by guest on may 3, 2016 ......schizotypal traits and schizotypal personality disorder (review in...

12
Neurocognitive Correlates of Schizotypy in First Degree Relatives of Schizophrenia Patients by Meinte Q. Vollema and Belinda Postma Abstract We examined neurocognitive correlates of three dimensions of schizotypy in 63 healthy first degree relatives of schizophrenia patients. Neurocognitive measures of attention, verbal memory, and prefrontal functioning were combined with self-report and interview measures of schizotypy. State-psy- chopathology (anxiety and depression) was a strong predictor for positive schizotypy (PS) and negative schizotypy (NS). PS was slightly correlated to verbal long-term memory, therefore weakly supporting the hypothesis that temporal-limbic malfunctioning underlies PS. NS was not correlated to any prefrontal measure, and therefore no evidence was found for the hypothesis that prefrontal malfunctioning underlies NS. Disorganization schizotypy (DS) was strongly correlated to the false alarm variable of the Continuous Performance Test (CPT), probably sup- porting the hypothesis of orbitofrontal malfunction- ing underlying DS. This correlational pattern of DS echoes closely two schizophrenia studies reporting a relationship between formal thought disorder and the false alarm CPT variable. This similarity, across schizophrenia and relatives samples, may be consid- ered as evidence that false alarms on the CPT and (subtle) problems in goal directedness of thinking are indicators of a genetically determined vulnerability to schizophrenia. Keywords: Schizotypy, multidimensionality, neu- rocognition, first degree relatives. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 28(3):367-377,2002. Schizophrenia and schizotypy are considered manifesta- tions of the same underlying, mainly genetically transmit- ted, biological vulnerability (Meehl 1990). If Meehl is right, it follows that schizophrenia patients and schizo- typal subjects share similar neurobiological and cognitive abnormalities, which may differ in severity. Empirical evidence suggests that schizotypy is a mul- tidimensional construct (Raine et al. 1994; Venables 1995; Vollema and van den Bosch 1995). Factor analytical stud- ies provide evidence for dimensions of PS (e.g., magical ideation, perceptual aberrations), DS (e.g., odd speech, odd behavior), and NS (e.g., social anxiety, no close friends). Empirical evidence also suggests that schizophre- nia itself is a multidimensional construct (review by Ven- ables 1995). It is composed of a positive symptom dimen- sion (hallucinations, delusions), a disorganization dimension (formal thought disorder, bizarre behavior), and a negative symptom dimension (blunted affect, poverty of speech). This similarity between the dimensions of schizo- typy and the dimensions of schizophrenia is striking. It may suggest that schizotypal traits and schizophrenia symptoms reflect different points on the same continuum. It may also suggest an underlying tridimensionality of eti- ology (Venables 1995). If these hypotheses are right, it would mean that there may be different pathogenetic determinants to the three dimensions of schizotypy and schizophrenia (i.e., positive, negative, and disorganiza- tion). For instance, both positive schizophrenia and posi- tive schizotypy may be caused by temporal-limbic dys- function. For 20 years, studies have been conducted into the neurocognitive correlates of schizotypy for two reasons: (1) to investigate the construct validity of schizotypy mea- sures (Lenzenweger and Moldin 1990), and (2) to explore the neurocognitive processes that might be related to dif- ferent dimensions of schizotypy. These studies may con- tribute to a better understanding of specific pathogenetic factors that underlie different dimensions of schizotypy and probably schizophrenia. In a meta-analytical review of the literature on the relationship between schizotypal dimensions and neu- rocognition (including 33 studies), we found that in nor- Send reprint requests to Dr. M.G. Vollema, Meerkanten GGZ Flevo- Veluwe, Veldwijk Psychiatric Hospital, P.O. Box 1000, 3850 BA, Ermelo, The Netherlands; e-mail: [email protected]. 367 by guest on May 3, 2016 http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/ Downloaded from

Upload: others

Post on 18-Jul-2020

4 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: by guest on May 3, 2016 ......schizotypal traits and schizotypal personality disorder (review in Benishay and Lencz 1995). However, it remains to be seen whether questionnaires and

Neurocognitive Correlates of Schizotypyin First Degree Relatives of

Schizophrenia Patients

by Meinte Q. Vollema and Belinda Postma

Abstract

We examined neurocognitive correlates of threedimensions of schizotypy in 63 healthy first degreerelatives of schizophrenia patients. Neurocognitivemeasures of attention, verbal memory, and prefrontalfunctioning were combined with self-report andinterview measures of schizotypy. State-psy-chopathology (anxiety and depression) was a strongpredictor for positive schizotypy (PS) and negativeschizotypy (NS). PS was slightly correlated to verballong-term memory, therefore weakly supporting thehypothesis that temporal-limbic malfunctioningunderlies PS. NS was not correlated to any prefrontalmeasure, and therefore no evidence was found for thehypothesis that prefrontal malfunctioning underliesNS. Disorganization schizotypy (DS) was stronglycorrelated to the false alarm variable of theContinuous Performance Test (CPT), probably sup-porting the hypothesis of orbitofrontal malfunction-ing underlying DS. This correlational pattern of DSechoes closely two schizophrenia studies reporting arelationship between formal thought disorder and thefalse alarm CPT variable. This similarity, acrossschizophrenia and relatives samples, may be consid-ered as evidence that false alarms on the CPT and(subtle) problems in goal directedness of thinking areindicators of a genetically determined vulnerabilityto schizophrenia.

Keywords: Schizotypy, multidimensionality, neu-rocognition, first degree relatives.

Schizophrenia Bulletin, 28(3):367-377,2002.

Schizophrenia and schizotypy are considered manifesta-tions of the same underlying, mainly genetically transmit-ted, biological vulnerability (Meehl 1990). If Meehl isright, it follows that schizophrenia patients and schizo-typal subjects share similar neurobiological and cognitiveabnormalities, which may differ in severity.

Empirical evidence suggests that schizotypy is a mul-tidimensional construct (Raine et al. 1994; Venables 1995;Vollema and van den Bosch 1995). Factor analytical stud-ies provide evidence for dimensions of PS (e.g., magicalideation, perceptual aberrations), DS (e.g., odd speech,odd behavior), and NS (e.g., social anxiety, no closefriends). Empirical evidence also suggests that schizophre-nia itself is a multidimensional construct (review by Ven-ables 1995). It is composed of a positive symptom dimen-sion (hallucinations, delusions), a disorganizationdimension (formal thought disorder, bizarre behavior), anda negative symptom dimension (blunted affect, poverty ofspeech). This similarity between the dimensions of schizo-typy and the dimensions of schizophrenia is striking. Itmay suggest that schizotypal traits and schizophreniasymptoms reflect different points on the same continuum.It may also suggest an underlying tridimensionality of eti-ology (Venables 1995). If these hypotheses are right, itwould mean that there may be different pathogeneticdeterminants to the three dimensions of schizotypy andschizophrenia (i.e., positive, negative, and disorganiza-tion). For instance, both positive schizophrenia and posi-tive schizotypy may be caused by temporal-limbic dys-function.

For 20 years, studies have been conducted into theneurocognitive correlates of schizotypy for two reasons:(1) to investigate the construct validity of schizotypy mea-sures (Lenzenweger and Moldin 1990), and (2) to explorethe neurocognitive processes that might be related to dif-ferent dimensions of schizotypy. These studies may con-tribute to a better understanding of specific pathogeneticfactors that underlie different dimensions of schizotypyand probably schizophrenia.

In a meta-analytical review of the literature on therelationship between schizotypal dimensions and neu-rocognition (including 33 studies), we found that in nor-

Send reprint requests to Dr. M.G. Vollema, Meerkanten GGZ Flevo-Veluwe, Veldwijk Psychiatric Hospital, P.O. Box 1000, 3850 BA,Ermelo, The Netherlands; e-mail: [email protected].

