christine cordle - the use of operant self-control in compulsive hair-pulling

Upload: irving-a-perez-mendez

Post on 02-Jun-2018

232 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/11/2019 Christine Cordle - The Use of Operant Self-control in Compulsive Hair-pulling

    1/4

    J. Behov. Ther. Exp. Psychior. Vol. II. pp. 127-130.

    k:Pergamon Press Ltd., 1980 . Printed in Great Britain.

    ooO5-7916/80/0604127 02 W/O

    THE USE OF OPERANT SELF CONTROL PROCEDURES IN THE

    TREATMENT OF COMPULSIVE HAIR PULLING

    CHRISTINE J. CORDLE and CLIVE G. LONG

    Leicester General Hospital and Walsgrave Hospital, Coventry, England

    Summary-Two young female patients with a lo- and 5-year history of compulsive hair pulling

    were individually treated by the same operant self-control procedure. Zero pulling was

    achieved at weeks 6 and 13, and maintained at follow-up 15 months later. These findings are

    discussed in the light of previous behavioural treatments, and the importance of eliciting an

    appropriate aversive consequence for habit control is stressed.

    The incidence of trichotillomania is extremely

    difficult to ascertain, partly because reported

    cases are divided between the dermatological and

    psychotherapeutic literature; and partly because

    many authors have made unverified estimates

    on the frequency of the symptoms ranging from

    rare (Philippopoulos, 1961) to commonly seen

    (Monroe and Abse, 1963).

    Although fiddling with hair may be considered

    a normal activity among social primates (Horne,

    1977), no normative data exists on the incidence

    of hair pulling among the non-institutionalized

    adult population. What is clear, however, is that

    the phenomenon of hair pulling, of a severity

    requiring treatment, is uncommon. For this

    reason, perhaps, there has never been a con-

    trolled trial of any form of therapy for

    trichotillomania.

    The benefits of psychoanalytically based

    psychotherapy have been generally extolled, yet

    most of these studies remark on the malignant

    and chronic nature of trichotillomania with

    frequent exacerbations and remissions (Green-

    berg and Sarner, 1965). Generally psychoanalytic

    procedures have met with little success, and in

    some cases the process involving introspection

    may accelerate the rate of self-destructive

    symptoms.

    Studies using behavioural intervention are

    mostly case histories, but in terms of quantitative

    data and attempts at specifying the relationship

    between symptom change and treatment the

    behaviour literature is undoubtedly superior.

    Taylor (1963) eliminated the habit by interrupt-

    ing the behavioural sequence through the

    practice of an antagonistic response. Bayer

    (1972) employed a technique of self-monitoring

    and a mild form of aversive control (saving hairs

    pulled) to treat a case of two years duration.

    Unfortunately no follow-up data were presented.

    Over the period 1974-1978 at least five

    English language reports on the behavioural

    treatment of trichotillomania in adults have

    appeared with encouraging results. Techniques

    used have included contingency contracting

    (Stabler and Warren, 1974), positive coverants

    and response cost (McLaughlin and Nay, 1975),

    covert sensitization (Levine, 1976), suggestive

    hypnosis (Horne, 1977), aversive conditioning

    (Horne, 1977), and cognitive desensitization

    (Bornstein and Rychtarik, 1978).

    The six patients treated were mostly young

    females with an age range of 14 to 42 years

    and with a 2 to 35 year history of hair pulling.

    In three cases the extent of the habit had resulted

    in the wearing of a wig or headscarf and the

    baseline rate of hair pulling ranged from 22.6

    Requests for reprints should be sent to Miss C. J. Cordle, Senior Psychologist, Psychology Department, Leicester

    General Has+*

    -h

    Road, Leicester LES 4PW, England.

    127

  • 8/11/2019 Christine Cordle - The Use of Operant Self-control in Compulsive Hair-pulling

    2/4

    128

    CHRISTINE J. CORDLE and CLIVE G. LONG

    to 220 hairs per week. Except in the cases cited

    by Horne (1977), where concurrent therapy for

    sexual and family problems makes evaluation

    difficult, treatment has usually been brief and

    effective with no remission at up to six months

    follow-up.

