associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

64
Peer relationships and quality of life Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and self-reported quality of life in 11-12 year-olds Natalia Lezhnieva Master thesis at the Department of Psychology UNIVERSITY OF OSLO Spring 2017

Upload: others

Post on 26-Dec-2021

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

Peer relationships and quality of life

Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship

and self-reported quality of life in 11-12 year-olds

Natalia Lezhnieva

Master thesis at the Department of Psychology

UNIVERSITY OF OSLO

Spring 2017

Page 2: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

II

Peer relationships and quality of life Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and self-reported quality of life in 11-

12-years olds

Page 3: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

III

© Natalia Lezhnieva

2017

Peer relationships and quality of life – Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship

and self-reported quality of life in 11-12-year-olds.

Natalia Lezhnieva

http://www.duo.uio.no

Printed by Reprosentralen, University of Oslo

Page 4: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

IV

Page 5: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

V

Page 6: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

VI

Abstract Author: Natalia Lezhnieva

Title: Peer relationships and quality of life

Supervisors: Mona Bekkhus and Per Morten Fredriksen (co-supervisor)

Social interactions are fundamental for our development and vital for our existence. Thus, it

is reasonable to assume that social interactions play a significant role in what could be

defined as “a good life”. Findings from existing studies on children’s quality of life have

shown that peer relations are associated with both positive and negative outcomes. Positive

aspects of peer relations, and especially friendship, are considered to promote one’s quality of

life, while negative aspects decrease children’s quality of life. The overall aim of this master

thesis was to examine the associations between peer relations and children’s quality of life.

More specifically, it focused on associations between popularity and friendships and self-

reported quality of life in 11-12-year-olds.

The current study was based on data collected for a larger ongoing research project – “The

Health Oriented Pedagogical Project” (HOPP). Principal investigator of the project is Per

Morten Fredriksen (Kristiania University College, Department of Health Science, Oslo;

Norway). At the baseline in 2015 there were 2297 children from 6 to 12 years old who

participated in the project. The HOPP is a longitudinal intervention study with multi-

informant and multi-method design.

Results show that both popularity and reciprocal friendship had a positive association with

children’s quality of life. Number of nominations (both for popularity and reciprocal

friendship) played a significant role for mentioned above associations. Findings from the

current study contribute in contemporary research focused on children’s quality of life and

may imply that schools can contribute beneficially in implementing interventions aimed to

promote children’s quality of life.

Page 7: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

VII

Page 8: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

VIII

Acknowledgements It has been such an experience writing this master thesis and I would like to express my

gratitude to those who have been by my side through the whole process. The thesis is based

on the data from ”The Health Oriented Pedagogical Project” (HOPP) and I would like to

express my gratitude to Per Morten Fredriksen and his colleagues from Kristiania University

College for the given opportunity to participate in such large, interesting, and thorough

project. This experience enriched my knowledge regarding practical aspects of conducting a

scientific project, data collection as well as coding processes. I appreciate his guidance and

support.

I want to express my sincere gratitude to my supervisor Mona Bekkhus. From the very

beginning, her competent guidance, constructive feedback, and support were both inspiring

and motivating. It has been a long sometimes even challenging process, yet it turned to be

rewording by the end. I am glad that I got to share this experience with Mona.

I definitely couldn’t do it without my family’s support and encouragement. Thank you all for

giving me time and space for writing. It has been a long journey during which I had to

constantly adjust my list of priorities.

Finally, I want to thank everyone else who had been involved in the process. You all made

this master thesis a reality.

Page 9: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

IX

Page 10: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

X

Content 1 Introduction ....................................................................................................................... 1

1.1 Operationalization ...................................................................................................... 2 1.2 Theoretical perspectives on peer relationships ........................................................ 5 1.3 Theoretical perspectives on quality of life ................................................................ 6 1.4 Peer relationships and quality of life ........................................................................ 6 1.5 Popularity and quality of life ..................................................................................... 8 1.6 Friendships and quality of life ................................................................................... 9 1.7 Quality of life and demographic factors ................................................................. 11 1.8 Research questions ................................................................................................... 13

2 Method ............................................................................................................................. 14 2.1 Design ........................................................................................................................ 14 2.2 Recruitment .............................................................................................................. 15 2.3 Measures .................................................................................................................... 15

2.3.1 Quality of life (QofL) – outcome variable ........................................................... 15 2.3.2 Reciprocal friendship and popularity variables ................................................... 16 2.3.3 Control variable ................................................................................................... 17

2.4 Preliminary analysis ................................................................................................. 17 2.5 Statistical analysis ..................................................................................................... 18

3 Results .............................................................................................................................. 19 3.1 Descriptive analyses ................................................................................................. 19 3.2 Bivariate correlation analyses ................................................................................. 20 3.3 Hierarchical linear regression analyses .................................................................. 21

3.3.1 Popularity and quality of life ............................................................................... 21 3.3.2 Reciprocal friendship and quality of life ............................................................. 22 3.3.3 Number of reciprocal nominations and quality of life ......................................... 22 3.3.4 Summarized results from multiple regression analyses ....................................... 23

3.4 Independent t-test ..................................................................................................... 23 3.4.1 Quality of life and reciprocal friendship .............................................................. 23 3.4.2 Quality of life and gender .................................................................................... 23 3.4.3 Quality of life and paternal education .................................................................. 24

3.5 One-way ANOVA ..................................................................................................... 24 3.5.1 Popularity and quality of life ............................................................................... 24 3.5.2 Reciprocal friendship and quality of life ............................................................. 25 3.5.3 Paternal education and quality of life .................................................................. 27

4 Discussion ......................................................................................................................... 28 4.1 Popularity and quality of life ................................................................................... 28 4.2 Reciprocal friendship and quality of life. ............................................................... 30 4.3 Associations between demographic factors and quality of life ............................. 33 4.4 Strengths, limitations and suggestions for future research .................................. 34 4.5 Implications ............................................................................................................... 36

5 Conclusion ........................................................................................................................ 38

References ............................................................................................................................... 39 Appendices ............................................................................................................................. 47

Page 11: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

XI

Table 1: Descriptive features of the sample……………………………………………...19-20 Table 2: Correlations between variable……………………………………………………...20 Table 3-5: Hierarchical regression analyses……………………………………………..21-22 Table 6-8: One-way ANOVA…………………………………………………………....24-26 Figure 1: Quality of life by popularity nominations………………………………………...25 Figure 2: Quality of life by reciprocal friend nominations……………………………….....26 Figure 3: Quality of life by father’s education………………………………………………27 Appendix I: Flowchart of sample selection……….…………………………………...........47 Appendix II: The Inventory of Life Quality in Children and Adolescents………………....48 Appendix III: Tests of normality………………………………..………………………….50 Appendix IV: One-way ANOVA post hoc comparison…..…………………………...........51

Page 12: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and
Page 13: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

1

1 Introduction Social interactions are fundamental for our development and vital for our existence. It is

reasonable to assume that social interactions play a significant role in what could be defined

as “a good life”. Indeed, numerous studies have shown that social relationships play an

important role for self-reported well-being (e.g.Ben-Arieh, Casas, Frønes, & Korbin, 2014;

Rubin, Bukowski, & Parker, 2006; Rubin, Bowker, McDonals, & Menzer, 2013).

This thesis focuses on the role of peer relations (popularity and friendships) and quality of

life in children aged 11-12 years old. To date research on peer relationships and well-being

has usually focused on either adolescence or young adults, thus less is known about such

association in middle childhood (e.g. Ben-Arieh et al., 2014). Therefore, more research on

associations between peer relations and children’s quality of life is needed.

Data from research based on adult populations indicate that social relations (family and

friends) essentially contribute to adult’s well-being (Holder, 2012; Lyubomirsky, Sheldon,

Schkade, Candland, & Baumeister, 2005). Thus, it is logical to assume that the same might

yield for children as well. However, questions arise as to what degree these findings could be

generalized to middle childhood, is there any differences or not? To answer these questions,

data based on a child population, needs to be collected in order to compare it to findings from

adult research. On the other hand, children at different age are at different stages of their

development (physiologically, psychologically and socially), thus the role of peer relations,

specifically popularity and friendship, might have a different impact on children themselves

and their quality of life.

It has already been established that functions of friendship vary with age (e.g. Dunn, 2004),

but much less is known about its impact on children’s quality of life. In fact, most of the

research that has been conducted to date and that has examined children’s quality of life, is

based on research sample of children with various physical or psychological impairments, i.e.

they carry a diagnosis, for example, cancer (Eilersten, Jozefiak, Rannestad, Indredavik, &

Vik, 2012), obesity (Herzer, Zeller, Rausch, & Modi, 2011), ADHD (Schei, Jozefiak, Nøvik,

Lydersen, & Indredavik, 2016). Therefore, there exists a gap between the research focusing

on ill-being and the research examining quality of life based on sample from a normal

population. The current study contributes to the latter one by examining associations between

Page 14: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

2

popularity, friendship and children’s self-reported quality of life among representative normal

sample of children. The knowledge about what is typical for normal population of children

might be beneficial for developing intervention programs, and for other health-related

research.

There has also been a tradition to primarily focus on negative correlates and consequences of

peer relations, such as peer rejection, bullying, and peer victimization in children (see Rubin

et al., 2006). It was not until more recent that an emerging number of studies started to focus

on well-being in children from general population (e.g. Ben-Arieh et al., 2014; Rubin et al.,

2013). The overall aim in this thesis is to examine the associations between peer relations and

children’s self-reported quality of life, in a large representative population based sample.

In the following sections I start with clarifying terms related to popularity, friendship and

quality of life. Further, theories relevant to peer relations and quality of life will be reviewed,

as well as existent empirical data. Children are engaged in different types of peer relations.

This thesis focuses on two of them – popularity and friendship.

1.1 Operationalization Popularity and friendship are two related phenomena, but are also distinct and separate ones,

and one cannot be substituted by the other and vice versa (Bukowski, Hoza, & Boivin, 1993;

Dunn, 2004; Dunn & McGuire, 1992; Fink, Begeer, Peterson, Slaughter, & Posnay, 2015;

Furman & Robbins, 1985). I am going to use terms ”popularity” and ”friendship”, ”reciprocal

friendship” when I refer to phenomena separately and ”peer relations” when I refer to them

together.

