attitudes to animals: views in animal...

21
ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFARE Attitudes to Animals provides a foundation that the reader can use to make ethical choices about animals. It will challenge readers to question their current views, attitudes and perspectives on animals and the nature of the human–animal relationship. Human perspectives on the human–animal relationships re ect what we have learned, together with spoken and unspoken attitudes and assump- tions, from our families, society, media, education and employment. This thought-provoking book will ask what it is to be human, what to be animal, and what is the nature of relationship between them. These ques- tions are addressed through philosophical and ethical discussions, scien- ti c evidence and dynamic theoretical approaches. Attitudes to Animals will also encourage us to think not only of our relationships to non-human animals, but also those with other, human, animals. . has taught psychology and psychobiology at Centre College in Danville, Kentucky. In addition to a long-term interest in animal welfare issues, her research has focused on spatial cognition and foraging in non-human primates. She has worked with captive primate populations in the United Kingdom and the United States, and has also conducted eld research in Peru, Costa Rica and Madagascar. Currently, Francine Dolins is the Program O cer for Research in the Animal Research Issues Section of The Humans Society of the United States.

Upload: vannhu

Post on 29-Jun-2018

214 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFAREassets.cambridge.org/97805214/73422/sample/9780521473422web.pdf · ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFARE Edited by ... Franc¸oise

job:LAY00 27-10-1998 page:1 colour:1 black–text

ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS:VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFARE

Attitudes to Animals provides a foundation that the reader can use to makeethical choices about animals. It will challenge readers to question theircurrent views, attitudes and perspectives on animals and the nature of thehuman–animal relationship.

Human perspectives on the human–animal relationships reflect what wehave learned, together with spoken and unspoken attitudes and assump-tions, from our families, society, media, education and employment.This thought-provoking book will ask what it is to be human, what to beanimal, and what is the nature of relationship between them. These ques-tions are addressed through philosophical and ethical discussions, scien-tific evidence and dynamic theoretical approaches. Attitudes to Animalswill also encourage us to think not only of our relationships to non-humananimals, but also those with other, human, animals.

. has taught psychology and psychobiology atCentre College in Danville, Kentucky. In addition to a long-term interestin animal welfare issues, her research has focused on spatial cognition andforaging in non-human primates. She has worked with captive primatepopulations in the United Kingdom and the United States, and has alsoconducted field research in Peru, Costa Rica and Madagascar. Currently,Francine Dolins is the Program Officer for Research in the AnimalResearch Issues Section of The Humans Society of the United States.

Page 2: ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFAREassets.cambridge.org/97805214/73422/sample/9780521473422web.pdf · ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFARE Edited by ... Franc¸oise

job:LAY00 27-10-1998 page:2 colour:1 black–text

For Christopher – for sharing his joy in all that is life

Page 3: ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFAREassets.cambridge.org/97805214/73422/sample/9780521473422web.pdf · ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFARE Edited by ... Franc¸oise

job:LAY00 27-10-1998 page:3 colour:1 black–text

ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS:VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFARE

Edited by

FRANCINE L. DOLINS

Page 4: ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFAREassets.cambridge.org/97805214/73422/sample/9780521473422web.pdf · ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFARE Edited by ... Franc¸oise

job:LAY00 27-10-1998 page:4 colour:1 black–text

The Pitt Building, Trumpington Street, Cambridge, United Kingdom

The Edinburgh Building, Cambridge CB2 2RU, UK http://wwww.cup.cam.ac.uk

40 West 20th Street, New York, NY 10011-4211, USA http://wwww.cup.org10 Stamford Road, Oakleigh, Melbourne 3166, Australia

© Cambridge University Press 1999

This book is in copyright. Subject to statutory exceptionand to the provisions of relevant collective licensing agreements,

no reproduction of any part may take place withoutthe written permission of Cambridge University Press

First published in 1999

Printed in the United Kingdom at the University Press, Cambridge

Typeset in Times New Roman 10/13pt []

A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library

Library of Congress Cataloguing in Publication Data

Dolins, Francine L. (Francine Leigh), 1964–Attitudes to animals: views in animal welfare/Francine L. Dolins

p. cm.Includes bibliographical references and index.

