the 1960s. The

effects gained from these experiments are only applicable to today’s citizens, and not to

the citizens who were living in the 1950s and the 1960s.

Finally, my findings in this dissertation are limited to one news medium: print

newspapers. During political campaign periods, citizens are exposed to various news

outlets including television, radio, magazines, blogs, etc. Although newspapers tend to be

the dominant agenda-setter for other news outlets (McCombs, 2004; Roberts &

McCombs, 1994), in order to fully understand the portrayals and the effects of voters in

the news, further investigation on how other news outlets feature electoral participation is

necessary. It should also be acknowledged that because there are so many different news

sources that citizens interact during election periods, the effects found in this study may

not be as potent in a real media environment as witnessed here.

QUESTIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH

This dissertation begins to unpack the portrayals of voters and electoral

participation and some of the implications of these patterns for citizens and the press.

Based on the findings discussed here, several questions for future research are raised.

First, what do journalists have to say about the patterns located in this study? It

would be interesting to interview news media professionals on what was found in the

content analysis, and ask fundamental questions, including: are voters newsworthy?

Page 170: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

157

How? When? Why? Additionally, it would be curious to share the key findings of the

experiment with journalists and solicit their feedback on this score. If my research has

identified a subtle means of increasing intentions to participate in politics and trust in the

media, what should and can journalists, their editors and newspaper ownership do about

this? Interviewing the news media professionals on the patterns and practices of the press

located in this study will provide important insights into what can be done to provide

electoral news coverage that is more democratic and empowering to the electorate.

Second, while the present study focuses on the portrayals of American electoral

participation in the news, it would also be helpful to conduct comparative analyses of

how voters and voting are portrayed in the news. Future studies could track how news

media portray voters in other democratic countries, and examine if news coverage of

voters differ among countries with higher and lower levels of voter turnout. Studies could

also assess portrayals of voters in various types of democratic systems.

Third, research could also focus on how voters, the vote and the voting process

are portrayed in film, television, literature, song and other forms of popular culture. Are

voters portrayed more prominently? With greater detail? In which media? During which

historical periods? Answers to these questions can round out the findings of this content

analysis of print news coverage.

Additionally, future studies could track the coverage surrounding the 2008

presidential election. Will voters be treated in thin ways during this exciting election

year? Or, will the coverage more closely resemble the empowered narratives of the 1950s

and the 1960s? If voters are depicted with greater agency, will the attributes look like

Page 171: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

158

those from the 1950s and the 1960s? Or will they be unique to this political moment?

Answers to these questions will help to set expectations for future campaigns and will

help inform how younger citizens’ political worlds will be shaped, now and into the

future.

CONCLUSION Americans have not been encouraged to think about their votes and their roles as

voters as central to democratic process through print news coverage. The “story” of

voting featured in the news has been one in which voters and their electoral participation

are sidelined. I believe that this major finding helps to explain the puzzle guiding this

project. Americans continue to be proud of their democracy—the larger structure that

makes democratic life possible. My data also lead me to suspect that Americans have also

become less mindful of their personal responsibility in safeguarding their system through

voting.

The news, here, is not all bad. This dissertation also shows that if citizens are

reminded of their role as voters in a print news article, several potentially exciting

patterns ensue. Adults express a higher likelihood of voting and of trusting the press;

college students report these positive outcomes and more.

These findings lead me to a normative statement: what Americans need now,

especially at the time of unstable voter turnout and political engagement, is more

democratic coverage of elections; stories that report on the experience, action and policy

preferences of candidates and the roles, responsibilities and political desires of voters. It

Page 172: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

159

should be noted that this is not to suggest that news must return to an earlier era—for the

romantic sake of returning to a less cynical time—but to encourage news organizations to

re-insert voters into the center of democratic discussion.

Political elites often talk about the importance of voters and voting (as President

Bush did at the outset of this project). More holistic coverage of elections could remind

Jill Davidson (also mentioned at the outset of this project) of her role, as well.