Friendship

Many definitions of “friendship” emphasize the fact that this type of relationship is typically

viewed as voluntarily and mutual (e.g. Bagwell, Newcomb, & Bukowski, 1998; Dunn, 2004;

Newcomb & Bagwell, 1995). Newcomb and Bagwell (1995) define friendship as mutual

dyadic relationship that is different from other peer relations in its level of affection. The

notion that friendship is assumed to be mutual relationship doesn’t represent the whole

spectrum of friendship relationships: not all friendships are created equal and definitely not

all friendships are reciprocal by default. As a result, researchers distinguish between

Page 15: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

3

reciprocal and unilateral friendships (Ladd, Emerson, & Scarr, 1984; Lodder, Scholte,

Goossens, & Verhagen, 2015; Sijtsema, 2016). A friendship can be described as reciprocal

when two children mutually nominate each other as friends while unilateral friendship occurs

when a child consider another person as a friend but doesn’t get a reciprocated nomination in

return (Asher, Guerry, & McDonald, 2014). Studies of friendship nominations report that 10

to 25 per cent of children aged from 8 to 15 years old don’t have reciprocal friendship

nomination (Ostberg, 2003; Parker and Asher, 1993; Rose, 2002). It is reasonable to assume

that reciprocated friendship outcomes would differ from unilateral friendship outcomes

(Hartup, 1996).

Popularity

Popularity, has been defined as “a multifaceted construct that is characterized by the level of

likeability among one’s peers, number of friends, and how well an individual gets along with

others” (Bunt & Donnellan, 2015, p. 151). Researchers often distinguish between

sociometric and perceived popularity (Cillessen, 2011; Cillessen & Bellmore, 2011;

Parkhurst & Hopmeyer, 1998, Rubin et al., 2006). They argue that sociometric popularity (or

peer acceptance) captures the individual level of the dyadic relationships between peers while

perceived popularity captures the group level of the relationships (social status of a child in

the group of peers). Popularity is described as sociometric when the question “Who do you

like/dislike most?” is posed, whereas perceived popularity is characterized by “Who do you

think is popular?” (Rubin et al., 2006). In this thesis sociometric popularity has been

measured.

Sociometric popularity constitutes what Bagwell et al. (1998) refer to as “peer acceptance”

and “peer rejection.” Thus, it refers to the degree in which the child is liked by peers.

Traditionally, popularity is accessed with the help of sociometric measures. Friendships are

also measured by using a similar set of methods – friend nominations – with the difference

that the reciprocity is also measured. Reciprocated friendships differ from other peer

nominations as they reflect intimate, voluntarily relationships and are characterized by a

special emotional connection (e.g. Dunn, 2004; Parker & Asher, 1993). Similar findings have

been reported by Fink et al. (2015). They found that friendship and popularity were different

phenomena: 53% of children who had low popularity scores had a reciprocal friend while

23% of children with high sociometric status had no reciprocal friend.

Page 16: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

4

Quality of life

Quality of life is an interdisciplinary term that is widely used by sociologists, economists,

psychologists, and psychiatrists, but no consensus currently exists among scholars what

exactly the term refers to. Some researchers point out that the term has been used from very

specific such as health-related to very wide e.g. well-being, living conditions (Gasper, 2010;

Helsedirektoratet, 2015; Svavarsdottir & Orlysgsdottir, 2006). Gasper (2010) argues that the

term ”well-being” refers to a description or perception of one’s own experience, while

“quality of life” describes contexts and circumstances of that experience. The author

emphasizes that the term “quality of life” is usually used in literature about living conditions

and health related topics while in psychological literature authors prefer to use the term

“well-being”. Other researchers regard both “well-being” and “quality of life” as

interchangeable and use them as an umbrella-term for both objective indicators (e.g. living

conditions) and subjective indicators (experiences, feeling and meanings) (Barstads, 2014;

Michalos, 2008). Frey and Stutzer (2009) define “quality of life” as to what degree a person

perceives his/her life as desirable. Norwegian Directorate of Health in its book “Well-being

på norsk” (2015) defines “quality of life” simply as “subjective experience of feeling good”

(about one’s life). The definition proposed by The Norwegian Institute of Public Health

(NIPH) is in concordance with the one above, and NIPH suggests that: “quality of life means

to experience joy and meaning, vitality and satisfaction, sense of belonging and safety, use

personal strengths, feel interest and engagement” (Nes, 2017). In this thesis the term “quality

of life” refers to children’s subjective perception of his/her life as desirable (or not) as a

whole, at the same time, such terms as “happiness, “life satisfaction”, “well-being” are also

used when referring to relevant studies where authors initially used these terms in their

research.

Page 17: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

5

1.2 Theoretical perspectives on peer relationships Already in the first part of 20th century such psychologists as Piaget (1932), Mead (1934),

Vygotsky (1978), and Sullivan (1953) discussed the importance of peer relationships for

children’s development. Experiences with peers evolve within children’s development – they

become more complex, diverse, and sophisticated. The role, which peers play in children’s

life, has been examined within different theoretical frameworks.

Cognitive theorists emphasize the role of peer relationships for a cognitive development of a

child. Piaget (1932) distinguished peer relationships as opposed to relationships with parents,

based on the idea that peer relationships can be described as more balanced in power and

egalitarian. This equality in relationships makes it possible to develop abilities to understand

other’s thoughts, emotions and intentions (Mead, 1934; Piaget, 1932). Peer relationships also

promote development of self-reflect (Mead, 1934). Through interactions with peers children

learn and adopt various patterns and models of social behavior, as well as rules and norms

(Bandura, 1977).

These perspectives on peer relationships and their role in child’s development have given

modern scholars a solid ground to build their models on, such as transactional models of

development, proposed by Rubin et al. (2013). They propose that the child characteristics, the

family characteristics, the quality of relationships within and outside of the family, culture,

stress and social support, all these factors would determine the relationships the child

establishes with peers. The process is dynamic and multidirectional, thus, the child is viewed

as an active agent of the social environment. Further, authors suggest that development could

go in two directions: a pathway to psychological adaptation and a maladaptive pathway. The

first one starts with secure parent-child relationships that with time promote and stimulate

engagement in establishing positive relationships outside of the family. Thus, by expanding

the environment for social interactions a child acquires various social skills (e.g.

understanding others, problem solving). The secure relationships with parents represent the

source of support and guidance in dealing with various challenges with peers when they

occur in more adaptive way. Hence, for transactional models it is essential to view the model

as a whole with all its components constantly interacting and influencing each other. The

maladaptive pathway is characterized by difficult temperament demonstrated by a child

and/or parents, development of insecure parent-child relationships, unfortunate parenting

Page 18: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

6

style (e.g. authoritarian), and family stress. All these factors are believed by authors to

contribute in incompetent behaviors toward peers, which may result in peer rejection and

other developmental maladjustments (Rubin et al., 2013).

1.3 Theoretical perspectives on quality of life Quality of life refers to what can be characterized as optimal functioning and experience and

simply described as a good life. (Helsedirektoratet, 2015). Western philosophical

perspectives on the quality of life can be divided in two different, yet related approaches:

hedonic and eudemonic. Hedonic approach emphasizes the importance of subjectively

determined positive experiences, feelings, and evaluations related to pleasure and happiness,

while eudemonic approach focuses on experiences that are objectively good for person (e.g.

personal strengths and potentials, meaning in life) (McMahan & Estes, 2012). Existing

empirical research implies that eudemonic approach may be more significant for quality of

life than hedonic one. For example, findings from McMahan and Estes study (2012), who

examined associations between both hedonic and eudemonic dimensions and adults’ quality

of life, indicated that eudemonic approach seemed to be more appropriate for the quality of

life. Peer relationships might provide experiences that are beneficial for one’s development;

therefore this study incorporates the eudemonic understanding of the quality of life.

1.4 Peer Relationships and Quality of Life Research suggests that peer relations influence children on individual, intrapersonal and

general adjustment levels (Rubin e al. 2013). Moreover, it is well established that peer

relations are important to social, cognitive and emotional development of children, youth, and

adults. Friends provide a context in which we learn necessary social skills and serve a source

of social support (Bagwell et al., 1998; Casas, Figuer, Gonzales, & Malo, 2007; Dunn, 2004;

Fink et al., 2015; Hartup, 1996).

Being accepted by peers becomes especially important for children during middle childhood

(e.g. Rubin el al., 2006). Research suggests that children prefer to spend time with those who

are similar to them in age, gender, behavioral patterns, and ethnicity (Abecassis, Hartup,

Haselager, Scholte, & Van Lieshout, 2002). During middle childhood most of peer

interactions happen within cliques (Rubin et al., 2006). Being a clique member has been

associated with psychological well-being and constructive coping with stress (Hansel, 1981).

Page 19: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

7

By the time of transitioning to adolescence, friendships represent unique relationships outside

of the family where adolescents are able to develop social identities, acceptance,

connectedness to others, at the same time providing arena and various paths to develop

autonomy from their parents.

Traditionally, research primarily focused on the negative consequences related to peer

relationships (e.g. peer rejection, bullying, victimization), however, for the past two decades

there has emerged a body of research where the emphasis started to shift from focusing on

dysfunctional outcomes to promoting positive aspects of peer relationships (e.g. Rubin el al.,

2006; Rubin et al., 2013). Nevertheless, findings from existing studies on children’s well-

being have shown that peer relations are associated with both positive and negative

outcomes. Positive aspects of peer relations, and especially friendship, are considered to

promote one’s well-being (e.g. Bukowski et al., 2011, Rubin et al., 2006; Rubin et al., 2013)

while negative aspects decrease children’s well-being (e.g. Hodges, Boivin, Vitaro, &

Bukowski, 1999; Ladd, 2009; Laursen, Bukowski, Aunola, & Nurmi, 2007; Rubin et al.,

2013, Schwartz et al., 2000). Findings from some studies provide evidence for a long-term

effect of peer relationships on children’s adjustment and development (e.g. Dunn & McGuire,

1992; Hay, Payne, & Chadwick, 2004; Rubin et al., 2013). Furthermore, there has not yet

been agreed on which direction the associations between peer relations and well-being goes.

That is, the associations may be bidirectional, which means that there is still uncertainty over

whether peer relationship influence quality of life or whether children who report high quality

of life also have better relationships with peers. Recent empirical work suggests that there

may be a bidirectional pattern between well-being and peer relationships. For example,

Dougherty (2006) in her meta-analytic review points out that the relationships between social

status and emotional well-being might be bidirectional, but more studies with a longitudinal

design are needed in order to examine causality and directionality.

Associations between peer relationships, more specifically, popularity and friendships, and

children’s self-reported quality of life are aimed to be examined in this master thesis, but first

let’s proceed by reviewing the existing studies on the relevant topics.

Page 20: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

8

1.5 Popularity and Quality of Life Kindergartens and schools represent the perfect conditions for children to establish and

maintain relations with peers. As stated by Sullivan (1953), the importance of peer relations

increases during middle childhood, therefore the role of peer may have a stronger impact on

the well-being in children as they grow older. Findings from empirical research also suggest

that during the middle childhood children become concerned about being liked by peers and

belonging to a certain peer group (e.g. Rubin et al., 2006).

