comm 239

Upload: ratulone

Post on 03-Apr-2018

216 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    1/44

    Questionnaire Design for Surveys and Laboratory Experiments:

    Social And Cognitive Perspectives

    Communication 239, Political Science 324R, Psychology 224

    Jon A. Krosnick

    Departments of Communication, Political Science, and Psychology

    Stanford University

    434 McClatchy Hall, 725-3031, [email protected]

    Spring, 2005

    Building 50, Room 52H

    Tuesdays, 3:15 6:05 pm

    _______________________________________________________________________

    This course will provide an introduction to general theories of the cognitive aspects of

    question-answering in surveys. The focus will be on the cognitive processes in whichrespondents engage when answering questions and on the social interactions among researchers,

    interviewers, and respondents. The course will provide an overview of issues that one should

    consider when designing a survey questionnaire or when interpreting the results of a survey.

    A great deal of research has been conducted during the last 50 years exploring the effects

    of different measurement strategies on the findings of survey research, and this work reveals agreat deal about the cognitive processes in which survey respondents engage during interviews.We will review the major findings of this work and discuss their implications for understanding

    cognitive aspects of question-answering. The primary goal of the discussions will be to identify

    issues to be considered when writing survey questions to measure specified constructs. Takentogether, this literature makes many useful recommendations regarding how to avoid sources of

    bias in measurement and how to maximize accuracy.

    Most of our discussions will focus on the measurement of subjective psychologicalphenomena, such as attitudes, beliefs, assessments of importance, and assessments of

    satisfaction. There will be some secondary discussion of the measurement of objective

    phenomena, primarily past behavior.

    Course requirements are: (1) reading all required reading assignments prior to class

    meetings when they will be discussed, (2) participating energetically and creatively in classdiscussion, and (3) a final sit-down exam asking you to critique a questionnaire that I will give

    you.

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    2/44

    Topics to be Covered

    1. General Introduction

    2. Open versus Closed Questions

    3. Closed Questions: Ratings versus Rankings

    4. Closed Questions: Number of Scale Points/Magnitude Scaling

    5. Closed Questions: Verbal vs. Numeric Scale Point Labels

    6. Closed Questions: Response Order Effects

    7. Attitudes and Non-attitudes

    8. Acquiescence and Other Response Biases

    9. Question Wording/Question Balance

    10. Question Order

    11. Attitude Recall Questions

    12. Asking Why?

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    3/44

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    4/44

    Warwick, D. P., & Lininger, C. A. (1975). The sample survey: Theory and practice.

    (Chapter 6, "Questionnaire design."). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    1. Introduction to Issues of Question Design

    Andrews, F. M. (1984). Construct validity and error components of survey measures: A

    structural modeling approach. Public Opinion Quarterly, 48, 409-442.

    *Hippler, H., & Schwarz, N. (1987). Response effects in surveys. In H. Hippler, N.

    Schwarz, & S. Sudman (Eds.), Social information processing and survey

    methodology. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Kalton, G., & Schuman, H. (1982). The effect of the question on survey responses: A

    review. The Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, 145, 42-73.

    *Krosnick, J. A. (1991). Response strategies for coping with the cognitive demands ofattitude measures in surveys. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 5, 213-236.

    Schuman, H. (1982). Artifacts are in the mind of the beholder. The American

    Sociologist, 17, 21-28.

    Schuman, H. (1986). Ordinary questions, survey questions, and policy questions. Public

    Opinion Quarterly, 50, 432-442.

    Schuman, H., & Kalton, G. (1985). Survey methods. In G. Lindzey and E. Aronson(Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (vol. 1). New York: Random House.

    Schuman, H., & Presser, S. (1977). Question wording as an independent variable insurvey analysis. Sociological Methods and Research, 6, 27-46.

    *Tourangeau, R. (1987). Attitude measurement: A cognitive perspective. In H. Hippler,N. Schwarz, & S. Sudman (Eds.), Social information processing and survey

    methodology. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    2. Open vs. Closed Questions

    Belson, W. A., & Duncan, J. A. (1962). A comparison of the checklist and the openresponse questioning systems. Applied Statistics, 11, 120-132.

    *Converse, J. M. (1984). Strong arguments and weak evidence: The open/closedquestioning controversy of the 1940s. Public Opinion Quarterly, 48, 267-282.

    Ehrlich, H. J., & Rinehart, J. W. (1965). A brief report on the methodology of stereotype

    research. Social Forces, 43, 564-575.

    2

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    5/44

    Elig, T. W., & Frieze, I. H. (1979). Measuring causal attributions for success and

    failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 621-634.

    Geer, J. G. (1988). What do open-ended questions measure? Public Opinion Quarterly,

    52, 365-371.

    Hurd, A. W. (1932). Comparisons of short answer and multiple choice tests covering

    identical subject content. Journal of Educational Psychology, 26, 28-30.

    Jenkins, J. G. (1935). Psychology in Business and Industry. New York: Wiley, p. 348-

    351.

    Lazarsfeld, P. E. (1944). The controversy over detailed interviews - an offer for

    negotiation. Public Opinion Quarterly, 8, 38-60.

    Lindzey, G. E., & Guest, L. (1951). To repeat - check lists can be dangerous. PublicOpinion Quarterly, 15, 355-358.

    Link, H. C. (1943). An experiment in depth interviewing. Public Opinion Quarterly, 7,

    267-279.

    Magill, W. H. (1934). The influence of the form of the item on the validity of

    achievement tests. Journal of Educational Psychology, 25, 21-28.

    Malmud, R. S. (1925). The controlled vs. the free completion. American Journal ofPsychology, 36, 401-411.

    Mason, R., Boersma, L., & Faulkenberry, G. D. (1988). The use of open and closedquestions to identify holders of crystallized attitudes: The case of adoption of

    erosion-control practices among farmers. Rural Sociology, 53, 96-109.

    Metzner, H., & Mann, F. (1952). A limited comparison of two methods of data

    collection: The fixed alternative questionnaire and the open-ended interview.

    American Sociological Review, 17, 486-491.

    Remmers, H. H., Marschat, L. E., Brown, A., & Chapman, I. (1923). An experimental

    study of the relative difficulty of true-false, multiple-choice, and incomplete-

    sentence types of examination questions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 14,367-372.

    Roslow, S., & Blankenship, A. B. (1939). Phrasing the question in consumer research:Experimental studies on the form of question. Journal of Applied Psychology, 23,

    612-622.

    3

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    6/44

    Roslow, S., Wulfeck, W. H., & Corby, P. G. (1940). Consumer and opinion research:

    Experimental studies on the form of the question. Journal of Applied Psychology,

    24, 334-346.

    Rugg, D., & Cantril, H. (1944). The wording of questions. In H. Cantril (Ed.), Gauging

    public opinion. Princeton: Princeton University Press, p. 37-38, 49.

    Schuman, H., Ludwig, J., & Krosnick, J.A. (1986). The perceived threat of nuclear war,

    salience, and open questions. Public Opinion Quarterly, 50, 519-536.

    *Schuman, H., & Presser, S. (1981). Questions and answers in attitude surveys. New

    York: Academic Press. Chapter 3, "Open versus closed questions."

    Schuman, H., & Scott, J. (1987). Problems in the use of survey questions to measure

    public opinion. Science, 236, 957-959.

    Skott, H. E. (1943). Attitude research in the Department of Agriculture. Public OpinionQuarterly, 7, 280-292.

    3. Closed Questions: Ratings vs. Rankings

    *Alwin, D. F., & Krosnick, J. A. (1985). The measurement of values in surveys: A

    comparison of ratings and rankings. Public Opinion Quarterly, 49, 535-552.

    Elig, T. W., & Frieze, I. H. (1979). Measuring causal attributions for success andfailure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 621-634.

    Feather, N. T. (1973). The measurement of values: Effects of different assessmentprocedures. Australian Journal of Psychology, 25, 221-231.

    Guilford, J. P. (1954). Psychometric methods. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    *Herzog, A. R., & Bachman, J. G. (1981). Effects of questionnaire length on response

    quality. Public Opinion Quarterly, 45, 549-559.

    *Krosnick, J. A., & Alwin, D. F. (1987). A test of the form-resistant correlation

    hypothesis: Ratings, rankings, and the measurement of values. Public Opinion

    Quarterly, 52, 526-538.

    McIntyre, S. H., & Ryans, A. B. (1977). Time and accuracy measures for alternative

    multidimensional scaling data collection methods: Some additional results.Journal of Marketing Research, 14, 607-610.

    Moore, M. (1975). Rating versus ranking in the Rokeach value survey: An Israeli

    comparison. European Journal of Social Psychology, 5, 405-408.

    4

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    7/44

    Munson, J. M., & McIntyre, S. H. (1979). Developing practical procedures for the

    measurement of personal values in cross-cultural marketing. Journal ofMarketing Research, 16, 48-52.

    Rankin, W. L., & Grube, J. W. (1980). A comparison of ranking and rating proceduresfor value system measurement. European Journal of Social Psychology, 10, 233-

    246.

    Reynolds, T. J., & Jolly, J. P. (1980). Measuring personal values: An evaluation of

    alternative methods. Journal of Marketing Research, 17, 531-536.

    Schulz, U., & May, T. (1989). The recording of social orientations with ranking and paircomparison procedures. European Journal of Social Psychology, 19, 41-59.

    4. Closed Questions: Number of Scale Points

    Andrews, F. M. (1984). Construct validity and error components of survey measures: Astructural modeling approach. Public Opinion Quarterly, 48, 409-442.

    Bass, B. M., Cascio, W. F., & O'Connor, E. J. (1974). Magnitude estimations ofexpressions of frequency and amount. Journal of Applied Psychology, 59, 313-

    320.

    Bendig, A. W. (1953). The reliability of self-ratings as a function of amount of verbalanchoring and of the number of categories on the scale. Journal of Applied

    Psychology, 37, 38-41.

    Bendig, A. W. (1954). Transmitted information and the length of rating scales. Journal

    of Experimental Psychology, 47, 303-308.

    Bendig, A. W., & Hughes, J. B. (1953). Effect of amount of verbal anchoring and

    number of rating-scale categories upon transmitted information. Journal of

    Experimental Psychology, 46, 87-90.

