bulls eye or ricochet? ethnically targeted campaign ads in

37
Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in the 2008 Election Matt A. Barreto University of Washington [email protected] Victoria M. DeFrancesco Soto Northwestern University [email protected] Jennifer L. Merolla Claremont Graduate University [email protected] Ricardo Ramirez University of Southern California [email protected] Author names are presented alphabetically, authorship is equal Prepared for Presentation at the 2009 Chicago Area Behavioral Workshop Evanston, IL, May 8, 2009

Upload: others

Post on 27-Apr-2022

3 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in the 2008 Election

Matt A. Barreto University of Washington

[email protected]

Victoria M. DeFrancesco Soto Northwestern University [email protected]

Jennifer L. Merolla

Claremont Graduate University [email protected]

Ricardo Ramirez

University of Southern California [email protected]

Author names are presented alphabetically, authorship is equal

Prepared for Presentation at the 2009 Chicago Area Behavioral Workshop

Evanston, IL, May 8, 2009

Page 2: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

Introduction

In an effort to court the Latino community, both political parties made a point to

campaign aggressively for Latino “swing” votes through advertisements and get-out-the-vote

(GOTV) drives starting in 2000 (Segal 2006; Martinez-Ruiz-Velasco 2007). The increased

commitment to utilize Spanish-language advertising and direct contact with Latino voters

since the 2000 election1 has yet to be followed by consistent evidence that this targeted

outreach has worked. The two main questions that result are, do targeted ethnic appeals

influence the vote choice of Latinos and what is the effect on non-Latinos?

The rich literature on campaign effects, especially in the form of political

advertisements has almost exclusively focused on non-Latino voters and English-language

materials. Yet the realities of the political campaign have changed markedly over the last

decade. Today, no viable candidate for President or for offices in states with a sizable Latino

population can compete without a “Hispanic Outreach Coordinator” and at least one

Spanish speaking member of their advertisement staff. Gone are the days of Gerald Ford’s

tamale eating incident. Instead, candidates and campaigns today understand they need to be

savvy to win Latino votes. What they many not understand is the means to achieve this goal.

Recently candidates have relied on their Portuguese wife (Kerry), their half-Mexican nephew

(Bush), and their own immigrant story (Schwarzenegger) to try to connect with Latino

voters. The delivery of messages in Spanish or a combination of Spanish and English has

also characterized the recent push for the Latino electorate. However, we still do not know

if these approaches work. Are ethnically targeted campaigns effective in mobilizing Latinos

or is it simply seen as pandering? More specifically, what cues matter for Latinos? How

1 Although Latinos had been active in presidential campaigns prior to 2000, such as the Viva Kennedy clubs in 1960, many scholars point to 2000 as the first election to implement a well organized and sustained Latino outreach and Spanish language advertisement campaign.

2

Page 3: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

important is the use of Spanish in the targeted outreach? How important are the

endorsements of co-ethnics for Latino vote choice? If either Spanish language or Latino

endorsements are utilized in political advertisement, what direct or indirect effects are there

among non-Latino voters? Lastly, are these ricochet effects constant across all non-Latino

racial groups?

The purpose of this project is to offer a more nuanced understanding of the effects

of the diversification in targeted campaign strategies that are increasingly making use of

Spanish-language and Latino ethnic appeals by presidential candidates, by answering the

questions noted above. We do this by drawing on the marketing, campaign effects, and

Latino politics literature to advance an account of the effects of language and ethnic cues on

Latino and non-Latino voter preferences, and explore possible mediating effects. More

important, we are able to discern the individual and combined effects of language and

targeted ethnic group appeals. The findings of our field experiment challenge conventional

wisdom about consistency of effects across racial groups and have important implications

for future outreach by candidates and parties. They suggest that candidates and campaigns

must further refine their approach to Latino voters, and at the same time, improve their

ability to micro-target their ads to the intended audience.

The remainder of the paper is divided into five parts. First, we review the extant

literature on campaign effects generally, as well as the emerging literature on mobilization

and persuasion or Latino voters. Second, given that this is the first study to directly measure

individual-level effects of Latino-targeted versus mainstream appeals, we present the

plausible expectations and relevant hypotheses of the measurable effects. We then review

the experimental design and relevant variables that will be considered. The fourth section

presents the results, and the final section concludes with an eye towards the considerations

3

Page 4: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

that candidates and campaigns must undertake as they decide whether or not to separately

target Latinos only in Spanish-language media or whether it makes sense to use limited

language and ethnic cues within their general media campaign.

Campaign Effects

Scholars of political behavior have long explored the effects of political

advertisements and messages on voters. With respect to political ads, early work by

Patterson and McClure (1976) focused on the effects that campaign ads had on the voting

preferences of citizens. These scholars found that campaign ads did little to change the

minds of voters, a finding that echoed the more general findings of minimal campaign

effects in the literature (e.g., Campbell et al. 1960; Lazarsfeld, Berelson and Gaudet 1944).

However, more recent work (e.g., Shaw 1999; West 2005) has found that ads have an impact

on evaluations of the candidates and vote choice. Furthermore, many studies have

demonstrated that ads can have an impact on voter learning about the candidates,

perceptions of candidates’ chances of winning, voter turnout, and by altering the standards

by which candidates are judged (e.g., Alvarez 1997; Ansolabehere and Iyengar 1995; Brians

and Wattenberg 1996; Finkel and Geer 1998; Johnston et al. 1992).

Within this general literature, scholars have only explored variation by the tone and

targeting of the message. For example, there is a debate on whether negative or positive

advertisements increase the likelihood of turnout (e.g., Ansolabehere and Iyengar 1995;

Finkel and Geer 1998; Freedman and Goldstein 1999; Geer 2006; Goldstein and Freedman

2002). Others have examined whether certain groups of voters are more receptive to

targeted messages (Clinton and Lapinski, 2004). However, they have not explored the

4

Page 5: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

effects of these messages on Latinos, nor on how Latino targeted messages might affect

Latinos and non-Latinos.