Peers and teachers perceive their popular and unpopular classmates in a different way. Well-

liked children often are characterized as helpful, good students both by schoolmates, teachers,

and parents (Wenzel & Asher, 1995). Popular children are viewed as prosocial, supportive,

empathic, communicative, cooperative and academically successful, while unpopular

children are aggressive, disruptive, withdrawn and socially anxious (e.g. Cillessen &

Bellmore, 2011; Gazelle, 2008; Rubin et al., 2013). Children who are precieved as unpopular,

with few nominations, have often been found to have difficulties of establishing friend

relationships, and have long-term psychological difficulties (Nangle, Erdley, Newman,

Mason, & Carpenter, 2003). Moreover, findings from studies examining negative outcomes

of peer relations show that being rejected by peers is associated with miscellaneous negative

outcomes (e.g. poor school adjustment, behavioral deviations and maladaptations,

psychopathology) that may have a long-term effect and consequences for their psychosocial

adjustment (Rubin et al. 2013).

Being accepted by peers, on the other hand, is usually associated with later positive

outcomes, sociable behavior, satisfactory school achievements and adjustment, as well as

with lack of pathology (Cillessen, 2011). Disliked children, on the other hand, have been

found to have lower levels of externalizing problems (Sandstrom & Cillessen, 2006) and

higher levels of behavior problems (Mayeux, Samdstrom, & Cillessen, 2008).

Research on the associations between one’s quality of life and popularity have been relatively

inconsistent. For example, Ostberg (2003) found associations between child’s status among

his/her peers in the class were positively correlated with one’s quality of life: the higher

popularity the higher well-being. Similar findings have also been reported in studies based on

children age 9 to 12 years and adolescents sample (Holder & Coleman, 2009; Proctor, Linley,

Page 21: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

9

& Maltby, 2010). Researches found that adolescents who were popular among peers reported

higher level of life satisfaction as compared to less successful classmates (Proctor et al.,

2010), and popularity predicted higher level of self-reported happiness (Holder & Coleman,

2009). Kasser and Ahuvia (2002), examined popularity and happiness in a sample of

university students. They found that associations between popularity and happiness had a

negative correlation. Those students who valued their status among peers the most reported

lower level of happiness as compared to other students. However, not all studies have found

popularity to have only positive outcomes. For example, in a study by Demir and Urberg

(2004) no direct associations was found between popularity and well-being. In other words,

findings are somewhat mixed, but this may only reflect the different age group, or the use of

different measurements. Therefore this study will add to the existing literature by focusing on

quality of life and popularity and friendship in a huge community-based population study.

To sum up, existing data on associations between one’s popularity and quality of life suggests

low to moderate associations (e.g. Holder 2012). On the other hand, friendship’s

contributions into one’s quality of life are considered much more significant (Ben-Arieh et

al., 2014).

1.6 Friendships and Quality of Life Many studies have confirmed that friends represent an important domain of children’s well-

being (Bukowski, Newcomb & Hartup, 1998; Goswami, 2011; Parker & Asher, 1993). In

Great Britain The Good Childhood Report 2012 (Children’s society, 2012) disclosed that

children who were happier with their friendships had significantly higher well-being. It also

showed that negative aspects of friendship negatively correlated to children’s well-being. It

has been reported that number of friends was one of the factors predicting higher level of

reported life satisfaction (Casas et al., 2007). Similar findings have been reported by Holder

and Coleman (2009). They found that having many friends predicted higher level of

happiness as compared to those with few friends. Friends might also be one of the factors that

contribute to overall life satisfaction in adolescence (Demir & Weitekamp, 2007). A recent

study conducted by Goswami (2012) investigated how children’s social relationships could

be linked to children’s subjective well-being. Results of this study showed that positive

aspects of friendship promoted subjective well-being, while negative aspects reduced

subjective well-being.

Page 22: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

10

The amount of time spent with friends increases during late childhood and adolescence

(Rubin et al., 2006) which is considered to be an indicator of why friends might have stronger

influence on each other during that period. Findings from different studies indicate that group

of peers and best friends differ in their level of influence on adolescents (e.g. Epstein, 1983).

It can be due to the fact that adolescents spend more time with their best friends than with

other peers. Other researchers argue that peer groups influences children and adolescents

positive adjustments, because a greater network of friends provides a greater number of

opportunities for forming positive relations outside of the family (Lansford, Criss, Pettit,

Dodge, & Bates, 2003). However, having at least one friend have been found to promote

prosocial behavior and psychological well-being (Berndt & Keefe, 1995; Bukowski et al.,

1998; Parker & Asher, 1993). Findings from different studies suggests that having a

reciprocal friend is important for positive psychological well-being, but it is especially

important for those who are at risk for maladaptation (Bowker, Thomas, Norman, & Spencer,

2011). A lack of friends is associated with depression and other mental health problems,

which in its turn tend to decrease quality of life (e.g. Rubin et al., 2006). Recent studies show

that youth with reciprocal prosocial friends were more than twice as likely to have positive

psychological functioning as those youth reporting no special friend (Hopkins, Zubrick, &

Taylor, 2014), and they were found to be less lonely (Asher et al., 2014). Thus, it seems that

the number of reciprocal friend nominations is associated with children’s overall quality of

life. However, although there have been some studies examining children’s social relations

and well-being/happiness (Demir & Weitekamp, 2007; Goswami, 2012; Holder & Coleman

2009; Ostberg, 2003; Uusitalo-Malmivaara & Lehto, 2013), much less attention has been

given to children`s quality of life. Participants in the majority of the studies investigating

children’s quality of life usually have various physical or psychological impairment and

diagnosis such as cancer (Eilersten et al., 2012), ADHD (Schei et al., 2016), deafness/hearing

loss (Fellinger, Holzinger, Sattel & Laucht, 2008) and other. Therefore more knowledge on

quality of life in a population-based sample would be an important addition to the current

literature.

Demir et al. (2007) found that best friends predicted happiness in a sample of college

students. They suggested that best friends are often valued the most; therefore a person would

gain more from such relationships. Other studies had further found that having more friends

was considered to be a key factor for increasing their own happiness (e.g. Uusitalo-

Page 23: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

11

Malmivaara&Lehto,2013). Children who had two or more close friends reported higher

level of happiness as compared with children who had only one or no close friends (Jover&

Thoilliez2010;Uusitalo-Malmivaara&Lehto,2013). This is in contrast to findings from

Hodges et al. (1999) study suggest that there is no difference in children’s adjustments if they

have only one friend or many. In a meta-analysis of studies focusing on reports from elderly,

Pinquart, SörensonandLight(2000),foundthatthequantityofsocialcontactswere

associatedwithsubjectivewell-being.

1.7 Quality of life and demographic factors It has been estimated that for adult population demographic factors like age, income, gender,

education, and so on explain from 10 to 15 per cent of the variety in quality of life variable.

(see Sirgy, 2012 for a review).

Income

Holder and Coleman (2009) have examined relationships between various demographic

factors and children’s happiness. They found that demographic variables show low to

moderate correlation to children’s happiness. Participants in their study were children 9 to 12

years old. However, reported findings from researches seem to be mixed. Most of the studies

report that people with higher incomes, on average, report higher level of life quality

compared to those who have lower income (e.g. Easterlin, 2011; Diener & Seligman, 2004;

Pinquart et al., 2000; Sirgy, 2012), at the same time as other researchers bring to our attention

the fact that the difference in reported happiness between very wealthy people and a control

group was significant but rather low (Diener, Horrowitz & Emmons, 1985, Rojas, 2011). A

large-scale study by Levin et al. (2011) examined the relationship between life satisfaction of

the 13-year-old adolescents and family’s wealth. Adolescents from 35 countries participated

and findings showed a strong relationship between family’s wealth and life satisfaction

reported by adolescents. On the national level there had also been observed a positive

association between life satisfaction and national income. The association between income

and quality of life has on the other hand been found to diminish over time (Easterlin, 2011,

Sirgy, 2012). There has been some evidence suggesting that an increase in income contribute

to happiness (especially in lower income countries or countries with a big gap between poor

and reach people), but beyond a certain level income doesn’t seem to have an impact on self-

reported quality of life (e.g. Dolan et al., 2008; Frey & Stutzer, 2009).

Page 24: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

12

Age

Many studies with adult population sample report U-shaped relationship between age and

quality of life, with a minimum at middle age and then going up again (see Frey & Stutzer,

2009). Some researchers suggest that subjective well-being increases or at least doesn’t drop

with age (Argyle, 1999, Diener et al., 1999), while others conclude that life satisfaction

doesn’t decline much during adulthood, but drops significantly among those who are over 70

years old (Baird et al., 2010). In other words, level of self-reported quality of life do change

with age, but it seems like researchers had focused mostly on adults: therefore, more research

based on children is needed.

Gender

Majority of the researchers based on adult population, agree that gender might have an effect

on reported levels of quality of life since it has been demonstrated that women report higher

level of happiness than men (see Sirgy, 2012), at the same time they admit that effects of

gender is rather caused by its interplay with other factors influencing subjective well-being

than by gender itself (e.g. Dollan et al., 2008). Research findings from studies based on

children and adolescents report no gender differences in subjective happiness (Holder &

Coleman, 2009; Uusitalo-Malmivaara & Lehto, 2013).

Other factors

There exists other demographic factors, like family composition, education, work, etc., that

have been associated with quality of life, but taken all together demographic factors in total

are responsible for not more than 15 % of the variance in self-reported quality of life (Holder

& Coleman, 2009; Sirgy, 2012) and besides income their effect on quality of life are reported

weak or even absent, but they tend to interact with other factors, like, for example, gender,

age, and religion, in predicting one’s quality of life (see Sirgy, 2012). Holder (2012) comes to

a conclusion that findings from studies based on children samples indicate that demographic

variables associate weakly with happiness.

Page 25: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

13

1.8 Research questions The overall aim of this thesis is to examine the associations between popularity and

friendships and children’s self-reported quality of life. Based on reviewed theory and

empirical research a hypothesis has been proposed that children who have at least one

reciprocal friend would report higher quality of life.

The current master thesis examines associations between quantitative components (i.e.

number of nominations) of popularity and friendship and children’s quality of life. To my

knowledge, this study will be the first one to examine associations solely between

friendships’ variables and self-reported quality of life, based on a sample of a representative

normal children population 11 to 12 years old. More specifically, this thesis will examine the

following research questions:

1. Is children’s popularity associated with higher self-reported quality of life?

2. Do children with at least one reciprocal friend report higher quality of life as

compared to children with no friends?

3. Are differences in number of reciprocal friend nominations (none, one, two, three or

four) related to differences in children’s reported quality of life?

4. Are there gender differences in self-reported quality of life?

5. Does parent’s level of education influence children’s self-reported quality of life?

With the starting point at existing relevant literature on this topic, it is expected to find

associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and self-reported quality of life.

Moreover, it is expected that children with at least one reciprocal friend would report higher

quality of life than children with unilateral friends or with no friends. There is further an

expectation that the majority of the children would report satisfactory and high levels of

quality of life, since the sample is a population-based community sample.