367

by guest on May 3, 2016

http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/D

ownloaded from

Page 2: by guest on May 3, 2016 ......schizotypal traits and schizotypal personality disorder (review in Benishay and Lencz 1995). However, it remains to be seen whether questionnaires and

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 28, No. 3, 2002 M.G. Vollema and B. Postma

mal subjects PS is more strongly associated with devian-cies in smooth pursuit eye movements and continuous per-formance tasks than DS and NS are. We also found thatNS is more strongly associated with abnormal perform-ance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) than PSand DS are (Vollema et al., submitted for publication).

A neurodevelopmental model of neurocircuitry mal-function underlying schizotypy and schizophrenia is pro-vided by Walker and Gale (1995). Two malfunctioningneural circuits are held responsible for the development ofschizotypal traits. First, malfunction of the temporal-lim-bic circuit may contribute to the development of PS traits.Evidence for this hypothesis is provided by Flaum andAndreasen (1995), who report a negative correlationbetween hippocampal volume and PS. Hemsley (1992)argued that an inability to make use of stored information(mediated by the hippocampus) of past regularities couldgive rise to positive symptoms. If temporal-limbic mal-function underlies PS, one would expect correlations withthe retrieval component of verbal long-term memory. Inour review, we did not report on temporal-limbic dysfunc-tion, because few studies were conducted into the relation-ship to schizotypy.

Second, Walker and Gale (1995) argued that malfunc-tion of the prefrontal circuit contributes to the develop-ment of NS traits. Evidence that NS is related to prefrontaldysfunction is provided by Raine et al. (1992), who reportstrong correlations between NS and WCST variables.These findings suggest that prefrontal (or executive) dys-function, including mental inflexibility and poor planning,is associated with, and probably may give rise to, thesocial-interpersonal deficits of NS. If Walker and Gale areright, correlations may be found between NS and neu-rocognitive measures of prefrontal functioning.

Walker and Gale did not hypothesize about neural cir-cuits underlying DS. It was argued by Frith et al. (1991)and Allen et al. (1993) that formal thought disorder inschizophrenia is related to the production of wrongresponses during a sustained attention task and to the pro-duction of incorrect words during a verbal fluency task.Problems in suppressing incorrect responses seem to beassociated with formal thought disorder in schizophrenia,which made Liddle and Morris (1991) suggest thatorbitofrontal dysfunction may underlie formal thought dis-order. If this holds for DS as well, it would be expectedthat DS is associated with cognitive disinhibition (indi-cated by premature and incorrect responses during atten-tion and memory tasks).

Schizotypy can be assessed with questionnaires andwith structured interviews. Questionnaires are mainly usedfor screening purposes, because they take less time toadminister (review in Vollema and van den Bosch 1995).Structured interviews are more time-consuming and there-

fore are mainly used as a second line for classification ofschizotypal traits and schizotypal personality disorder(review in Benishay and Lencz 1995). However, itremains to be seen whether questionnaires and interviews(with identical items) for schizotypy are equivalent andassess schizotypy similarly. Kendler et al. (1996) arguedthat questionnaires are not well suited to measuring allschizotypal signs (e.g., goal directedness of thinking),which can be rated reliably by only a well-trained inter-viewer. In a confirmatory factor analysis, using a schizo-typy interview and a schizotypy questionnaire (see Instru-ments portion of Method section), we predicted that fivefactors would describe the data the best: three trait factors(PS, NS, and DS) and two method factors (interview andquestionnaire). Questionnaire and interview PS and NS(respectively) loaded onto the same trait and method fac-tors, suggesting that they are highly equivalent. Self-reportand interview measures for PS assess PS similarly. Self-report and interview measures for NS assess NS similarly.However, questionnaire and interview DS loaded onto dif-ferent trait factors, suggesting that they are not equivalent,both measuring very different aspects of schizotypy(Vollema and Ormel, submitted for publication). A highlevel of trait variation indicates that the trait of interest(e.g., positive schizotypy) is a main determinant of the testscore. A high level of method variation indicates that theformat of the test is a main determinant of the test score.

The aim of this study was to test some hypotheses andto further explore the neurocognitive correlates of dimen-sions of schizotypy assessed by questionnaire and inter-view. We hypothesized that (1) PS would correlate nega-tively with the retrieval component of verbal memory, (2)DS would correlate positively with cognitive disinhibition,and (3) NS would correlate positively with perseverativeerrors, as part of executive functioning.

Method

Subjects. First degree relatives of schizophrenia patientshave a 5 to 15 percent chance of developing schizophreniaand therefore are considered a biological high-risk group(Gottesman 1991). First degree relatives were selected forthis study because more variation in scores on measures ofschizotypy (questionnaire, interview, and neurocognitivetests) was expected to occur than in a general populationsample or a college student sample known by their restrictedrange of neurocognitive abilities (Lencz et al. 1995).

The subjects were 63 (28 males and 35 females)healthy first degree relatives of schizophrenia patients.The schizophrenia patients, classified according toDSM-IV (APA 1994), were admitted to our hospital, andall of their first degree relatives were asked to participate.

368

by guest on May 3, 2016

http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/D

ownloaded from

Page 3: by guest on May 3, 2016 ......schizotypal traits and schizotypal personality disorder (review in Benishay and Lencz 1995). However, it remains to be seen whether questionnaires and

Neurocognitive Correlates of Schizotypy Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 28, No. 3, 2002

Subjects were recruited in two phases. First, we askedthe Family Board of our hospital to assist in recruiting thefirst degree relatives. Second, by letter we asked all fami-lies of schizophrenia patients in our hospital to participate.Letters (including three application sheets) were sent to140 families, and we received 139 in return: 85 relativesagreed to participate and 54 relatives refused. Sixty-threerelatives fulfilled the inclusion criteria and participated.

Exclusion criteria were as follows: age over 65 years,IQ lower than 70, organic cerebral or neurological disor-der, and chronic alcohol or drug abuse. The mean age ofthe relatives was 46 years (range 17-64 years), their meanIQ was 102 (standard deviation 14), the mode of educa-tional level was secondary (18 subjects), most subjects(45) were married, and subjects did not use psychiatricmedication.

We cannot be sure whether this is a representativesample of first degree relatives of schizophrenia patients.Some relatives remarked that the relative most similar tothe patient refused to participate. This was in line withexpectations put forward by Lenzenweger (1993), whoargued that the most severe schizotypal subjects are tooavoidant or too anxious to participate in research. Thosewho participated seemed surprisingly cooperative andcompliant.

InstrumentsSchizotypy. The Schizotypal Personality Question-

naire (SPQ; Raine 1991) is a 74-item self-report question-naire with a dichotomous response format (yes/no). TheSPQ is developed to measure all nine DSM-IH-R criteriafor schizotypal personality disorder (referential thinking,social anxiety, magical ideation, unusual perceptual expe-riences, odd behavior, no close friends, odd speech, con-stricted affect, suspiciousness). The SPQ is used as ascreening instrument in the general population for identifi-cation of individuals with schizotypal traits. It can alsoserve as a measure of individual differences in schizotypalpersonality. Recent factor analytical studies with the SPQsuggest that the nine original subscales can be reduced tothree schizotypal dimensions (Raine et al. 1994; Vollemaand Hoijtink 2000). The first dimension is called PS andcontains referential thinking, delusional mood, magicalideation, unusual perceptual experiences, and suspicious-ness. The second dimension is called DS and contains oddspeech and odd behavior. The last dimension is called NSand consists of constricted affect, no close friends, socialanxiety, referential thinking, and suspiciousness.