Page 173: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

160

References

Abramson, P. R. (1983). Political attitudes in America: formation and change. San

Francisco, CA: Freesman.

Abramson, P. R., & Aldrich, J. H. (1982). The decline of electoral participation in

America. American Political Science Review, 76(3), 502-521.

Almond, G. A., & Verba, S. (1963). The civic culture: political attitudes and democracy

in five nations. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

American National Election Studies (www.electionstudies.org). The ANES guide to

public opinion and electoral behavior. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan,

Center for Political Studies [producer and distributor].

Andolina, M. W., Jenkins, K., Zukin, C., & Keeter, S. (2003). Habits from home, lessons

from school: influences on youth civic engagement. Political Science& Politics,

36(2), 275-280.

Atkin, C. K., & Gantz, W. (1978). Television news and political socialization. Public

Opinion quarterly, 42, 183-197.

Barnhurst, K. G., & Mutz, D. (1997). American newspapers and the decline in event-

centered reporting, Journal of Communication, 47(4), 27-53.

Beck, P. A. (1977). The role of agents in political socialization. In S. A. Renshon (Ed.),

Handbook of political socialization: theory and research (pp.115-141). New

York, NY: Free Press.

Page 174: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

161

Beck, P. A., & Jennings, M. K. (1975). Parents as ‘middlepersons’ in political

socialization. Journal of Politics, 37, 81-107.

Beck, P. A., & Jennings, M. K. (1982). Pathways to participation. American Political

Science Review, 76(1), 94-108.

Beekman, H. R. (1891). An act making voting compulsory, Annals of the American

Academy of Political and Social Science, 1, 613.

Belli, R. F., Traugott, M. W., Young, M., & McGonagle, K. A. (1999). Reducing vote

over-reporting in surveys: Social desirability, memory failure, and source

monitoring. Public Opinion Quarterly, 63, 90-108.

Bennett, S. E. (2000). Political apathy and avoidance of news media among Generations

X and Y: America’s continuing problem. In S. Mann & J. J. Patrick (Eds.),

Education for civic engagement in democracy: Service learning and other

practices. Bloomington, IN: ERIC Clearinghouse for Social Studies/Science

Education.

Bennett, W. L. (1988/2001). News: The politics of illusion. New York, NY: Longman.

Bennett, W. L. (2008). Changing citizenship in the digital age. In W. L. Bennett (Ed),

Civic life online: Learning how digital media can engage youth (pp. 1-24).

Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Berelson, B. (1971). Content analysis in communication research. New York, NY:

Hafner Publishing Company.

Page 175: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

162

Brody, R. A. (1978). The puzzle of political participation in America. In Anthony King

(Ed.), The new American political system. Washington, DC: American Economic

Institute Press.

Brosius, H., & Hans Kepplinger, M. (1990). Agenda-setting function of television news.

Communication Research, 17(2), 183-211.

Buckingham, D. (1997). News media, political socialization and popular citizenship:

towards a new agenda. Critical Studies in Mass Communication, 14, 344-366.

Buckingham, D. (1999). Young people, politics and news media: beyond political

socialisation. Oxford Review of Education, 25(1&2). 171-184.

Calavita, M. (2003). Within the context of many contexts: family, news media

engagement, and the ecology of individual political development among

‘Generation Xers’ Communication Review, 6(1), 23-43.

Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., Miller, W. E., & Stokes, D. E. (1960). The American

voter. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Campbell, A., Gurin, G., & Miller, W. E. (1954). The voter decides. Evanston, Ill.: Row,

Peterson.

Cappella, J. B., & Jamieson, K. H. (1996). News frames, political cynicism, and media

cynicism. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, Vol.

546, 71-84.

Cappella, J. B., & Jamieson, K. H. (1997). Spiral of cynicism: The press and the public

good. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Page 176: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

163

Chaffee, S. H., Ward, L. S., & Tipton, L. (1970). Mass communication and political

socialization. Journalism Quarterly, 47, 647-659.