ISBN 0 521 47342 X. – ISBN 0 521 47906 1 (pbk.)1. Animal welfare – moral and ethical aspects. 2. Human–animal relationships – moral

and ethical aspects. I. TitleHV4708.D63 1999

[email protected]—dc21 97-32155 CIP

ISBN 0 521 47342 X hardbackISBN 0 521 47906 I paperback

Page 5: ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFAREassets.cambridge.org/97805214/73422/sample/9780521473422web.pdf · ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFARE Edited by ... Franc¸oise

job:LAY00 27-10-1998 page:5 colour:1 black–text

Contents

List of contributors page viiAcknowledgements ix

Part I Attitudes to animals1 A look back in the mirror: perspectives on animals and ethics

Francine L. Dolins 32 Me and my totem: cross-cultural attitudes towards animals

Phyllis Passariello 123 Sheep in wolves’ clothing? Attitudes to animals among farmers

and scientists James A. Serpell 26

Part II Animal awareness4 The problem of animal subjectivity and its consequences for

the scientific measurement of animal sufferingFrancoise Wemelsfelder 37

5 Environmental enrichment and impoverishment:neurophysiological effects Susan D. Healy andMartin J. Tovee 54

6 The behavioural requirements of farm animals for psychologicalwell-being and survival Robert J. Young 77

7 Personality and the happiness of the chimpanzeeJames E. King 101

8 Primate cognition: evidence for the ethical treatment ofprimates Richard W. Byrne 114

Part III Animal welfare9 Animal welfare: the concept of the issues Donald M. Broom 129

v

Page 6: ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFAREassets.cambridge.org/97805214/73422/sample/9780521473422web.pdf · ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFARE Edited by ... Franc¸oise

job:LAY00 27-10-1998 page:6 colour:1 black–text

10 New perspectives on the design and management of captiveanimal environments David Shepherdson 143

11 Should we let them go? Mary Midgley 152

Part IV Research and education12 Humane education: the role of animal-based learning

Andrew J. Petto and Karla D. Russell 16713 ‘Minding animals’: the role of animals in children’s mental

development M. Patricia Hindley 18614 Alternatives to using animals in education David Dewhurst 20015 Animals in scientific education and a reverence for life

Henk Verhoog 218

Part V Epilogue: the future of wild animals16 Human sentiment and the future of wildlife David E. Cooper 23117 In the absence of animals: power and impotence in our dealings

with endangered animals Charles Bergman 244

Index 258

vi Contents

Page 7: ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFAREassets.cambridge.org/97805214/73422/sample/9780521473422web.pdf · ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFARE Edited by ... Franc¸oise

job:LAY00 27-10-1998 page:7 colour:1 black–text

Contributors

Charles BergmanDepartment of English, Pacific Lutheran University, Tacoma, Washington,WA 98477, USA

Donald M. BroomDepartment of Clinical Veterinary Medicine, University of Cambridge,Madingley Road, Cambridge CB3 0ES, UK

Richard W. ByrneScottish Primate Research Group, School of Psychology, University of StAndrews, St Andrews, Fife KY16 9JU, UK

David E. CooperDepartment of Philosophy, University of Durham, 50 Old Elvet Road,Durham DH1 3HN, UK

David DewhurstFaculty of Health and Environment, Leeds Metropolitan University, Calver-ley Street, Leeds LS1 3HE, UK

Francine L. DolinsThe Humane Society of the United States, Animal Issues Research Section,Animal Issues Research Section, 2100 L Street, NW, Washington, DC20037, USA

Susan D. HealyDepartment of Psychology, University of Newcastle-upon-Tyne, Newcastle-upon-Tyne NE1 7RU UK

M. Patricia HindleySchool of Communication, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, British Col-umbia, V5A 1S6, Canada

vii

Page 8: ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFAREassets.cambridge.org/97805214/73422/sample/9780521473422web.pdf · ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFARE Edited by ... Franc¸oise

job:LAY00 27-10-1998 page:8 colour:1 black–text

Mary Midgley1a Collingwood Terrace, Newcastle-upon-Tyne NE2 2JP, UK

James E. KingDepartment of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ 85721, USA

Mary MidgleyPhyllis PassarielloAnthropology Program, Centre College, Danville, 600 West Walnut Street,Danville, KY 40422, USA

Andrew J. PettoCenter for Science Education, PO Box 8880 National, Madison, WI 53708-8880, USA

Karla D. RussellRRI, Box 87, Intervale, NH 03845, USA

James A. SerpellDepartment of Clinical Studies, School of Veterinary Medicine, Universityof Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104, USA