Although there are few studies about campaign effects on Latino voters, we are not

the first to take note of the new campaign strategies targeting Latinos. A handful of research

projects and articles focus specifically on Spanish-language and ethnically targeted campaigns

(Shaw et. al. 2001; Michelson 2003; Ramírez 2005; 2007; DeFrancesco Soto and Merolla

2006). From a variety of perspectives, these studies all tend to conclude that ethnic-based

appeals are more successful at mobilizing Latinos to vote. Just as the English-language

studies fall short of providing data for Latinos, the Latino politics research does not

investigate the effects of ethnically targeted ads for non-Latinos. Moreover, the Latino

politics research in this domain has tended to focus exclusively on turnout.

This research is among the first to consider the effects that general and ethnically-

targeted campaign ads have on Latinos, as well as among non-Latinos, and to extend this

research to vote choice. Nuño (2007) finds that ethnic partisan mobilization significantly

affected candidate preference among Latino voters in the 2000 election. However, it is

unclear what the content of the mobilization message was, given that Nuño relies on self-

reported mobilization measures from a telephone survey. In fact, he speculates that Latino

canvassers used different, more effective campaign appeals, making it difficult to compare to

the Anglo canvassers. In contrast, by using a controlled experiment, this study can examine

the effect of the actual campaign ads and messages.

In the past, the two streams of research were conducted in isolation of one another.

General studies examined the effect of “mainstream” campaign appeals on the general

electorate, providing no insight into effect on Latino voters. Similarly, the aforementioned

Latino mobilization research has limited its inquiry into how Latino voters respond to ethnic

5

Page 6: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

campaign appeals. We argue that the new demographic realities of the American electorate

make it much harder to neatly segregate Latino and non-Latino voters. Campaign billboards

high above California freeways prominently advertise their candidate in English and in

Spanish for all potential voters to see. Bilingual radio spots run on many popular English

and Spanish-language radio stations hoping to attract bilingual Latino listeners. Spanish-

language or bilingual campaign mailers to zip codes with a high percentage of Latino

households may inadvertently be received by non-Latinos voters. Finally, some campaigns

may experiment with using “Spanglish” in their television commercials to reach out to

Latino voters.

In 1994 when Republican Pete Wilson ran for re-election as Governor of California

his campaign was coordinated with the divisive anti-immigrant Proposition 187. He ran

multiple television ads depicting Latino immigrants as intruders and criminals, running

across the Mexican border with no regard for U.S. laws. The goal was to mobilize

conservative anti-immigrant voters, without regard for Latino voters who were viewed as an

inconsequential segment of the electorate. Twelve years later Republican Arnold

Schwarzenegger ran for re-election as Governor of California and campaigned heavily for

Latino votes, emphasized his own immigration story, and employed a Spanish-

language/bilingual campaign in print, radio and TV, even while Pete Wilson served as an

advisor. Campaign strategies have clearly changed, however due to the complexity of

modern campaigns scholars have not appropriately categorized the types and effects of

campaign appeals when the Latino electorate is in play.

Approaches to Campaigning for Latino Votes

Over time, marketing research began to more carefully craft messages to target

particular audiences. Studies in both marketing research and political science have

6

Page 7: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

demonstrated that targeted appeals are more effective than non-targeted appeals among the

targeted population, though the effects vary with respect to the non-targeted audience. For

example, in a controlled experiment, Aaker et. al. (2000) examined the effect of targeted

commercials on the intended audience as well as the “non-target market.” Although their

treatment ads are related to soft drinks and movies, their results are quite instructive. They

find targeted appeals are received favorably with the intended audience and unfavorably by

the non-target market. Similarly, Brumbaugh (2002) compared the effect of receiving ads

which cued themes from the dominant (Anglo) culture and from a subculture (i.e. targeted).

She found members of the dominant and subculture to be receptive to the dominant culture

ads (i.e. mainstream), but that only members of the subculture were receptive to the targeted

ads. The subculture targeted ads were not perceived negatively by the dominant culture, but

rather had no effect. With respect to political science research, Clinton and Lapinski (2004)

found that targeted ads in the 2000 presidential election were more effective than non-

targeted ads in increasing turnout, while Merolla and DeFranceso (2006) found that ads

targeted to Latinos had a stronger effect in increasing the likelihood of turnout. However,

neither study focused on the effects of targeted messages on those from the non-targeted

group.

Most recently, White (2007) compares exposure to racial cues in political messages

about the war in Iraq and welfare among both Whites and Blacks. The basic premise of

White’s research fits well with our expectations, that racial priming has significantly different

effects on the in-group and out-group. Through a controlled experiment, White finds that

both explicit and implicit racial priming activates different racial attitudes among Whites and

Blacks. While overt racial primes result in in-group identification among Blacks, the same

treatments result in out-group resentment among Whites. In a comparison of Latinos and

7

Page 8: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

non-Latinos, we might expect the cue of Spanish-language materials to elicit similar

responses.

The studies above inform some of our expectations as we turn to the question of

how political candidates can appeal to the Latino population. We identify four general

approaches to campaigning for Latino voters that candidates might employ. First, they may

rely on their traditional, English campaign ads and simply rely on these general ads for both

their Latino and non-Latino outreach. Second, they may take their traditional campaign ads

and translate them into Spanish or make them bilingual, without changing the content.

Third, they may design a new targeted campaign ad for Latino voters with images of Latino

families, children, or prominent Latino endorsements, while keeping the ads in English.

Fourth, they may translate their targeted ad, with Latino specific content, into Spanish or

make it bilingual. Although there are dozens of variations to campaign ads (i.e. tone,

content), these four general categories capture the substantive differences between each, and

provide a framework for scholars interested in the effects of Latino targeted messages.

Below, we explore a 2 x 2 typology of campaign ads, and the expected effects for

both Latino and non-Latino voters when compared to a control group. We turn first to

potential effects of these different types of appeals among Latinos. In general, drawing from

the literature discussed above, we expect Latino targeted messages to resonate more with

Latino voters (relative to the control), with bilingual targeted messages potentially having the

most noticeable effects. There is some empirical support to this claim with respect to

Latinos. Studies have shown that individuals are more likely to be mobilized by a co-ethnic

(Fraga 1988; Shaw, de la Garza and Lee 2000, Michelson 2003, and Ramírez 2005; 2007) or

given exposure to targeted ads (Merolla and DeFrancesco Soto 2006). As a result, we

specifically hypothesize that ethnically targeted ads will have a positive effect.