Chong, D., Citrin, J., & Conley, P. (2001). When self-interest matters, Political

Pscychology, 22(3), 541-570.

Chyi, H. I., & McCombs, M. E. (2004). Media salience and the process of framing:

coverage of the Columbine school shooting. Journalism and Mass

Communication Quarterly, 81(1), 22-35.

Cohen, B. (1963). The press and foreign policy. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University

Press.

Conover, P. J., & Searing, D. D. (2000). A political socialization perspective. In L.

McDonnell, P. M. Timpane & R. W. Benjamin (Eds.), Rediscovering the

Democratic Purposes of Education, Lawrence, KA: University of Kansas Press.

Converse, P. E. (1972). Change in the American electorate. In A. Campbell & P. E.

Converse (Eds.), The Human Meaning of Social Change (pp. 263-337). New

York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.

Conway, M. M., & Damico, A. J. (2001). Building blocks: the relationship between high

school and adult associational life, a paper presented at the Annual Meeting of

the American Political Science Association, San Francisco, CA.

Cook, T. E., Gronke, P., & Rattliff, J. (2000). Disdaining the media: The American

public’s changing attitudes toward the news. Paper presented at the annual

meeting of the American Political Science Association, Washington, DC.

Page 177: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

164

Cooper, A. (2001). Nominating presidential candidates: The primary season compared to

two alternatives, in Political Research Quarterly, 54(4), 771-793.

Craig, S. C. (1979). Efficacy, trust, and political behavior: An attempt to resolve a

lingering conceptual dilemma, American Politics Quarterly, 7(2), 225-239.

Craig, S. C., & Maggiotto, M. A. (1982). Measuring political efficacy, Political

Methodology, 8(3), 85-109.

Crespi, I. (1980). Polls as journalism. Public Opinion Quarterly, 44(4), 462-476.

Dahl, R. (1976). Modern political analysis. 3rd ed. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall,

Inc.

Dahl, R. (1998). On democracy. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

Dalton, R. (2007). The good citizen: How a younger generation is reshaping American

politics. Washington, DC: Congressional Quarterly.

Dawson, R. E., & Prewitt, K. (1969). Political socialization. Boston, MA: Little Brown.

Delli Carpini, M. X. (2000). Gen.com: Youth, civic engagement, and the new

information environment. Political Communication, 17, 341-349.

Delli Carpini, M. X., & Keeter, S. (1996). What Americans know about politics and why

it matters. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

Dominick, J. (1972). Television and political socialization. Educational Broadcasting

Review, 6, 48-56.

Doppelt, J., & Shearer, E. (2001). America’s no-shows—nonvoters: who they are, why

they don’t vote and what it could take to bring them to the polls. Retrieved June

18, 2004, from

Page 178: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

165

http://www.pewtrusts.com/ideas/ideas_item.cfm?content_item_id=732&content_type_id=8&issue_name=Yo

uth%20voting&issue=7&page=8&name=Grantee%20Reports

Downs, A. (1957). An economic theory of democracy. New York, NY: Harper.

Drew, D., & Reese, S. (1984).Children’s learning from a television newscast. Journalism

Quarterly, 67(1), 83-88.

Easton, D., & Dennis, J. (1969). Children in the political system. New York, NY:

McGraw-Hill.

Eaton, H. Jr. (1989) Agenda-setting with bi-weekly data on content of three national

media. Journalism Quarterly 66, 942-948.

Egan, A. (2008, March 26). How Youth Rule An Election. Forbes.com. Retrieved May 4,

2008, from http://www.forbes.com/opinions/2008/03/25/campaign-young-staffers-election-oped-

cx_ae_0326egan.html

Fallows, J. (1997). Breaking the news. New York, NY: Vintage.

Feldman, K. A., & T. M. Newcomb. (1969). The impact of college on students: an

analysis of four decades of research. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Finkel, S. E. (1985). Reciprocal effects of participation and political efficacy: a panel

analysis. American Journal of Political Science, 29(4), 891-913.