David ShepherdsonMetro Washington Park Zoo, Portland, Oregon, OR 97221, USA

Martin J. ToveeDepartment of Psychology, University of Newcastle-upon-Tyne, Newcastle-upon-Tyne NE1 7RU, UK

Henk VerhoogTheoretical Biology Division, Leiden University, Kaiserstraat 63, 2311 GPLeiden PO Box 9516, 2300 RA Leiden, The Netherlands

Francoise WemelsfelderGenetics and Behavioural Sciences Department, Scottish Agricultural Col-lege, Bush Estate, Penicuik EH26 0QE, UK

Robert J. YoungSchool of Agriculture and Horticulture, De Montfort University, Caythorpe,Grantham, Lincolnshire NG32 3EP, UK

viii Contributors

Page 9: ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFAREassets.cambridge.org/97805214/73422/sample/9780521473422web.pdf · ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFARE Edited by ... Franc¸oise

job:LAY00 27-10-1998 page:9 colour:1 black–text

Acknowledgements

There are many people both professionally and personally whom I wouldlike to acknowledge and extend my deepest thanks to in helping to makethis book possible. First, I would like to thank each of the contributorswho gave their time and energy in researching and writing their chapters. Iwould like to thank our editor at Cambridge University Press, Dr TraceySanderson, who has shown the greatest creativity and patience in dealingwith every aspect of the production of this book. I would like to acknowl-edge the work of Ms Mary Sanders, who copy-edited this volume withgreat diligence. And, I would especially like to acknowledge the guidanceand assistance that Dr Francoise Wemelsfelder has generously shown increating this volume, through her insight, thoughts and wisdom aboutanimal behaviour and welfare, and her friendship. Dr Liz Williamson wasan inspiration that one could ‘act’ positively to create changes for thewelfare of animals and humans alike; she inspires through example, asdoes Dr Hannah Buchanan-Smith.

During the planning stages when this project was just an idea, Jean-Marie Stassin and his small coterie of creatures (Kilda, Samson, Nenni,Penther) living at Ash Cottage provided much support and encourage-ment, as did dear friends and colleagues – Emma Clery, Sanjoy Roy, AnitaRoy, Jim Gardner and Gavin Baxter, Marion Smith, Sandra Gibb, Mari-on Dow, Betty Bruce, Mamie Prentice, Rosie Wilson, Dario Floreano,Lynn Hanley, Vickie Glidewell, Pratap Rughani, Paul Garber, Jim Kingand Christopher Klimowicz. I am also deeply grateful to Merelyn Dolinsand Eva Trupin, who have great faith in me. Philip Trupin will alwaysremain as an inspiration, especially when it comes to thinking aboutanimals.

Finally, I would like to acknowledge my deepest thanks to the animalsfor whom this book is really written. Many animals, as individuals, have

ix

Page 10: ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFAREassets.cambridge.org/97805214/73422/sample/9780521473422web.pdf · ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFARE Edited by ... Franc¸oise

job:LAY00 27-10-1998 page:10 colour:1 black–text

touched my life: as a token of the trusting relationships that have beenbestowed on me and the entrance into their ‘mysteries’ that I have beenallowed to observe, I wanted this book to exist. In my education, I have‘used’ animals in studying them. This relationship has always left meuneasy: an animal is not a book of verse to be opened and shut at will. Theanimal exists beyond the classroom hours, laboratory work and research –it behaves, lives, dies. I was and am determined that students’ education infuture times will not be fraught with similar decisions based on supposedlymutually exclusive alternatives of whether to pass a course or fail byaligning themselves with certain beliefs about animals and the meaning oftheir lives. An education can promote both ethical and scientific thinkingsimultaneously; they are definitely not mutually exclusive concepts. Thus,this book emerged to assist students with their own beliefs about howanimals should be treated in education, in research, and in our society, andthis book emerged to assist those who teach courses on ethics and animalwelfare. Those who love animals are students of animals’ nature, and Ihope that this book enlightens those outside of the structured educationalestablishments as well.