8

Page 9: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

H1: Targeted messages affect the preferences of Latinos.

H1a: This effect is more pronounced for the most targeted appeal (Latino Bilingual).

Table 1: Typology of Campaign Ads and Expected Effects on Voters

English Spanish/Bilingual

Traditional “Mainstream”

Latino: limited to null effects

Non-Latino: significant effects

Latino: significant effects

Non-Latino: H1: no effect H2: negative effect

H3: positive effect

Ethnically Targeted

Latino: significant effects

Non-Latino: H1: no effect H2: negative effect

H3: positive effect

Latino: significant (and potentially stronger) effects

Non-Latino: H1: no effect H2: negative effect H3: positive effect

Traditional English ads may also have a positive effect on Latino vote choice,

although we expect the effect to be somewhat limited (Brumbaugh 2002).

H2: Non-targeted messages have null to minimal effect on the preferences of Latinos.

In contrast, we expect non-Latino voters to be most persuaded by the Traditional

“Mainstream” English-language campaign ads. These are the ads non-Latino voters are

accustomed to seeing, and in reality, are designed to target non-Latino voters.

H3: Non-targeted messages affect the preferences of non-Latinos.

We are agnostic as to what the effects might be for the targeted appeals. First, if

non-Latinos perceive a message to be targeted to a different group (i.e. in Spanish/bilingual,

Latino endorser), then the message may have null effects (Brumbaugh 2002). Second, non-

Latinos may react negatively to the Latino-targeted ad (Aaker et al. 2000), especially if there

are implicit racial or ethnic cues. This expectation is consistent with work by Valentino

9

Page 10: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

1999, Valentino, Hutchings, and White 2002 and Mendelberg 2001 who find that white

voters react negatively to implicit racial campaign images. Third, and equally plausible, is

that non-Latinos may react positively to an ad clearly targeting Latinos and view this as

evidence of a candidate’s appeals to racial inclusion (Fraga and Leal 2004). This plausible

explanation of symbolic outreach efforts, or “ricochet pandering” for minorities, but

specifically Latino voters, has not been empirically tested. Given the lack of definitive

patterns in the literature, we consider all three hypotheses as plausible.

H4a: Targeted messages have a null effect on non-Latinos. H4b: Targeted messages have a positive effect on non-Latinos. H4c: Targeted messages have a negative effect on non-Latinos. Experimental Design

We implemented our study online from September 9th to the 16th with a sample of

registered voters in Los Angeles County who provided their email addresses when they

registered to vote. Subjects were randomly assigned to a control or treatment group. Those

in the treated groups were exposed to either an advertisement from Barack Obama or John

McCain, which was either in English or was Bilingual, and was either a mainstream ad with

an Anglo endorser or a targeted ad with a Latino endorser. The subjects filled out a pre-

survey, watched the advertisement, and then filled out a post-test which contained evaluation

questions about both candidates.

Participants and Design

We obtained the email addresses of about 675,000 registered voters in LA County

who provided this information when they registered to vote. While those who provided

their email addresses closely resembled the full sample of registered voters on almost all

dimensions, there were many more people ages 30 and younger. Furthermore, our goal was

10

Page 11: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

to have a large enough sample of Latinos to look at the effects of targeted ads on the target

population. To achieve both objectives, we selected a random stratified sample by age of

75,000 Latino surnames and 75,000 non-Latino surnames to receive our email solicitation.

All 150,000 individuals received an email solicitation in which we asked them to participate

in a study on the 2008 Presidential election.2 We randomized incentives in the email

solicitation as part of another study.3 The total number of subjects recruited was 5343, and

4672 had a computer that was capable of handling the study materials.

With respect to demographic and socioeconomic factors, the average respondent

was 35, graduated from college, and had an income level between 60,000 and 80,000. 50% of

the sample was male; 34% identified as Latino; 51% identified as white; 10% identified as

Asian; 4% as African-American; and, 78% were born in the U.S. With respect to

partisanship, 16% identified as Republican, 55% as Democrat, and the remaining 19% as

Independent. In terms of ideology, our mean respondent was slightly liberal. These sample

characteristics compare reasonably well with the population of registered voters in Los

Angeles County. We have a slight over representation of Democrats and a slight under

representation of Republicans; there are 51% of Democrats and 20% of Republicans in the

voter file. Our sample is also slightly younger than the full voter file sample, even though we

stratified our sampling by age. While we are pleased that the sample closely approximates

the electorate in Los Angeles County, it is not meant to be a representative sample of all

2 Those with a Spanish surname received the invitation in English and in Spanish. 3 The control group was not given any monetary compensation, while the treatment groups had some form of monetary compensation for the first 250 people in each condition to respond to the survey. Three conditions were some form of a $10 dollar gift card to a store, to give to a charity of their choice, or for a choice between the two. Two conditions were a lottery for a 1 in 50 chance of winning a $100 or $250 dollar visa gift card. The response rate by condition varied from a low of 2.47% for those in the control group, which had no monetary compensation, to a high of 3.74% for those who received the 10 gift card for a store.

11

Page 12: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

voters in L.A. because we are most interested in the effects of the experiment on registered

voters.

Subjects were randomly assigned to the control group or one of eight political

advertisements by the computer: Obama English Miller; Obama Bilingual Miller; Obama

English Mendoza; Obama Bilingual Mendoza; McCain English Miller; McCain Bilingual

Miller; McCain English Mendoza; and, McCain Bilingual Mendoza. An investigation of

whether subjects were evenly distributed across conditions, for each study, according to

potentially relevant variables (i.e., invitation compensation condition, party identification,

ideology, gender, education, age and income) revealed a significant difference only in the

distribution of gender (p=0.09).4

Procedures

Subjects who wanted to take part in the study clicked on a link to the survey, which

they could take in English or Spanish. Subjects did an audio visual test to make sure that

they could hear and see a test video. If they were not able to see or hear the test video, they

were thanked for being willing to participate and were excused from the study (on-screen).