Fiorina, M. P. (1981). Retrospective voting in American national elections. New Haven,

CT: Yale University Press.

Franklin, C. H. (1991). Efficient estimation in experiments. Political Methodologist, 4(1),

1-3.

Freeman, J. L., & Frazer, S. C. (1966). Compliance without pressure: the foot-in-the-door

technique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4(2), 195-202.

Page 179: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

166

Gans, H. J. (2003). Democracy and the news. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Gerber, A. S., & Green, D. P. (2000). The effects of canvassing, telephone calls, and

direct mail on voter turnout: a field experiment. The American Political Science

Review, 94(3), 653-663.

Gerber, A. S., Green, P. D., & Shachar, R. (2003). Voting may be habit-forming:

evidence from randomized field experiment. American Journal of Political

Science, 47(3), 540-550.

Golan, G., & Wanta, W. (2001). Second-level agenda setting in the New Hampshire

primary: a comparison of coverage in three newspapers and public perceptions of

candidates. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 78, 247-59.

Goodwin-Gill, G. S. (2006). Free and fair elections. Geneva: Inter-Parliamentary Union.

Graber, D. (2000). Adapting political news to the needs of the twenty-first century

Americans. In L. Bennett & R. M. Entman (Eds.), Mediated politics:

Communication in the future of democracy (pp. 433-452). Cambridge, MA:

Cambridge University Press.

Greenstein, F. I. (1968). Political socialization. In D. L. Sills (Ed.), International

encyclopedia of the social sciences. New York, NY: Macmillan and Free Press.

Gronke, P., & Cook, T. E. (2007). Disdaining the media: the American public’s changing

attitudes toward the news. Political Communication, 24(3), 259-281.

Hais, M., & Winograd, M. (2008, February 7). Millennials Are About to Give American

Politics an Extreme Makeove. The Huffington Post. Retrieved May 4, 2008, from

http://www.huffingtonpost.com/michael-hais-and-morley-winograd/millennials-are-about-to-_b_85556.html

Page 180: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

167

Hart, R. P. (1999). Seducing America: how television charms the modern voter. Revised

edition, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Hart, R. P. (2000). Campaign talk: why elections are good for us. Princeton, NJ:

Princeton University Press.

Hart, R. P., Jarvis, S. E., Jennings, W. P., & Smith-Howell, D. (2005). Political

keywords: using language that uses us, New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Hart, R. P., Smith-Howell, D., & Llewellyn, J. (1991). The mindscape of the presidency:

Time magazine, 1945-1985, Journal of Communication, 41(3), 6-25.

Hess, R. D., & Torney, J. V. (1967). The development of political attitudes in children.

Chicago, IL: Aldine.

Hester, J. B., & Gibson, R. (2003). The economy and second-level agenda-setting: a

time-series analysis of economic news and public opinion about the economy.

Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly, 80(1), 73-90.

Hoffman, J. (2008, April 8). Young Obama Backers Twist Parents’ Arms. The New York

Times. Retrieved May 4, 2008 from

http://www.nytimes.com/2008/04/08/us/politics/08kids.html?ex=1208318400&en=b9601c284cd9e852&ei=5

070&emc=eta1

Hollander, N. (1971). Adolescents and the war: the sources of socialization. Journalism

Quarterly, 48, 472-479.

Hyman, H. (1959). Political socialization. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.

Inglehart, R. (1997). Modernization and postmodernization. Princeton, NJ: Princeton

University Press.

Page 181: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

168

Iyengar, S. (1991) Is anyone responsible? How television frames political issues.

Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.

Iyengar, S., & Kinder, D. R. (1987). News that matters. Chicago, IL: University of

Chicago Press.

Jarvis, S. E. (2005). The talk of the party: political labels, symbolic capital, and

American life. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers, Inc.