In the current study, summed scores for three dimensions(further referred to as Questionnaire Positive Schizotypy[QPS], Questionnaire Disorganization Schizotypy [QDS],and Questionnaire Negative Schizotypy [QNS]) will be usedin the analyses instead of scores on the nine subscales.

The Structured Interview for Schizotypy-Revised(SIS-R; Kendler et al. 1989; Vollema and Ormel 2000) isa semistructured interview for assessing schizotypal symp-toms and signs. The SIS-R contains 20 schizotypal symp-toms and 11 schizotypal signs, which are rated on a four-point scale. Symptoms are defined as verbal responses tostandardized questions concerning, for instance, magicalideation, illusions, and referential thinking. Signs refer tobehaviors that are rated by the interviewer, for instance,goal directedness of thinking and flatness of affect. Ques-tions and rating procedures are standardized. Manyschizotypal symptoms and some signs can be assessedreliably using the SIS-R (Vollema and Ormel 2000).

Item scores were reduced to three dimensional scores,according to the three dimensionality of schizotypy (asdiscussed earlier in this section). The first SIS-R dimen-sion concerns PS and contains symptoms of referentialthinking, delusional mood, magical ideation, illusions, andsuspiciousness. The second SIS-R dimension concernsDS and contains signs of goal directedness of thinking,loosening of associations, and oddness. The last SIS-Rdimension concerns NS and contains symptoms of socialisolation, social anxiety, introversion, restricted affect,paranoid ideation, and referential thinking. The variablesare further referred to as Interview Positive Schizotypy(IPS), Interview Disorganization Schizotypy (IDS), andInterview Negative Schizotypy (INS).

Dimensions of PS and NS. We also calculated twodimensional scores for each subject, which were derivedfrom a confirmatory factor analytical study with the samesample. Two (latent) dimensions of schizotypy, using theSPQ and the SIS-R, best described (the structure of) thedata. The PS dimension score is calculated as follows: PS= 0.76QPS + O.55QDS + 0.76IPS. The NS dimensionscore is calculated as follows: NS = 0.84QNS + 0.84INS(parameter values are factor loadings from the factor ana-lytical study). Questionnaire and interview DS were notcorrelated, and therefore DS did not appear as a separatedimension from this factor analytical study. QDS loadedon PS, and therefore we selected IDS as the indicator ofDS.

Psychopathology. Two scales of the SymptomsChecklist-90 (SCL-90; Derogatis et al. 1973) wereused—anxiety and depression. These subscales provideindications for state-related anxiety and depression. It wasargued by Meehl (1990) that schizotypal subjects may suf-fer from high levels of anxiety and depression. Thesesymptoms may influence the performance of schizotypeson neurocognitive tests. There are three reasons the rela-tives in this study might have suffered from high levels ofanxiety and depression. First, they might have been anx-ious and depressed as a result of being a family member ofa schizophrenia patient. Second, they might have devel-

369

by guest on May 3, 2016

http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/D

ownloaded from

Page 4: by guest on May 3, 2016 ......schizotypal traits and schizotypal personality disorder (review in Benishay and Lencz 1995). However, it remains to be seen whether questionnaires and

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 28, No. 3, 2002 M.G. Vollema and B. Postma

oped anxiety and depression in reaction to schizotypalsymptoms. Finally, they might have been anxious becausethey were being assessed. For these reasons, we includedstate-anxiety and state-depression as variables to be con-trolled for.

Neurocognitive Tests. The National Adult Reading Test(NART; Dutch version NLV; Schmand et al. 1992) wasused to assess the global level of intellectual functioning.It served as a tool to assess whether subjects fulfilled theinclusion criterion of IQ higher than 70. In addition, IQwas used as a variable to be controlled for in the correla-tional analyses.

Tests sensitive to frontal lobe performance. We usethe terms tests sensitive to frontal lobe performance andtests sensitive to temporal-limbic performance to stayclose to the neurodevelopmental model of Walker andGale. We are aware that performance on the selected neu-rocognitive tests (see below) is not restricted to malfunc-tion of one cerebral area but has more determinants. It isnot our aim to explore neuroanatomical substrates ofdimensions of schizotypy but to investigate associatedneurocognitive mechanisms.

CPT. The 3-7 version of the CPT (Nuechterlein 1991), atest of sustained attention, was used. It is a relatively easy ver-sion of the CPT without high perceptual or information pro-cessing loads, but it requires working memory activity. CPTvariables used were false alarm rate (CPTF), prematureresponses (CPTP), and d' (CPTD). False alarm rate refers tothe percentage of nontargets that are incorrectly responded toas targets. We used a restricted false alarm variable as well todistinguish between general false alarms and false alarmsresulting from impulsive reactions. The premature responsesvariable refers to those false alarms occurring when subjectsreacted immediately after a 3 was presented. Finally, the gen-eral CPT variable of d' (an indicator of perceptual sensitivity)was used, referring to the degree to which subjects discrimi-nate between target combinations (3-7) and nontargets.

Consistent evidence suggests that there is deviantCPT performance by schizophrenia patients, their firstdegree relatives, and schizotypal subjects (for a review,see Nuechterlein 1991). CPT performance in schizophre-nia spectrum subjects is stable; therefore, the CPT quali-fies as a vulnerability indicator. We wanted to examinewhether CPTD is related to PS (according to the reviewfindings) or to NS (according to the Walker and Gale pre-frontal hypothesis and the assumption that d' has strongprefrontal determinants, according to Buchsbaum 1990)and whether false alarms and premature reactions arerelated to DS according to the (dis)inhibition hypothesis(Frith etal. 1991).

Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. The Wisconsin CardSorting Test (WCST; Heaton 1981) is sensitive to frontallobe functioning (McPherson and Cummings 1998) and

was administered in paper-and-pencil form according tothe standard instructions described by Heaton. The WCSTmeasures so-called executive functions like conceptualability, problem solving, and mental flexibility.

WCST variables used were number of categoriescompleted (WCC; indicates conceptual ability) and num-ber of perseverative errors (WCP; indicates mental flexi-bility). WCST deviancies are found throughout the schizo-phrenia spectrum (van der Does and van den Bosch 1992).In schizotypy studies, WCST deviancies of normal sub-jects are related to all three dimensions, but the strongestassociation is with NS (Vollema et al., submitted for publi-cation). According to the Walker and Gale prefrontalhypothesis, NS should be related to the WCST, and weinvestigated this. We also explored whether DS is relatedto number of perseverative errors, which may represent afailure to inhibit a learned response, according to the(dis)inhibition hypothesis of Frith et al. (1991).

Verbal Fluency. Verbal Fluency (Borkowski et al.1967) is a letter fluency test (using N, A, and M) for repro-duction of words from semantic memory. According toLezak (1983), letter fluency tests qualify as prefrontal testsand are more difficult and more sensitive to problems instrategy-guided search processes in semantic memory thancategory fluency tests are. Variables used were total num-ber of correct words (VFC; indicates ability to retrieve andgenerate words) and total number of incorrect words (VFI;indicates inability to inhibit incorrectly retrieved words).

Liddle and Morris (1991) and Allen et al. (1993)found that schizophrenia patients with negative symptomsproduced few words and those with formal thought disor-der produced many incorrect words. The associations ofVF with NS (according to the prefrontal hypothesis ofWalker and Gale) and DS (according to the [dis] inhibitionhypothesis of Frith et al.) will be investigated.