We obtained joy in creating this book, but the ultimate joy will occurwhen we no longer have the need to write books such as these, whenanimals as separate entities, in their separate existences, are acknowledgedin their respectful place.

x Acknowledgements

Page 11: ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFAREassets.cambridge.org/97805214/73422/sample/9780521473422web.pdf · ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFARE Edited by ... Franc¸oise

job:LAY01 27-10-1998 page:1 colour:1 black–text

Part IAttitudes to animals

Page 12: ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFAREassets.cambridge.org/97805214/73422/sample/9780521473422web.pdf · ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFARE Edited by ... Franc¸oise

job:LAY01 27-10-1998 page:2 colour:1 black–text

MMMM

Page 13: ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFAREassets.cambridge.org/97805214/73422/sample/9780521473422web.pdf · ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFARE Edited by ... Franc¸oise

job:LAY01 27-10-1998 page:3 colour:1 black–text

1A look back in the mirror: perspectives on

animals and ethics

FRANCINE L. DOLINS

A bundle of grey and gold hair, three Eulemur fulvus rufus, the red-frontedbrown lemur, were huddled on an angled branch at least 50 feet abovewhere I sat. Most lemur species are highly endangered; I watched thesefulvus, recognizing that their fate relied not on their survival by findingfood and shelter when necessary, but on the fate of a conservation projectwhose success precariously balances between various interconnected pro-grammes of education, health, research, eco-tourism, and foreign invest-ment. This delicate balancing act attempts to keep itself upright amidst anonslaught of human needs, desires, and at times, greed.

Lemurs are endemic only in Madagascar. This makes it a special placefor those who are concerned about the conservation and welfare of wildspecies of non-human primates. Yet, for those humans who inhabit thisland where the forest is their primary resource for survival, their needscome into direct conflict with the lemurs, a westernized version of a valued‘flagship’ species. The Malagasy who live in rural areas depend almostentirely on the land, what they can grow and what they can extract from it.In the cities, the people are dependent on those who have grown andextracted from the land. Their immediate survival needs depend on landand forest clearance, and are not always in parallel with those of the manyspecies of lemurs. The lemurs’ state of conservation, however, may be oflittle consequence when human poverty and sickness are more than appar-ent. This example, when the needs of human animals come into conflictwith those of non-human animals, is just one type of relationship in whichhumans have come to find themselves; just one side of a multi-faceted issuereflecting the many possibilities of human relationships to non-humananimals. There are many other ways that humans relate to animals, such asby keeping them as pets, subjects in experiments, objects of study orreverence, the basis for subsistence, or financial gain.

3

Page 14: ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFAREassets.cambridge.org/97805214/73422/sample/9780521473422web.pdf · ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFARE Edited by ... Franc¸oise

job:LAY01 27-10-1998 page:4 colour:1 black–text

In a recently published book, The Frankenstein Syndrome: Ethical andSocial Issues in the Genetic Engineering of Animals, the well-known ethicalphilosopher, Bernard Rollin (1995), made the important point that mostpeople consider ethical issues to be ‘dilemmas’. A dilemma, according toRollin is ‘a situation or question where there are two and only two choicesor answers, both of which lead to unsatisfactory conclusions’ (1995, p. 9).Luckily, most ethical issues are not dilemmas, but are matters of choice(Rollin, 1995). An important point made by Rollin (1995) is that, indealing with most ethical issues, we are in the process of choosing from aselection of many possible choices. Moreover, any choice made, whetherinformed or not, will have impact. The purpose of this book, Attitudes toAnimals, is to inform in order to provide a foundation from which to judgein making ethical choices regarding animals. Most importantly, this bookshould prompt the reader to question their current views, attitudes, andperspectives from which their long-held beliefs may have been derived.

The two threads of this narrative so far can be understood as theintertwined fates of animals and humans as a consequence of ethicalchoices. Underlying these choices is the nature of the human–animalrelationship and how (understood only from the humans’ point of view),this relationship developed. Human perspectives on the human–animalrelationship reflect what has been learned, both spoken as well as un-spoken attitudes and assumptions, from our families, society, media,education, and in our type of employment. For instance, in James Serpell’schapter, ‘Sheep in wolves’ clothing? Attitudes to animals among farmersand scientists’, he discusses how agricultural workers employ distancingmechanisms in relationship to the animals they tend from birth untilslaughter, as an answer to their ethical choice. Likewise, Serpell illustrateshow scientists and technicians working with animals in laboratories typi-cally use similar psychological devices, also compensating for the sacrificeof ‘the many’ by keeping a few animals as ‘mascots’. This portrays thedifficult emotional states of animal ‘exploiters’ in relation to the animalsthey tend, as well as their response to public criticism and pressure.