Jarvis, S. E., Montoya, L.M., & Mulvoy, E. (2005). The political participation of working

youth and college students. Working Paper. The Center for Information &

Research on Civic Learning & Engagement (CIRCLE). Retrieved June 2, 2008

from http://www.civicyouth.org/index.php?s=montoya

Jennings, M. K., & Niemi, R. G. (1981). Generations and politics. Princeton, NJ:

Princeton University Press.

Kam, C. D. (2007). When duty calls, do citizens answer? Journal of Politics, 69 (1), 17-

29.

Kernell, S. (1997). Going public: New strategies of presidential leadership. Washington,

DC: CQ Press.

Key, V. O. (1966). The responsible electorate: rationality in presidential voting, 1936-

1960. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.

Kim, K., & McCombs, M. E. (2007). News story descriptions and the public’s opinions

of political candidates. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 84(2),

299-314.

Page 182: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

169

Kim, S., Scheufele, D. A., & Shanahan, J. (2002). Think about it this way: attribute

agenda-setting function of the press and the public’s evaluation of a local issue.

Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly, 79(1), 7-25.

Kinder, D. R., & D. R. Kiewiet. (1979). Economic discontent and political behavior: the

role of personal grievances and collective economic judgments in congressional

voting. American Journal of Political Science, 23(3), 495-527.

Kiousis, S. (2001). Public trust or mistrust? Perceptions of media credibility in the

information age. Mass Communication and Society, 4, 381-403.

Kiousis, S. (2002). Killing the messenger: an exploration of presidential newspaper

coverage and public confidence in the press, Journalism Studies, 3(4), 557-572.

Kiousis, S. (2003). Job approval and favorability: the impact of media attention to the

Monica Lewinsky scandal and public opinion of President Bill Clinton, Mass

Communication & Society, 6(4), 435-51.

Kiousis, S., Bantimaroudis, P., & Ban, H. (1999). Candidate image attributes:

experiments on the substantive dimension of second level agenda-setting.

Communication Research, 26(4), 414-428.

Kornberg, A., & Clarke, H. D. (1992). Citizens and community. New York, NY:

Cambridge University Press.

Krippendorff, K. A. (1980). Content analysis: an introduction to its methodology.

Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.

Lane, R. (1959). Political life: why people get involved in politics. Glencoe, IL: The Free

Press.

Page 183: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

170

Lawrence, R. G. (2000). Game-framing the issues: tracking the strategy frame in public

policy news, Political Communication, 17, 93-114.

Leighley, J. E. (1995). Attitudes, opportunities and incentives: a field essay on political

participation. Political Research Quarterly, 48(1), 181-209.

Leighley, J. E. (2001). Strength in numbers? The political mobilization of racial and

ethnic minorities. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Levine, P. (2007). The future of democracy: Developing the next generation of American

citizens. Hanover, NH: Tufts University Press.

Lipset, S. M. (1963). Political man. New York, NY: Doubleday & Company, Inc.

Litt, E. (1963). Civic education, community norms and political indoctrination. American

Sociological Review, 28, 69-75.

Lopez, M. H., Kirby, E., & Sagoff, J. (2005). The Youth Vote 2004. The Center for

Information & Research on Civic Learning & Engagement (CIRCLE). Retrieved

July 11, 2008, from http://www.civicyouth.org/quick/youth_voting.htm

Lopez, M. H., Levine, P., Both, D., Kiesa, A., Kirby, E., & Marcelo, K. (2006). The 2006

civic and political health of the nation: A detailed look at how youth participate in

politics and communities. The Center for Information & Research on Civic

Learning & Engagement (CIRCLE). Retrieved July 11, 2008, from

http://www.civicyouth.org/research/products/youth_index.htm

Luskin, R. C. (1987). Measuring political sophistication. American Journal of Political

Science, 31, 856-899.