Among those who were able to watch and hear the test video, they then proceeded to fill out

a brief survey which asked basic demographic information, group identity evaluations, as

well as political predisposition questions. Treated groups were then exposed to one of the

eight advertisements. All groups then filled out a post-treatment survey that contained

questions that would allow for a manipulation check, evaluations of the candidates, and

behavior relevant to the upcoming election.

4 To be certain that slight differences in partisanship did not impact the final results, the data were weighted to party identification across each of the eight treatment cells to match the party identification in the control group, within each racial group. This way we can be certain that the exact same proportion of democrats and republicans are found in each of the eight treatment groups.

12

Page 13: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

Treatments As stated previously, subjects in the treated groups received one of eight

advertisements. The advertisements all contained the same script, with variations only with

respect to our key conditions: the candidate being sponsored, the language of the

advertisement, and the presence of Latino content. The language manipulation and

candidate manipulation were straightforward. The same script was used (see Appendix), but

certain phrases were repeated in Spanish and we varied the candidate’s name and picture.

For example, the first line in the script (which is replicated in the appendix) noted: “John

McCain/Barack Obama, he’s a once in a generation leader, un lider unico (added for

bilingual), with a bold agenda.” The first image to appear on the screen was of John McCain

or Barack Obama. We chose images of the candidates that had similar backgrounds; and, in

some instances, we were able to swap just the candidates heads onto the same background.

[table 1 about here]

We varied the presence of Latino content in two ways. First, we had either an Anglo

male, Harold Miller, or a Latino male, Hector Mendoza, endorse the candidate. Second, we

scattered the phrase Latino throughout some sections of the script. For example, the Anglo

endorser was President of the National Advocacy Alliance, while the Latino endorser was

President of the Latino Advocacy Alliance. One limitation to this design is that we can not

say specifically whether it is the endorser or the use of the word Latino throughout the script

that may drive differences for this condition. However, the current structure of the Latino

content more closely mirrors the approach used in Latino targeted ads, so the ad should be

more similar to what voters would see in actual elections, making it higher in external

validity.

13

Page 14: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

Manipulation Check The programming of the on-line survey forced treated respondents to watch the

advertisement, so they could not just skip it. However, we wanted to test whether subjects

were paying attention to the political advertisements. One indicator we can use to see if

subjects were paying attention to the ads is whether they recalled the endorser. In the post-

test, we asked respondents to identify the individuals who have endorsed each candidate

from a list of seven names, which included the endorsers in the ads as well as other

individuals who endorsed the candidates during the election, such as Ted Kennedy

endorsing Barack Obama and George Bush endorsing John McCain. If respondents paid

attention to the ad content, then we should find that those who received the Miller condition

(either English or Bilingual) should be more likely to select Miller as endorsing the given

candidate relative to those who did not receive the Miller ad. The same logic would apply

for those in the conditions with the Mendoza endorsement. In figure 1, we present the

proportion of individuals who said Miller or Mendoza endorsed each candidate by those

who received an ad with or without the given endorsement.

[Insert Figure 1 about here]

The first thing that is clear for both candidates is that individuals were far more likely

to say that Miller or Mendoza endorsed the given candidate when they were exposed to a

political advertisement with that endorser. For example, 60% of individuals identified Miller

as endorsing Obama when they saw an ad with him as the endorser and only 1.95% of those

who were not exposed to such an ad said Miller endorsed the candidate. All of these

differences in identifying the endorser for those in and not in the condition are statistically

significant according to difference in proportions tests. Furthermore, the pattern of results

is similar across all of the racial and ethnic groups in our sample. However, the percentage

14

Page 15: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

of respondents who correctly identified the endorser they received does appear to vary

across the conditions. A lower percentage of those in the Miller condition were able to

correctly identify Miller as endorsing McCain, 40.56%, or Obama, 60%, relative to those

who were exposed to the Mendoza conditions, where 80.98% correctly identified him as

endorsing Obama and 73.67% correctly identified him as endorsing McCain. These

differences are statistically significant according to difference in proportions tests. Thus, it

appears that recall of the endorser was much higher when the endorser was Mendoza for

both candidates. This pattern of higher recall for Mendoza was similar across all racial and

ethnic groups in the sample.

As another test of whether individuals paid attention to the content of the

advertisements, we asked them to rate (on a seven point scale) how much importance the

candidates placed on four issues with higher values being more important: education, the

economy, health care, and immigration. The first three issues were discussed in the

advertisements, with education receiving the most coverage and health care the least amount

of coverage. Thus, we should find that those exposed to the given candidate ads think the

candidates place more importance on these issues relative to the control group. We did not

discuss the issue of immigration, so we should not observe differences between the control

and the groups exposed to the given candidate ad. We plot the mean importance

respondents thought each candidate placed on the four issues for the control group and

those exposed to the ads of each candidate in Figure 2. For Obama, we see that individuals

exposed to the ad did think he placed more importance on the issues of education and the

economy relative the control group, as expected. However, the differences are not

significant for the issue of health care. Meanwhile, as expected, there are no significant

differences on the issue of immigration. A similar pattern emerges for those individuals

15

Page 16: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

exposed to the McCain ad, who think he places more importance on the issues of education

and health care relative to those in the control group, while they perceive no differences on

the issue of immigration. However, they do not perceive any significant differences on the

issue of the economy for McCain. Overall, it seems that respondents linked the content in

the ads to the importance candidates placed on the issues, with the exception of health care

for Obama and the economy for McCain. However, health care was mentioned very briefly

in the ad, and McCain was being criticized for his position on the economy around the time

of our study.

[Insert Figure 2 about here]

Vote Choice

Close to the end of our survey, preceding questions of political sophistication,

education, and income, respondents were asked their vote choice intention. Survey

participants were asked: “If the 2008 election were being held today, and you had to decide,

would you vote for Barack Obama, John McCain, or somebody else?” The overwhelming

majority, 70% indicated that they would cast their ballot for Obama, while 21% indicated

that they would cast their ballot for the Republican candidate. The remaining 9% of the

participants either did not know, 4%, or would cast their ballot for someone else, 5%.