Temporal-Limbic Test, California Verbal LearningTest. The Verbal Learning and Memory Test (VLGT;Dutch version of the California Verbal Learning Test;Mulder et al. 1996) is a verbal memory test, which is usedin a shortened version. Subjects were presented the list of16 words once. After 30 minutes they were asked whichwords they still knew. Variables used were total numberof correct words retrieved (VLMC; indicates ability toretrieve information from verbal long-term memory) andtotal number of incorrect words retrieved (VLMI; indi-cates incorrectly retrieved information from verbal long-term memory, probably because of disinhibition).

Sass et al. (1992) found an association betweenretrieval from verbal long-term memory and loss of neu-rons in the hippocampus. The associations of the VLGTwith PS (according to the temporal-limbic hypothesis ofWalker and Gale) and with DS (according to the (dis)inhi-bition hypothesis of Frith et al.) were investigated.

370

by guest on May 3, 2016

http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/D

ownloaded from

Page 5: by guest on May 3, 2016 ......schizotypal traits and schizotypal personality disorder (review in Benishay and Lencz 1995). However, it remains to be seen whether questionnaires and

Neurocognitive Correlates of Schizotypy Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 28, No. 3, 2002

ResultsCorrelational Analyses. We started with the usual corre-lational analyses (Pearson; 2-tailed), which showed manyassociations between schizotypy, neurocognition, anddemographic variables. For instance, age and IQ corre-lated significantly with CPTD (r = -0.28 and 0.50, respec-tively) and with IDS (r = 0.32 and r = -0.29, respectively).Stratta et al. (1997) showed that age and education (andtherefore IQ) also have strong effects on WCST perform-ance. Therefore, partial correlational analyses were exe-cuted controlling for the effects of age, IQ, state-anxietyand state-depression, because these variables might influ-ence neurocognitive performance.

Table 1 shows partial correlations (Pearson; 1-tailedbecause we knew in advance the directions of the correla-tions) between dimensions of schizotypy and neurocogni-tive measures. Seventy-two correlations were calculated,and ten were significant at the p < 0.05 level (including thecorrelation of PS with VLMC at p - 0.05) and four at thep < 0.01 level.

The highest correlations were found between disorga-nization and CPT. For QDS we found a significant correla-tion with CPTP (r = 0.33, p = 0.006). IDS was signifi-cantly correlated with CPTF (r = 0.59, p = 0.00), CPTD(r = -0.30, p = 0.011), and CPTP (r = 0.35, p = 0.004). IPSand INS were not correlated with any of the neurocogni-tive measures.

Multiple Regression Analyses. To investigate whetherschizotypy scores could be predicted by performance onneurocognitive tests, multiple regression analyses wereexecuted.

Variables used in the analyses (except the nominalvariable of gender) were standardized to z scores in orderto use standard variances in the regression analyses. Forall schizotypal measures (QPS, QDS, QNS, IPS, IDS,INS, PS, and NS), multiple regression analyses were exe-cuted in two steps. First, the variables of gender, age, IQ,state-anxiety, and state-depression were used as indepen-dent variables (all model Is). Thereafter, the neurocogni-tive variables CPTF, CPTD, CPTP, WCP, WCC, VFC,VFI, VLMC, and VLMI were added in the analyses foreach measure of schizotypy separately (all model 2s).

For all regression analyses, values of multiple r(absolute value of correlation coefficient between depen-dent and independent variables), r2 (proportion of varianceexplained by the model), r2 adjusted (r2 for this sample,corrected for population parameters), F (test that all coeffi-cients are 0 and that no linear relationship exists betweendependent and independent variables), p (significancelevel for F), and beta (partial regression coefficient) areshown in table 2. Betas are provided only when their prob-ability level is p < 0.05 and when t values are lower than-2 or higher than 2 (Norusis 1995).

Many variables were included in the model 2 analy-ses. In particular, the neurocognitive variables might inter-

Table 1. Means on neurocognitive tests and partial correlations between dimensional measures ofschizotypy and neurocognitive variables in first degree relatives (n = 63; corrected for age, IQ, state-depression, and state-anxiety)

Schizotypy Measures

Neurocognitive Variables Mean QPS QDS QNS IPS IDS INS PS NS

CPTF

CPTD

CPTP

WCP

WCC

VFC

VFI

VLMC

VLMI

0.98

3.64

1.71

21.78

4.37

44.21

2.73

4.86

0.87

0.33**

-0.23*

0.59**

-0.30*

0.35**

-0.29* -0.30* -0.22* -0.23*

0.32**

Note.—CPT = Continuous Performance Test; CPTD = d' (CPT); CPTF = false alarm rate (CPT); CPTP = premature responses (CPT); IDS= Interview Disorganisation Schizotypy (SIS-R); INS = Interview Negative Schizotypy (SIS-R); IPS = Interview Positive Schizotypy(SIS-R); NS = negative schizotypy, dimension score (SPQ and SIS-R); PS = positive schizotypy, dimension score (SPQ and SIS-R); QDS= Questionnaire Disorganisation Schizotypy (SPQ); QNS = Questionnaire Negative Schizotypy (SPQ); QPS = Questionnaire PositiveSchizotypy (SPQ); SIS-R = Structured Interview for Schizotypy-Revised; SPQ = Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire; VF = Verbal Flu-ency; VFC = total number of correct words (VF); VFI = total number of incorrect words (VF); VLGT = Verbal Long-Term Memory Test; VLMC= number of correct words retrieved (VLGT); VLMI = number of incorrect words retrieved (VLGT); WCC = number of categories completed(WCST); WCP = number of perseverative errors (WCST); WCST = Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.

•p < 0.05 (1 -tailed); " p < 0.01 (1 -tailed)

371

by guest on May 3, 2016

http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/D

ownloaded from

Page 6: by guest on May 3, 2016 ......schizotypal traits and schizotypal personality disorder (review in Benishay and Lencz 1995). However, it remains to be seen whether questionnaires and

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 28, No. 3, 2002 M.G. Vollema and B. Postma

correlate; therefore, we tested for collinearity. Testing forcollinearity did not reveal condition indexes for indepen-dent variables higher than 15, indicating that the indepen-dent variables in this study did not intercorrelate signifi-cantly.

In predicting QPS, the multiple r2 of 0.22 for model1 is significant at the p < 0.05 level but rather low. Ascan be seen in table 2, only depression (beta = 0.33) con-tributed significantly to the prediction of QPS. Adding

the neurocognitive variables (model 2) resulted in ahigher multiple r2of 0.37, but the F statistic wasinsignificant. However, betas were significant fordepression (beta = 0.43) and verbal long-term memory(beta = -0.34). The addition of neurocognitive variablesto demographic and state-psychopathology variables didnot improve the prediction of QPS. With respect to thesignificant beta for verbal long-term memory and thesignificant partial correlation at the p < 0.05 level (table

Table 2. Multiple regressions predicting scores on dimensional measures of schizotypy

QPS

Model 1Model 2

QDS

Model 1Model 2

QNS

Model 1Model 2

IPS

Model 1Model 2

IDS

Model 1

Model 2

INS

Model 1Model 2

PS

Model 1Model 2

NS

Model 1

Model 2

r

0.4690.611

0.5000.640

0.6100.700

0.3700.460

0.490

0.760

0.4500.510

0.5200.590

0.580

0.650

0.220.37

0.250.41

0.380.50

0.140.22

0.24

0.58

0.210.26

0.270.35

0.34

0.42

r adjusted

0.150.1

0.180.22

0.320.33

0.06-0.02

0.17

0.44

0.130.04

0.200.15

0.28

0.25

F

3.091.80

3.702.10

6.702.96

1.790.9

3.44

4.17

2.801.17

4.071.74

5.71

2.41

P

0.0160.067

0.0060.027

0.0000.003

0.130.57

0.009

0.000

0.0240.333

0.0030.080

0.000

0.013

Beta*

0.33 (SCL-90 depression)0.43 (SCL-90 depression)