Page 184: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

171

Macedo, S., Alex-Assensoh, Y. M., Berry, J. M., Brintnall, M., Campbell, D. E., Fraga,

L. R., Fung, A., Galston, W. A., Karpowitz, C. F., Levi, M., Levinson, M., &

Lipsitz, K. (2005). Democracy at risk: How political choices undermine citizen

participation, and what we can do about it. Washington, DC: Brookings

Institution Press.

MacManus, S. A. (1996). Young v. Old: Generational combat in the 21st century.

Boulder, CO: Westview Press.

Maisel, L. S., & Buckley, K. Z. (2004). Parties and elections in America: the electoral

process. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.

Marcus, G. E., Neuman, R. W., & Mackuen, M. (2000). Affective intelligence and

political judgment. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Martinelli, K. A., & Chaffee, S. H. (1995). Measuring new-voter learning via three

channels of political information. Journalism & Mass Communication, 72, 18-44.

Mayo, H. B. (1959). A note on the alleged duty to vote. The Journal of Politics, 21(2),

319-323.

McCombs, M. E. (2004). Setting the agenda: The mass media and public opinion.

Malden, MA: Polity Press.

McCombs, M. E., & Shaw, D. (1972). The agenda-setting function of mass media. Public

Opinion Quarterly, 36, 176-187.

McCombs, M. E., Llamas, J. P., Lopez-Escobar, E., & Lennon, F. R. (1997). Candidate

images in Spanish elections: second-level agenda-setting effects. Journalism and

Mass Communication Quarterly, 74, 703-16.

Page 185: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

172

McCombs, M. E., Lopez-Escobar, E., & Llamas, J. P. (2000). Setting the agenda of

attributes in the 1996 Spanish general election, Journal of Communication, 77-92.

McDonald, M. P., & Popkin, S. L. (2001). The myth of the vanishing voter. American

Political Science Review, 95(4), 963-974.

McGuire, W. J. (1964). Inducing resistance to persuasion: Some contemporary

approaches. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology,

Vol. 1. New York: Academic Press.

Merelman, R. M. (1980). The family and political socialization: toward a theory of

exchange. Journal of Politics, 42(2), 461-486.

Meyer, P. (2002). Precision Journalism (4th ed.). Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield

Publishers, Inc.

Miller, W. E., & Shanks, J. M. (1996). The new American voter. Cambridge, MA:

Harvard University Press.

Mindich, D. T. Z. (2005). Tuned Out: Why Americans under 40 don’t follow the news.

New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Moy, P., & M. Pfau. (2001). With malice toward all. Westport, CT: Praeger.

National Association of Secretaries of State. (1999). New millennium survey: American

youth attitudes on politics, citizenship, government & voting. Retrieved June 18,

2004 from www.stateofthevote.org/survey

Nelson, T. E., Clawson, R. A., & Oxley, Z. M. (1997). Media framing of a civil liberties

conflict and its effect on tolerance. American Political Science Review, 91, 567-

84.

Page 186: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

173

Nie, N. H., Junn, J., & Stehlik-Barry, K. (1996). Education and democratic citizenship in

America. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Nie, N. H., Verba, S., & Petrocik, J. R. (1976). The changing American voter.

Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

O’Donnell, G. (2007). The perpetual crises of democracy. Journal of Democracy,18, 5-9.

Paletz, D. L., Short, J. Y., Baker, H., Campbell, B. C., Cooper, R. J., & Oeslander, R. M.

(1980). Polls in the media: content, credibility, and consequences. Public Opinion

Quarterly, 44(4), 495-513.

Parker, R. D., & Buriel, R. (1997). Socialization in the family: ethnic and ecological

perspectives. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child

psychology, 3, New York, NY: Wiley, 463-52.

Pateman, C. (1970). Participation and democratic theory. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge

University Press.

Patterson, T. E. (1993). Out of order. New York, NY: Alfred A. Knopf.

Patterson, T. E. (2002). The vanishing voter: public involvement in an age of uncertainty.

New York, NY: Alfred A. Knopf.