In order to begin to address our hypotheses we examine how vote choice was (or

was not) influenced by the exposure to the different advertisement treatments. In this

analysis we will focus on the main effects of the advertisements. As we will elaborate in the

conclusion, having examined the ad main effects on vote choice and other relevant political

measures we will turn to the incorporation of moderators. However, below we will

concentrate on how the different ads alone influenced both Latinos and non-Latinos.

16

Page 17: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

Results for Latino Voters

We first examine the ad effects among the Latino sub-sample, considering whether

the ethnically targeted ads hit their “bulls eye.” With regards to vote choice, we are

interested in whether the targeted ads for each of the candidates, Obama and McCain, had a

positive effect on their respective levels of support. In other words, we expect that Latinos

who were exposed to targeted Obama ads would be more likely to vote for Obama than

those not exposed to an advertisement; we hold the same expectation in reference to

McCain and his targeted ads. Looking first at the vote choice for Obama, we see that in the

control group the percent of Latinos voting for Obama is 66%. Each of the Obama ad cells

sees an increase in the percentage voting for Obama, but only two are statistically significant,

according to a difference of proportions test – both of the bilingual ads. The level of

support for Obama was 75% for both the Bilingual Miller and the Bilingual Mendoza

treatments, an increase of over ten percentage points. The trend among the different groups

indicated that Obama’s ethnically targeted ads that employed Spanish had a positive and

significant mobilizing effect on Latinos, however there was no added effect for the Mendoza

bilingual ad, and the English Mendoza had no statistically significant effect at all. These

results are presented in figure 3A.

[Insert figures 3A & 3B about here]

Turning to McCain we see a much lower baseline level of support, 17% of Latinos

voting for McCain in the control. Once again, we find a similar pattern where all of the

treatments register an increase in support for McCain but only the two bilingual ethnically

targeted McCain ads are statistically significant increases. McCain’s support increases to

22% in the bilingual Miller ad and 25% in the bilingual Mendoza ad (see figure 3B). The

English Mendoza ad did not have a statistically significant effect.

17

Page 18: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

Results for White Voters

Among non-Latino Whites a new pattern emerges with both the Obama and the

McCain ads. The generic “mainstream” ads have no statistically significant effect, and the

overtly ethnically targeted ads – bilingual Mendoza do not cause an increase or decrease in

support. However, the two remaining Latino targeted ads seem to backfire completely. The

English ad featuring a Latino endorser, and the bilingual ad featuring an Anglo endorser

both cause a demobilizing effect for White voters – across both candidates. For Obama ads,

his support among Whites starts at 70% in the control and decreases to 64% with both the

English Mendoza and the bilingual Miller treatments. These ads, which we describe as

“mixed message” ads do not resonate with White voters, and seem to go so far as to reduce

their level of support for the candidate. However, it is not outright hostility towards Latinos

driving this, but perhaps the sense of competition, or spillover that the mixed-message ad

creates. The overtly targeted ad has no negative effect, however that ad may be sidestepped

completely by the White voter, easily dismissing, or approving of it as a Latino targeted ad.

The mixed-message ads are not as easy to dismiss. With the control adjusted downwards

about 45 points for McCain, the exact same pattern emerges whereby the two mixed-

message ads lead to reduced support for McCain while the clearly targeted ad does not have

a negative effect. The fact that this pattern is consistent across both Obama and McCain ads

suggests this is not simply mixed partisan cues or expectations coloring the outcome, but

rather a deeper response to the Latino outreach ads that cross over into mainstream ads.

[Insert figures 4A & 4B about here]

18

Page 19: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

Results for Black Voters

For African Americans, the election of Barack Obama was truly historic. Blacks,

long the largest and most significant minority group in the United States were

hypermobilized by Obama’s candidacy. At the same time, there was a sense that Obama

would not be able to run as a “Black” candidate, for fear of inciting backlash from White

voters, so expectations were not overly high that Obama would run an aggressive Black

targeted campaign, in particular in television ads. However, Latinos were often discussed as

an important coalition partner, perhaps even a reluctant coalition partner, and thus

considerable attention would need to be paid to Latino outreach. How would African

American voters respond to Obama targeting Latino voters? Our results indicate a backlash

by Black voters to Latino targeted ads. Not surprising, in the control group, Obama

received 98% of the Black vote, and after viewing the four ads, each treatment group did

decrease in support for Obama. Even the “mainstream” English Miller ad saw a reduction

in support for Obama from 98% to 89%, though the drop was not statistically significant.

More telling were the sharp decreases witnessed after Black voters viewed the three Latino

targeted ads. The bilingual Miller ad saw support for Obama decrease to 81%, while the two

ads featuring Mendoza both saw support decrease further, down to 78%. The bilingual

Mendoza ad seemed to have the most negative effect whereby the votes lost by Obama were

transferred directly over to McCain. In contrast, in the other two Latino targeted ads a large

number transferred to the undecided or other category instead of directly to McCain. With

regard to the McCain ads, too few African Americans supported McCain to observe any

meaningful data across the different treatments.

[Insert figure 5 about here]

19

Page 20: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

Results for Asian Voters

Finally, we turn to Asian American voters. Asian voters in our dataset reflect the

diversity of the Asian American community in Southern California, with large proportions of

Chinese, Filipino, Korean, and Japanese ancestry. However, sample size considerations

prevent us from being able to drill down into each national origin group, and instead we

examine the data for Asian Americans as a collective minority. Nonetheless, some

interesting results were observed. With respect to the ads for Obama, the two bilingual ads

resulted in an eight point increase in support. Asians in the control group supported Obama

at 56% compared to 64% in each the bilingual Mendoza and the bilingual Miller treatments

(see figure 6). No change is noted in vote support in the English Mendoza or the English

Miller ads. Two possible explanations exist. First, skeptics may argue that Filipinos are

more likely to have an affinity for the Spanish language outreach as descendants of a former

Spanish colony where some Spanish words are common. Further, the name Mendoza is also

a commonly occurring Filipino surname. However this does not appear to be the

explanation for a couple reasons. First, Filipinos are only one part of the larger Asian

sample, approximately 20%, leaving 80% of non-Filipino Asians in the sample. Second, any

confusion over Mendoza should have also registered in the English Mendoza ad, which was

not statistically different from the control. Third, when we pull Filipinos out and estimate

Asian vote choice again, the same results are present. Instead, we think a deeper aspect is at

play related to shared minority status. In particular, Asians and Latinos share their status as

linguistic minorities who both have strong use of their native language. While the ads are

not Asian language outreach, they do represent an effort by the candidate to reach out in a

non-English language to a minority group. Perhaps some Asian voters see this as a future

20

Page 21: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

promise of outreach to their own community as the number and influence of their electorate

grows.