-0.34 (VLMC)

0.30 (SCL-90 anxiety)0.28 (gender)0.32 (SCL-90 anxiety)0.33 (CPTP)

0.43 (SCL-90 depression)0.46 (SCL-90 depression)

-0.30 (VLMC)

——

0.24 (age)-0.29 (IQ)

0.56 (CPTF)

0.43 (SCL-90 depression)0.38 (SCL-90 depression)

0.33 (SCL-90 depression)0.37 (SCL-90 depression)

0.46 (SCL-90 depression)-0.23 (IQ)

0.44 (SCL-90 depression)

Note.—CPT = Continuous Performance Test; CPTF = false alarm rate (CPT); CPTP = premature responses (CPT); IDS = Interview Disor-ganisation Schizotypy (SIS-R); INS = Interview Negative Schizotypy (SIS-R); IPS = Interview Positive Schizotypy (SIS-R); NS = negativeschizotypy, dimension score (SPQ and SIS-R); PS = positive schizotypy, dimension score (SPQ and SIS-R); QDS = Questionnaire Disor-ganisation Schizotypy (SPQ); QNS = Questionnaire Negative Schizotypy (SPQ); QPS = Questionnaire Positive Schizotypy (SPQ);SCL-90 = Symptom Checklist-90; SIS-R = Structured Interview for Schizotypy-Revised; SPQ = Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire;VLMC = number of correct words retrieved (Verbal Long-Term Memory Test). Model 1s: One measure of schizotypy as dependent variableand as independent variables gender, age, IQ, state-depression, and state-anxiety. Model 2s: One measure of schizotypy as dependentvariable and as independent variables those from model 1 added with nine neurocognitive measures.

* Betas are provided only when f > 2 or / < -2 and their p < 0.05.

372

by guest on May 3, 2016

http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/D

ownloaded from

Page 7: by guest on May 3, 2016 ......schizotypal traits and schizotypal personality disorder (review in Benishay and Lencz 1995). However, it remains to be seen whether questionnaires and

Neurocognitive Correlates of Schizotypy Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 28, No. 3, 2002

1), we argue that a modest association exists betweenthis variable and QPS.

In predicting QDS, the multiple r2 of 0.25 for model 1was rather low but highly significant at the p < 0.01 level.Anxiety contributed significantly to this prediction (beta =0.30). Adding the neurocognitive variables resulted in asignificant (p < 0.05) and higher multiple r2 of 0.41. Sig-nificant contributors in this second analysis of QDS weregender (beta = 0.28), anxiety (beta = 0.32), and CPTP(beta = 0.33).

In predicting QNS, the multiple r2 of 0.38 for model 1is highly significant at the p < 0.01 level and represents amodest correlation. Table 2 shows that depression (beta =0.43) contributed significantly to the prediction of QNS.Adding the neurocognitive variables resulted in a signifi-cant and higher multiple r2 of 0.50: this addition improvedthe prediction. It was caused by verbal long-term memory(beta = -0.30).

In predicting IPS, multiple r^s of both model 1 andmodel 2 were insignificant. Demographic, state-psy-chopathology, and neurocognitive variables seemed to beunrelated to IPS.

In predicting IDS, the multiple r2 of 0.24 for model 1is low but significant at the/? < 0.01 level. The majority ofthe variance of IDS was contributed by age (beta = 0.24)and IQ (beta = -0.29). Adding the neurocognitive vari-ables resulted in a highly significant (p < 0.01) and ratherhigh multiple r2 of 0.58. The majority of the variance ofIDS in this second analysis was contributed by CPTF (beta= 0.56).

In predicting INS, the multiple r2 of 0.21 for model 1was rather low but significant (p < 0.05). The majority ofthe variance of INS was contributed by depression (beta =0.43). The addition of the neurocognitive variablesresulted in a significant (p = 0.033) and slightly highermultiple r2 of 0.26. But the majority of the variability ofINS was contributed by depression.

In predicting PS, the multiple r2 of 0.27 for model 1was rather low but significant (p < 0.05). The majority ofvariance of PS was contributed by depression (beta =0.33). The addition of the neurocognitive variables tomodel 1 resulted in nonsignificant r values (p = 0.080).

In predicting NS, the multiple r2 of 0.34 for model 1was highly significant (p < 0.01). The majority of varianceof NS was contributed by depression (beta = 0.46) and IQ(beta = -0.23). The addition of the neurocognitive vari-ables resulted in a higher r2. The majority of variance wascontributed by depression (beta = 0.44).

DiscussionThe aim of this study was to explore some neurocognitivecorrelates of dimensions of schizotypy in first degree rela-

tives of schizophrenia patients. Three general conclusionscan be drawn. First, for DS we found correlations inexpected directions (for PS to a much lesser extent, and forNS we found an unexpected association). Second, the pat-tern of correlations differed from the pattern found inschizotypy studies with normal subjects. Third, state-psy-chopathology is significantly correlated to all three dimen-sions of schizotypy. We will discuss the general conclu-sions, then the limitations of this study and theimplications of the findings.

Dimensions of Schizotypy and Their NeurocognitiveMechanisms. PS was hardly related to any neurocogni-tive measure in this study. It was only slightly related toverbal long-term memory. Self-report PS tends to go withpoorer retrieval from verbal long-term memory. It con-verges with a report by Flaum and Andreasen (1995)reporting a reduced hippocampal volume in subjects withhigh PS. Our findings only weakly support the temporal-limbic hypothesis of PS postulated by Walker and Gale.This hypothesis links PS, verbal retrieval deficits, and thehippocampus. It may agree with the hypothesis ofHemsley (1992), who argued that hippocampal dysfunc-tion, psychotic tendencies, and an obstructed access topast regularities about common-sense knowledge arerelated to each other. Our findings suggest that the ten-dency to develop psychotic-like experiences seems onlyweakly related to problems in retrieving information fromverbal long-term memory. However, verbal long-termmemory was related to (self-report) NS as well.Therefore, the (weak) association with PS is not specific.This relationship is mainly restricted to paranoid ideation,which loads on PS and NS as well, and may suggest thatduring the formation and maintenance of paranoid ideassubjects do not have sufficient access to stored common-sense knowledge in verbal long-term memory. Therefore,their ideas cannot be consensually validated by this inac-cessible information.

NS too was hardly related to any neurocognitive mea-sure. We found only a modest relation with verbal long-term memory. Higher scores on self-report NS tend to gowith poorer retrieval from verbal long-term memory. Thisis mainly due to the correlation between paranoid ideation(loading on PS and NS as well) and verbal long-termmemory (r = -0.29, p < 0.05).

NS was not related to any test sensitive to frontal lobeperformance. Therefore, we found no evidence for the pre-frontal hypothesis of NS. In line with our findings are earlierreports about first degree relatives by Keefe et al. (1994) andStratta et al. (1997). They reported normal WCST perform-ance of first degree relatives. Stratta et al. (1997) argued thatthe WCST is related to the disease process of schizophreniaonly because of its close connection to negative symptoms.

373

by guest on May 3, 2016

http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/D

ownloaded from

Page 8: by guest on May 3, 2016 ......schizotypal traits and schizotypal personality disorder (review in Benishay and Lencz 1995). However, it remains to be seen whether questionnaires and

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 28, No. 3, 2002 M.G. Vollema and B. Postma

Our findings support the hypothesis of Stratta et al. (1997)that the WCST may not be a good trait marker of vulnerabil-ity to schizophrenia. Executive dysfunction, including men-tal inflexibility and poor planning, does not seem to underliethe social-interpersonal deficits of NS. Our findings suggestthat variables from other domains may turn out to bestronger determinants of social withdrawal and social anxi-ety and the like. One might think of personality factors likecoping and motivation.