Patterson, T. (2007). Young people and news. The Joan Shorenstein Center on the Press,

Politics and Public Policy. Retrieved July 17, 2008 from

http://www.hks.harvard.edu/presspol/research_publications/papers.htm

Peiser, W. (2000). Cohort replacement and the downward trend in newspaper readership.

Newspaper Research Journal, 21(2), 11-22.

Page 187: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

174

Pew Research Center For the People & the Press. (1992). Campaign ’92: Generation

divide. Washington, DC.

Pew Research Center For the People & the Press. (2007). How young people view their

lives, futures and politics: A portrait of “Generation Next.” Washington: DC.

Polsby, N. W. (1983). Consequences of Party Reform. New York, NY: Oxford University

Press.

Pomper, G. M. (1968). Elections in America. New York, NY: Dodd, Mead & Company.

Popkin, S. D. (1991). The reasoning voter: communication and persuasion in

presidential campaigns. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Presser, M. (2008, February 26). Youth Vote Surge in Austin and Across the Country.

The Austin American Statesman, Retrieved May 4, 2008, from

http://www.statesman.com/news/content/region/legislature/stories/02/26/0226youngvote.html

Price, V., & Zaller, J. R. (1993). Who gets the news? Alternative measures of news

reception and their implications for research. Public Opinion Quarterly, 57, 133-

164. .

Putnam, R. D. (2000). Bowling alone: the collapse and revival of American community.

New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.

Rahn, W. M., & Transue, J. E. (1998). Social trust and value change: the decline of social

capital in American youth, 1976-1995. Political Psychology, 19(3), 545-565.

Riker, W., & Ordeshook, P. (1968). A theory of the calculus of voting. American

Political Science Review, 62, 25-43.

Page 188: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

175

Rimmer, T., & Weaver, D. (1987). Different questions, different answers? Media use and

media credibility. Journalism Quarterly, 64, 28-36.

Roberts, M., & McCombs, M. (1994). “Agenda setting and political advertising: origins

of the news agenda,” Political Communication, 11, 249-262.

Rogers, E. M., Dearing, J. W., & Bregman, D. (1993). The anatomy of agenda-setting

research. Journal of Communication, 43, 68-84.

Rosenstone, R. J., & Hansen, J. M. (1993). Mobilization, participation, and democracy in

America. New York, NY: Macmillan.

Rubin, A. (1976). Television in children’s political socialization. Journal of

Broadcasting, 20, 51-59.

Sax, L. J., Astin, A., Korn, W., & Mahoney, K. (1997). The American freshman national

norms for fall 1997. Los Angeles, CA: Higher Education Research Institute.

Schudson, M. (1998). The good citizens: a history of American civic life. Cambridge,

MA: Harvard University Press.

Sears, D. O. (1975). Political socialization. In F. I. Greenstein & N. W. Polsby (Eds.),

Handbook of Political Science, 2, Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley Publishing.

Sears, D. O. (1986). College sophomores in the laboratory: Influences of a narrow data

base on psychology’s view of human nature. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 51, 515-530.

Sears, D. O., & Valentino, N. A. (1997). Politics matters: political events as catalysts for

preadult socialization. American Political Science Review, 91, 45-62.

Page 189: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

176

Sherr, S., & Staples, M. (2004). News for a new generation report 1: Content analysis,

interviews, and focus groups. The Center for Information & Research on Civic

Learning & Engagement.

Sigel, R. S. (1965). Assumptions about the learning of political values, Annals of the

American Academy of Social and Political Science, 361, 165, 1-9.

Smith, K. (1987). Newspaper coverage and public concern about community issues,

Journalism Monographs, 101.

Smith, T. W., & Kim, S. (2006). National pride in cross-national and temporal

perspective. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 18, 127-136.

Soroka, S. N, (2001) Media, public opinion, and foreign policy, paper presented to the

American Political Science Association, San Francisco.