    Birkett, N. J. (1986). Selecting the number of response categories for a Likert-type

    scale. Proceedings of the American Statistical Association 1987 Annual

    Meetings, Section on Survey Research Methods.

    Bishop, G. F. (1987). Experiments with the middle response alternative in survey

    questions. Public Opinion Quarterly, 51, 220-232.

    Champney, H., & Marshall, H. (1939). Optimal refinement of the rating scale. Journal

    of Applied Psychology, 23, 323-331.

    5

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    8/44

    Cox, E. P. (1980). The optimal number of response alternatives for a scale: A review.

    Journal of Marketing Research, 17, 407-422.

    Eagly, A. H., & Steffen, V. J. (1988). A note on assessing stereotypes. Personality and

    Social Psychology Bulletin, 14, 676-680.

    Finn, R. H. (1972). Effects of some variations in rating scale characteristics on the

    means and reliabilities of ratings. Educational and Psychological Measurement,

    32, 255-265.

    Garner, W. R. (1960). Rating scales, discriminability, and information transmission.

    Psychological Review, 67, 343-352.

    Ghiselli, E. E. (1939). All or none versus graded response questionnaires. Journal of

    Applied Psychology, 23, 405-413.

    Green, P. E., & Rao, V. R. (1970). Rating scales and information recovery - how manyscales and response categories to use? Journal of Marketing, 34, 33-39.

    Kalton, G., Roberts, J., & Holt, D. (1980). The effects of offering a middle response

    option with opinion questions. Statistician, 29, 65-78.

    Komorita, S. S., & Graham, W. K. (1965). Number of scale points and the reliability of

    scales. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 25, 897-995.

    Lehmann, D. R., & Hulbert, J. (1972). Are three-point scales always good enough?Journal of Marketing Research, 9, 444-446.

    Lipset, S. M., & Schneider, W. (1983). The confidence gap. New York: The Free Press,p. 89-96.

    Lissitz, R. W., & Green, S. B. (1975). Effect of the number of scale points on reliability:A monte carlo approach. Journal of Applied Psychology, 60, 10-13

    +Lodge, M. (1981). Magnitude scaling: Quantitative measurement of opinions. Beverly

    Hills: Sage.

    Lodge, M., & Tursky, B. (1979). Comparisons between category and magnitude scaling

    of political opinion employing SRC/CPS items. American Political ScienceReview, 73, 50-66.

    Lodge, M., & Tursky, B. (1981) The social-psychophysical scaling of political opinion.In B. Wegener (Ed.), Social attitudes and psychophysical measurement.

    Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.

    6

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    9/44

    Lodge, M., Cross, D., Tursky, B., & Tannenhaus, J. (1975). The psychophysical scaling

    and validation of a political support scale. American Journal of Political Science,

    19, 611-649.

    Lodge, M., Cross, D., Tursky, B., Foley, M. A., & Foley, H. (1976). The calibration and

    cross-model validation of ratio scales of political opinion in survey research.Social Science Research, 5, 325-347.

    Lodge, M., Cross, D., Tursky, B., Tannenhaus, J., & Reeder, R. (1976). Thepsychophysical scaling of political support in the 'real world'. Political

    Methodology, 2, 159-182.

    Matell, M. S., & Jacoby, J. (1971). Is there an optimal number of alternatives for Likertscale items? Study I: Reliability and validity. Educational and Psychological

    Measurement, 31, 657-674.

    Matell, M. S., & Jacoby, J. (1972). Is there an optimal number of alternatives for Likert-scale items? Effects of testing time and scale properties. Journal of Applied

    Psychology, 56, 506-509.

    McGuire, W. J. (1981). The probabilogical model of cognitive structure and attitude

    change. In R. E. Petty, T. M. Ostrom, and T. C. Brock (Eds.), Cognitiveresponses in persuasion. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Peterson, B. L. (1985). Confidence: Categories and confusion. GSS Technical Report

    No. 50, National Opinion Research Center, University of Chicago.

    Ramsay, J. O. (1973). The effect of number of categories in rating scales on precision of

    estimation of scale values. Psychometrika, 38, 513-532.

    Rosenstone, S. J., Hansen, J. M., & Kinder, D. R. (1986). Measuring change in personal

    economic well-being. Public Opinion Quarterly, 50, 176-192.

    *Schuman, H., & Presser, S. (1981). Questions and answers in attitude surveys. New

    York: Academic Press. Chapters 6, "Measuring a middle position."

    Smith, T. W., & Peterson, B. L. (1985). The impact of number of response categories on

    inter-item associations: Experimental and simulated results. Paper presented at

    the American Sociological Association annual meetings, Washington, D.C.

    Stember, H., & Hyman, H. (1949-1950). How interviewer effects operate through

    question form. International Journal of Opinion and Attitude Research, 3, 493-512.

    Symonds, P. M. (1924). On the loss of reliability in rating due to coarseness of the scale.

    Journal of Experimental Psychology, 456-461.

    7

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    10/44

    Warr, P., Barter, J., & Brownridge, G. (1983). On the independence of positive and

    negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 644-651.

    Watson, D. (1988). The vicissitudes of mood measurement: Effects of varying

    descriptors, time frames, and response formats on measures of positive andnegative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 128-141.

    *Wedell, D. H., & Parducci, A. (1988). The category effect in social judgment:Experimental ratings of happiness. Journal of personality and Social Psychology,

    55, 341-356.

    Welch, R. E. (1972). The use of magnitude estimation in attitude scaling: Constructing ameasure of political dissatisfaction. Social Science Quarterly, 76-87.

    5. Closed Questions: Verbal vs. Numerical Scale Point Labels

    Albaum, G., & Murphy, B. D. (1988). Extreme response on a Likert scale.Psychological Reports, 63, 501-502.

    Andrews, F. M. (1984). Construct validity and error components of survey measures: Astructural modeling approach. Public Opinion Quarterly, 48, 409-442.

    Bass, B. M., Cascio, W. F., & O'Connor, E. J. (1974). Magnitude estimations of

    expressions of frequency and amount. Journal of Applied Psychology, 59, 313-320.

    Bendig, A. W. (1953). The reliability of self-ratings as a function of amount of verbalanchoring and of the number of categories on the scale. Journal of Applied

    Psychology, 37, 38-41.

    Bendig, A. W., & Hughes, J. B. (1953). Effect of amount of verbal anchoring and

    number of rating-scale categories upon transmitted information. Journal of

    Experimental Psychology, 46, 87-90.

    *Bradburn, N. M., & Miles, C. (1979). Vague qualifiers. Public Opinion Quarterly, 43,

    92-101.

    Bryant, G. D., & Norman, G. R. (19??). Expressions of probability: Words and

    numbers. New England Journal of Medicine, 302, 411.

    Budescu, D. V., & Wallsten, T. S. (1985). Consistency in interpretation of probabilistic

    phrases. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Making, 36, 391-405.

    Chase, C. I. (1969). Often is where you find it. American Psychologist, 24, 1043.

    8

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    11/44

    Cliff, N. (1959). Adverbs as multipliers. Psychological Review, 66, 27-44.

    Cohen, J. Dearnley, E. J., & Hansel, C. E. M. (1958). A quantitative study of meaning.

    British Journal of Educational Psychology, 28, 141-148.

    Eagly, A. H., & Steffen, V. J. (1988). A note on assessing stereotypes. Personality and

    Social Psychology Bulletin, 14, 676-680.

    Finn, R. H. (1972). Effects of some variations in rating scale characteristics on the

    means and reliabilities of ratings. Educational and Psychological Measurement,

    32, 255-265.

    Goocher, B. E. (1965). Effects of attitude and experience on the selection of frequency

    adverbs. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 4, 193-195.

    Hakel, M. D. (1968). How often is often? American Psychologist, 23, 533-534.

    Koltko, M. E. (1989). How vagueness can ruin a survey: Comment on Pope,Tabachnick, & Keith-Spiegel. American Psychologist, 44, 845-846.

    Lichtenstein, S., & Newman, J. R. (1967). Empirical scaling of common verbal phrasesassociated with numerical probabilities. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 9,

    563-564.

    Madden, J. M., & Bourdon, R. D. (1964). Effects of variations in scale format onjudgment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 48, 147-151.

    Miller, P. V. (1984). Alternative question forms for attitude scale questions in telephonesurveys. Public Opinion Quarterly, 48, 766-778.

    Moore, P. G. (1977). The manager's struggles with uncertainty. Journal of the RoyalStatistical Association, 140, 129-165.

    Murphy, K. R., & Constans, J. I. (1987). Behavioral anchors as a source of bias in

    ratings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72, 573-577.

    Ostrom, T. M., & Upshaw, H. S. (1968). Psychological perspective and attitude change.

    In A. G. Greenwald, T. C. Brock, and T. M. Ostrom (Eds.), PsychologicalFoundations of Attitudes. New York: Academic Press.

    Parducci, A. (1968). Often is often. American Psychologist, 23, 828.

    Pepper, S., & Prytulak, L. S. (1974). Sometimes frequently means seldom: Context

    effects in the interpretation of quantitative expressions. Journal of Research in

    Personality, 8, 95-101.

    9

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    12/44

    Peters, D. L., & McCormick, E. J. (1966). Comparative reliability of numerically

    anchored versus job-task anchored rating scales. Journal of Applied Psychology,50, 92-96.

    Pope, K. S., Tabachnick, B. G., & Keith-Spiegel, P. (1989). Reply to Koltko. AmericanPsychologist, 44, 846-847.

    Reyna, V. F. (1981). The language of possibility and probability: Effects of negation onmeaning. Memory and Cognition, 9, 642-650..

    Roslow, S., Wulfeck, W. H., & Corby, P. G. (1940). Consumer and opinion research:

    Experimental studies on the form of the question. Journal of Applied Psychology,24, 334-346.

    Schwarz, N., & Scheuring, B. (1988). Judgments of relationship satisfaction: Inter- and

    intraindividual comparisons as a function of questionnaire structure. EuropeanJournal of Social Psychology, 18, 485-496.

    Simpson, R. H. (1944). The specific meanings of certain terms indicating different

    degrees of frequency. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 30, 328-330.

    Stone, D. R., & Johnson, R. T. (1959). A study of words indicating frequency. Journal

    of Educational Psychology, 50, 224-227.