Page 26: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

14

2 Method The current master thesis is based on data collected for a larger research project – “The

Health Oriented Pedagogical Project” (HOPP). Principal investigator of the project is Per

Morten Fredriksen (Kristiania University College, Department of Health Science, Oslo,

Norway). The main goal of the study is to examine the effect of increased physical activity as

a part of the pedagogical approach (both from health related and academic perspectives).

The HOPP research project is funded by several sources including Horten municipality,

Kristiania University College, Institute of Health Sciences, Norwegian Order of Odd Fellows,

and Norwegian Fund for Post-Graduate Training in Physiotherapy. The Regional Committees

for Medical and Health Research Ethics approved the study protocol (2014/2064/REK sør-

øst).

2.1 Design The Health Oriented Pedagogical Project (later referred to as HOPP) is an ongoing,

prospective longitudinal intervention study, started in 2015. The study was designed as a

case-control physical activity intervention in all (seven) elementary schools in the Horten

and two control schools in Bærum and Asker. The study has a multi-informant and multi-

method design. At the baseline in 2015, a total number of all students in all schools was

2817, where 2297 (82%) participated in the project. Children from 1st to 6th grade were

recruited to participate in the study. All participants are followed longitudinally from baseline

in spring 2015 for as long as they stay in elementary school (2015-2021) (Fredriksen, Hjelle,

Mamen, Meza, & Westerberg, 2017).

Data for the HOPP project is a multi-method project including computer-based

questionnaires and physiological and academic tests. Data collection will be administrated at

local school settings each year. All questionnaires and tests are conducted by trained research

assistants.

Data in the current study is based on data gathered during the second year of data collection

(2015-2016), because the information about friendship and its variables had not yet been

included at the baseline of the project. Due to the project design, some tests are limited to

certain age groups. Our thesis is based on children’s answers about their quality of life,

Page 27: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

15

number of friends, and popularity. The number of participants, for this master thesis, consists

of 691 children from 11 to 12 years old (For more detailed information about number of

participants see Appendix I).

2.2 Recruitment Seven elementary schools in Horten municipality, Vestfold County, Norway and two schools

in Akershus County took part in the HOPP project. All schools in the Horten municipality

were enrolled in the HOPP intervention program, while two schools in Akershus County

were control schools, and took part solely in annual testing.

During the year 2014, there was an informational campaign about the HOPP for parties

involved including authorities, school principals and teachers, and children’ parents. For a

child to be considered as a participant of the project written parental consent had been

required.

Elementary education is compulsory in Norway, and this gives an opportunity to reach all

children, regardless of their socio-economic background. All schools were public, and the

number of pupils in the schools varied from just above 100 (Fagerheim school, Horten

municipality) to almost 600 children (Eiksmarka school, Bærum municipality).

2.3 Measures 2.3.1 Quality of life (QofL) – outcome variable Quality of life was measured using the Norwegian version of the Inventory of Life Quality in

Children and Adolescents (ILC; Mattejat & Remschmidt, 2006, Jozefiak 2012; Jozefiak,

Larsson & Wichstrom, 2009). Children reported on seven items addressing different aspects

of quality of life: school, family, social integration, interests and recreational activities,

physical health, mental health, and global quality of life item (Appendix II). Each item is

rated on a 5-point scale from 1 – ”very good” to 5 – ”very bad” using ”smiley” icon as an

anchor. The mean score was calculated, and items were reversed coded if necessarily in such

a way that the higher the mean score the higher is reported quality of life. Children used

computers in the school setting to answer questions. The reliability showed acceptable

Cronbach’s alpha .66, which is similar to previous studies examining the psychometric

properties of the Norwegian version of the ILC on general population of children, reporting

Page 28: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

16

Cronbach’s alpha .63 (e.g. Jozefiak et al., 2009). Jozefiac and colleagues (2009, 2012) have

previously reported satisfactory norms and measures of validity and reliability of Norwegian

version of the ILC.

Originally, the ILC questionnaire was developed as a practical instrument that would be able

to access children’s subjective quality of life over the past week. Target group of the ILC is

children and adolescents with mental and somatic disorders aged from 6 to 18 years, but

lately it had also been administrated on the general population samples (Jozefiak et al., 2009).

The Norwegian adaptation of the ILC was translated by Jozefiak and Linnemann in 2008 and

was based on an original German ILC version (Mattejat & Remschmidt, 2006). The

Nowegian version of the ILC was approved by original authors.

2.3.2 Reciprocal friendship and popularity variables Information about children’s friends had been accessed by using a modification of the

”The Bus Story” procedure developed by Perren and Alsaker (2006). Children were given

the following instruction: ”You are going on a trip. Whom of your friends from your class

could you think of inviting with you?” According to the instruction, they could nominate up

to five friends from the class (both girls and boys), but children actually nominated no more

than four friends.

Friendship and popularity ranking was examined by different procedures to investigate the

meaning of popularity, directionality and reciprocity of the nominations. All produced

variables required manual scoring - looking at each child’s response one by one and counting

for each nomination and its directionality within one class.

Based on procedures from previous studies (e.g. Ostberg, 2003, Parker & Asher 1993) a sum

score of the number of nominations was used for each child in order to compute a popularity

variable. It varied from 0 to 9 nominations. In the next step reciprocal friendship variable was

computed. It was counted as a number of reciprocal friend nominations. By analyzing

children’s popularity nominations we were able to detect directional links between nominated

children and the child who nominated those children as his/her friends and whether those

links were reciprocal or not, and if they were reciprocal then to what degree. As a result, the

following scale had emerged: ‘0’ for those who didn’t have any reciprocal friend

Page 29: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

17

nominations, ‘1’ meant that the child got 1 reciprocal scoring back from those who had been

nominated by him or her, ‘2’ indicated that 2 out four nominated friends nominated him or

her as a friend as well, ‘3’ – the child was nominated back as a friend by 3 out 4 of his/her

friends on the list, finally, ‘4’ means an absolute reciprocal match as it describes the situation

when the child was nominated as a friend by all the children that he or she nominated as

friends.

2.3.3 Control variable All children provided the information about their age and gender. In addition, both mothers

and fathers were asked about their level of education that had been coded in 4 categories:

elementary school (1), high school (2), university degree up to 3 years (3), and university

degree 4 years and more (4).

2.4 Preliminary analysis The analyses of collected data were made by using IBM SPSS Statistics, version 22. Prior to

the main statistical analyses, the outcome variable (quality of life) was examined for its

distribution, outliers, missing and reliability.

For missing data pairwise exclusion had been applied (Pallant, 2013). As for normal

distribution’s tests, a skewness of -1.00 and a kurtosis of 1.21 were found. It means that the

scores are clustered to the right of the high values, which is not unusual when the majority of

participants report high level of quality of life. In normative samples, low levels of self-

reported quality of life are rare (e.g. Jozefiak et al., 2009). The positive kurtosis value

indicates that the distribution is clustered in the center, shaped like a peak. These features are

reported to be common for cases when the large samples are utilized (Tabachnick & Fidell,

2013). A Kolmogorov-Smirnov statistic showed a significant p-value of .000 suggesting that

the distribution is significantly different from a normal distribution, which is not unusual for

the large samples (Pallant, 2013, Tabachnick & Fidell, 2013).

Histogram (Appendix III) confirms the negative skew. In Normal Q-Q plots for quality of life

variable a deviation tale of lower values from the zero line is observed (Appendix III). Data

was also checked for outliers.

Maximum of Cook’s distance was .036, which is under the critical 1.0 (Tabachnik & Fidell,

2013).

Page 30: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

18

Chronbach’s alpha coefficient was used as an indicator for the internal consistency of the

quality of life scale. It showed a good internal consistency of .63, which is within .48-.76 that

is considered to be high for questioners with less then 10 items (Pallant, 2013).

In order to perform one way-way ANOVA, the Levene’s test for Equality of variances was

conducted. From its results, we can reject the null hypothesis that there are no differences in

the variance between the groups and accept the alternative hypothesis that there is a

difference between the groups.

2.5 Statistical analysis Descriptive analysis was made in order to examine the basic features of the variables.

Followed by bivariate correlation analysis to determine if there exist any statistically

significant correlations between variables and to examine their strength. Further, hierarchical

linear regression analysis was conducted to find out if popularity and number of reciprocal

friends were associated with self-reported quality of life. The demographic variables like age,

gender and parents’ level of education have also been included in the analysis. In was

performed a set of hierarchical regression analyses – one with popularity as an independent

variable and quality of life as dependent, than one with dichotomous reciprocal friendship

variable and quality of life, and finally, associations between actual number of reciprocal

friend nominations and quality od life was . In order to examine if a gender had an effect on

reported by children quality of life an independent t-test was conducted. T-test had been also

conducted in order to examine whether having one friend was associated with higher quality

of life or not. Further, a series of One-Way ANOVA was performed to determine whether

there were any statistically significant differences in reported quality of life between groups

of children with different popularity scores, reciprocal nominations and father’s level of

education respectively.

Page 31: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

19

3 Results 3.1 Descriptive analyses The detailed information about descriptive features of variables is presented in Table 1. As it

is shown in the table, the majority of participants reported high level of quality of life. Only

9% of the respondents were unpopular, while mean score for popularity was 3 nominations.

16.4 % of the children didn’t have a reciprocal friend in the class, at the same time over 75%

of children had 1-3 reciprocal nominations, where mean score showed to be approximately 2

nominations. Equal number of girls and boys participated in the study. Majority of their

parents had a university degree.

Table 1. Descriptive features of the sample

Variables n % Mean SD Skewness Kurtosis

1. Quality of life 4.44 .41 -1.00 1.21

low (1-4) 65 9.4

high (4-5) 626 90.6

2. Popularity nominations 3.08 2.00 .47 -.24

0 62 9.0

1 108 15.6

2 115 16.6

3 136 19.7

4 113 16.4

5 77 11.1

6 33 4.8

7 33 4.8

8 10 1.4

9 4 0.6

3. Number of reciprocal nominations 1.81 1.20 .09 -.96

0 113 16.4

1 180 26.0

2 181 26.2

3 159 23.0

4 58 8.4

Page 32: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

20

4. Age (in years) 11.70 .46 -.88 -1.23

11 207 29.9

12 484 70.1

5. Gender .50 .50 .00 -2.01

Boys 346 50.1

Girls 345 49.9

6. Maternal education 3.00 .76 -.18 -.79

Elementary school 7 1.0

High school 137 19.8

University (up to 3 years) 257 37.2

University (4 years +) 152 22.0

Missing 138 20.0

7. Paternal education 2.94 .81 -.14 -.92

Elementary school 12 1.7

High school 160 23.2

University (up to 3 years) 222 32.1

University (4 years +) 150 21.7

Missing 147 21.3

3.2 Bivariate correlation analyses Table 2. Correlations between variables

Variables: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

1. Quality of life -

2. Popularity .196** -

3. Number of reciprocal nominations .238** .743** -

4. Education (mother) .082 .060 .037 -

5. Education (father) .163** .129** .161** .464** -

6. Gender .021 .031 .075* .100* .097* -

7. Age .016 .065 .054 .057 .010 .028 -

Note. bold type *p<.05, **p<.01

Prior to the main analysis, the bivariate correlations between variables had been examined

(Table 2). Bivariate correlations mostly showed weak positive associations between

Page 33: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

21

variables. Associations between quality of life and children’s age and gender, as well as level

of mother’s education didn’t reach the statistical significance and their Pearson correlation

coefficient was close to 0 indicating no associations between variables. On the other hand,

popularity, number of reciprocal nominations, as well as father´s level of education had

reached the statistical significance with Pearson correlation coefficient under .30.