DS had the highest correlations with neurocognitivemeasures. It was strongly related to cognitive disinhibitionduring CPT performance. The findings differ quantita-tively for IDS and QDS. Premature responses were relatedto QDS: higher scores on this schizotypy measure (includ-ing self-judgments about odd speech and odd behavior)tended to go with an increase in premature reactions on theCPT. False alarms were strongly related (r = 0.59) to inter-view DS: higher scores on IDS (including problems ingoal directedness of thinking) tended to go with anincrease of incorrect responses during CPT performance.Although both CPT measures were intercorrelated (r =0.56), the divergent findings for DS measures are not sur-prising because their intercorrelation was very low (r =-0.11). The findings strongly support the cognitive(dis)inhibition hypothesis of DS: odd speech (by SPQ) andproblems in goal directedness of thinking (by SIS-R) arerelated to the production of incorrect responses duringCPT performance. A general failure of cognitive inhibitionmay underlie both the lack of goal directedness in schizo-typal thinking and the premature and incorrect psychomo-tor responses during the CPT. Additional evidence for cog-nitive disinhibition associated with DS was found.Incorrect retrieval from verbal long-term memory was sig-nificantly correlated to IDS in the correlational analyses,although in the multiple regression it fell to just below sta-tistical significance (t = 1.9).

Patterns of Neurocognitive Correlations in DifferentSamples. The pattern of neurocognitive correlates ofdimensions of schizotypy in this relatives study seems todiffer from the pattern found in normal subjects. Wefound verbal long-term memory correlated to PS and NS,and CPT to DS, and we found no correlations with theWCST for NS. In a meta-analysis on neurocognitive cor-relates of schizotypy in normal subjects, we concludedthat PS is correlated to CPT and NS to WCST (Vollema etal., submitted for publication). Few studies have investi-gated the neurocognitive correlates of DS.

There may be various reasons for this divergency offindings. First, studies using normal subjects mainly usedone particular subscale instead of dimensional scales.Summing scores on subscales and transforming them intodimensional scores may mask associations between a neu-

rocognitive test and subtraits. For example, the ParanoidIdeation subscale of the SPQ correlated with the CPT vari-able of d' (r = -0.45, p < 0.001), but the correlation of thedimensional score PS with CPT d' was insignificant. Sec-ond, the current study controlled for the effects of age,gender, IQ, state-anxiety, and state-depression. As can beseen in table 2, state-depression was the strongest predic-tor for PS and NS. In schizotypy studies using normal sub-jects, these variables have seldom been controlled for,except, for instance, by Lenzenweger and Korfine (1994).Third, sample differences may contribute to the differentfindings. Kendler et al. (1991) and Grove et al. (1991) alsodid not find an association between CPT and PS in firstdegree relatives. This would indicate that different sam-ples may have different patterns of neurocognitive correla-tions of schizotypal dimensions.

The pattern of correlations from this study seemsmore in line with the pattern of neurocognitive correlatesof schizophrenia symptom dimensions. In schizophreniastudies, significant associations between positive symp-toms and Latent Inhibition (as a test sensitive to hip-pocampal function; Hemsley 1992), between formalthought disorder and CPT (Frith et al. 1991), and betweennegative symptoms and WCST (Wagman et al. 1987) arereported. The findings diverge with respect to the WCSTand the negative dimensions, but with respect to the posi-tive and disorganization dimensions, the findings convergestrongly. In particular, the findings with respect to DS andcognitive disinhibition (during CPT performance) arehighly similar across both samples. Nuechterlein et al.(1986) found a strong association between formal thoughtdisorder and false alarms on the CPT in schizophreniapatients. Frith et al. (1991) confirmed this finding and con-cluded that formal thought disorder is associated with theproduction of incorrect responses during CPT perform-ance. This similarity of a relation between CPT and DS infirst degree relatives and CPT and formal thought disorderin schizophrenia patients is striking. It supports thehypothesis that in first degree relatives, false alarms andpremature reactions on the CPT and problems in goaldirectedness of thinking are manifestations of the familialbiological vulnerability to schizophrenia.

State-Psychopathology. State-psychopathology (in par-ticular depression) appeared to be the best predictor forscores on PS and NS of first degree relatives. Althoughdepression and anxiety scores of the relatives were justabove normal limits (mean for SCL-90 depression = 50thpercentile and mean for SCL-90 anxiety = 60th percentilecompared to normal controls), they heavily determinedschizotypy scores. For example, depression correlated sig-nificantly with dimensions of PS (r = 0.34, p < 0.01) andNS (r = 0.45, p < 0.01). As hypothesized by Meehl

374

by guest on May 3, 2016

http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/D

ownloaded from

Page 9: by guest on May 3, 2016 ......schizotypal traits and schizotypal personality disorder (review in Benishay and Lencz 1995). However, it remains to be seen whether questionnaires and

Neurocognitive Correlates of Schizotypy Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 28, No. 3, 2002

(1990), state-psychopathology is likely to co-occur withschizotypal phenomena. Some additional explanations canbe given. First, during the assessments they may feardetection of schizotypal phenomena, which they are sofamiliar with in their relative with schizophrenia. Second,their anxiety and depression may have a trait character.Many subjects indicated that in reaction to the schizophre-nia disorder of their relative and its consequences (e.g.,long-term hospitalization, medication, deterioration), theydeveloped symptoms of anxiety and depression. For thesereasons it is well advised to control for the effects of anxi-ety and depression in future studies into schizotypy usingfirst degree relatives of schizophrenia patients.

Limitations and Implications. Limitations of this studyare as follows. First, a type I error may have occurred:Did some of the significant relationships occur by chance,because many correlational analyses (72) were executed?We stressed only significant associations between schizo-typal dimensions and neurocognitive measures thatoccurred in both the correlational and multiple regressionanalyses. The correlations between DS and CPT (CPTFand CPTP) and between PS and verbal memory (VLMC)were all in the predicted directions, therefore reducing thechance for a type I error.

Second, unfortunately we are not sure whether oursample is representative. Some subjects indicated that therelative most similar to the schizophrenia patient refusedto participate. If some highly schizotypal relatives wereadded to the sample, correlations between schizotypaldimensions and neurocognitive measures would likelyincrease. We have indications that relatives differ on SPQdimensions compared to normal controls (from Kremen etal. 1998). We found means for PS (5.2), DS (3.9), and NS(8.1); and Kremen et al. found 3.4, 1.9, and 4.9, respec-tively. Relatives in the Kremen et al. study reached meanscores of 3.8, 2.3, and 5.8, respectively. Although we real-located some of the SPQ items based on a confirmatoryitem factor analysis (see Instruments section), our scorescan be (globally) compared to those of Kremen et al.(1998). This comparison shows in particular that thescores of our relatives differ from the scores of the normalcontrols from Kremen et al. and even seem somewhathigher than the scores of their study's relatives. Withrespect to the neurocognitive measures, our study can becompared with the study of Keefe et al. (1994), in whichrelatives' scores on the WCST variables categories com-pleted and perseverative errors were 4.8 and 18, respec-tively, while normal controls' scores were 5.2 and 15.6,respectively. We found 4.4 for categories completed and21.8 for perseverative errors. So our WCST findings aremore in line with the performances of the relatives thanwith the controls in the study by Keefe et al. (1994). The

same holds for the performance on letter fluency. Keefe etal. (1994) reported a mean amount of t words of 13.3 forrelatives and 17.9 for normal controls. Our total amount ofn words was 14.2. In sum, with respect to schizotypaltraits and neurocognitive measures, our relatives per-formed much the way relatives in other studies did andwere more deviant than controls (Keefe et al. 1994; Kre-men et al. 1998).