    Teigen, K. H. (1988). When are low-probability events judged to be 'probable?' Effectsof outcome-set characteristics on verbal probability estimates. Acta

    Psychologica, 68, 157-174.

    *Wallsten, T. S., Budescu, D. V., Rapoport, A., Zwick, R., & Forsyth, B. (1986).

    Measuring the vague meanings of probability terms. Journal of Experimental

    Psychology: General, 115, 348-365.

    Warr, P., Barter, J., & Brownridge, G. (1983). On the independence of positive and

    negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 644-651.

    Watson, D. (1988). The vicissitudes of mood measurement: Effects of varying

    descriptors, time frames, and response formats on measures of positive and

    negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 128-141.

    Wedell, D. H., & Parducci, A. (1988). The category effect in social judgment:

    Experimental ratings of happiness. Journal of personality and Social Psychology,55, 341-356.

    Weisberg, H. F., & Miller, A. H. (1980). Evaluation of the feeling thermometers: A

    report to the National Election Study Board based on data from the 1979 pilot

    10

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    13/44

    study. Unpublished manuscript, Center for Political Studies, Institute for Social

    Research, University of Michigan.

    Zaller, J. (1988). Vague questions get vague answers: An experimental attempt to

    reduce response instability. Unpublished manuscript, University of California at

    Los Angeles.

    6. Closed Questions: Acquiescence and Other Response Biases

    Bachman, J. G., & O'Malley, P. M. (1984). Black-white differences in self-esteem: Are

    they affected by response styles? American Journal of Sociology, 90, 624-639.

    Bachman, J. G., & O'Malley, P. M. (1984). Yea-saying, nay-saying, and going to

    extremes: Black-white differences in response styles. Public Opinion Quarterly,

    48, 491-509.

    Bass, B. M. (1955). Authoritarianism or acquiescence? Journal of Abnormal and Social

    Psychology, 51, 616-623.

    Bentler, P. M., Jackson, D. N., & Messick, S. (1971). Identification of content and style:

    A two-dimensional interpretation of acquiescence. Psychological Bulletin, 76,186-204.

    Bentler, P. M., Jackson, D. N., & Messick, S. (1972). A rose by any other name.

    Psychological Bulletin, 77, 109-113.

    Bishop, G. F., Oldendick, R. W., & Tuchfarber, A. J. (1982). Effects of presenting one

    versus two sides of an issue in survey questions. Public Opinion Quarterly, 46,69-85.

    Bishop, G. F., Oldendick, R. W., & Tuchfarber, A. J. (1982). Effects of presenting oneversus two sides of an issue in survey questions. Public Opinion Quarterly, 46,

    69-85.

    Blankenship, A. B. (1940). The influence of the question form upon the response in apublic opinion poll. Psychological Record, 3, 345-422.

    Block, J. (1965). The challenge of response sets. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Block, J. (1971). On further conjectures regarding acquiescence. Psychological

    Bulletin, 76, 205-210.

    Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., & Rogers, W. (1976). The quality of American life:

    Perceptions, evaluations, and satisfaction. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Chapter 4.

    11

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    14/44

    Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., Miller, W. E., & Stokes, D. E. (1960). The American

    Voter. New York: Wiley. Chapter 18.

    Clancy, K. J., & Wachsler, R. A. (1971). Positional effects in shared-cost surveys.

    Public Opinion Quarterly, 35, 258-265.

    Couch, A., & Keniston, K. (1960). Yeasayers and naysayers: Agreeing response set as a

    personality variable. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 60, 151-174.

    Couch, A., & Keniston, K. (1961). Agreeing response set and social desirability.

    Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 62, 175-179.

    Crandall, J. E. (1965). Some relationship among sex, anxiety, and conservatism of

    judgment. Journal of Personality, 33, 99-107.

    Cronbach, L. J. (1941). An experimental comparison of the multiple true-false andmultiple multiple-choice tests. Journal of Educational Psychology, 32, 533-543.

    Cronbach, L. J. (1942). Studies of acquiescence as a factor in the true-false test. Journal

    of Educational Psychology, 33, 401-415.

    Cronbach, L. J. (1946). Response sets and test validity. Educational and Psychological

    Measurement, 6, 475-494.

    Cronbach, L. J. (1950). Further evidence on response sets and test design. Educationaland Psychological Measurement, 10, 3-31.

    Foster, R. J. (1961). Acquiescent response set as a measure of acquiescence. Journal ofAbnormal and Social Psychology, 63, 155-160.

    Gage, N. L., & Chatterjee, B. B. (1960). The psychological meaning of acquiescenceset: Further evidence. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 60, 280-283.

    Goldsmith, R. E., Stith, M. T., & White, J. D. (1987). The Rokeach value survey and

    social desirability. Journal of Social Psychology, 127, 553-554.

    Goldsmith, R. E., White, J. D., & Stith, M. T. (1987). Yeasaying and the Rokeach value

    survey: Interactions with age and race. Journal of Social Behavior andPersonality, 2, 251-258.

    Hamilton, D. L. (1968). Personality attributes associated with extreme response style.Psychological Bulletin, 69, 192-203.

    Hedges, B. M. (1979). Question wording effects: Presenting one or both sides of the

    case. Statistician, 28, 83-99.

    12

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    15/44

    Hitlin, R. (1976). On question wording and stability of response. Social Science

    Research, 5, 39-41.

    Hui, C. H., & Triandis, H. C. (1985). The instability of response sets. Public Opinion

    Quarterly, 49, 253-260.

    Jackman, M. R. (1973). Education and prejudice or education and response-set?

    American Sociological Review, 38, 327-339.

    Jackson, D. N., & Messick, S. J. (1957). A note on ethnocentrism and acquiescence

    response sets. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 54, 132-134.

    Jackson, D. N., & Messick, S. J. (1958). Content and style in personality assessment.

    Psychological Bulletin, 55, 243-252.

    Jackson, J. E. (1979). Bias in closed-ended issue questions. Political Methodology, 6,393-424.

    Lenski, G. E., & Leggett, J. C. (1960). Caste, class, and deference in the research

    interview. American Journal of Sociology, 65, 463-467.

    Lentz, T. F. (1938). Acquiescence as a factor in the measurement of personality.

    Psychological Bulletin, 35, 659.

    Lewis, N. A., & Taylor, J. A. (1955). Anxiety and extreme response preferences.Educational and Psychological Measurement, 15, 111-116.

    Martin, J. (1964). Acquiescence: Measurement and theory. British Journal of Social andClinical Psychology, 3, 216-225.

    McClendon, M. J. (1989). Acquiescence: A test of the cognitive limitations and questionambiguity hypotheses. Paper presented at the 1989 meetings of the American

    Association for Public Opinion Research, St. Petersburg, Florida.

    McClendon, M. J. (1991). Acquiescence and recency response-order effects in interviewsurveys. Sociological Methods and Research.

    Moum, T. (1988). Yea-saying and mood-of-the-day effects in self-reported quality oflife. Social Indicators Research, 20, 117-139.

    O'Grady, K. E. (1988). The Marlowe-Crowne and Edwards social desirability scales: Apsychometric perspective. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 23, 87-101.

    13

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    16/44

    Peabody, D. (1961). Attitude content and agreement set in scales of authoritarianism,

    dogmatism, anti-Semitism, and economic conservatism. Journal of Abnormal and

    Social Psychology, 63, 1-11.

    Peabody, D. (1964). Models for estimating content and set components in attitude and

    personality scales. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 24, 255-269.

    Peabody, D. (1966). Authoritarianism scales and response bias. Psychological Bulletin,

    65, 11-23.

    Phillips, D. L. (1973). Abandoning method. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. (Chapters 2

    and 3).

    Raskin, E., & Cook, S. W. (1938). A further investigation of the measurement of an

    attitude toward fascism. Journal of Social Psychology, 9, 201-206.

    Ray, J. J. (1979). Is the acquiescent response style not so mythical after all? Someresults from a successful balanced F scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 43,

    638-643.

    Ray, J. J. (1983). Reviving the problem of acquiescent response bias. Journal of Social

    Psychology, 121, 81-96.

    Ray, J. J., & Pratt, G. J. (1979). Is the influence of acquiescence on "Catchphrase" type

    attitudes scale items not so mythical after all? Australian Journal of Psychology,

    31, 73-78.

    *Rorer, L. G. (1965). The great response-style myth. Psychological Bulletin, 63, 129-

    156.

    Roslow, S., Wulfeck, W. H., & Corby, P. G. (1940). Consumer and opinion research:

    Experimental studies on the form of the question. Journal of Applied Psychology,24, 334-346.

    Schuman, H., & Duncan, O. D. (1974). Questions about attitude survey questions. In H.

    Costner (Ed.), Sociological Methodology 1973-1974. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Schuman, H., & Presser, S. (1981). Questions and answers in attitude surveys. NewYork: Academic Press. Chapter 7, "Balance and imbalance in questions."

    *Schuman, H., & Presser, S. (1981). Questions and answers in attitude surveys. NewYork: Academic Press. Chapter 8, "The acquiescence quagmire."

    Schuman, H., & Scott, J. (1989). Response effects over time: Two experiments.

    Sociological Methods and Research, 17, 398-408.

    14

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    17/44

    Small, D. O., & Campbell, D. T. (1960). The effect of acquiescence response set upon

    the relationship of the F-scale and conformity. Sociometry, 23, 69-71.

    Stagner, R. (1941). A comparison of the Gallup and Fortune polls regarding American

    intervention policy. Sociometry, 4, 239-258. (p. 247)

    Toner, B. (1987). The impact of agreement bias on the ranking of questionnaire

    response. Journal of Social Psychology, 127, 221-222.

    Wright, J. D. (1975). Does acquiescence bias the 'index of political efficacy?' Public

    Opinion Quarterly, 39, 219-226.

    7. Closed Questions: Response Order Effects

    Becker, S. L. (1954). Why an order effect. Public Opinion Quarterly, 18, 271-278.

    Belson, W. A. (1966). The effects of reversing the presentation order of verbal ratingscales. Journal of Advertising Research, 6, 30-37.

    Bishop, G. F. (1987). Experiments with the middle response alternative in surveyquestions. Public Opinion Quarterly, 51, 220-232.

    Brook, D., & Upton, G. J. G. (1974). Biases in local government elections due to

    position on the ballot paper. Applied Statistics, 23, 414-419.