[Insert figure 6 about here]

Possible Mediating Effects

The results presented thus far are interesting, but only scratch the surface of what is

happening in terms of ethnically targeted campaign ads. Do all Latinos or Whites respond

the same, or do some mediating independent variables create significant cleavages in how

subgroups respond to the ads? In particular, we are interested in how factors related to race,

racial identity, and immigration may interact with the exposure to the treatment and create

the positive or negative mobilizing effects noted above.

For Latinos, our first cut at the analysis included language preference and ethnic

identity. Language usage is an obvious variable to consider given that the two targeted ads

that worked were both the Spanish/bilingual ads. It may be that Latinos who consume

Spanish media are driving this effect and it is simply a communication barrier, not

necessarily a symbolic mechanism in outreach. To this extent we subdivided the Latino

sample into those who reported usage of Spanish media (TV, Radio, Newspaper or Internet)

and those who reported no such Spanish media usage. For the Spanish dominant group the

bilingual ads performed well, leading to higher support for Obama than either of the two

English ads. However, the effect was even stronger for English dominant Latinos were the

increase over the control for the bilingual ads was over 12 points. Thus, regardless of

language usage, the bilingual ads were popular among Latinos. Likewise, we noted high

support for the two bilingual ads for Latinos across varying degrees of ethnic identity.

Understandably, the level of ethnic awareness and identity is naturally high among Latinos in

21

Page 22: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

Los Angeles, however comparing lower and higher ethnic identifiers, similarly high levels of

response to the bilingual ads was found.

Among Whites and Blacks more distinguishable patterns emerged. For Whites,

cutting the sample by their degree of racial identity, we find those who state their racial

identity is important witnessed a statistically significant decrease of 15 points in support for

Obama after viewing the mixed-message Latino ads. However, similar to the full sample,

even this subgroup of Whites did not have a negative reaction to the overtly Latino targeted

ad (see figure 7A). In contrast, Whites who stated their racial identity is not important to

them did not have a negative reaction to the mixed-message ads, in fact none of the

treatments were statistically different than the control among this subgroup.

[Insert figures 7A and 7B about here]

Among African Americans, attitudes towards immigration seem to have the largest

mediating effect on the Latino targeted treatments. When the Black sample is split in half,

those reporting below or above the mean on an immigrant feeling thermometer question,

significant differences emerge (see figures 8A/8B). Black voters who hold lower than

average feelings towards immigrants are driving the negative results for the two bilingual ads.

It seems the introduction of Spanish into the Obama ad creates discontent among those

who are anti-immigrant. Remarkably, the intended vote rate for Obama goes from a perfect

100% in the control to only 50% after viewing the bilingual Mendoza ad, for Black voters

with low opinions of immigrants. Meanwhile, African American who have a positive view of

immigrants show no significant decrease in support for Obama after viewing the bilingual

ads.

[Insert figures 8A and 8B about here]

22

Page 23: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

Discussion

The Latino targeted advertisement treatments do not have a uniformly positive or

negative effect across our sample. An understanding of these effects requires the

differentiation of racial and ethnic sub-groups; a non-Latino grouping is too blunt of a

categorization and the examination of Latino targeted effects requires disaggregation not

only based on ethnicity but also race. Non-Latinos is simply too heterogeneous of a

category and based only on this category we would conclude that the Obama targeted ads

had no effect on non-Latinos and that only one of McCain’s targeted ads had a negative

effect.

A different picture emerges once we break down non-Latinos into racial sub-groups.

A clear pattern of the negative effect of ethnically targeted ads emerges. This effect is most

visible among blacks, but is also seen among Whites with mixed-message ads. The ad

treatment that had a consistently statistically negative effect was the bilingual Miller

treatment. The vote shares for both Obama and McCain among whites decreased in moving

from the control to their respective targeted bilingual Miller treatment. Among blacks,

Obama’s bilingual Miller ad also had a negative effect. Based on this analysis we can

tentatively conclude that ethnically targeted ads will not have a positive effect on the non-

targeted group. The expectation should instead be narrowed down to either null or negative

effects.

The bilingual ads had a different effect when we look at Latinos. Obama’s bilingual

ads were both significant, seeing close to a 10 percentage increase in support for Obama.

Among Latinos, McCain’s bilingual treatments also had a small mobilizing effect. Our initial

hypothesis that ethnically targeted ads would have a positive effect on Latinos is partially

supported. Not all of the ethnically targeted ads have a positive effect. In fact, the bilingual

23

Page 24: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

ads seem to be the only ones resonating with Latinos, however we did not find any negative

effects with ethnically targeted ads.

It is interesting that the most consistently significant treatment across groups was not

the most ethnically targeted, combining both Spanish and a Latino endorser. Instead the ad

treatment that featured Miller in a bilingual narrative elicited the most consistent and

significant effect. Across groups the language of the ad appears to be the most mobilizing or

demobilizing aspect. Close to half of the Latinos in the sample are English dominant, 30%

are Spanish dominant, and the remainder bilingual. Based only on the main effects we can

initially conclude that the effectiveness of the Bilingual ad was not so much a result of

comprehension facilitation but of targeting based on linguistic symbolism. We posit that the

cultural symbol of Spanish exerts a powerful mobilizing effect among Latinos.