Associations between popularity and quality of life were weakly and positively correlated.

Positive and weak correlations were observed between number of reciprocal friend

nominations and quality of life. Father’s education also showed a small, albeit significant

association with quality of life. The strongest correlation to the quality of life variable

showed the reciprocal friend nominations variable, followed by popularity and father’s

education.

3.3 Hierarchical linear regression analyses In order to investigate how popularity and reciprocal friendship contribute to quality of life

variable, a series of multiple regression analyses was examined.

3.3.1 Popularity and quality of life Table 3. Quality of life predicted by popularity variable

Variables Model 1 Model 2

B SE Beta B SE Beta

Popularity .037 .009 .181** .034 .009 .164**

Age -.007 .042 -.008

Gender -.026 .037 -.032

Education (mother) -.003 .028 -.005

Education (father) .078 .026 .154**

R square .032** .056*

Note. Bold type *p<.05. **p<.01

When examining whether popularity predicted quality of life, hierarchical regression analyses

showed that popularity nominations significantly predicted higher quality of life reported by

children. The effect remained to be significant after controlling for potential confounders.

Paternal education was also significantly associated with quality of life.

Page 34: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

22

3.3.2 Reciprocal friendship and quality of life Table 4. Quality of life predicted by reciprocal friendship (dichotomous) variable

Variables Model 1 Model 2

B SE Beta B SE Beta

Reciprocal friend nomination

(dichotomous) .198 .052 .173** .178 .009 .156**

Age -.001 .041 -.001

Gender -.023 .037 -.029

Education (mother) .002 .028 .004

Education (father) .078 .026 .155**

R square .030** .054*

Note. Bold type *p<.05. **p<.01

When examining whether reciprocal friendship (dichotomous) predicted quality of life,

hierarchical regression analyses showed that reciprocal friendship (dichotomous)

significantly predicted higher quality of life reported by children. The effect remained to be

significant after controlling for other demographic variables. Level of father’s education was

also significantly associated with quality of life.

3.3.3 Number of reciprocal nominations and quality of life Table 5. Quality of life predicted by number of reciprocal nominations

Variables Model 1 Model 2

B SE Beta B SE Beta

Reciprocal friend nominations .084 .015 .248** .079 .016 .232**

Age -.008 .041 -.009

Gender -.035 .037 -.043

Education (mother) .004 .027 .008

Education (father) .068 .026 .134**

R square .062** .081*

Note. Bold type *p<.05. **p<.01

Page 35: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

23

When examining whether number of reciprocal nominations could predict quality of life,

hierarchical regression analyses showed that number of reciprocal nominations significantly

predicted higher quality of life reported by children. The effect remained to be significant

after controlling for potential confounders. Paternal education was also significantly

associated with quality of life.

3.3.4 Summarized results from multiple regression analyses Results showed that both popularity and reciprocal friendship were positively correlated to

quality of life variable even when controlled for age, gender and parents’ education. All

regression models reached statistical significance level. Out of all regression models, the

model that included reciprocal friend nominations contributed best to explain the variance in

the quality of life variable. Age and gender didn’t have any statistical significant influence on

quality of life.

3.4 Independent t-test 3.4.1 Quality of life and reciprocal friendship An independent t-test was conducted to compare children’s reports on quality of life in

having a reciprocal friend and not having a reciprocal friend conditions. From the results of

Levene’s Test for Equality of Variances, we can reject the null hypothesis that there is no

difference in the variances between the groups and accept the alternative hypothesis that there

is a difference between the groups. There was a significant difference in the scores for having

a reciprocal friend (M=4.47, SD=0.38) and not having a reciprocal friend (M=4.30, SD=0.48)

conditions; t(141)=-3.5, p=.001. These results suggest that having a reciprocal friend has an

effect on children’s quality of live. Specifically, our results suggest that when children have

at least one reciprocal friend they report higher quality of life as compared to those who have

no reciprocal friends.

3.4.2 Quality of life and gender An independent t-test was conducted to compare children’s reports on quality of life

depending on their gender. There has not been found a significant difference in the quality of

life scores for boys (M=4.43, SD=0.40) and girls (M=4.45, SD=0.41) conditions; t(689)=-.54,

p=.59. These results suggest that being a boy or a girl doesn’t have an effect on children’s

Page 36: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

24

quality of live. Specifically, our results suggest that children of both genders report similar

level of quality of life.

3.4.3 Quality of life and paternal education Since the linear regression analysis showed that father’s education was significantly

associated with children’s self-reported quality of life, an independent t-test was conducted to

compare children’s reports on quality of life depending on father’s education level: high

school versus university degree. There was a significant difference in the scores for high

school (M=4.34, SD=0.46) and university degree (M=4.50, SD=0.37) conditions; t(232)=-

3.78, p=.000. These results suggest that level of father’s education has an effect on children’s

quality of live. In other words, our results suggest that children report on average higher

quality of life when their fathers have a degree from university as compared to those children

whose fathers finished high school.

3.5 One-way ANOVA To answer the research questions whether there were any differences in self-reported quality

of life and peer relation variables, one-way ANOVA group comparison had been performed.

3.5.1 Popularity and quality of life Table6.One-wayANOVA:popularity–comparinggroup SS Df F P η²

Between groups 5.359 6 5.628 .000 .05

Within groups 108.562 684

Total 113.921

Note: group sizes are unequal

A one-way between subjects ANOVA was conducted to compare the effect of popularity on

children's quality of life in having no nominations, having one, two three, four, five and six

and more nomination conditions. There was found a significant effect of popularity on

reported quality of life at the p<.001 level for the seven conditions [F(6,684)=5.63, p=.000].

Page 37: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

25

Figure 1. Mean level of reported QoL by number of popularity nominations

Post hoc comparisons using the Bonferroni test (Appendix IV) indicated that the mean score

for being unpopular (having no nominations at all) condition was significantly different from

any other condition (except for having two nominations). In other words, these results

suggest that unpopular children tend to report the lowest quality of life compared to those

who have got at least one and more popularity nomination from their peers. However, the

number of nominations doesn’t seem to play a significant role in this comparison, even

though overall tendency for reported quality of life increases with number of nominations

(except for two and 6 and more nominations) – see Figure 1

3.5.2 Reciprocal friendship and quality of life Table7.One-wayANOVA:numberofreciprocalnominations–comparinggroup SS Df F P η²

Between groups 7.027 4 11.273 .000 .06

Within groups 106.895 686

Total 113.921

Note: group sizes are unequal

A one-way between subject ANOVA was conducted to compare the effect of number of

reciprocal friends on children's quality of life in having no reciprocal friend, having one, two

three or four reciprocal friends conditions (due to measuring and scoring procedures, latter

condition describes an absolute reciprocity condition). There was a significant effect of

4

4.1

4.2

4.3

4.4

4.5

4.6

Popularity (number of nominations)

Qua

lity

of li

fe

0

1

2

3

4

5

6 and more

Page 38: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

26

number of reciprocal friends on reported quality of life at the p<.001 level for the five

conditions [F(4, 693)=11.34, p=.000].

Figure 2. Mean level of reported QoL by number of reciprocal nominations

Post hoc comparisons using the Bonferroni test (Appendix IV) indicated that the mean score

for having no reciprocal friend condition was significantly different than having two, three or

four reciprocal friends conditions, while having one reciprocal friend condition didn’t differ

significantly from having no reciprocal friend condition. However, having one reciprocal

friend was significantly different than having four reciprocal friends and didn’t differ

significantly from having no, two or three reciprocal friends. Having two reciprocal friends

condition was significantly different from having no and having four reciprocal friends

conditions. Further, having three reciprocal friends condition was significantly different from

having no reciprocal friend, one and four reciprocal friends conditions, while it didn’t differ

significantly from having two reciprocal friends condition. Finally, having four reciprocal

friends condition was significantly different from all other reciprocal friend conditions. In

other words, these results suggest that when children have reciprocal friends they tend to

report higher quality of life compared to those who don’t have any friends. Specifically, the

bigger the difference in the number of friends, the bigger the difference in quality of life

scores.

4 4.1 4.2 4.3 4.4 4.5 4.6 4.7

Friendship (number of reciprocal nominations)

Qua

lity

of L

ife

0

1

2

3

4

Page 39: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

27

3.5.3 Paternal education and quality of life

Table8.One-wayANOVA:father’slevelofeducation–comparinggroup SS Df F P η²

Between groups 2.719 2 8.36 .000 .03

Within groups 76.610 471

Total 79.329

Note: group sizes are unequal

Since it was only 10 persons that had elementary school education elementary and high

school education were combined together in to high school category. Further a one-way

between subject ANOVA was conducted to compare the effect of father’s level of education

on children's quality of life in having high school and university degree (up to 3 years and 4

years and more) conditions. There was a significant effect of father’s level of education on

reported by children quality of life at the p=.000 level for the four conditions [F(2,

471)=8.36, p=.000].

Figure 3. Mean level of reported QoL by father’s level of education

Post hoc comparisons using the Bonferroni test indicated that the mean score in the reported

quality of life in reported quality of life was significantly different for those children whose

fathers finished high school, as compared to those who had fathers with a degree from

university. However, number of yeas spent at the university didn’t seem to matter, as

children, on average, reported exactly the same level of quality of life.

4.2

4.3

4.4

4.5

4.6

Father's level of education

Qua

lity

of L

ife

High school

University (up to 3 years)

University (4 years and more)

Page 40: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

28

4 Discussion The overall goal of the current study was to examine associations between popularity and

reciprocal friendship and children’s self-reported quality of life. There are two main findings

that stand out. First, both popularity and reciprocal friendship had a positive association with

children’s quality of life. Second, number of nominations (both for popularity and reciprocal

friendship) played a significant role for mentioned above associations.

4.1 Popularity and quality of life A positive but weak association was found for popularity and children’s quality of life,

suggesting that the higher the popularity level among classmates the higher quality of life

was reported. This finding is similar to previous studies, which also reported weak but

positive associations (e.g. Holder, 2012, Ostberg, 2003,). Although, the main tendency was

that quality of life increased with the number of popularity nominations, the findings also

suggest that the social status i.e. popular versus unpopular, rather than the number of

nominations, was related to higher levels of quality of life. Thus, the social position in a class

is also relevant to the children`s quality of life.