    Campbell, D. T., & Mohr, P. J. (1950). The effect of ordinal position upon responses to

    items in a check list. Journal of Applied Psychology, 34, 62-67.

    Carp, F. M. (1974). Position effects on interview responses. Journal of Gerontology, 29,

    581-587.

    Cochrane, R., & Rokeach, M. (1970). Rokeach's value survey: A methodological note.

    Journal of Experimental Research in Personality, 4, 159-161.

    Coney, K. A. (1977). Order-bias: The special case of letter preference. Public Opinion

    Quarterly, 41, 385-388.

    Coney, K. A. (1979). Reply. Public Opinion Quarterly, 43, 408-409.

    Greenhalgh, C. (1979). On letter preference order bias. Public Opinion Quarterly, 43,407-408.

    Johnson, J. D. (1981). Effects of the order of presentation of evaluative dimensions for

    bipolar scales in four societies. Journal of Social Psychology, 113, 21-27.

    15

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    18/44

    *Krosnick, J. A., & Alwin, D. F. (1987). An evaluation of a cognitive theory of

    response order effects in survey measurement. Public Opinion Quarterly, 51,201-219.

    Mathews, C. O. (1929). The effect of the order of printed response words on an interestquestionnaire. Journal of Educational Psychology, 20, 128-134.

    McClendon, M. J. (1986). Response-order effects for dichotomous questions. SocialScience Quarterly, 67, 205-211.

    *McClendon, M. J. (1991). Acquiescence and recency response-order effects in

    interview surveys. Sociological Methods and Research, 20, 60-103.

    Mingay, D. J., & Greenwell, M. T. (1989) Memory bias and response-order effects.

    Journal of Official Statistics, 5, 253-263.

    Monsees, M. L., & Massey, J. T. (1979). Adapting procedures for collecting

    demographic data in a personal interview to a television interview. Proceedingsof the American Statistical Association 1979 Annual Meetings.

    Morgan, R. (1948). A note on question wording. Public Opinion Quarterly, 12, 38.

    Mueller, J. E. (1970). Choosing among 133 candidates. Public Opinion Quarterly, 34,

    395-402.

    Payne, J. D. (1971). The effects of reversing the order of verbal rating scales in a postal

    survey. Journal of the Marketing Research Society, 14, 30-44.

    Payne, S. L. (1949). Case study in question complexity. Public Opinion Quarterly, 13,

    653-658.

    Payne, S. L. (1951). The art of asking questions. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Perreault, W. D. (1975). Controlling order-effect bias. Public Opinion Quarterly, 39,

    544-551.

    Powers, E. A., Morrow, P., Goudy, W. J., & Keith, P. M. (1977). Serial order preference

    in survey research. Public Opinion Quarterly, 41, 80-85.

    Quinn, S. B., & Belson, W. A. (1969). The effects of reversing the order of presentation

    of verbal rating scales in survey interviews. London: Survey Research Centre.

    Rugg, D., & Cantril, H. (1944). The wording of questions. In H. Cantril (Ed.), Gauging

    public opinion. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    16

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    19/44

    *Schuman, H., & Presser, S. (1981). Questions and answers in attitude surveys. New

    York: Academic Press. Chapters 2, "Question order and response order," p. 56-74

    only.

    Upton, G. J. G., & Brook, D. (1974). The importance of positional voting bias in British

    elections. Political Studies, 22, 178-190.

    Upton, G. J. G., & Brook, D. (1975). The determination of the optimum position on a

    ballot paper. Applied Statistics, 24, 279-287.

    8. Attitudes and Non-Attitudes

    Andrews, F. M. (1984). Construct validity and error components of survey measures: A

    structural modeling approach. Public Opinion Quarterly, 48, 409-442.

    Bishop, G. F., Oldendick, R. W., & Tuchfarber, A. J. (1980). Experiments in filteringpolitical opinions. Political Behavior, 2, 339-369.

    Bishop, G. F., Oldendick, R. W., & Tuchfarber, A. J. (1982). Effects of presenting one

    versus two sides of an issue in survey questions. Public Opinion Quarterly, 46,

    69-85.

    Bishop, G. F., Oldendick, R. W., & Tuchfarber, A. J. (1983). Effects of filter questions

    in public opinion surveys. Public Opinion Quarterly, 47, 528-546.

    Bishop, G. F., Oldendick, R. W., Tuchfarber, A. J., & Bennett, S. E. (1980). Pseudo-

    Opinions on public affairs. Public Opinion Quarterly, 44, 198-209.

    Bishop, G. F., Tuchfarber, A. J., & Oldendick, R. W. (1986). Opinions on fictitious

    issues: The pressure to answer survey questions. Public Opinion Quarterly, 50,

    240-250.

    Bishop, G. F., Tuchfarber, A. J., & Oldendick, R. W., & Bennett, S. E. (1979). Effects

    of opinion filtering and opinion floating: Evidence from a secondary analysis.

    Political Methodology, 6, 293-309.

    Blankenship, A. B. (1940). The influence of the question form upon the response in a

    public opinion poll. Psychological Record, 3, 345-422.

    Budd, R. J. (1987). Response bias and the theory of reasoned action. Social Cognition,

    5, 95-107.

    *Converse, P. E. (1964). The nature of belief systems in mass publics. In D. E. Apter

    (Ed.), Ideology and discontent. New York: Free Press.

    17

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    20/44

    Converse, P. E. (1970). Attitudes and non-attitudes: Continuation of a dialogue. In E.

    R. Tufte (Ed.), The quantitative analysis of social problems. Reading, MA:

    Addison-Wesley.

    Edwards, J. D., & Ostrom, T. M. (1971). Cognitive structure of neutral attitudes.

    Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 7, 36-47.

    Ehrlich, H. J. (1964). Instrument error and the study of prejudice. Social Forces, 43,

    197-206.

    Eysenck, H. J., & Crown, S. (1948). National stereotypes: An experimental and

    methodological study. International Journal of Opinion and Attitude Research, 2,

    26-39.

    Gill, S. (1947). How do you stand on sin? Tide, 21, 72.

    Kolson, K. L., & Green, J. J. (1970). Response set bias and political socializationresearch. Social Science Quarterly, 51, 527-538.

    Mason, R., Boersma, L., & Faulkenberry, G. D. (1988). The use of open and closed

    questions to identify holders of crystallized attitudes: The case of adoption of

    erosion-control practices among farmers. Rural Sociology, 53, 96-109.

    *Schuman, H., & Presser, S. (1981). Questions and answers in attitude surveys. New

    York: Academic Press. Chapters 4 and 5, "The assessment of no opinion," and

    "The fine line between attitudes and non-attitudes."

    Schuman, H., & Scott, J. (1989). Response effects over time: Two experiments.

    Sociological Methods and Research, 17, 398-408.

    Smith, T. W. (1984). Nonattitudes: A review and evaluation. In C. F. Turner & E.

    Martin (Eds.), Surveying subjective phenomena. New York: Russell SageFoundation.

    9. Question Wording

    Aldrich, J. H., Niemi, R. G., Rabinowitz, G., & Rohde, D. W. (1982). The measurement

    of public opinion about public policy: A report on some new issue questionformats. American Journal of Political Science, 26, 391-414.

    Blankenship, A. B. (1940). The influence of the question form upon the response in apublic opinion poll. Psychological Record, 3, 345-422.

    Blankenship, A. B. (1940). Does the question form influence public opinion poll

    results? Journal of Applied Psychology, 24, 27-30.

    18

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    21/44

    Chaplin, W. F., & Buckner, K. E. (1988). Self-ratings of personality: A naturalistic

    comparison of normative, ipsative, and idiothetic standards. Journal ofPersonality, 56, 509-530.

    Converse, P. E., & Pierce, R. (1985). Measuring partisanship. Political Methodology,11, 143-166.

    Fee, J. F. (1981). Symbols in survey questions: Solving the problems of multiple wordmeanings. Political Methodology, 7, 71-95.

    *Hippler, H. J., & Schwarz, N. (1986). Not forbidding isn't allowing: The cognitive

    basis of the forbid-allow asymmetry. Public Opinion Quarterly, 50, 87-96.

    Hitlin, R. (1976). On question wording and stability of response. Social Science

    Research, 5, 39-41.

    Houser, J. D. (1938). Measurement of the vital products of business. Journal of

    Marketing, 2, 181-189.

    Kenney, P. J., & Rice, T. W. (1988). The evaporating independents: Removing the

    'independent' option from the NES party identification question. Public OpinionQuarterly, 52, 231-239.

    Kolson, K. L., & Green, J. J. (1970). Response set bias and political socialization

    research. Social Science Quarterly, 51, 527-538.

    +Krosnick, J. A. (1989). Question wording and reports of survey results: The case of

    Louis Harris and Aetna Life and Casualty. Public Opinion Quarterly, 53, 107-113.

    Langer, G. (1989). Polling on prejudice: Questionable questions. Public Opinion,May/June, 18-19, 57.

    Link, H. C., & Freiberg, A. D. (1942). The problem of validity vs. reliability in public

    opinion polls. Public Opinion Quarterly, 6, 87-98.

    Lurie, W. A. (1938). The measurement of prestige and prestige-suggestibility. Journal

    of Social Psychology, 9, 219-225.

    Menefee, S. C. (1936). The effect of stereotyped words on political judgments.

    American Sociological Review, 1, 614-621.

    Morgan, R. (1948). A note on question wording. Public Opinion Quarterly, 12, 38.

    19

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    22/44

    Mullen, B., Driskell, J. E., & Smith, C. (1989). Availability and social projection: The

    effects of sequence of measurement and wording of question on estimates of

    consensus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 15, 84-90.

    Payne, S. L. (1951). The art of asking questions. Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University

    Press.

    *Petty, R. E., Rennier, G. A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1987). Assertion versus interrogation

    format in opinion surveys: Questions enhance thoughtful responding. PublicOpinion Quarterly, 51, 481-494.

    Quattrone, G. A., & Tversky, A. (1988). Contrasting rational and psychological analyses

    of political choice. American Political Science Review, 82, 719-736.

    Roslow, S., Wulfeck, W. H., & Corby, P. G. (1940). Consumer and opinion research:

    Experimental studies on the form of the question. Journal of Applied Psychology,

    24, 334-346.