As a primatologist, I am often asked why I study primate behaviour andhow the information I learn is useful for improving the lives of humans.Underlying these questions are certain implicit assumptions. The first isthat non-human primates are not worthy of consideration in their ownright; and the second assumption is that non-human animal life has nointrinsic value because the world was created for humans’ use. Theseassumptions maintain a human-centred perspective. The origins of thesebeliefs, however, are not necessarily pervasive across all peoples, but rather

4 F. L. Dolins

Page 15: ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFAREassets.cambridge.org/97805214/73422/sample/9780521473422web.pdf · ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFARE Edited by ... Franc¸oise

job:LAY01 27-10-1998 page:5 colour:1 black–text

arise from particular concepts present in particular cultures. In the chapterby Phyllis Passariello, ‘Me and my totem: cross-cultural attitudes towardsanimals’, perspectives on the human–animal relationship are examinedthrough an anthropological looking glass. As examples of humans’ per-spectives towards animals and our shaping of this long relationship, Pas-sariello discusses the domestication of animals and totemism. What char-acteristics have we idealized and shaped in generations of animals, andwhat characteristics of animals have we taken on, to wear or to hidebehind, as disguises? As Passariello states, ‘Attitudes, of course, do notfossilize’, and so our study across cultures is a window on our personal andcultural evolution of attitudes towards animals: the tension between thecultural and the natural. It is, of course, the attitudes that we eschew thatcreate the foundations for our choices.

Considering and even producing animals’ existence merely for our ownuse suggests that we have taken on the role of ‘creator’, ‘protector’, or‘steward’, tending and preserving nature for our own purposes. Controlover animals is one consequence of our anthropocentric attitude towardanimals. Humans have exerted their control over what was once wildlife, inzoos, sanctuaries, private menageries, and in laboratories for conserva-tion, breeding, cloning, as well as for experimental purposes, respectively.Our goal has been, and is, to maintain this control in order to preserveendangered species. This is even true in the face of the likelihood that it wasour control in the first place that has brought many species to the very edgeof their existence. The problem that now faces us is how to keep endan-gered species from disappearing. The answer eludes us, as we can only butwatch many species of animals and plants head toward extinction.

Do some humans have the right to maintain control over animals’ livesin order to save them from other humans and/or from extinction? CharlesBergman’s chapter ‘In the absence of animals: power and impotence in ourdealings with endangered animals’ investigates this thorny issue of controlover animal life, and where it may lead us and them. He examines theplight of the last wild California condors as an example. David Cooper’schapter ‘Human sentiment and the future of wildlife’ takes the argumentof natural preservation from yet another perspective. Wildlife manage-ment exemplified our concern for animals’ continuing presence on earth.Cooper discusses human concerns about the rights of animals to exist,highlighting the four reasons that dominate human concerns: the right ofindividual animals, the right of a species, the benefit to humans, and thebenefit to the ecosystem. The chapter concludes that the final realistic (notidealistic) solution to the future of wildlife is to create and maintain

5A look in the mirror

Page 16: ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFAREassets.cambridge.org/97805214/73422/sample/9780521473422web.pdf · ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFARE Edited by ... Franc¸oise

job:LAY01 27-10-1998 page:6 colour:1 black–text

‘relative’ wildernesses which are inhabited by managed wild animals. Thereader is called upon to view this solution not as second best, but as apragmatic means which allows for human interactions with animals andfor species survival. Under these circumstances Cooper considers this to bea limited interaction, although one which provides an enriching life experi-ence. Mary Midgley’s chapter ‘Should we let them go?’ takes yet anotherperspective on human control over animals. She discusses control inrelation to the ethical problem of whether it is correct to keep animals incaptivity at all, and if so, in what manner. Thus, taken together, thesechapters provide various and at times contradictory views on the subject ofhuman control over animal lives and destinies.