Steele, J. E. (1995). Experts and the operational bias of television news: the case of the

Persian Gulf war, Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 72(4), 799-812.

Sullivan, J. L., & Riedel, E. (2001). Efficacy: Political. In N. J. Smelser & P. B. Baltes

(Eds.), International encyclopedia of social & behavioral sciences (pp. 4353-

4356). New York, NY: Elsevier.

Takeshita, T. (1993) ‘Agenda-setting effects of the press in a Japanese local election.’

Studies of Broadcasting, 29, 193-216.

Teixeira, R. A. (1987). Why Americans don’t vote. Westport, CT: Greenwood.

Teixeira, R. A. (1992). The disappearing American voter. Washington, DC: the

Brookings Institution.

Page 190: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

177

Thompson, D. (1970). The democratic citizen. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University

Press.

Tsfati, Y., & Peri, Y. (2006). Mainstream media skepticism and exposure to sectorial and

extranational news media: the case of Israel. Mass Communication & Society,

9(2), 165-187.

U.S. Census Bureau. (2002). Voting and registration in the election of November 2000.

Retrieved May 14, 2006, from http://www.census.gov/prod/2002pubs/p20-542.pdf

Valentino, N. A., Beckmann, M. N., & Buhr, T. A. (2001). A spiral of cynicism for

some: the contingent effects of campaign news frames on participation and

confidence in government. Political Communication, 18, 347-367.

Verba, S., & Nie, N. H. (1972). Participation in America: political democracy and social

equality. New York, NY: Harper & Row.

Verba, S., Schlozman, K. L., & Brady, H. E. (1995). Voice and equality: civic

voluntarism in American Politics. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Wattenberg, M. P. (1998). The decline of American political parties, 1952-1996.

Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Wattenberg, M. P. (2002). Where have all the voters gone? Cambridge, MA: Harvard

University Press.

Weaver, D. H (1996). What voters learn from media, Annals of the American Academy of

Political and Social Science, 546, 34-37.

Page 191: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

178

Weaver, D. H., Graber, D. A., McCombs, M. E., & Eyal, C. H. (1981). Media agenda-

setting in a presidential election: Issues, images and interest. New York, NY:

Praeger.

Why Young Voters Care Again: And why their vote matters. (2008, February 11). Time

Magazine, cover page.

Wimmer, R. D., & Dominick, J. R. (1994). Mass media research and introduction. 4th ed.

Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.

Winter, J. P., & Eyal, C. H. (1981). Agenda-setting for the civil rights issue. Public

Opinion Quarterly, 45, 376-383.

Wolfinger, R. E., & Rosenstone, S. J. (1980). Who votes? New Haven, CT: Yale

University Press.

Yates, M., & Youniss, J. (1998). Community service and political identity development

in adolescence. The Journal of Social Issues 54(3), 495-512.

Zaller, J. R. (1992). The nature and origins of mass opinion. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge

University Press.

Zukin, C. (1997). Generation X and the news. Washington, DC: Radio and Television

News Directors Foundation.

Zukin, C., Keeter, S., Andolia, M., Jenkins, K., & Delli Carpini, M. X. (2006). A new

engagement? Political participation, civic life, and the changing American

citizen. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Page 192: Copyright by Soo-Hye Han 2008

179

Vita

Soo-Hye Han was born in Gifu, Japan as the daughter of Jong-Hak Han and

Young-Ja Lee. After completing her work at Aichi Korean High School in Nagoya,

Japan, she attended and earned degrees from Kyoto University of Foreign Studies (B.A.)

and the University of Pittsburgh (M.P.I.A.). In 2001 she entered the Graduate School of

the University of Texas at Austin. During this time, she taught and assisted various

courses in the Department of Communication Studies and the Department of Asian

Studies and worked as a research assistant at the Annette Strauss Institute for Civic

Participation.

Permanent address: 2021 Kitagomizuka, Kusu-cho, Yokkaichi-shi, Mie 510-0103 Japan

This dissertation was typed by the author.