In this study only children who didn’t get any popularity nominations from their classmates

reported lower quality of life. One possible explanation might be that being accepted in

general is perceived as more desirable than not being accepted by anyone. Thus, the number

of nominations is not as important as being liked by at least one of your peers. During middle

childhood, preferences in peer relationships start to shift toward being concerned with

whether one is accepted by peers or not (Rubin et al., 2013). The need to belong is considered

to be one of the basic needs (Nesdale, Durkin, Maass, & Griffiths, 2004). A desire to fulfill

this need represents the driving force for establishing various social interactions, including

interactions with peers. Relationships with peers are thought to influence children’s vision

and perception of themselves (Rubin et al., 2006). By continuing with this logic it is possible

to assume that low versus high social status among classmates would affect the way a child

thinks and feels about himself/herself. Findings from previous studies support such a

conclusion (e.g. Rubin et al., 2013).

Page 41: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

29

Other possible factors that might play a role here is that the school is compulsory for

everyone, and children spend on average 5-6 hours per day at school. In other words, children

have to spend a great amount of time at school. Thus, social interactions between classmates

are an every-day life experience. Therefore, if this environment is unfortunate in terms of

social contacts, it might have a significant impact on one’s perception of the overall quality

of life. In fact, disliked children have been found to evaluate their social experiences as less

positive and often view themselves as less competent in social interactions as compared to

the more liked classmates (e.g Rubin et al., 2013). Unpopular children are more likely to face

unpleasant experiences with their peers as being bullied and victimized, which is associated

with higher internalizing and externalizing problems (Cillessen, 2011; Rubin et al., 2013).

Hence, decreasing the children´s quality of life (e.g. Ladd, 2009; Rubin et al., 2013).

Another key issue worth considering is the measure instrument of popularity itself. In the

current study, popularity measures as a continuum beginning with positive nominations

(Perren & Alsaker, 2006). Negative peer nominations are not included, and usually are not

used in Norway because of ethical reasons (we do not ask children to point out classmates

that they dislike, therefore in a Norwegian setting a lack of peer nominations indicates

children that are disliked). Hence, the method used in the current study differs from that of

the classic sociometric popularity method (Coie, Dodge, Coppotelli, & Scarr, 1982). One

could argue that excluding negative nominations made it difficult to compare findings from

this study with those that examine sociometric popularity and its outcomes. The number of

nominations was limited up to five, but the children nominated no more than four classmates.

This kind of limitation in children’s answers is rather beneficial as it strengthens the validity

of such reports (Cilllessen, 2011). Moreover, the unlimited number of nominations is

preferred to be used in large groups, while a limited number of nominations is considered to

fit better in smaller groups (Cillesen, 2011).

However, it is worth to mention that since the current study is based on a normal sample,

even children with no nominations from their peers on average evaluated their quality of life

as “good”. These findings seem to go in line with findings from the previous study based on

the normal population of Norwegian children (Jozefiak et al., 2009). Findings from their

study indicate that children on average report satisfactory to high levels of their quality of life

reported by children age from 8 to 16 years old.

Page 42: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

30

One possibility in the explanation of such a high level of the quality of life is that the children

could have established successful peer relationships outside of their classrooms, or school.

Therefore one limitation of our finding is that we did not have measures of peer relations

outside the child’s classroom, such as friends in a neighborhood, or children’s involvements

in leisure time activities (hobbies and sport engagement). Thus, there is a possibility that they

have successful relations elsewhere. In fact, in a Norwegian study conducted by Ogden

(1995), was found that 95 % of children in 4th grade had between 2 to 5 friends outside of the

school. However, others have found a much greater overlap in children’s network of friends

at school and outside of the school (Kvello, 2006). Participants in that study were children

from 4th, 7th and 9th grades. On average 50% of friends that children had at school were also

reported as preferable to spend their leisure time with.

To sum up, results from the current study seem to be consistent with previous studies that

showed a positive association between popularity and the quality of life (e.g. Holder &

Coleman, 2009; Ostberg, 2003). Overall, few studies have however examined the link

between quality of life specifically and popularity in the age group studied here (11-12-year-

olds). The current finding suggests that popularity is as important for children’s self-reported

quality of life in middle childhood as it have been reported for adolescents and young

adulthood (Demir & Urberg, 2004; Kasser & Ahuva, 2002).

4.2 Reciprocal friendship and quality of life. Similarly to popularity, a moderately weak positive associations were found for reciprocal

friendship and quality of life. Such associations indicate that the increased number of

reciprocal friend nominations results in higher scores of self-reported quality of life. Similar

findings have been reported by a number of recent studies (Ben-Arieh et al., 2014; Casas et

al., 2007; Children’s society, 2012; Holder & Coleman, 2009; Goswami, 2012). For example,

findings from the Holder and Coleman study (2009) show that children with many friends

reported a higher level of happiness as compared to those with few friends. In that study, the

author’s questionnaire had just one question on whether a child had many friends or not,

without specifying about the number of friends or if those relationships were reciprocal or not

(very often friendships are considered reciprocal by default). The current study examines

more closely the friendship nominations and their relations to children’s quality of life.

Page 43: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

31

Indeed, results from the current study suggest that the number of reciprocal nominations have

an impact on the children’s quality of life. Those children who didn’t have any reciprocal

friends in their class reported the lowest level of quality of life as compared to their

classmates with reciprocal friends. Interestingly, children with no friends didn’t differ from

those with one reciprocal friend. This particular finding contradicts with what has been

reported in previous studies, as having at least one reciprocal friend is considered to promote

children’s adjustment and quality of life (Bowker et al, 2011; Bukowski et al., 1998, Parker

& Asher, 1993). In the current study, an interesting trend emerged. Level of quality of life

didn’t differ between groups of children close to each other in number of nominations, except

four reciprocal nominations (Appendix IV). One question is whether this could reflect a

desire to achieve a certain level in social interactions among one’s peers? The current finding

suggests that more reciprocal nominations appear as more desirable than only having one.

This particular finding contradicts to what has been commonly found in previous studies,

where having at least one reciprocal friend has been found to be a protective factor for

maladjustment (e.g. Hodges et al., 1999; Parker & Asher, 1993). Perhaps in a normative

sample, with a relatively high rate of quality of life, multiple reciprocal friendships become

more important to feel good about oneself, and thus, the more reciprocal nominations a child

gets, the higher is reported level of quality of life. On the other hand, there is further a

possibility that this finding reflects other individual or group characteristics. For example,

Ostberg (2003) suggests that unpopular children may indicate that there exists a greater

pressure for conformity, a higher degree of social control and a lower tolerance in a group.

An interesting pattern, however, was that those children who got four out of four possible

reciprocal nominations also were those who reported the highest level of quality of life. This

level of quality of life was significantly different from any other number of reciprocal

nominations. For example, in a study by Uusitalo-Malmivaara and Lehto (2013) findings

suggests that children who were least happy considered having more friends as a crucial

factor for increasing levels of happiness. Friends therefore may become a resourceful asset in

terms of providing assistance and support and promoting the development of necessary social

skills. They are also important for affect regulation and constructive conflict resolution

(Rubin et al., 2013). Therefore, it is reasonable to assume, that with the increased number of

reciprocal friends the chance that children’s needs for social interactions with peers are also

met. Such fulfillment of one’s social needs and desires might positively influence the child’s

level of quality of life.

Page 44: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

32

Previous studies confirm that children perceive their first choice to nominate as a friend, as

more positively valued as compared to second or third ones (e.g. Kiesner, Nicotra & Notari,

2005). Children’s first choice in friend nominations is considered to have a stronger impact

on the children’s adjustment and well-being as compared to other friends (e.g. Urberg, 1992).

Such conclusions somewhat contradict with findings from the current study as at least two

reciprocal nominations and not one was found to reach the significance level as compared to

those children with no reciprocal friends in the class. A possible explanation why two and not

one reciprocal friend were important in contrast to no friend nomination is related that the

amount of time spend with friends increases during late childhood and adolescence. Thus,

having two friends versus having just one doubles the chances of spending leisure time

together. However, researchers have pointed to the need for further research that examines

friendship reciprocity and relative correlates, in order to gain a better understanding of its

influence on adjustment throughout childhood and into adolescence (e.g. Holder & Coleman,

2009, Rubin et al., 2013)

However, we should also consider that growing up in a digital age it is necessary to take into

consideration the importance of Internet technologies for both one’s quality of life as well as

establishing social relationships online. Researchers emphasize the influential role of digital

technologies, as they are viewed as common devices for every-day activities, both for

children and adults (e.g. Ben-Arieh et al., 2014; Demir, 2015). Overall, it seems like Internet-

based interactions show positive correlation with positive friendship experiences both online

and offline (Asher et al., 2014; Ben-Arieh et al., 2014). In other words, Internet represents

unlimited possibilities to maintain and develop social relationships. Thus, those who might

experience challenges in establishing reciprocal friendships at school might benefit from

online friendships, and therefore friendlessness at school would not affect the overall quality

of life. However, we did not have such data in the current study, but it could be an interesting

future perspective to gain a better understanding of friendship in children´s every-day lives.

Rubin et al. (2013) mention several key factors for relations between friendship and quality

of life, such as, individual characteristics of the child, the characteristics of the friend, and the

friendship’s stability and intimacy. They also suggest that friendship represents the safe

environment where the children’s basic need for social interactions beside family is met.

Other researchers emphasize the importance of friendship quality and its role in child quality

of life (Demir, 2015) or feeling of closeness and satisfaction (Lyubomirsky et al., 2005).

Page 45: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

33

However, in the current study these possible correlates have not been measured, but would be

interesting to examine in further research.

4.3 Associations between demographic factors and

quality of life Findings from the current study seem to correspond with the findings from earlier research,

and suggest that the father’s level of education was positively, but weakly associated with

children’s quality of life.

The level of education might be a proxy for income, which in its turn is considered to

contribute to the person’s quality of life. Children, whose fathers reported having bachelor

degree or more, reported a higher level of quality of life as compared to those children whose

fathers just completed high school education. Though, no statistically significant differences

within the levels of university education have been found, which suggest amount of years

spent by children’s fathers at university does not impact differently on the quality of life of

their children (i.e. there were no difference for a bachelor or a master degree or above).

Similar findings have been reported in terms of associations between income and happiness

(Dolan et al., 2008; Frey & Stutzer, 2009). It has been shown that increases in income

contribute to happiness, but beyond a certain level, it starts to diminish. In other words, a

father completing a bachelor degree seems to be sufficient for the children’s quality of life.

Children’s age and gender didn’t show any associations with quality of life. It indicates that

both girls and boys evaluate their quality of life equally. It is consistent with findings from

previous studies that also report no gender differences in the levels of happiness (Holder &

Coleman, 2009; Uusiato-Malmivaara & Lehto, 2013), however, they did not measure quality

of life as it was done in the current study.