Some radical animal rights groups suggest that we should limit or ceaseour interaction with animals altogether, for animals’ benefit. They ques-tion whether it is necessary for humans to interfere with animals at all intheir daily lives. However, if we do limit these experiences, are we limitingour development as human beings as well? Patricia Hindley’s chapter‘ ‘‘Minding animals’’: the role of animals in children’s mental development’deals with the issues of human–animal interactions as it affects the devel-opment of a moral self. Correspondingly, Andrew Petto and Karla Rus-sell, in their chapter entitled ‘Humane education: the role of animal-basedlearning’ explore the challenge of educating children humanely usingconcern for animals as the basis for ethical behaviour. They do notprescribe a set of rules but invite creative means by which to achieve apathway to this goal. Petto and Russell define the aims in two ways. Thefirst is to have the learning process provide the opportunity to understandthe ‘natural’ and the ‘cultural’ animal. Secondly, they suggest modifyingteaching methods with a view towards developing humane attitudes inchildren about the natural lives of animals, the effects of captivity, theirrole in the ecosystem, and resultant costs and benefits to both humans andanimals. Similarly, David Dewhurst’s chapter, ‘Alternatives to using ani-mals in education’ is devoted to viable alternative methods for teachingbiology to students while maintaining humane standards, ethical responsi-bility, and respect for animal life.

Through these chapters, I am reminded of a conversation in which acolleague bemoaned the fact that, once we begin the study of any system,we irrevocably alter that system. He explained that, once we imposemeasurements or observations even with careful regard, we cannot knowwith complete certainty the original state of that system. This, my col-league argued, was the irony of science: that we can never know what asystem was like prior to our study of it (also see Mackenzie, 1997).

6 F. L. Dolins

Page 17: ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFAREassets.cambridge.org/97805214/73422/sample/9780521473422web.pdf · ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFARE Edited by ... Franc¸oise

job:LAY01 27-10-1998 page:7 colour:1 black–text

Similarly, our interactions with animals will invariably create changes intheir lives, which may be for better or for worse. And, our study of animalswill create changes in our human lives, which may ultimately prove to bean enriching process, or not, depending on our subjective perspectives andon the framework through which we perceive and define our experiences.

The scientific view of animals, from the influence of Behaviourism, isone of objectivism, ignoring the subjective qualities of animals altogether.However, this perspective often leads to animals being treated as ‘blackboxes’, responding purely to environmental stimuli without reason orsentience. In this manner, the question of animal subjectivity affectingtheir actions and behaviour was not taken into account in scientificthought and methodology. Currently, scientific perspectives on animalsare being re-examined. Henk Verhoog’s chapter, ‘Animals in scientificeducation and a reverence for life’ carries us through logical argument,detailing the basis of scientific inquiry of animals’ behaviour and thelimitations of this methodology. In doing so, Verhoog makes suggestionsfor a paradigmatic shift in scientific inquiry. Similarly, Fran#oise Wemels-felder in her chapter ‘The problem of animal subjectivity and its conse-quences for the scientific measurement of animal suffering’ describes aresearch model that uses animal subjectivity as a means to understand themotivation for animals’ behavioural actions. ‘Are animals aware of theirown suffering?’ is a fundamental question in animal welfare, and Wemels-felder together with Verhoog provide a theoretical framework and experi-mental basis for investigating the subjective states of animals. It is onlythrough research of animals’ subjective states that this important questioncan be addressed.

The underlying subjective states of animals may also be investigated viaother methods. Susan Healy and Martin Tovee present a chapter discuss-ing the environmental effects on neurophysiology during development,‘Environmental enrichment and impoverishment: neurophysiological ef-fects’. Their discussion centres on experimental evidence from animalsraised in impoverished (versus enriched) environments where interactionwith the environment and stimulation were minimal or highly controlled.The result is that, not only is the animal’s ability to problem solve severelyimpaired, but its overall behavioural repertoire is significantly reduced.The behavioural deficits are reflected in developmental neurophysiologicaldeficits as well. The authors point out that the major neurophysiologicaldifference between an animal raised in an impoverished environment fromthat of an enriched environment is a significant decrease in cortical connec-tions. Thus, the lack of variability in the behavioural repertoires of captive

7A look in the mirror

Page 18: ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFAREassets.cambridge.org/97805214/73422/sample/9780521473422web.pdf · ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFARE Edited by ... Franc¸oise

job:LAY01 27-10-1998 page:8 colour:1 black–text

animals inhabiting impoverished environments can be correlated with, ifnot explained by, a lack of neuronal connections and density in importanthigher order processing areas of the brain. The benefit of this chapter inthis book is to provide the reader with an understanding of the neuro-physiological mechanism(s) directing behavioural variability. This chap-ter also provides empirical evidence of the long-term effects of impoveri-shed environments on animals, not only behaviourally, but also neur-ophysiologically. It enables us to have a greater understanding of how toalter the environments to provide stimulation for captive animals. Inciden-tally, information gained through this chapter may aid in establishingadditional means to measure and enhance the welfare of animals on aneurophysiological plane in correlation with their behavioural repertoires,as well as provide benefits to diminish effects of disease and trauma inhumans and animals.