    Rossi, P. H., & Wright, J. D. (19??). Social science research and the politics of guncontrol. In R. L. Shotland and M. M. Mark (Eds.), Social science and social

    policy. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

    Rugg, D. (1941). Experiments in wording questions: II. Public Opinion Quarterly, 5, 91-

    92.

    Schaffer, N. C. (1982). A General Social Survey experiment in generic words. PublicOpinion Quarterly, 46, 572-581.

    Schuman, H. (1988). Rejoinder. Public Opinion Quarterly, 52, 579-581.

    Schuman, H., & Bobo, L. (1988). Survey-based experiments on white racial attitudes

    toward residential integration. American Journal of Sociology, 94, 273-299.

    Schuman, H., & Duncan, O. D. (1974). Questions about attitude survey questions. In H.

    Costner (Ed.), Sociological Methodology 1973-1974. San Francisco: Jossey-

    Bass.

    *Schuman, H., & Presser, S. (1981). Questions and answers in attitude surveys. New

    York: Academic Press. Chapters 11, "Tone of wording."

    Sigelman, L., & Presser, S. (1988). Measuring public support for the new Christian

    right: The perils of point estimation. Public Opinion Quarterly, 52, 325-337.

    Simpson, J. H. (1988). A reply to 'Measuring public support for the new Christian right:

    The perils of point estimation." Public Opinion Quarterly, 52, 338-342.

    20

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    23/44

    Singer, E. (1988). To the editor of POQ. Public Opinion Quarterly, 52, 576-579.

    Smith, T. W. (1987). That which we call welfare by any other name would smellsweeter: An analysis of the impact of question wording on response patterns.

    Public Opinion Quarterly, 51, 75-83.

    Sniderman, P. M., Piazza, T., & Kendrick, A. (1987). The dynamics of racial policy

    attitudes. Paper presented at the annual meetings of the Midwest Political Science

    Association, Chicago, Illinois.

    Stagner, R. (1941). A comparison of the Gallup and Fortune polls regarding American

    intervention policy. Sociometry, 4, 239-258. (p. 247)

    Stagner, R. (1941). The prestige value of different types of leadership. Sociology and

    Social Research, 404-413.

    Studenski, P. (1939). How polls can mislead. Harpers, 180, 80-83.

    Uhrbrock, R. S. (1950). Standardization of 724 rating scale statements. PersonnelPsychology, 2, 285-316.

    Warr, P., Barter, J., & Brombridge, G. (1983). On the independence of positive andnegative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 644-651.

    Wembridge, E. R., & Means, E. R. (1918). Obscurities in voting upon measures due to

    double-negative. Journal of Applied Psychology, 2, 156-163.

    Winer, G. A., Hemphill, J., & Craig, R. K. (1988). The effect of misleading questions in

    promoting nonconservation responses in children and adults. DevelopmentalPsychology, 24, 197-202.

    10. Question Order

    Abramson, P. R., Silver, B. D., & Anderson, B. A. (1987). The effects of question order

    in attitude surveys: The case of the SRC/CPS citizen duty questions. AmericanJournal of Political Science, 31, 900-908.

    Baron, R. S., & Roper, G. (1976). Reaffirmation of social comparison views of choiceshifts: Averaging and extremity effects in an autokenetic situation. Journal of

    Personality and Social Psychology, 33, 521-530.

    Bishop, G. F. (1987). Context effects on self-perceptions of interest in government and

    public affairs. In H. Hippler, N. Schwarz, & S. Sudman (Eds.), Social

    information processing and survey methodology. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    21

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    24/44

    Bishop, G. F., Oldendick, R. W., & Tuchfarber, A. J. (1980). Experiments in filtering

    political opinions. Political Behavior, 2, 339-369.

    *Bishop, G. F., Oldendick, R. W., & Tuchfarber, A. J. (1982). Political information

    processing: Question order and context effects. Political Behavior, 4, 177-200.

    Bishop, G. F., Oldendick, R. W., & Tuchfarber, A. J. (1984). What must my interest in

    politics be if I just told you "I don't know"? Public Opinion Quarterly, 48, 510-

    519.

    Bishop, G. F., Oldendick, R. W., & Tuchfarber, A. J. (1985). The importance of

    replicating a failure to replicate: Order effects on abortion items. Public Opinion

    Quarterly, 49, 105-114.

    Blankenship, A. B. (1940). The influence of the question form upon the response in a

    public opinion poll. Psychological Record, 3, 345-422.

    Bradburn, N. M., & Mason, W. M. (1964). The effect of question order on responses.

    Journal of Marketing Research, 1, 57-61.

    *Budd, R. J. (1987). Response bias and the theory of reasoned action. Social Cognition,

    5, 95-107.

    Carp, F. M. (1974). Position effects on interview responses. Journal of Gerontology, 29,

    581-587.

    Carpenter, E. H., & Blackwood, L. G. (1979). The effect of question position on

    responses to attitudinal questions. Rural Sociology, 44, 56-72.

    Carper, J., & Doob, L. W. (1953-1954). Intervening responses between questions and

    answers in attitude surveys. Public Opinion Quarterly, 17, 511-519.

    Crespi, I., & Morris, D. (1984). Question order effect and the measurement of candidate

    preference in the 1982 Connecticut elections. Public Opinion Quarterly, 48, 578-

    591.

    Feldman, J. M., & Lynch, J. G. (1988). Self-generated validity and other effects of

    measurement on belief, attitude, intention, and behavior. Journal of Applied

    Psychology, 73, 421-435.

    Ferber, R. (1952). Order bias in a mail survey. Journal of Marketing, 17, 171-178.

    Granberg, D. (1985). An anomaly in political perception. Public Opinion Quarterly, 49,

    504-516.

    22

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    25/44

    Green, D. P., & Waxman, L. M. (1987). Direct threat and political tolerance: An

    experimental analysis of the tolerance of blacks toward racists. Public Opinion

    Quarterly, 51, 149-165.

    Gross, E. J. (1964). The effect of question sequence on measures of buying interest.

    Journal of Advertising Research, 4, 40-41.

    Hayes, D. P. (1964). Item order and Guttman scales. American Journal of Sociology,

    70, 51-58.

    Higgins, E. T., & Lurie, L. (1983). Context, categorization, and memory: The "change-

    of-standard" effect. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 525-547.

    Higgins, E. T., & Stangor, C. (in press). A "change-of-standard" perspective on the

    relations among context, judgment, and recall. Journal of Personality and Social

    Psychology.

    Hitlin, R. (1976). On question wording and stability of response. Social Science

    Research, 5, 39-41.

    Hyman, H. H., & Sheatsley, P. B. (1950). The current status of American public

    opinion. In J. C. Payne (Ed.), The teaching of contemporary affairs: Twenty-firstyearbook of the national council for the social studies. Washington, D. C.:

    National Council for the Social Studies.

    Johnson, W. R., Sieveking, N. A., & Clanton, E. S. (1974). Effects of alternativepositioning of open-ended questions in multiple-choice questionnaires. Journal of

    Applied Psychology, 59, 776-778.

    Kalton, G., Collins, M., & Brook, L. (1978). Experiments in wording opinion questions.

    Applied Statistics, 27, 149-161.

    Kirchner, W. K., & Uphoff, W. H. (1955). The effect of grouping scale items in union-

    attitude measurement. Journal of Applied Psychology, 39, 182-183.

    Knowles, E. S. (1988). Item context effects on personality scales: Measuring changesthe measure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 312-320.

    Knowles, E. S., Coker, M. C., Cook, D. A., Diercks, S. R., Irwin, M. E., Lundeen, E. J.,Neville, J. W., & Sibicky, M. E. (1991). Order effects within personality

    measures. In N. Schwarz & S. Sudman (Eds.), Order effects in social and

    psychological research. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Kraut, A. I., Wolfson, A. D., & Rothenberg, A. (1975). Some effects of position on

    opinion survey items. Journal of Applied Psychology, 60, 774-776.

    23

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    26/44

    Mansbridge, J. J. (1986). Why we lost the ERA. Chicago: University of Chicago Press,

    pp. 14-19.

    McCauley, C., Kogan, N., & Teger, A. I. (1971). Order effects in answering risk

    dilemmas for self and others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 20,

    423-424.

    McClendon, M. J., & O'Brien, D. J. (1988). Question-order effects on the determinants

    of subjective well-being. Public Opinion Quarterly, 52, 351-364.

    McFarland, S. G. (1981). Effects of question order on survey responses. Public Opinion

    Quarterly, 45, 208-215.

    Metzner, H., & Mann, F. (1953). Effects of grouping related questions in questionnaires.

    Public Opinion Quarterly, 17, 136-141.

    Moore, D. (1987). Question order effects and form resistant correlations: Somediscouraging findings. Paper presented at the annual meetings of the American

    Association for Public Opinion Research, Hershey, PA.

    Mullen, B., Driskell, J. E., & Smith, C. (1989). Availability and social projection: The

    effects of sequence of measurement and wording of question on estimates ofconsensus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 15, 84-90.

    Nakamura, C. Y. (1959). Salience of norms and order of questionnaire items: Their

    effect on responses to the items. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 59,139-142.

    Newman, S. E., & Benassi, V. A. (1989). Putting judgments of control into context:Contrast effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 876-889.

    Pligt, J. V. D., Eiser, J. R., & Spears, R. (1987). Comparative judgments andpreferences: The influence of the number of response alternatives. British Journal

    of Social Psychology, 26, 269-280.

    Sayre, J. (1939). A comparison of three indices of attitude toward radio advertising.Journal of Applied Psychology, 23, 23-33.

    Schuman, H., Kalton, G., & Ludwig, J. (1981). Context and contiguity in surveyquestionnaires. Public Opinion Quarterly, 47, 112-115.

    *Schuman, H., & Ludwig, J. (1983). The norm of even-handedness in surveys as in life.American Sociological Review, 48, 112-120.

    24

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    27/44

    Schuman, H., & Presser, S. (1981). Questions and answers in attitude surveys. New

    York: Academic Press. Chapter 2, "Question order and response order," p. 23-56

    only.

    Schuman, H., Presser, S., & Ludwig, J. (1981). Context effects on survey responses to

    questions about abortion. Public Opinion Quarterly, 45, 216-223.