    Unfortunately, the nature of many of these

    studies precludes the possibility of isolating

    active agents of therapeutic change, and con-

    clusions are based on anecdotal reports by the

    patients themselves. Most treatment procedures

    include positive and negative reinforcement,

    anxiety reduction and feedback.

    While self-monitoring and hair collection

    may account for initial decline in target be-

    haviour, long term maintenance has been attri-

    buted to the use of a cognitive desensitization

    procedure (Bornstein and Rychtarik, 1978);

    while Horne (1977) suggests that more effective

    treatment techniques may be developed by

    investigating the relationship between anxiety

    and habit strength. In Levines (1976) rapid

    treatment, an anxiety management technique

    (relaxation) was only incidentally used as a

    part of covert sensitization (imagined self-

    mutilation and nausea following hair pulling).

    Use of covert sensititation in altering symbolic

    representations of undesired performance, how-

    ever, may require determination of the natural-

    istic occurrence and functional role of such

    representations (Mahoney, 1974). Application

    of operant self-conlrol proccdurcs lo the prob-

    lems of obesity, for example, found that verbal

    descriptions of aversive comcquences their

    subject5 had actually experienced (social rejection,

    sarcastic treatment, demeaning infcrenccs) were

    more potent variables in treatment than statis-

    tically probable consequences such as diabetes

    (Fcrstcr, Nurnbcrgcr and Levitt, 1962).

    Following the analysis of Fcrstcr e/ (I/. (1962)

    an individual will manipula(c the factor\ which

    dctcrminc the frequency and amount of an

    undesired behaviour if this control is reinforcing

    to him-if it cnablcs him to ejcapc from the

    ultimate avcrsivc con\equcnces (U.A.C..). In

    scvcrc cases of tricllotillornania, whcrc basc-

    lint rates of hair-pulling are very high and where

    the aversive results of the habit are covered by a

    wig or headscarf, it would seem that the

    pa ient is

    protected

    from the social con-

    sequences of the habit (e.g. embarrassment,

    anxiety) as well as from social reinforcement

    for habit reduction. It seems likely that penetra-

    tion of this disguise is for many extremely

    aversive and it is surprising that no treatment

    procedure has dealt with manipulation of this

    variable.

    In accordance with these considerations, the

    present study reports the results of two cases ot

    severe trichotillomania treated (one by each

    author) within an operant self-control regime.

    METHOD

    ub jec ts

    Client A was a 25year-old married woman who described

    a IO-year history of hair pulling which she had never been

    able completely to inhibit. She reported that she had

    started pulling her hair out when she was bullied at school.

    Eyelash pul ling occurred for a short period but spon-

    taneously remitted. The usual pattern was for the hair to be

    played with pulled out examined for root removal

    chewed and finally discarded. Hair was pulled from the

    top of her head and above the right and left ear. In all 3

    areas the hair was thin and patchy. Diary recordings

    revealed that hair was usually pulled in the evenings last

    thing at night and first thing in the morning. The habit

    occurred most typically when she was alone and the fre-

    quency increased when she was angry depressed or worried.

    Over the years she disguised her problem with a variety of

    hair styles a scarf and eventually for the last 3 years a wig.

    Embarrassment and shame led to constant worry that her

    problem which she hid from everyone except her husband

    and mother would be discovered. The client reported that

    her husband was very supportive and willing to help her in

    any way possible.

    Client B was an attractive 19-year-old girl with a 5.year

    history of trichotillomania which started soon after the

    death of her mother. Prior to this she had been in the habit

    of fiddling with her hair a great deal. The hair pul ling had

    persisted with only a 2-week period of abstinence which

    followed a hurtful comment made about her hair. Hairs

    were singly pulled from the top of her scalp back of her

    head and above both ears where the hair was very patchy.