Third, relatives could have responded defensively.Various authors have pointed to this particular problem(Grove et al. 1991; Lenzenweger 1993). Relatives mayfear the detection of schizotypal phenomena. We have noindications that the relatives responded defensively toSPQ and SIS-R (see above). Furthermore, in a multitrait-multimethod study, method variation (including a defen-sive response set and the like) contributed only moderatelyto the total variance on schizotypy measures (Vollema andOrmel, submitted for publication). It turned out that thescores on SPQ and SIS-R are mainly determined by traitfactors (i.e., PS, DS, and NS) and to a much lesser extentby method factors (i.e., the format of items and responsecategories as well as test-taking attitudes).

Different versions of the CPT have been used in stud-ies of schizotypy and schizophrenia. The 3-7 version has aworking memory load and is easier than, for example, theDegraded Stimulus version (Nuechterlein 1991). This lat-ter version requires higher levels of mental effort.Nuechterlein argued that the Degraded Stimulus versionwould be more sensitive to deviancies in high-risk sam-ples. Therefore, it is surprising that we found strong corre-lations between DS and false alarms on the CPT 3-7. Itsuggests that the 3-7 version is able to detect subtledeviancies in first degree relatives.

Future research into the neurocognition of schizotypycan be directed to the following areas. First, the discrep-ancy between the patterns of neurocognitive correlates ofschizotypy in normal subjects versus first degree relativesdeserves attention. Normal studies should control for vari-ables like sex, age, and IQ before making more definitiveconclusions. Increasing evidence for this discrepancy willlead to questions about the usefulness of normal subjects toschizotypy research. Second, the modest associationbetween PS and verbal long-term memory needs to befirmly replicated before hypotheses about hippocampal(and verbal memory) dysfunctioning underlying psychotic-like schizotypy can be tested. Alternative tests sensitive tohippocampal dysfunction may be used. Third, studies maybe executed to disentangle the subcomponents of CPT per-formance responsible for the strong correlation with DS.Fourth, more studies are needed to assess the ability of theWCST as a vulnerability marker. Finally, it is still advisedto make use of observational scores for DS. The evidencefrom this study supports Kendler et al.'s (1996) hypothesis

375

by guest on May 3, 2016

http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/D

ownloaded from

Page 10: by guest on May 3, 2016 ......schizotypal traits and schizotypal personality disorder (review in Benishay and Lencz 1995). However, it remains to be seen whether questionnaires and

Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 28, No. 3, 2002 M.G. Vollema and B. Postma

that interviews are better suited for the assessment ofschizotypal signs than questionnaires are. And we have towait until questionnaires are available assessing disorgani-zation in a valid way.

We investigated the neurocognitive correlates ofdimensions of schizotypy in healthy first degree relativesof schizophrenia patients. This sample has many advan-tages (over samples of schizophrenia patients) for investi-gating the vulnerability to schizophrenia. Our subjectswere motivated and able to participate, showed no psy-chotic symptoms (or any psychiatric disorder), were med-ication-free, and were all well-functioning citizens. How-ever, in these healthy subjects we found some highlysignificant associations between dimensions of schizotypyand neurocognitive measures. The strong associationsbetween DS and incorrect responses during an attentionaltask may indicate a genetic vulnerability to schizophrenia.

References

Allen, H.A.; Liddle, P.F.; and Frith, CD. Negative features,retrieval processes and verbal fluency in schizophrenia.British Journal of Psychiatry, 163:769-775, 1993.

American Psychiatric Association. DSM—IV: Diagnosticand Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed.Washington, DC: APA, 1994.

Benishay, D.S., and Lencz, T. Semistructured interviewsfor the measurement of schizotypal personality. In: Raine,A.; Lencz, T.; and Mednick, S.A., eds. SchizotypalPersonality. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge UniversityPress, 1995. pp. 463-481.

Borkowski, J.G.; Benton, A.L.; and Spreen, 0. Word fluencyand brain damage. Neurvpsychologia, 5:135-140,1967.

Buchsbaum, M.S. The frontal lobes, basal ganglia, andtemporal lobes as sites for schizophrenia. SchizophreniaBulletin, 16(3):379-389, 1990.

Derogatis, L.R.; Lipman, R.S.; and Covi, I. SCL-90, anoutpatient psychiatric rating scale: Preliminary report.Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 9:13-27, 1973.

Flaum, M., and Andreasen, N.C. Brain morphology inschizotypal personality as assessed by magnetic resonanceimaging. In: Raine, A.; Lencz, T.; and Mednick, S.A., eds.Schizotypal Personality. Cambridge, MA: CambridgeUniversity Press, 1995. pp. 385-406.Frith, C D . ; Leary, J.; Cahill, C ; and Johnstone, E.Performance on psychological tests: Demographic andclinical correlates of the results of these tests. BritishJournal of Psychiatry, 159 (Suppl 13):26-29, 1991.

Gottesman, I. Schizophrenia genesis. In: Gottesman, I.The Origins of Madness. New York, NY: Freeman, 1991.pp. 82-104.

Grove, W.M.; Lebow, B.S.; Clementz, B.A.; Cerri, A.;Medus, C; and Iacono, W.G. Familial prevalence and coag-gregation of schizotypy indicators: A multitrait family study.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100:115-121,1991.

Heaton, R.K. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual.Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, 1981.

Hemsley, D.R. Disorders of perception and cognition inschizophrenia. Revue Europeene de PsychologieAppliquee, 42:105-114, 1992.

Keefe, R.S.E.; Silverman, J.M.; Roitman, S.E.L.; Harvey,P.D.; Duncan, M.A.; Alroy, D.; Siever, L.J.; Davis, K.L.;and Mohs, R.C. Performance of nonpsychotic relatives ofschizophrenic patients on cognitive tests. PsychiatryResearch, 53:1-12, 1994.

Kendler, K.S.; Lieberman, J.A.; and Walsh, D. TheStructured Interview for Schizotypy (SIS): A preliminaryreport. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 15(4):559-571, 1989.

Kendler, K.S.; Ochs, A.L.; Gorman, A.M.; Hewitt, J.K.;Ross, D.E.; and Mirsky, A.F. The structure of schizotypy:A pilot multitrait twin study. Psychiatry Research,36:19-36,1991.

Kendler, K.S.; Thacker, L.; and Walsh, D. Self-report mea-sures of schizotypy as indices of familial vulnerability toschizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 22(3):511-520,1996.

Kremen, W.S.; Faraone, S.V.; Toomey, R.; Seidman, L.J.;and Tsuang, M.T. Sex differences in self-reported schizo-typal traits in relatives of schizophrenic probands.Schizophrenia Research, 34:27-37, 1998.

Lencz, T; Raine, A.; Benishay, D.S.; Mills, S.; and Bird,L. Neuropsychological abnormalities associated withschizotypal personality. In: Raine, A.; Lencz, T ; andMednick, S.A., eds. Schizotypal Personality. Cambridge,MA: Cambridge University Press, 1995. pp. 289-329.

Lenzenweger, M.F. Explorations in schizotypy and thepsychometric high-risk paradigm. In: Chapman, L.J.;Chapman, J.P.; and Fowles, D., eds. Progress inExperimental Personality and Psychopathology Research.New York, NY: Springer, 1993. pp. 66-116.

Lenzenweger, M.F., and Korfine, L. Perceptual aberra-tions, schizotypy, and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.Schizophrenia Bulletin, 20(2):345-357, 1994.

Lenzenweger, M.F., and Moldin, S.O. Discerning thelatent structure of hypothetical psychosis pronenessthrough admixture analysis. Psychiatry Research,33:243-257, 1990.