Among non-Latinos the bilingual Mendoza ad (Obama), the most ethnically targeted

ad, only had an effect among African Americans. The lack of a larger backlash effect from

non-Latinos suggests a number of possible explanations. First, an ad that is mixed—either a

combination of Latino content or Spanish—may appear as pandering while an ad that is

both targeted in its language and content may appear as a more genuine multicultural

outreach effort. These are just two of many potential explanations of why different types

and levels of ethnically targeted ad treatments differing effects across groups and by

candidates.

The other consistent effect across groups was that the non-targeted English Miller

ads did not have any effect. A crude take home point for a campaign strategist based on

this analysis would be that trying to kill two birds with one stone is not possible. The

general ads are not focused enough to have an effect on any group, while the ethnically

24

Page 25: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

targeted ads can elicit support among Latinos but then this comes at the cost of a negative

backlash among certain non-Latino segments.

Conclusion

In the closing weeks of this election cycle, the Latino mass outreach was capped by a

unique political ad where the Democratic candidate for president, Barack Obama fully

conveyed the message himself and did so in Spanish. Did Obama’s time and effort in

rehearsing his message in Spanish actually pay off? Based on the survey experiment we

conducted it is very possible that the symbolic gesture of using Spanish to communicate his

message potentially had a positive effect in increasing Obama’s support. While McCain did

not narrate a complete Spanish language ad, his use of bilingual and translated ads also likely

had positive effects among the Latino electorate. However, these Spanish language and

bilingual ads if seen by non-Latinos had the potential of hurting of each of the candidates.

Campaigns seek to maximize their effectiveness while minimizing their cost and

effort. As a result of the diversity of the present day electorate one-size-fits all messages are

no longer feasible and candidates must engage in increasing micro-targeting efforts. Latino

targeted messages as seen in the above experiment results and recent studies do in fact have

positive results. Candidates seeking out the support of Latinos can effectively reach this

group through careful targeting but they must also be vigilant that exposure to these ads is

limited in its reach beyond Latinos. A key set of considerations of ethnically targeted ads,

which we did not consider here but will do so in the next iteration of our analysis is that of

individual level racial/ethnic identity. Latinos vary in their level of ethnic group

identification and this variance is likely to moderate the receipt of Latino targeted messages.

At the same time non-Latinos vary in their own racial group identity and feelings of

25

Page 26: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

ethnocentrism both of which we must also include.

26

Page 27: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

Table 1: Experimental Design

McCain ObamaEnglish/Anglo Harold Miller

English; no Latino appeals Harold Miller

English; no Latino appeals Bilingual/Anglo Harold Miller

Bilingual; Latino appeals Harold Miller

Bilingual; Latino appeals English/Latino Hector Mendoza

English; no Latino appeal Hector Mendoza

English; no Latino appeal Bilingual/Latino Hector Mendoza;

Latino appeals Hector Mendoza; Latino appeals

Control No ad given No ad given

27

Page 28: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

Figure 1: Percentage identifying Miller or Mendoza as Endorsing Obama and McCain, by Experimental condition

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

80

90

Miller condition Not Miller Condition Mendoza Condition Not Mendoza Condition

Experimental Condition

ObamaMcCain

28

Page 29: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

Figure 2: Mean Importance Candidate Place on Issues, by Condition

3

4

5

6

7

Education Economy Health Care Immigration

Obama ControlObama AdMcCain ControlMcCain Ad

29

Page 30: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

Figure 3A

Vote for Obama by Treatment - LATINOS

73.5

69.9

66.7

75.275.1

62

64

66

68

70

72

74

76

BO Bi MillerBO Bi MendozaBO En MillerBO En MendozaControl ( Pr=.771 ) ( Pr=.297 ) ( Pr=.020) ( Pr=.051)

Figure 3B

Vote for McCain by Treatment - LATINOS

21.7

24.7

18.317.8

21.1

12

14

16

18

20

22

24

26

JM Bi MillerJM Bi MendozaJM En MillerJM En MendozaControl ( Pr=.272 ) ( Pr=.513 ) ( Pr=.076) ( Pr=.087)

30

Page 31: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

Figure 4A

Vote for Obama by Treatment - WHITES

70.1

69.2

64.664.2

70.5

62

64

66

68

70

72

BO Bi MillerBO Bi MendozaBO En MillerBO En MendozaControl ( Pr=.087 ) ( Pr=.519 ) ( Pr=.228) ( Pr=.096)

Figure 4B Vote for McCain by Treatment - WHITES

21.8

26.8

20.4

25.125.4

16

18

20

22

24

26

28

JM Bi MillerJM Bi MendozaJM En MillerJM En MendozaControl ( Pr=.032 ) ( Pr=.256 ) ( Pr=.087) ( Pr=.066)

31

Page 32: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

Figure 5

Vote for Obama by Treatment - BLACKS

81.3

77.7

88.8

98.0

78.2

72

76

80

84

88

92

96

100

BO Bi MillerBO Bi MendozaBO En MillerBO En MendozaControl ( Pr=.051 ) ( Pr=.255 ) ( Pr=.068) ( Pr=.089)

Figure 6

Vote for Obama by Treatment - ASIANS

64.164.1

57.5

53.6

56.3

50

52

54

56

58

60

62

64

66

BO Bi MillerBO Bi MendozaBO En MillerBO En MendozaControl ( Pr=.684 ) ( Pr=.116 ) ( Pr=.084) ( Pr=.077)

32

Page 33: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

Figure 7A

Vote for Obama by Treatment - WHITES{ Who state their racial identity is important }

71.5

52.2

69.5

53.8

67.2

48

52

56

60

64

68

72

76

BO Bi MillerBO Bi MendozaBO En MillerBO En MendozaControl

Figure 7B

Vote for Obama by Treatment - WHITES{ Who state their racial identity is NOT important }

68.669.669.171.671.8

48

52

56

60

64

68

72

76

BO Bi MillerBO Bi MendozaBO En MillerBO En MendozaControl

33

Page 34: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

Figure 8A

Vote for Obama by Treatment - BLACKS{ Who have below average rating of Immigrants }

50.0

88.9100.0

87.5

66.7

40

50

60

70

80

90

100

BO Bi MillerBO Bi MendozaBO En MillerBO En MendozaControl

Figure 8B

Vote for Obama by Treatment - BLACKS{ Who have above average rating of immigrants }