    She would chew the hair before discarding it. The habit was

    most likely to occur when working at her desk or at home

    when she was reading or watching T.V. The frequency

    increased when she was anxious. Her state of mind when

    she performed the hair pulling seemed to vary from absent-

    mindedness when she would suddenly discover that she had

    been pulling her hair without thinking about it to a state

    in which she felt the urge to pull her hair and did so in

    spite of some sense of resistance. She was self-conscious

    and embarrassed by her problem and had attempted to

  • 8/11/2019 Christine Cordle - The Use of Operant Self-control in Compulsive Hair-pulling

    3/4

    THE TREATMENT OF COMPULSIVE HAIR-PULLING

    129

    disguise it with various hairstyles and a headscarf. Nail

    biting apart she showed no other neurotic features.

    Procedure

    Basel ine.

    After the initial assessment interview each

    client was asked to record daily the number of hairs pulled

    out over a 2-week period.

    Treafmenl.

    The clients were then seen once a week for

    % hourly treatment sessions. Client A was treated by one

    of the authors and Client B by the other, using the same

    treatment techniques:

    (I) Feedback to increase awareness of behaviour including

    self-monitoring by weekly collection of hairs in an envelope

    (following Bayer, 1972) and visual display of progress via a

    graph in the therapists office.

    (2) Self-targeting: Client and therapist agreed on the

    maximum number of hairs to be pulled each week. This was

    to decrease each time the target was achieved.

    (3) Stimulus control: The use of incompatible responses

    when the urge to pull hair occurred (a prepotent repertoire

    including knitting, isometric and relaxation exercises) and,

    in the case of Client A, removal to a predetermined safe

    environment, e.g. the room in which her husband was

    working.

    (4) Escalating response cost: (a) After each occasion

    of hair-pulling the client was instructed to examine a

    photograph of herself which showed very clearly the results

    of her trichotillomania. (b) Use of the U.A.C. should the

    client pull more than the self-targeted number of hairs per

    week. In both cases this was found to be removal of their

    respective disguises and a scalp examination by a young

    male member of the hospital staff.

    (5) Positive reinforcement consisting of coffee and

    general pratre at each session when the stated target was

    achieved.

    (6) Phasing out of wig and scarf wearing as control was

    achieved and hair grew back.

    Fol low up

    After 4 weeks of zero hair pulling, each client was

    followed up every month for 6 months and at 3-monthly

    intervals thereafter for a total of 15 months.

    RESULTS

    The number of hairs pulled decreased dramatic-

    ally in the case of Client A from a baseline mean

    of 515/week to zero/week after 5 treatment

    sessions. In the case of Client B the number of

    hairs pulled decreased more gradually from a

    baseline mean of 692/week to zero/week over a

    period of 13 weeks (see Table 1). Phasing out

    the wig and headscarf in the case of Client A

    was gradually achieved over a period of 12

    months. Initially, 5 months after zero hair

    pulling, she was able to discard the wig at

    Table

    I.

    Number of hairs pulled out

    Week

    Client A

    Client B

    Baseline mean 515

    692

    I

    17

    531

    2

    16

    487

    3

    10

    455

    4 7 420

    5 1

    22

    6 0

    I90

    7

    0

    86

    8 0

    58

    9 0

    52

    IO

    0

    50

    II 0

    39

    12 0

    35

    13

    0

    0

    16

    0

    0

    home, substituting it for a headscarf. After a

    further 2 months she was able to go out in the

    evenings and then at weekends with only a

    headscarf and after another month she paid her

    first visit for 11 years to the hairdressers. By

    this time, there had been substantial hair-growth

    at the back and sides of her head but the hair on

    the top of her scalp remained very thin and

    patchy. After 12 months she was able to go to

    work without a wig or headscarf, with her hair

    styled to cover the bald patches. Since then the

    wig has been completely discarded and she

    and her husband reported strong negative

    feelings towards it.

    Client Bs disguise was less total than Client A

    and the phasing out process was therefore

    quicker. Three months after zero hair pulling,

    she only used a headscarf on very windy days

    and after a further three months she no longer

    used any form of disguise.

    At 15 months follow-up there had been no

    relapse with either client.

    DISCUSSION

    The present study provides support for the

    use of operant self-control procedures in the

    treatment of two severe cases of trichotillo-

    mania. The length of follow-up, which greatly

    exceeded that of previous studies, was sufficient

    to monitor phasing out of habit disguise, which

    was particularly difficult for Client A. As a

  • 8/11/2019 Christine Cordle - The Use of Operant Self-control in Compulsive Hair-pulling

    4/4

    130

    CHRISTINE J. CORDLE and CLIVE G. LONG

    result of treatment, both clients were able to

    engage in activities previously restricted, such

    as swimming and various other outdoor pursuits.