    Schwarz, N., & Wyer, R. S. (1985). Effects of rank ordering stimuli on magnitude

    ratings of these and other stimuli. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 21,30-46.

    Sigelman, L. (1981). Question-order effects on presidential popularity. Public Opinion

    Quarterly, 45, 199-207.

    Smith, R. H., Diener, E., & Wedell, D. H. (1989). Intrapersonal and social comparison

    determinants of happiness: A range-frequency analysis. Journal of Personality

    and Social Psychology, 56, 317-325.

    Smith, T. W. (1982). Conditional order effects. Paper presented at the annual meetingof the World Association for Public Opinion Research, Hunt Valley, Maryland,

    May, 1982.

    Strack, F., & Martin, L. L. (1987). Thinking, judging, and communicating: A process

    account of context effects in attitude surveys. In H. Hippler, N. Schwarz, & S.

    Sudman (Eds.), Social information processing and survey methodology. New

    York: Springer-Verlag.

    Swann, W. B., Pelham, B. W., & Chidester, T. R. (1988). Change through paradox:

    Using self-verification to alter beliefs. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 54, 268-273.

    Tourangeau, R., & Rasinski, K. A. (1988). Cognitive processes underlying contexteffects in attitude measurement. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 299-314.

    *Tourangeau, R., Rasinski, K. A., & Bradburn, N. (1991). Measuring happiness in

    surveys: A test of the subtraction hypothesis. Public Opinion Quarterly, 55, 255-266.

    Turner, C. F., & Krauss, E. (1978). Fallible indicators of the subjective state of thenation. American Psychologist, 33, 456-470.

    Weinstein, N. D. (1984). Why it won't happen to me: Perceptions of risk factors andsusceptibility. Health Psychology, 3, 431-457.

    25

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    28/44

    Willick, D. H., & Ashley, R. K. (1971). Survey question order and the political party

    preferences of college students and their parents. Public Opinion Quarterly, 35,

    189-199.

    11. Attitude Recall Questions

    Abramson, P. R., & Inglehart, R. (1992). Generational replacement and value change in

    eight West European societies. British Journal of Political Science, 22, 183-228.

    Aderman, D., & Brehm, S. S. (1976). On the recall of initial attitudes following

    counterattitudinal advocacy: An experimental reexamination. Personality andSocial Psychology Bulletin, 2, 59-62.

    Allegeier, A. R., Byrne, D., Brooks, B., & Revnes, D. (1979). The waffle phenomenon:

    Negative evaluations of those who shift attitudinally. Journal of Applied Social

    Psychology, 9, 170-182.

    Allsop, D., & Weisberg, H. F. (1988). Measuring change in party identification in an

    election campaign. American Journal of Political Science, 32, 996-1017.

    Andersen, K. (1979). Generation, partisan shift, and realignment: A glance back to thenew deal. In N. Nie, S. Verba, & J. Petrocik (Eds.). The changing American

    voter. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Anderson, R. C., & Pichert, J. W. (1978). Recall of previously unrecallable information

    following a shift in perspective. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior,

    17, 1-12.

    Andrews, F. M., & Withey, S. B. (1976). Social indicators of well-being. New York:Plenum Press.

    Baddeley, A. D., Lewis, V., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1978). When did you last ...? In M.M. Gruneberg, P. E. Morris, & R. N. Sykes (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory.

    London: Academic Press.

    Bain, R. (1931). Stability in questionnaire response. American Journal of Sociology,

    37, 445-453.

    Baltes, P. (1968). Longitudinal and cross-sectional sequences in the study of age and

    generation effects. Human Development, 11, 145-171.

    Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology.Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Bem, D. J. (1965). An experimental analysis of self-persuasion. Journal ofExperimental Social Psychology, 1, 199-218.

    26

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    29/44

    Bem, D. J. (1967). Self-perception: An alternative interpretation of cognitive

    dissonance phenomena. Psychological Review, 74, 183-200.

    *Bem, D. J., & McConnell, H. K. (1970). Testing the self-perception explanation of

    dissonance phenomena: On the salience of premanipulation attitudes. Journal of

    Personality and Social Psychology, 14, 23-31.

    Benewick, R. J., Birch, A. H., Blumler, J. G., & Ewbank, A. (1969). The floating voterand the liberal view of representation. Political Studies, 17, 177-195.

    Berent, M. K., & Krosnick, J. A. (1990). The accuracy of attitude recollections.

    Unpublished data, The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio.

    Blair, E., & Burton, S. (1987). Cognitive processes used by survey respondents to

    answer behavioral frequency questions. Journal of Consumer Research, 14, 280-

    288.

    Blair, J., Menon, G., & Bickart, B. (1991). Measurement effects in self vs. proxyresponses to survey questions: An information-processing perspective. In P. P.

    Biemer, R. M. Groves, L. E. Lyberg, N. A. Mathiowetz, & S. Sudman (Eds.),

    Measurement errors in surveys. New York: Wiley.

    Booth, A. (1971). The recall of news items. Public Opinion Quarterly, 34, 604-610.

    Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129-148.

    Bradburn, N. M., Rips, L. J., & Shevell, S. K. (1987). Answering autobiographical

    questions: The impact of memory and inference on surveys. Science, 236, 157-

    161.

    Breksted, A. (1966). Factors influencing the reliability of anamnestic recall. ChildDevelopment, 37, 601-612.

    Bridge, R. G., Reeder, L. G., Kanouse, D., Kinder, D. R., Nagy, V. T., & Judd, C. M.(1977). Interviewing changes attitudes - sometimes. Public Opinion Quarterly,

    41, 56-64.

    Burton, S., & Blair, E. (1991). Task conditions, response formulation processes, and

    response accuracy for behavioral frequency questions in surveys. Public OpinionQuarterly, 55, 50-79.

    Butler, D., & Stokes, D. (1969). Political change in Britain. New York: St. Martin'sPress.

    Cialdini, R. B., Braver, S. L., & Lewis, S. K. (1974). Attributional bias and the easily

    persuaded other. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 631-637.

    27

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    30/44

    Cialdini, R. B., Levy, A., Herman, C. P., & Evenbeck, S. (1973). Attitudinal politics:

    The strategy of moderation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 25,

    100-108.

    Cannell, C. F., & Fowler, F. J. (1963). A comparison of a self-enumerative procedure

    and a personal interview: A validity study. Public Opinion Quarterly, 27, 250-264.

    Cannell, C. F., Oksenberg, L., & Converse, J. M. (1978). Experiments in interviewing

    techniques: Field experiments in health reporting, 1971-1977. Washington, D.C.:

    U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, DHEW Publication No.(HRA) 78-3204.

    Cialdini, R. B., Braver, S. L., & Lewis, S. K. (1974). Attributional bias and the easily

    persuaded other. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 631-637.

    Cialdini, R. B., Levy, A., Herman, P., & Evenbeck, S. (1973). Attitudinal politics: The

    strategy of moderation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 25, 100-108.

    Conway, M., & Ross, M. (1984). Getting what you want by revising what you had.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 738-748.

    Crowder, R. G. (1976). Principles of learning and memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Dakin, R. E., & Tennant, D. (1968). Consistency of response by event-recall intervalsand characteristics of respondents. Sociological Quarterly, 9, 73-84.

    Duncan, O. D. (1975). Measuring social change via replication of surveys. In K. C.Land & S. Spilerman (Eds.), Social indicator models. New York: Russell Sage.

    Duncan, O. D., Schuman, H. & Duncan, B. (1973). Social change in a metropolitan

    community. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Eich, E., Reeves, J. L., Jaeger, B., & Graff-Radford, S. B. (1985). Memory for pain:

    Relation between past and present pain intensity. Pain, 23, 375-380.

    Faunce, W. A. (1958). Automation and the automobile worker. Social Problems, 6, 68-

    78.

    Ferber, R. (1966). The reliability of consumer reports of financial assets and debts.Urbana, IL: Bureau of Economic and Business Research, University of Illinois.

    Ferber, R., Forsythe, J., Guthrie, H., & Maynes, E. S. (1969). Validation of a nationalsurvey of consumer financial characteristics: savings accounts. Review of

    Economics and Statistics, 51, 436-444.

    Fink, R. (1960). The retrospective question. Public Opinion Quarterly, 24, 143-148.

    28

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    31/44

    Fischhoff, B. (1975). Hindsight =/ foresight: The effect of outcome knowledge on

    judgment under uncertainty. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human

    Perception and Performance, 1, 288-299.

    Fischhoff, B. (1977). Perceived informativeness of facts. Journal of Experimental

    Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3, 349-358.

    Goethals, G. R., & Reckman, R. F. (1973). The perception of consistency in attitudes.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 9, 491-501.

    Granberg, D., & Holmberg, S. (1991). Self-reported turnout and voter validation.

    American Journal of Political Science, 35, 448-459.

    Granberg, D. & Holmberg, S. (1992). The Hawthorne effect in election studies: The

    impact of survey participation on voting. British Journal of Political Science, 22,

    240-247.

    Gutek, B. A. (1978). On the accuracy of retrospective attitudinal data. Public OpinionQuarterly, 42, 390-401.

    Hardin, E. (1960a). Computer automation, work environment, and employeesatisfaction: A case study. Industrial and Labor Relations Review, 13, 559-567.

    Hardin, E. (1960b). The reactions of employees to office automation. Monthly Labor

    Review, 83, 925-932.

    Hardin, E. (1965). Perceived and actual change in job satisfaction. Journal of Applied

    Psychology, 49, 363-367.

    Hardin, E., & Hershey, G. L. (1960). Accuracy of employee reports on changes in pay.

    Journal of Applied Psychology, 44, 269-275.

    Hastie, R., & Park, B. (1986). The relationship between memory and judgment depends

    on whether the judgment task is online or memory-based. Psychological Review,93, 258-268.

    Hastorf, A., & Cantril, H. (1954). They saw a game. Journal of Abnormal and Social

    Psychology, 47, 129-143.

    Hawkins, S. A., & Hastie, R. (1990). Hindsight: Biased judgments of past events after

    the outcomes are know. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 311-327.

    Himmelweit, H. T., Biberian, M. J., & Stockdale, J. (1978). Memory for past vote:

    Implications of a study of bias in recall. British Journal of Political Science, 8,365-375.