Descartes declared that animals did not possess the capacity to processrational thoughts or to experience pain (1956). An extension of the Car-tesian ideology set the past criteria for distinguishing humans from otheranimals, based on four major differences. Humans alone were thought tohave the ability to use tools and language, to possess self-awareness andconsciousness, and to be able to anticipate and plan intentionally forfuture events. In contrast to Descartes, Darwin (1859) believed that therewere biological, psychological as well as emotional continuities betweenhumans and all other animals. Conceptually, this placed all animals,including humans, along the same plane of subjective existence.

Following Darwinian tradition, the research of Jane Goodall (vanLawick-Goodall, 1976) and others (e.g. McGrew, 1992) have providedstrong evidence for chimpanzees’ abilities to both use and create tools.Additionally, language use in apes has been studied extensively by numer-ous researchers (e.g. Savage-Rumbaugh et al., 1986; Fouts, 1973; Patter-son & Cohn, 1990). While the controversy still exists about whether apesgenerate novel sentences flexibly using syntactical structures (Terrace etal., 1979; Miles, 1994), recent evidence from studies of captive, language-reared pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) has provided strong support fortheir use of complex communication, their ability to use semantics insign-based languages, and to comprehend syntax in human speech (Rum-baugh, 1996). Studies of the cognitive aspects of non-human animal be-haviour complement both the study of language abilities and these ani-mals’ capacity for self-awareness and/or consciousness (see Parker,Mitchell & Boccia, 1994). The two remaining frontiers are the areas ofself-awareness and consciousness, including empathy, and anticipation

8 F. L. Dolins

Page 19: ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFAREassets.cambridge.org/97805214/73422/sample/9780521473422web.pdf · ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFARE Edited by ... Franc¸oise

job:LAY01 27-10-1998 page:9 colour:1 black–text

and intentionality. These are not wholly separate from each other: ananimal that can anticipate a future can anticipate a future event where it islikely to be aware of its own suffering.

In Richard Byrne’s chapter ‘Primate cognition: evidence for the ethicaltreatment of primates’ the themes discussed are non-human primate intel-ligence and capacity for anticipating future unpleasant events. Byrneprovides evidence for classifying whether a non-human primate is sentientof its own future pain and thus worthy of ‘welfare’ considerations. Thischapter should be read in relation to Fran#oise Wemelsfelder’s ideas forcomparison. Donald Broom’s chapter ‘Animal welfare: the concept of theissues’ suggests that the welfare of an animal should be attended to inrelation to its ‘‘needs, freedoms, happiness, coping, control, predictability,feelings, suffering, pain, anxiety, fear, boredom, stress and health’’. Broomprovides a detailed conceptual and theoretical discussion of ways in whichto assess the welfare of captive animals. On a related theme, in ‘Newperspectives on the design and management of captive animal environ-ments’, David Shepherdson’s chapter is on the theoretical background andexamples of environmental enrichment for captive animals in zoologicalparks. By modifying the environment in which captive animals live, theirbehavioural repertoire may be transformed from a paucity of stereotypicalbehaviours to a greater number of more natural types of behaviours thatcorrespond to their wild counterparts.

It is obvious from the widely different backgrounds of the authorsincluded in these chapters that opinions vary greatly amongst them. Theauthors of the chapters derive from interdisciplinary and varying back-grounds. Within these multiple viewpoints, the reader may choose lines ofthought to follow on theoretical, conceptual and practical levels.