70.0

100.090.9

100.0 100.0

40

50

60

70

80

90

100

BO Bi MillerBO Bi MendozaBO En MillerBO En MendozaControl

34

Page 35: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

Appendix: Script Senator Barack Obama/John McCain, he’s a once-in-a-generation leader/UN LIDER UNICO, with a bold agenda. In times like these, we need a leader who is willing to make the right decisions to lead our country back to prosperity. Images: Picture of candidate alone and in crowd That is why Harold Miller/Hector Mendoza, president of the National/Latino Advocacy Alliance, endorses Barack Obama/John McCain for President. Image 3: Picture of endorser alone with title An increasing number of children/Latino children can’t read at their grade level. So every child can succeed, he’ll fight for education reforms hailed as the most sweeping in a decade/ LUCHARA PARA REFORMAS EDUCATIVAS Images: School House, Children in Class Senator Obama/McCain has a history of reaching across the aisle and will do so to tackle the pressing problems that confront our community/Latino community/nuestra comunidad/nuestra comunidad Latina such as soaring health care costs, predatory mortgage lenders, and our dependence on foreign oil. Images: Congress in session, Gas Prices, home foreclosures, oil rig, As President, Senator Obama/McCain, will fight for all the people/PARA TODOS, not the powerful or the special interests. Image 16: Candidate in crowd, Group of people Keeping the best solutions, doing what’s right. HE’S ON OUR SIDE/(EL ESTA A NUESTRO LADO). That is why Harold Miller/Hector Mendoza supports Barack Obama/John McCain for President. Images: Candidate with workers, candidate alone

35

Page 36: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

References Aaker, Jennifer, Anne Brumbaugh, and Sonya Grier. 2000. “Nontarget Markets and Viewer

Distinctiveness: The Impact of Target Marketing on Advertising Attitudes.” Journal of Consumer Psychology. 9(3): 127-140.

Ansolabehere, Stephen, Shanto Iyengar, Adam Simon, and Nicholas Valentino. 1994. “Does

Attack Advertising Demobilize the Electorate?” American Political Science Review 88: 829-38.

Ansolabehere, Stephen, and Shanto Iyengar. 1995. Going Negative: How Political Advertisements

Shrink and Polarize the Electorate. New York: The Free Press. Ansolabehere, Stephen, Shanto Iyengar, and Adam Simon. 1999. “Replicating Experiments

Using Aggregate and Survey Data: The Case of Negative Advertising and Turnout.” American Political Science Review 93: 901-909.

Brians, Craig Leonard and Martin P. Wattenberg. 1996. “Campaign Issue Knowledge and

Salience: Comparing Reception from TV Commercials, TV News and Newspapers.” American Journal of Political Science. 40(1):172-193.

Brumbaugh, Anne. 2002. “Source and Nonsource Cues in Advertising and Their Effects on

the Activation of Cultural and Subcultural Knowledge on the Route to Persuasion.” Journal of Consumer Research. 29(Sept): 258-269.

DeFrancesco Soto, Victoria M. and Jennifer L. Merolla. 2006. “Vota por tu Futuro: Partisan

Mobilization of Latino Voters in the 2000 Presidential Election.” Political Behavior 28:285–304

DeFrancesco Soto, Victoria 2006. “Partisanship with Accents: When Does Ethnicity Trump

Partisanship?” Paper presented at American Political Science Association annual conference. Philadelphia, PA.

Forehand, Mark and Rohit Desphandé. 2001. “What We See Makes Us Who We Are:

Priming Ethnic Self-Awareness and Advertising Response.” Journal of Marketing Research. 38(Aug): 336-348.

Fraga, Luis R. and David Leal. 2004. “Playing the ‘Latino Card’: Race, Ethnicity, and

National Party Politics,” Du Bois Review: Social Science Research on Race, v1: 297-317. Fraga, Luis R. 1988. “Domination through democratic means: Nonpartisan slating groups in city electoral politics.” Urban Affairs Quaterly. 23(4): 528-555. Jamieson, Kathleen Hall. 1992. Dirty Politics: Deception, Distraction, and Democracy. New York:

Oxford University Press. Mendelberg, Tali. 2001. The Race Card: Campaign Strategy, Implicit Messages, and the Norm of

Equality. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

36

Page 37: Bulls Eye or Ricochet? Ethnically Targeted Campaign Ads in

Nuño, Stephen. 2007. “Latino Mobilization and Vote Choice in the 2000 Presidential

Election.” American Politics Research. 35 (March): 273-293. Ramírez, Ricardo. 2005. “Giving Voice to Latino Voters: A Field Experiment on the

Effectiveness of a National Non-Partisan Mobilization Effort.” Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science. 601:66-84.

Ramírez, Ricardo. 2007. “Segmented Mobilization: Latino Non-Partisan Get Out The Vote

Efforts in the 2000 General Election.” American Politics Research. 35(2):155-175.. Segal, Adam. 2006. Total Spanish-Language TV Spending by Market in the 2004 Presidential

Election. Baltimore: Hispanic Voter Project, Johns Hopkins University. Shaw, Daron, de la Garza, Rodolfo O. Lee, Jongho. 2000. “Examining Latino turnout in

1996: a three-state, validated survey approach (California, Florida, Texas).” American Journal of Political Science. 44:2 338-46.

Valentino, Nicholas A., Vincent L. Hutchings, and Ismail K. White. 2002. “Cues that Matter:

How Political Ads Prime Racial Attitudes during Campaigns.” American Political Science Review 96: 75-90.

Valentino, Nicholas A. 1999. “Crime News and the Priming of Racial Attitudes During Evaluations of the President.” Public Opinion Quarterly. 63: 293-320. Wattenberg, Martin P. and Craig Leonard Brians. 1999. “Negative Campaign Advertising:

Demobilizer or Mobilizer?” American Political Science Review. 93(4):891-899. White, Ismail K. 2007. “When Race Matters and When It Doesn’t: Racial Group Differences

in Response to Racial Cues.” American Political Science Review. 101(May): 339-354.

37