Lezak, M.D. Neuropsychological Assessment. 2nd ed.Oxford, England: Oxford University Press, 1983.

Liddle, P.F., and Morris, D.L. Schizophrenic syndromesand frontal lobe performance. British Journal ofPsychiatry, 158:340-345, 1991.

376

by guest on May 3, 2016

http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/D

ownloaded from

Page 11: by guest on May 3, 2016 ......schizotypal traits and schizotypal personality disorder (review in Benishay and Lencz 1995). However, it remains to be seen whether questionnaires and

Neurocognitive Correlates of Schizotypy Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 28, No. 3, 2002

McPherson, S.E., and Cummings, J.L. The neuropsychol-ogy of the frontal lobes. In: Ron, M.A., and David, A.S.,eds. Disorders of Brain and Mind. Cambridge, MA:Cambridge University Press, 1998. pp. 121-135.

Meehl, P.E. Toward an integrated theory of schizotaxia,schizotypy and schizophrenia. Journal of PersonalityDisorders, 4:1-99, 1990.

Mulder, J.L.; Dekker, R.; and Dekker, PH. Verbale leer engeheugen test. Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets andZeitlinger, 1996.

Norusis, M.J. SPSS 6.1 Guide to Data Analysis.Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall, 1995.

Nuechterlein, K.H. Vigilance in schizophrenia and relateddisorders. In: Nasrallah, H.A., ed. Handbook ofSchizophrenia. Vol. 5. Amsterdam, The Netherlands:Elsevier Science, 1991. pp. 397-433.

Nuechterlein, K.H.; Edell, W.S.; Norris, M.; and Dawson,M.E. Attentional vulnerability indicators, thought disor-der, and negative symptoms. Schizophrenia Bulletin,12(3):408^26, 1986.

Raine, A. The SPQ: A scale for the assessment of schizo-typal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria.Schizophrenia Bulletin, 17(4):555-564, 1991.

Raine, A.; Reynolds, C ; Lencz, T.;. Scerbo, A.; Triphon,N.; and Kim, D. Cognitive-perceptual, interpersonal, anddisorganized features of schizotypal personality.Schizophrenia Bulletin, 20(1): 191-201, 1994.

Raine, A.; Sheard, C ; Reynolds, G.P.; and Lencz, T. Pre-frontal structural and functional deficits associated withindividual differences in schizotypal personality.Schizophrenia Research, 7:237-247, 1992.

Sass, K.J.; Sass, A.; Westerveld, M.; Lencz, T; Novelly,R.A.; Kim, J.H.; and Spencer, D.D. Specificity in the cor-relation of verbal memory and hippocampal neuron loss:Dissociation of memory, language and verbal intellectualability. Journal of Clinical and ExperimentalNeuropsychology, 14:662-672, 1992.

Schmand, B.; Lindeboom, J.; and van Harskamp, F.Nederlandse leestest voor volwassenen. Lisse, TheNetherlands: Swets and Zeitlinger, 1992.

Stratta, P.; Daneluzzo, E.; Mattei, P.; Bustini, M.; Casacchia,M.; and Rossi, A. No deficit in Wisconsin Card Sorting Testperformance of schizophrenic patients' first-degree rela-tives. Schizophrenia Research, 26:147-151, 1997.

Van der Does, A.J.W., and van den Bosch, R.J. Whatdetermines Wisconsin Card Sorting performance inschizophrenia? Clinical Psychology Review, 12:567-583,1992.

Venables, P.H. Schizotypal status as a developmentalstage in studies of risk for schizophrenia. In: Raine, A.;Lencz, T.; and Mednick, S.A., eds. SchizotypalPersonality. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge UniversityPress, 1995. pp. 107-135.

Vollema, M.G.; Aleman, A.; and van den Bosch, R.J.Neurocognitive correlates of dimensions of self-reportschizotypy: A quantitative review. Submitted for publica-tion.

Vollema, M.G., and van den Bosch, R.J. The multidimen-sionality of schizotypy. Schizophrenia Bulletin,21(1):19-31, 1995.

Vollema, M.G., and Hoijtink, H. The multidimensionalityof self-report schizotypy in a psychiatric population: Ananalysis using multidimensional Rasch models.Schizophrenia Bulletin, 26(3):565-575, 2000.

Vollema, M.G., and Ormel, J. The reliability of theStructured Interview for Schizotypy-Revised (SIS-R).Schizophrenia Bulletin, 26(5):619-629, 2000.

Vollema, M.G., and Ormel, J. A multitrait-multimethodanalysis of three dimensions of schizotypy. Submitted forpublication.

Wagman, A.M.I.; Heinrichs, D.W.; and Carpenter, W.T.Deficit and non-deficit forms of schizophrenia:Neuropsychological evaluation. Psychiatry Research,22:319-330, 1987.

Walker, E.F., and Gale, S. Neurodevelopmental processesin schizophrenia and schizotypal personality disorder. In:Raine, A.; Lencz, T ; and Mednick, S.A., eds. SchizotypalPersonality. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge UniversityPress, 1995. pp. 56-79.

The Authors

Meinte G. Vollema, Ph.D., is psychologist and Head of theDepartment of Psychological Assessment of MeerkantenGGZ Flevo-Veluwe, Ermelo, the Netherlands; BelindaPostma is technician, Department of PsychologicalAssessment of Meerkanten GGZ Flevo-Veluwe, Ermelo,the Netherlands.

377

by guest on May 3, 2016

http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/D

ownloaded from

Page 12: by guest on May 3, 2016 ......schizotypal traits and schizotypal personality disorder (review in Benishay and Lencz 1995). However, it remains to be seen whether questionnaires and

Minority Research Training in Psychiatry

Through a five-year, $2.5 million grant from the National Institute of Mental Health, theAmerican Psychiatric Institute for Research and Education (APIRE) is seeking through the Programfor Minority Research Training in Psychiatry (PMRTP) to increase the number of minority psychia-trists entering the field of psychiatric research.

The program provides medical students with funding for stipends, travel expenses, and tuitionfor an elective or summer experience in a research environment, with special attention paid totrainees' career development in research. In addition, stipends are available for a limited number ofone-or two-year postresidency fellowships for minority psychiatrists. Residents may engage in full-year research training during the last year of psychiatric residency or in "year off' research training.

Training takes place at research-oriented departments of psychiatry in major U.S. medicalschools and other appropriate sites throughout the country. An individual at the site (the research"mentor") is responsible for overseeing the research training experience.

Administered by the American Psychiatric Institute for Research and Education, the programincludes outreach efforts to identify minority medical students and residents who are potentialresearchers and to put them in touch with advisors who counsel them about careers in psychiatricresearch. Additional activities assist fellows and alumni in their research career development.

The director of the PMRTP is James Thompson, M.D., M.P.H.; the project manager is ErnestoGuerra. An advisory committee of senior researchers and minority psychiatrists developed guide-lines for applicants and criteria for selection. The members of this committee evaluate and selecttrainees, oversee the research training experiences, and play a role in evaluating the effectiveness ofthe program.

December 1 is the deadline for applications for residents seeking a year or more of training andfor postresidency fellows. For medical students, applications are due three months before training isto begin. Summer medical students who will start their training by June 30 should submit theirapplications by April 1.

For more information about the PMRTP, call the toll-free number for the PMRTP, 1-800-852-1390, or 202-682-6225, e-mail [email protected], or write to PMRTP at the American PsychiatricInstitute for Research and Education, 1400 K Street, NW, Washington, DC 20005.

378

by guest on May 3, 2016

http://schizophreniabulletin.oxfordjournals.org/D

ownloaded from