The section headed ‘Animal awareness’, a play on words, invites specu-lation on whether animals are self-aware, and our awareness of animals’behaviour and capacity for suffering. Robert Young’s chapter ‘The behav-ioural requirements of farm animals for psychological well-being andsurvival’ addresses areas of concern in the welfare of captive animals, andin particular, agricultural animals. Given that the main purpose of farmanimals is to provide financial benefit to their owner and food for people,the question of farm animal welfare is often only considered in relation tothe monetary costs and/or economic effects. However, Young’s chaptertakes us beyond the financial concern of costs and benefits to the humanowners and consumers, and deals with the underlying psychological statesof these farm animals. Approved methods of animal husbandry for farmanimals often deprive these animals of basic needs. The questions that

9A look in the mirror

Page 20: ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFAREassets.cambridge.org/97805214/73422/sample/9780521473422web.pdf · ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFARE Edited by ... Franc¸oise

job:LAY01 27-10-1998 page:10 colour:1 black–text

Young answers are whether farm animals have specific behavioural needs,what kind of suffering will be incurred if these needs are not met, and, whatenvironmental modifications can be achieved to enhance these animals’behavioural repertoire? But, what if the behavioural needs of a captiveanimal are satisfied? Will the basic satisfaction of needs be enough toensure an animal’s ‘happiness’? James King’s chapter ‘Personality and thehappiness of the chimpanzee’ deals with this question: where can we drawthe line between when an animal has had its welfare addressed sufficientlyso that we can claim that it exhibits ‘psychological well-being’, and when itis actually ‘happy’? Also, are the two the same or can they be distinguishedfrom each other? If we were to establish psychological well-being forhumans, would we also be able to claim that they were happy? By taking anovel twist on the almost cliched term ‘psychological well-being’, Kingevaluates previous research in this area and presents findings, illuminatingthe contexts and personalities that lead to chimpanzees’ states of happi-ness.

The intentions of the following chapters, as the titles of this book andintroduction suggest, is for the reader to take a look in the mirror oncemore, asking, what is it to be human, what to be animal, and what are thenature of the relationships therein? The text should provide the reader withthe means to glance back to past assumptions, to analyse, test and chal-lenge both new and old perspectives on these issues by following thediversity of its many authors’ thoughts. Finally, the subject matter of thisbook should pose a challenge to the reader to make choices, ethicalchoices, about their own human stance in their relationship to non-humananimals, and by expansion to other human animals.

References

Darwin, C. (1859). The Origin of Species. New York: Hurst.Descartes, R. (1956/1637). Discourse on Method. New York: Liberal Arts Press.Fouts, R. S. (1973). Acquisition and testing of gestural signs in four young

chimpanzees. Science, 180, 973–80.Lawick-Goodall, J. van (1976). Early tool use in chimpanzees. In Play, ed. J.

Bruner, A. Jolly & K. Sylva, pp. 19–46. Cambridge: Cambridge UniversityPress.

Mackenzie, D. (1997). Through the looking glass. The Sciences, May/June, 32–7.McGrew, W. C. (1992). Chimpanzee Material Culture: Implications for Human

Evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Miles, H. L. W. (1994). ME CHANTEK: The development of self-awareness in a

signing orangutan. Self-awareness in Animals and Humans: DevelopmentalPerspectives, ed. S. T. Parker, R. W. Mitchell & M. L. Boccia, pp. 254–72.Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

10 F. L. Dolins

Page 21: ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFAREassets.cambridge.org/97805214/73422/sample/9780521473422web.pdf · ATTITUDES TO ANIMALS: VIEWS IN ANIMAL WELFARE Edited by ... Franc¸oise

job:LAY01 27-10-1998 page:11 colour:1 black–text

Parker, S. T. , Mitchell, R. W. & Boccia, M. L. (1994). Self-awareness in Animalsand Humans: Developmental Perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge UniversityPress.

Patterson, F. G. & Cohn, R. H. (1990). Language acquisition in a lowlandgorilla: Koko’s first ten years of vocabulary development. Word, 41, 97–143.

Rollin, B. E. (1995). The Frankenstein Syndrome: Ethical and Social Issues in theGenetic Engineering of Animals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Rumbaugh, D. M. (1996). Beast machines of the monkey wars. ContemporaryPsychology, 41(4), 316–18.

Savage-Rumbaugh, S., McDonald, K., Sevcik, R. A., Hopkins, W. D. & Rubert,E. (1986). Spontaneous symbol acquisition and communicative use bypygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus). Journal of Experimantal Psychology:General, 115(3), 211–35.

Terrace, H. S. , Petitto, L. , Sanders, R. & Bever, T. (1979). Can an ape create asentence? Science, 206, 809–902.

11A look in the mirror