    29

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    32/44

    Himmelweit, H. T., Humphreys, P., & Jaeger, M. (1985). How voters decide: A

    longitudinal study of political attitudes and voting extended over fifteen years.

    Philadelphia: Open University Press.

    Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L., Prohaska, V. (1988). Hierarchical organization in ordered

    domains: Estimating the dates of events. Psychological Review, 95, 471-484.

    Katz, R. S., Niemi, R. G., & Newman, D. (1980). Reconstructing past partisanship inBritain. British Journal of Political Science, 10, 505-537.

    Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. In D. Levine (Ed.),

    Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 15, pp. 192-240). Lincoln: University

    of Nebraska Press.

    Kenny, D. A., & Campbell, D. T. (1989). On the measurement of stability in over-time

    data. Journal of Personality, 57, 445-481.

    Ladd, E. C., & Hadley, C. D. (1975). Transformations of the American party system.New York: Norton.

    Lansing, J. B., Ginsburg, G., & Braaten, K. (1961). An investigation of response error.Urbana, IL: Bureau of Economic and Business Research, University of Illinois.

    Locander, W., Sudman, S., & Bradburn, N. (1976). An investigation of interview

    method, threat, and response distortion. Journal of the American Statistical

    Association, 71, 269-275.

    Loftus, E. F. (1975). Leading questions and the eyewitness report. Cognitive

    Psychology, 7, 560-572.

    Loftus, E. F., & Zanni, G. (1975). Eyewitness testimony: The influence of the wordingof a question. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 5, 86-88.

    Lyons, T. F., & Dickinson, T. L. (1973). A comparison of perceived- and computed-change measures over a three-year period. Journal of Applied Psychology, 58,

    318-321.

    Mann, F. C., & Hoffman, L. R. (1960). Automation and the worker. New York: Holt.

    Markus, G. B. (1982). Political attitudes during an election year: A report of the 1980

    NES panel study. American Political Science Review, 23, 338-359.

    *Markus, G. B. (1986). Stability and change in political attitudes: Observed, recalled,

    and "explained". Political Behavior, 8, 21-44.

    McFarland, C., & Ross, M. (1987). The relation between current impressions andmemories of self and dating partners. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,

    13, 228-238.

    30

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    33/44

    McFarland, C., Ross, M., & Conway, M. (1985). Self-persuasion and self-presentation

    as mediators of anticipatory attitude change. Journal of Personality and Social

    Psychology, 46, 529-540.

    McFarland, C., Ross, M., & Giltrow, M. (1992). Biased recollections in older adults:

    The role of implicit theories of aging. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 62, 837-850.

    Means, B., Nigam, A., Zarrow, M., Loftus, E. F., & Donaldson, M. S. (1989).

    Autobiographical memory for health-related events. Washington, D.C.: U.S.

    Department of Health and Human Services, DHHS Publication No. (OHS) 89-1077.

    Mednick, S. A., & Shaffer, J. B. P. (1963). Mothers' retrospective reports in child-

    rearing research. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 33, 457-461.

    Mischel, W., Ebbesen, E. B., & Zeiss, A. M. (1976). Determinants of selective memory

    about the self. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 44, 92-103.

    Murphy, G., & Likert, R. (1967). Public opinion and the individual. New York: Russell

    and Russell.

    Natale, M., & Hantas, M. (1982). Effect of temporary mood states on selective memory

    about the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 927-934.

    National Center for Health Statistics. (1965b). Health interview responses comparedwith medical records. Vital and Health Statistics, PHS Publication No. 1000-

    Series 2-No. 8. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office.

    National Center for Health Statistics. (1965c). Comparison of hospitalization reporting

    in three survey procedures. Vital and Health Statistics, PHS Publication No.

    1000-Series 2-No. 8. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office.

    National Center for Health Statistics. (1971). Effect of some experimental interviewingtechniques on reporting in the Health Interview Survey. Vital and Health

    Statistics, PHS Publication No. 1000-Series 2-No. 41. Washington, D.C.: U.S.

    Government Printing Office.

    National Center for Health Statistics. (1972a). Effect of an extensive questionnaire and a

    diary procedure. Vital and Health Statistics, Series 2-No. 49, DHEW Publication

    No. (HSM) 72-1049 Health Services and Mental Health Administration.Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office.

    National Center for Health Statistics. (1972b). Optimum recall period for reporting

    persons injured in motor vehicle accidents. Vital and Health Statistics, Series 2-

    No. 50, DHEW Publication No. (HSM) 72-1050. Washington, D.C.: U.S.Government Printing Office.

    31

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    34/44

    National Center for Health Statistics. (1977). A summary of studies of interviewing

    methodology. Vital and Health Statistics, Series 2-No. 69, DHEW Publication

    No. (HRA) 77-1343. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office.

    National Center for Health Statistics. (1965a). Reporting of hospitalization in the health

    interview survey. Vital and Health Statistics, PHS Publication No. 1000-Series 2-No. 6. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office.

    Neter, J., & Waksberg, J. (1964). A study of response errors in expenditures data from

    household interviews. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 59, 18-55.

    Newcomb, T. M. (1943). Personality and social change. New York: Dryden.

    Newcomb, T. M., Koenig, K. E., Flacks, R., & Warwick, D. P. (1967). Persistence and

    change: Bennington College and its students after twenty-five years. New York:

    Wiley.

    Newman, W. R. (1976). Patterns of recall among television news viewers. PublicOpinion Quarterly, 40, 115-123.

    Niemi, R. G., Katz, R. S., & Newman, D. (1980). Reconstructing past partisanship: Thefailure of the party identification recall questions. American Journal of Political

    Science, 24, 633-651.

    Olson, J. M., & Cal, A. V. (1984). Source credibility, attitudes, and the recall of past

    behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 14, 203-210.

    Parry, H. J., & Crossley, H. M. (1950). Validity of responses to survey questions.

    Public Opinion Quarterly, 14, 61-80.

    Powers, E., & Bultena, G. (1972). Characteristics of decreased dropouts in longitudinalresearch. Journal of Gerontology, 27, 530-535.

    Powers, E. A., Goudy, W. J., & Keith, P. M. (1978). Congruence between panel andrecall data in longitudinal research. Public Opinion Quarterly, 42, 380-389.

    Reiter, H. L. (1980). The perils of partisan recall. Public Opinion Quarterly, 44, 385-

    388.

    Robbins, L. C. (1963). The accuracy of parental recall of aspects of child development

    and of child rearing practices. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66,261-270.

    Roberts, J. V. (1985). The attitude-memory relationship after 40 years: A meta-analysisof the literature. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 6, 221-241.

    Robinson, J. A. (1976). Sampling autobiographical memory. Cognitive Psychology, 8,

    578-595.

    32

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    35/44

    Rogers, T. F. (1976). Interviews by telephone and in person: Quality of responses and

    field performance. Public Opinion Quarterly, 40, 51-65.

    Ross, M. (1989). Relation of implicit theories to the construction of personal histories.

    Psychological Review, 96, 341-357.

    Ross, M., & Shulman, R. F. (1973). Increasing the salience of initial attitudes:

    Dissonance versus self-perception theory. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 28, 138-144.

    *Ross, M., & Conway, M. (1986). Remembering one's own past: The construction of

    personal histories. In R. M. Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of

    motivation and cognition (pp. 122-144). New York: Guilford Press.

    Ross, M., McFarland, C., & Fletcher, G. J. O. (1981). The effect of attitude on the recall

    of personal histories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 627-634.

    Ross, M., McFarland, C., Conway, M., & Zanna, M. P. (1983). Reciprocal relationbetween attitudes and behavior recall: Committing people to newly formed

    attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 257-267.

    Shaffer, D. R. (1975a). Another look at the phenomenological equivalence of pre- and

    postmanipulation attitudes in the forced-compliance experiment. Personality and

    Social Psychology Bulletin, 1, 497-500.

    Shaffer, D. R. (1975b). Some effects of consonant and dissonant attitudinal advocacy oninitial attitude salience and attitude change. Journal of Personality and Social

    Psychology, 32, 160-168.

    Skowronski, J. J., Betz, A. L., Thompson, C. P., & Shannon, L. (1991). Social memory

    in everyday life: Recall of self-events and other-events. Journal of Personality

    and Social Psychology, 60, 831-843.

    Smith, T. W. (1984). Recalling attitudes: An analysis of retrospective questions on the1982 GSS. Public Opinion Quarterly, 48, 639-649.

    Snyder, M., & White, P. (1982). Moods and memories: Elation, depression, and theremembering of the events of one's life. Journal of Personality, 50, 149-167.

    Staw, B. M., & Ross, J. (1980). Commitment in an experimenting society: A study of

    the attribution of leadership from administrative scenarios. Journal of AppliedPsychology, 65, 249-260.

    Streib, G. (1966). Participants and drop-outs in a longitudinal study. Journal of

    Gerontology, 21, 200-209.

    Sudman, S., & Bradburn, N. (1973). Effects of time and memory factors on response insurveys. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 68, 805-815.

    33

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    36/44

    Sudman, S., & Bradburn, N. M. (1974). Response effects in surveys: A review and

    synthesis. Chicago: Aldine.

    Teasdale, J. D., & Fogarty, S. J. (1979). Differential effects of induced mood on

    retrieval of pleasant and unpleasant events from episodic memory. Journal of

    Abnormal Psychology, 88, 248-257.

    Teasdale, J. D., & Taylor, R. (1981). Induced mood and accessibility of memories: Aneffect of mood state or of induction procedure? British Journal of Clinical

    Psychology, 20, 39-48.

    Tedeschi, J. T., Schlenker, B. R., & Bonoma, T. V. (1971). Cognitive dissonance:

    Private retiocination or public spectacle. American Psychologist, 26, 685-695.

    Trussell, R. E., & Elinson, J. (1959). Chronic illness in the United States, Vol. III.

    Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Wanous, J. P., & Reichers, A. E. (1992). Measuring "change": Remembered change vs.longitudinal data. WPS 92-112, Working Paper Series, College of Business, Ohio

    State University, Columbus, Ohio.

    Weir, B. T. (1975). The distortion of voter recall. American Journal of Political

    Science, 19, 53-62.

    Weiss, C. (1968). Validity of welfare mothers' interview responses. Public Opinion

    Quarterly, 32, 622-633.