    They both reported an increase in self-confidence

    and assertiveness, and Client B initiated her first

    serious relationship. Her embarrassment con-

    cerning her hair had previously led her to

    avoid such contact. The differential rate of

    reduction in hair pulling between the 2 clients

    was thought to reflect the greater support Client

    A received from her husband. Further, the

    U.C.A (wig removal) was possibly a greater

    deterrent than that for Client B whose disguise

    was less total.

    Observation and the clients own report

    showed a number of factors to be of importance

    in treatment success. The cases support Hornes

    (1977) hypothesis that where a strong habitual

    component exists, strong forms of feedback

    (hair collection and photograph examination)

    are required. As in previous studies (McLaughlin

    and Nay, 1975; Levine, 1976) both patients

    commented on increased feelings of self-control

    made possible by self-targeting; using a margin

    of error of I or 2 hairs per week clients felt

    safe and more capable of maintaining zero

    pulling. Habit-related anxiety was minimal in

    both cases. The most important treatment

    component identified by the clients was threat

    she clearly found the process extremely stressful,

    she felt it was the critical factor in continued

    abstinence. In the case of Client A, the threat

    alone of the U.A.C. was a sufficient deterrent.

    This study suggest that a self-control pro-

    cedure combining feedback with appropriate

    negative reinforcement can be a potent and

    lasting form of intervention for trichotillomania.

    REFERENCES

    Bayer C. A. (1972) Self-monitoring and mild aversion

    treatment of trichotillomania, J. Behov. T/w. & Exp.

    Psychiut. 3, 139-141.

    Bornstein P. H. and Rychtarik R. G. (1978) Multi-

    component behavioural treatment of trichotillomania:

    A case study, Behav. Res. Ther. 16, 217-220.

    Ferster C. B.. Nurnberger J. E. and Levitt E. B. (1962)

    The control of eating, J.

    Math.

    1 87-100.

    Greenberg H. R. and Sarner C. A. (1965) Trichotillomania:

    Symptoms and syndrome,

    Ar chs Gen. Psychiat. 12,

    482-489.

    Horne D. J. (1977) Behaviour theraov for trichotiliomania.

    Behuv. Rei. Thtb. I S, 192-196.

    .

    Levine B. A. (1976) Treatment of trichotillomania bv covert

    sensitization,

    J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiat. 7; 75-76.

    Mahoney M. J. (1974)

    Cogniti on and Behaviour Modifica-

    tion,

    Ballinger, Cambridge, Massachusetts.

    McLaughlin J. G. and Nay W. R. (1975) Treatment of

    trichotillomania using positive coverants and response

    cost: A case report,

    Behuv. Ther. 6, 87-91.

    Monroe J. T. and Abse D. W. (1963) The psychopathology

    of trichotillomania and trichophagy,

    Psychiatry 26,

    95-103.

    of exposure or disguise penetration as a result

    Philippopoulos G. S. (1961) A case of trichotillomania

    of failure to achieve their weekly goal. This

    (hair pulling),

    Aclu Psychorher. Psychosom. Orthopued.

    aversive consequence, carefully identified prior

    (Basef) 9,30 3 12.

    to treatment, was in fact only used with Client B

    Stabler B. and Warren A. B. (1974) Behavioural contracting

    in treating trichotillomania: Case note,

    Psycho/. Rep. 34,

    and its employment resulted in a dramatic

    4OL402.

    reduction in hair pulling that was maintained

    Seager C. P.

    (1970)

    Treatment of compulsive gamblers

    by

    electrical aversion,

    Er . J. Psychiut. 117. 545-553.

    through treatment. During scalp examination,

    Taylor J. A. (1963) A behavioural interpretation

    the observer was informed that the condition

    obsessive-compulsive neurosis,

    Behuv. Rex Ther.

    was not a medical one and on occasion the client

    237-244.

    stated that she pulled out her hair. Although

    of

    1.