    +Wells, G. L., & Loftus, E. F. (1984). Eyewitness testimony: Psychological

    perspectives. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Wenar, C., & Coulter, J. B. (1962). A reliability study of developmental histories. ChildDevelopment, 33, 453-462.

    White, L., & Edwards, J. N. (1990). Emptying the nest and parental well-being: Ananalysis of national panel data. American Sociological Review, 55, 235-242.

    Whyte, W. F. (1951). Pattern for industrial peace. New York: Harper.

    Williams, M. D., & Hollan, J. D. (1981). The process of retrieval from very long-term

    memory. Cognitive Science, 5, 87-119.

    Withey, S. B. (1954). Reliability of recall of income. Public Opinion Quarterly, 18,

    197-204.

    Wixon, D. R., & Laird, J. D. (1976). Awareness and attitude change in the forced-

    compliance paradigm: The importance of when. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 34, 376-384.

    34

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    37/44

    Yarrow, M. R., Campbell, J. D., & Burton, R. V. (1970). Recollections of childhood: A

    study of the retrospective method. Monographs of the Society for Research in

    Child Development, 35, 1-83.

    12. Asking Why

    Bem, D. J. (1967). Self-perception: An alternative interpretation of cognitive dissonance

    phenomena. Psychological Review, 74, 183-200.

    Bem, D. J. & McConnell, H. K. (1970). Testing the self-perception explanation of

    dissonance phenomena: On the salience of premanipulation attitudes. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 14, 23-31.

    Bishop, G. F. (1989). Think-aloud responses to survey questions: Illustrations of a newqualitative technique. Presented at the annual conference of the American

    Association for Public Opinion Research, St. Petersburg, Florida.

    Blair, E. & Burton, S. (1987). Cognitive processes used by survey respondents to

    answer behavioral frequency questions. Journal of Consumer Research, 14, 280-288.

    Boring, E. G. (1953). A history of introspection. Psychological Bulletin, 50, 169-189.

    Bornstein, R. F., Leone, D. R., & Galley, D. J. (1987). The generalizability ofsubliminal mere exposure effects: Influence of stimuli perceived without

    awareness on social behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53,

    1070-1079.

    Brown, R. (1962). Models of attitude change. In R. Brown, E. Galanter, E. H. Hess, &

    G. Mandler (Eds.), New directions in psychology (Vol. 1). New York: Holt,Rinehart & Winston.

    Burt, C. L. (1925). The young delinquent. London: University of Toronto Press.

    Byrne, D. (1959). The effects of a subliminal food stimulus on verbal responses.

    Journal of Applied Psychology, 43, 249-252.

    Chaiken, S. & Yates, S. (1985). Affective-cognitive consistency and thought-induced

    attitude polarization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1470-

    1481.

    Chapman, D W. (1932). Relative effects of determinate and indeterminate "Aufgaben."American Journal of Psychology, 44, 163-174.

    35

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    38/44

    Comstock, C. (1921). On the relevancy of imagery to the processes of thought.

    American Journal of Psychology, 32, 196-230.

    Comte, A. (1896). The positive philosophy of Comte. London: Bell, (Originally

    published, 1830-1842).

    Davis, J. A. (1964). Great aspirations: The graduate school plans of America's college

    students. Chicago: Aldine.

    Dreben, E. K., Fiske, S. T., & Hastie, R. (1979). The independence of evaluative anditem information: Impression and recall order effects in behavior-based

    impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1758-

    1768.

    Duval, S. & Wicklund's, R. A. (1972). A theory of objective self awareness. New York:

    Academic.

    Ericsson, K. A. & Simon, H. A. (1984). Protocol analysis: Verbal reports as data.Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.

    Fazio, R. H., Chen, J. M., McDonel, E. C. & Sherman, S. J. (1982). Attitudeaccessibility, attitude-behavior consistency, and the strength of the object-

    evaluation association. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 18, 339-357.

    Fishbein, M. & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An

    introduction to theory and research. Reading, MASS: Addison-Wesley.

    Gaudet, H. (1955). A model for assessing changes in voting intention. In P. F.

    Lazarsfeld & M. Rosenberg (Eds.), The language of social research. New York:Free Press of Glencoe.

    Gavanski, I. & Hoffman, C. (1986). Assessing influences on one's own judgments: Is

    there greater accuracy for either subjectively important or objectively influential

    variables? Social Psychology Quarterly, 49, 33-44.

    Gavanski, I. & Hoffman, C. (1987). Awareness of influences on one's own judgments:

    The roles of covariation detection and attention to the judgment process. Journalof Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 453-463.

    Genest, M. & Turk, D. C. (1981). Think-aloud approaches to cognitive assessment. In

    T. Merluzzi, C. Glass, & M. Genest (Eds.), Cognitive assessment. N.Y.:Guilford.

    Goethals, G. R. & Reckman, R. F. (1973). The perception of consistency in attitudes.

    Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 9, 491-501.

    Goode, W. J. (1956). After divorce. New York: Free Press of Glencoe.

    36

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    39/44

    Hastie, R. & Park, B. (1986). The relationship between memory and judgment depends

    on whether the judgment task is memory-based or on-line. Psychological

    Review, 93, 258-268.

    Hawkins, D. (1970). The effects of subliminal stimulation on drive level and brand

    preference. Journal of Marketing Research, 7, 322-326.

    Hothersall, D. (1984). History of psychology. New York: Random House.

    Hume, D. (1739). A treatise of human nature. London: Noon.

    James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Holt.

    Jamieson, D. W., Lydon, J. E. & Zanna, M. P. (1987). Attitude and activity preference

    similarity: Differential bases of interpersonal attraction for low and high self-monitors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1052-1060.

    Kadushin, C. (1958). Individual decisions to undertake psychotherapy. AdministrativeScience Quarterly, 3, 379-411.

    Kelman, H. (1966). Deception in social research. Transaction, 3, 20-24.

    Kimble, G. A. & Perlmuter, L. C. (1970). The problem of volition. Psychological

    Review, 77, 361-384.

    Kraut, R. E. & Lewis, S. H. (1981). Person perception and self-awareness: knowledge

    of influences on one's own judgments. Journal of Personality and Social

    Psychology, 42, 448-460.

    Krosnick, J. A. (1991). Response strategies for coping with the cognitive demands ofattitude measures in surveys. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 5, 213-236.

    Krosnick, J. A., Betz, A. L., Jussim, L. J., & Lynn, A. R. (1992). Subliminalconditioning of attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 152-

    162.

    Kulpe, O. (1895). Outlines of psychology. Originally published, 1893. E. B. Titchener

    (Trans.). New York: Macmillan.

    Kunst-Wilson, W. R., & Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Affective discrimination of stimuli that

    cannot be recognized. Science, 207, 557-558.

    Langer, E. J. (1989). Mindfulness. Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley.

    Latane, B. & Darley, J. (1970). The Unresponsive bystander: Why doesn't he help?

    New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Lazarsfeld, P. F. (1935). The art of asking why: Three principles underlying theformulation of questionnaires. National Marketing Review, 1, 32-43.

    37

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    40/44

    Leahey, T. H. (1991). A history of modern psychology. New Jersey: Prentice Hall.

    Lessler, J. T. (1987). Use of Laboratory Methods and Cognitive Science for the Design

    and Testing of Questionnaires. Handbook of methods. Sweden.

    Lessler, J., Tourangeau, R., & Salter, W. (1989). Questionnaire design in the cognitive

    research laboratory. Vital and Health Statistics, Series 6, No. 1 (HHS Publication

    No. PHS 89-1076). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.

    Lieberman, D. A. (1979). Behaviorism and the mind. A (limited) call for a return tointrospection. American Psychologist, 34, 319-333.

    Locke, J. (1959). An essay concerning human understanding. New York: Dover,

    (Originally published, 1690.)

    Lodge, M., McGraw, K. M., & Stroh, P. (1989). An impression-driven model of

    candidate evaluation. American Political Science Review, 83, 399-419.

    Lusk, C. & Judd, C. M. (1988). Political expertise and the structural mediators of

    candidate evaluations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 24, 105-126.

    Mackie, D. M. & Asuncion, A. G. (1990). On-line and memory-based modification of

    attitudes: determinants of message recall-attitude change correspondence.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 5-16.

    Markus, G. B. (1986). Stability and change in political attitudes: Observed, recalled,

    and "Explained". Political Behavior, 8, 21-44.

    Mayer, A., & Orth, J. (1901). Zur qualitativen Untersuchung der Association. Z.

    Psychol. Physiol. Sinnesorg., 26, 1-13.

    McDougall, W. (1922). Prolegomena to psychology. Psychological Review, 29, 1-43.

    McGraw, K., Lodge, M. & Stroh, P. (1990). Processes of candidate evaluation: On-line

    or memory based? A review and synthesis of four experiments. Unpublished

    manuscript, Department of Political Science, State University of New York atStony Brook.

    Millar, M. G. & Tesser, A. (1986). Effects of affective and cognitive focus on the

    attitude-behavior relationship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51,

    270-276.

    Millar, M. G. & Tesser, A. (1989). The effects of affective-cognitive consistency and

    thought on the attitude-behavior relation. Journal of Experimental SocialPsychology, 25, 189-202.

    Newport, F. (1991). The Gallup poll monthly (no. 313). Princeton, NJ: The Gallup

    Organization.

    38

  • 7/28/2019 Comm 239

    41/44

    *Nisbett, R. E. & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports

    on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84, 231-259.

    Pryor, J. B., Gibbons, F. X., Wicklund, R. A. Fazio, & Hood, R. (1977). Self-focused

    attention and self-report validity. Journal of Personality Psychology, 45, 513-527.

    Quattrone, G. A. (1985). On the congruity between internal states and action.

    Psychological Bulletin, 98, 3-40.

    Rahn, W. M. (1990). Perception and evaluation of political candidates: A social-cognitive perspective. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Minnesota.

    Robles, R., Smith, R., Carver, C. S., & Wellens, A. R. (1987). Influence of subliminal

    visual images on the experience of anxiety. Personality and Social Psychology

    Bulletin, 13, 399-410.

    Rossi, P. H. (1955). Why families move: A study in the social psychology of urban

    residential mobility. New York: Free Press of Glencoe.

    Rousseau, J., Johnson, E., & Stevens, D. (1984). The validity of verbal protocols.

    Manuscript described in Zajonc, R. B.