It has been expected that age wouldn’t be associated with the quality of live, since the

majority of participants was 12 years old. Samples with more diverse age groups would be a

good further step in follow-up analyses in HOPP.

Page 46: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

34

4.4 Strengths, limitations and suggestions for future

research There are several strengths with this study that should be mentioned. It is based on a

relatively large representative sample (number of valid participants after obtaining parent’s

consent and counting for missing was 691 children). The data was collected from a normal

population of children (and not a clinical subgroup), which suggests that findings are valid

for generalization. Measure instruments like the Inventory of Life Quality in Children and

Adolescents (ILC), sociometric and reciprocal nominations represent the well-known and

internationally recognized set of methods. In other words, such methods make it easier to

compare findings not only across Norway, but also with studies from other countries.

Another strength of the current study is that there are actual counts of the number of

nominations for each child as well as the directionality of nominations is detected (i.e.

reciprocity) as compared to other studies that had only one question about number of friends

and just assume reciprocity of the friendship without verifying it (e.g. Holder & Coleman,

2009). Thus, the current study provides a unique perspective by not only examining

associations between friendship and quality of life in young children, but specifically,

investigating the role of friendship’s reciprocity for children’s quality of life.

The number of students in schools varied from school to school, but the number of students

in a class was on average the same, indicating that children’s opportunities for maintaining

peer relationships were equally distributed for all children. Missing, on the other hand, is

considered to have a much greater impact on obtained results. The participation in this study

was voluntarily, but in order for child to be considered as eligible for participations, a written

consent from at least one of his or her parents was also required. This resulted in that not all

students in the class were eligible to participate. Even more, due to various reasons not all

participants showed up during the data collection. Such missing could lead that popularity

and reciprocal friendship scoring wouldn’t reflect the actual social interactions within the

class.

Another strength of the current study is that the data is based on children’s reports, which are

believed to be the most adequate way to access personal experiences, thoughts and beliefs

(e.g. Ben-Arieh et al., 2014). Even though debates about reliability and validity of children as

Page 47: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

35

informants are still ongoing, the majority of researchers recommend children to be the key

informant about their own feelings and relationships (Ben-Arieh et al., 2014). However, for

studies where children are informants, a multi-informant design is often used and to be

preferred (Ben-Arieh et al., 2014). Popularity, friendship and quality of life variables

represent phenomena strongly related to personal experiences, thus children’s self-reports are

therefore considered a major strength in the current study. Nevertheless, one should bear in

mind that some of the key concepts could for some children be difficult to understand and to

separate from each other (i.e. what is a friend, and best friend). To counterbalance such

potential bias, there were trained research assistants present during the assessments that took

place in the schools classrooms. Indeed, there were some of the children who asked the

research assistants to clarify the meaning of some of these concepts.

It is important to notice that there exist other factors that have not been measured in the

current study, but that have shown correlations with one’s relations with peers. Therefore,

this study cannot address causation, and thus study findings should be interpreted with

caution.

On the other hand, since findings show that popularity and reciprocal friendship explain

respectively only about 3% and 6% of the variance in quality of life variable, it is reasonable

to assume that there exist other variables, not measured in this study, contributing to the

children’s quality of life. Findings from other studies suggest that such core factors for the

quality of life may be family relationships and friendship quality (e.g. Goswami, 2012,

Diener et al., 2008). Therefore, more research on children’s quality of life and its correlates is

needed.

Another limitation is the study sample is cross-sectional, and therefore, the direction of the

associations is not known. It also implies that the scores in the quality of life are more likely

to reflect the short-term quality of life tied to the situation there and then. Thus the question

whether the quality of life is stable over time remains to be answered by future research. Peer

relationships are not static but under the constant development, therefore a longitudinal

design is optimal. Over the next years, however, longitudinal information will be available.

To sum up, the current study contributes with examining associations between quality of life

and peer relations, more specifically, popularity and reciprocal friendship each of which

Page 48: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

36

uniquely predicted the quality of life. The research on children’s quality of life and its

correlates is rather novel, therefore studies investigating ways of promoting children’s quality

of life are much needed. School settings seem like a perfect place in implementing the

interventions based on current and previous research.

4.5 Implications Findings from this study suggest that the design and implementation of prevention and

intervention programs aimed at fostering positive development and promoting high level of

quality of life should consider peer relationships, and especially, friendship as a part of those

programs. Both schools and parents may be active agents of such programs, by promoting

positive aspects within peer relationships (like popularity and friendship) one would expect to

increase quality of life as well. Parents may contribute by helping a child to expand the areas

where it is possible to establish successful relationships, for example by signing in to a new

sport section or reading club at school. Teachers may influence by organizing school

activities in such a way that children with few or no friends would have bigger chances of

establishing new ones.

Since the present study is based on sample of children from normal population, result may

also describe general tendencies for normal population of children. Latter can be rather

beneficial for clinical practice and research focusing on health-related quality of life.

Knowing how children on average score their quality of life makes it comparable to reports

from clinical subgroup of children.

Although research that is focused on peer relationships and quality of life seems to be

important both from preventive and clinical perspectives, it is necessary to pay attention to

what kind of unexpected consequences it may lead. Finding from this study describe general

tendencies in groups of a certain population, therefore it cannot define each and every person

from that population. It is important to remember in order to avoid possible stigmatization of

participants. Self-reported questionnaires may put a child in situation when he or she needs to

focus on unpleasant experiences, which can negatively impact child’s general condition and

state of mood. The children that didn’t get a parental consent in order participate in the study

may feel themselves excluded from the much larger group of those who could participate in

the project. It is important to have in mind that such situations may potentially take place.

Page 49: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

37

Therefore a reaction plan needed to be developed in order to successfully cope with these

situations and their participants.

Page 50: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

38

5 Conclusion The current thesis examined the associations between quantitative features of popularity and

reciprocal friendship (number of nominations) and quality of life reported by children. Both

popularity and reciprocal friendship had a positive association with children’s quality of life.

Findings suggest that the social status i.e. popular versus unpopular, rather than the number

of nominations, was related to the children’s quality of life. The number of popularity

nominations was not as important as being liked by one of peers. On the contrary, number of

reciprocal friend nominations played a significant role in quality of life. Those children who

didn’t have any reciprocal friends in their class reported the lowest level of quality of life as

compared to their classmates with reciprocal friends. Even more, those children who received

four out of four possible reciprocal nominations reported the highest level of quality of life.

However, it is worth to mention, that participants in this study were children from normal

population and both children who are disliked by their peers and children with no reciprocal

friends in the class evaluated on average their quality of life as ”good”. Results from this

study also suggest that both popularity and reciprocal friendship were not strong predictors of

quality of life, indicating that there exist other variables, not measured in this study,

contributing to the children’s quality of life. Thus, identifying these fundamental factors,

which influence children’s quality of life, could be beneficial for developing programs

promoting high quality of life and hence preventing possible maladjustments in a long-term

perspective. School settings seem to be a perfect place to implement such programs.

Page 51: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

39

References

Abecassis, M., Hartup, W. W., Haselager, G. J. T., Scholte, R. H. J., & Van Lieshout, C. F. M. (2002). Mutual antipathies and their significance in middle childhood and adolescence. Child Development, 73(5), 1543. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00489

Argyle, M. (1999). Causes and correlates of happiness. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N.

Schwartz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 353–373). New York: Russell Sage.

Asher, G., Guerry W. B., & McDonald, K. L. (2014). Children as friends. In G. B. Melton, A.

Ben-Arieh, J. Cashmore, G. S. Goodman, & N. K. Worley (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of child research. doi:10.4135/9781446294758.n10

Baird, B., Lucas, R., & Donnellan, M. (2010). Life satisfaction across the lifespan: Findings

from two nationally representative panel studies. An International and Interdisciplinary Journal for Quality-of-Life Measurement, 99(2), 183-203. doi:10.1007/s11205-010-9584-9

Bagwell, C. L., Newcomb, A. F., & Bukowski, W. M. (1998). Preadolescent friendship and

peer rejection as predictors of adult adjustment. Child Development, 69(1), 140-153. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06139.x

Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, N.J: Prentice Hall. Barstad, A. (2014). Levekår og livskvalitet: Vitenskapen om hvordan vi har det. Oslo:

Cappelen Damm akademisk. Ben-Arieh, A., Casas, F., Frønes, I., & Korbin, J. E. (2014). Handbook of child well-being:

Theories, methods and policies in global perspective: Springer Netherlands. Retrieved from https://link.springer.com/book/10.1007/978-90-481-9063-8/page/1

Berndt, T. J., & Keefe, K. (1995). Friends' influence on adolescents' adjustment to school.

Child Development, 66(5), 1312-1329. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00937.x Bowker, J. C., Thomas, K. K., Norman, K. E., & Spencer, S. V. (2011). Mutual best

friendship involvement, best friends' rejection sensitivity, and psychological maladaptation. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40(5), 545-555. doi:10.1007/s10964-010-9582-x

Bukowski, W. M., Buhrmester, D., & Underwood, M. K. (2011). Peer relations as a

developmental context. In M. K. Underwood & L. H. Rosen (Eds.), Social development: Relationships in infancy, childhood, and adolescence (pp. 153–179). New York: The Guilford.

Bukowski, W. M., Hoza, B., & Boivin, M. (1993). Popularity, friendship, and emotional

adjustment during early adolescence. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 60, 23-37. doi:10.1002/cd.23219936004

Page 52: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

40

Bukowski,W. M., Newcomb, A. F., & W. W. Hartup, W. W. (1998). The company they keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Burt, S. A. & Donnellan, M. B. (2015) Toward a Developmentally Sensitive and Genetically

Informed Perspective on Popularity. In B.N. Horwitz & J.M. Neiderhiser (Eds.), Gene-environment interplay in interpersonal relationships across the lifespan, Advances in behavior genetics Vol. 3 doi:10.1007/978-1-4939-2923-8_7

Casas, F., Figuer, C., Gonzalez, M., & Malo, S. (2007). The values adolescents aspire to,

their well-being and the values parents aspire to for their children. Social Indicators Research, 84(3), 271-290. doi:10.1007/s11205-007-9141-3

Cillessen, A. (2011). Popularity. In R. Levesque (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Adolescence (pp.

2104-211). Retrieved from https://link.springer.com/book/10.1007/978-1-4419-1695-2/page/27

Cillessen, A. H. N., & Bellmore, A. D. (2011). Social skills and social competence in

interactions with peers. In P. K. Smith & C. H. Hart (Eds.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of childhood social development (2nd ed., pp. 393–412). Malden, MA: Blackwell.

Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., Coppotelli, H., & Scarr, S. (1982). Dimensions and types of social

status: A cross-age perspective. Developmental Psychology, 18(4), 557-570. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.18.4.557

Demir, M. (2015). Friendship and happiness: Across the life-span and cultures. Retrieved

from https://link.springer.com/book/10.1007%2F978-94-017-9603-3 Demir, M., & Urberg, K. A. (2004). Friendship and adjustment among adolescents. Journal

of Experimental Child Psychology, 88(1), 68-82. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2004.02.006 Demır, M., & Weitekamp, L. (2007). I am so happy ’cause today I found my friend:

Friendship and personality as predictors of happiness. An Interdisciplinary Forum on Subjective Well-Being, 8(2), 181-211. doi:10.1007/s10902-006-9012-7

Diener, E., Horwitz, J., & Emmons, R. (1985). Happiness of the very wealthy. An

International and Interdisciplinary Journal for Quality-of-Life Measurement, 16(3), 263-274. doi:10.1007/BF00415126

Diener, E., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2004). Beyond money: Toward an economy of well-being.

Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 5(1), 1-31. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00501001.x

Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., Smith, H. L., & Eisenberg, N. (1999). Subjective Well-

Being: Three Decades of Progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125(2), 276-302. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.2.276

Dolan, P., Peasgood, T., & White, M. (2008). Do we really know what makes us happy? A

review of the economic literature on the factors associated with subjective well-being. Journal of Economic Psychology, 29(1), 94-122. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2007.09.001

Page 53: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

41

Dougherty, L. R (2006). Children’s emotionality and social status: a meta-analytic review.

Social Development, 15(3), 394-417. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2006.00348.x Dunn, J. (2004). Children’s friendships: The beginning of intimacy. Massachusetts:

Blackwell Publishing. Dunn, J. & McGuire, S. (1992). Sibling and peer relationships in childhood. Journal of Child

Psychology and Psychiatry, 33(1), 67-105. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1992.tb00859.x Easterlin, R. A. (2011). Happiness, growth, and the life cycle. New York: Oxford University

Press. Eilertsen, M., Jozefiak, T., Rannestad, T., Indredavik, M. S., & Vik, T. (2012). Quality of life

in children and adolescents surviving cancer. European Journal of Oncology Nursing, 16, 185-193. doi:10.1016/j.ejon.2011.08.001

Epstein, J. L. (1983). Friends in school: Patterns of selection and influence in secondary

schools. New York, NY: Academic Press. Fellinger, J., Holzinger, D., Sattel, H., & Laucht, M. (2008). Mental health and quality of life

in deaf pupils. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 17(7), 414-423. doi:10.1007/s00787-008-0683-y

Fink, E., Begeer, S., Peterson, C. C., Slaughter, V., & Rosnay, M. (2015). Friendlessness and

theory of mind: A prospective longitudinal study. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 33(1), 1-17. doi:10.1111/bjdp.12060

Fredriksen, P. M., Hjelle, O. P., Mamen, A., Meza, J.T., & Westerberg A.C. (2017) The

Health Oriented Pedagogical Project (HOPP). A controlled longitudinal intervention trial of a school-based physical activity program. BMC Public Health 17:370. DOI:10.1186/s12889-017-4282-z

Frey, B. & Stutzer, A. (2009). Should national happiness be maximized? In A. K. Dutt og B.

Radcli (Red.), Happiness, economics and politics: Towards a multi-disciplinary approach (s. 301-323). Cheltenham: Edward Elgar.

Gasper, D. (2010). Understanding the diversity of conceptions of well-being and quality of

life. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 39(3), 351-360. doi:10.1016/j.socec.2009.11.006

Gazelle, H. (2008). Behavioral profiles of anxious solitary children and heterogeneity in peer

relations. Developmental Psychology, 44(6), 1604-1624. doi:10.1037/a0013303 Goswami, H. (2012). Social relationships and children's subjective well-being. Social

Indicators Research, 107(3), 575-588. doi:10.1007/s11205-011-9864-z Hansell, S. (1981). Ego development and peer friendship networks. Sociology of Education,

54(1), 51-63. doi:10.2307/2112512

Page 54: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

42

Hartup, W. W. (1996). The company they keep: friendships and their developmental significance. Child Development, 67(1), 1-13. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01714.x

Hay, D. F., Payne, A. & Chadwick, A. (2004). Peer relations in childhood. Journal of Child

Psychiatry, 45(1), 84-108. doi: 10.1046/j.0021-9630.2003.00308.x Helsedirektoratet. (2015). Well-being på norsk. Retrieved from

https://helsedirektoratet.no/Lists/Publikasjoner/Attachments/971/Well-being%20på%20norsk%20IS-2344.pdf

Herzer, M., Zeller, M. H., Rausch, J. R., & Modi, A. C. (2011). Perceived social support and

its association with obesity-specific health-related quality of life. Journal of developmental and behavioral pediatrics, 32(3), 188-195 doi:10.1097/DBP.0b013e318208f576

Hodges, E. V. E., Boivin, M., Vitaro, F., & Bukowski, W. M. (1999). The power of

friendship: Protection against an escalating cycle of peer victimization. Developmental Psychology, 35(1), 94-101. doi:10.1037//0012-1649.35.1.94

Holder, M. D. (2012). Happiness in children: measurement, correlates and enhancement of

positive subjective well-being. Springer Netherlands. Retrieved from https://link.springer.com/book/10.1007/978-94-007-4414-1/page/1

Holder, M., & Coleman, B. (2009). The contribution of social relationships to children’s

happiness. An Interdisciplinary Forum on Subjective Well-Being, 10(3), 329-349. doi:10.1007/s10902-007-9083-0

Hopkins, K. D., Zubrick, S. R., & Taylor, C. L. (2014). Resilience amongst Australian

Aboriginal youth: An ecological analysis of factors associated with psychosocial functioning in high and low family risk contexts. PLoS ONE, 9(7). doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0102820

Jozefiak, T. (2012). ILC - Inventory of Life Quality in Children and Adolescents. Stockholm:

Hogrefe Psykologiförlaget AB. Jozefiak, T., Larsson, B., & Wichstrom, L. (2009). Changes in quality of life among

Norwegian school children: a six-month follow-up study. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 7, 7-7. doi:10.1186/1477-7525-7-7

Kasser, T., & Ahuvia, A. (2002). Materialistic values and well‐being in business students.

European Journal of Social Psychology, 32(1), 137-146. doi:10.1002/ejsp.85 Kiesner, J., Nicotra, E., & Notari, G. (2005). Target specificity of subjective relationship

measures: Understanding the determination of item variance. Social Development, 14(1), 109-135. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2005.00293.x

Page 55: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

43

Kvello, Ø. (2006) Barns og unges vennskap: Empiri- og teorigjennomgang, en kvantitativ og kvalitativ studie av barns oppfatninger og presentasjoner av sosiale relasjoner til jevnaldre[n]de og voksne i forhold til noen sentrale mål på psykososial tilpasning. Retrieved from https://brage.bibsys.no/xmlui/handle/11250/269154

Ladd, G. W. (2009). Trends, travails, and turning points in early research on children’s peer

relashionships. In K. H. Rubin, W. M. Bukowski, & B. Laursen (Eds.), Handbook of peer interactions, relationships, and groups (pp. 20-41). New York: Guilford.

Ladd, G. W., Emerson, E. S., & Scarr, S. (1984). Shared knowledge in children's friendships.

Developmental Psychology, 20(5), 932-940. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.20.5.932 Lansford, J. E., Criss, M. M., Pettit, G. S., Dodge, K. A., & Bates, J. E. (2003). Friendship

quality, peer group affiliation, and peer antisocial behavior as moderators of the link between negative parenting and adolescent externalizing behavior. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 13(2), 161–184. doi:10.1111/1532-7795.1302002

Laursen, B., Bukowski, W. M., Aunola, K., & Nurmi, J. E. (2007). Friendship moderates

prospective associations between social isolation and adjustment problems in young children. Child Development, 78(4), 1395-1404. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01072.x

Levin, K. A., Torsheim, T., Vollebergh, W., Richter, M., Davies, C. A., Schnohr, C. W., . . .

Currie, C. (2010). National income and income inequality, family affluence and life satisfaction among 13 year old boys and girls: A multilevel study in 35 Countries. Social Indicators Research, 104, 179–194. doi:10.1007/s11205-010-9747-8

Lodder, G. M. A., Scholte, R. H. J., Goossens, L., & Verhagen, M. (2015). Loneliness in

early adolescence: Friendship quantity, friendship quality, and dyadic processes. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 1-12. doi:10.1080/15374416.2015.1070352

Lyubomirsky, S., Sheldon, K. M., Schkade, D., Candland, D. K., Baumeister, R. F., &

Simonton, D. K. (2005). Pursuing happiness: The architecture of sustainable change. Review of General Psychology, 9(2), 111-131. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.9.2.111

Mattejat F, Remschmidt H: Das Inventar zur Erfassung der Lebensqualität bei Kindern und

Jugendlichen (ILK) – (in German). (The inventory of life quality in children and adolescents ILC) Bern: Hans Huber Verlag; 2006

Mayeux, L., Sandstrom, M. J., & Cillessen, A. H. N. (2008). Is being popular a risky

proposition? Journal of Research on Adolescence, 18(1), 49-74. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2008.00550.x

McMahan, E. A. og Estes, D. (2012). Age-related differences in lay conceptions of well-

being and experienced well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies, 13(1), 79-101. doi:0.1007/s10902-011-9251-0

Mead G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Page 56: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

44

Michalos, A. C. (2008). Education, happiness and well-being. Social Indicators Research, 87(3), 347-366. doi:10.1007/s11205-007-9144-0

Nangle, D. W., Erdley, C. A., Newman, J. E., Mason, C. A., & Carpenter, E. M. (2003).

Popularity, friendship quantity, and friendship quality: interactive influences on children's loneliness and depression. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 32(4), 546-555. doi:10.1207/S15374424JCCP3204_7

Nes, R. B. (2017). Livskvalitet og trivsel i Norge. Folkehelseinstituttet. Retrieved from

https://www.fhi.no/fp/psykiskhelse/psykiskhelse/livskvalitet-og-trivsel-i-norge/ Nesdale, D., Durkin, K., Maass, A., & Griffiths, J. (2004). Group status, outgroup ethnicity

and children's ethnic attitudes. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 25(2), 237-251. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2004.02.005

Newcomb, A. F. & Bagwell, C. L. (1995). Children's friendship relations: a meta-analytic

review. Psychological Bulletin, 117(2), 306-347. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.117.2.306 Ogden, T. (1995). Kompetanse i kontekst. En studie av risiko og kompetanse hos 10- og 13-

åringer. Oslo: Barnevernets Utviklingssenter, Rapport nr. 3. Ostberg, V. (2003). Children in classrooms: Peer status, status distribution and mental well-

being. Social Science and Medicine, 56, 17–29. doi:10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00006-0 Pallant, J. (2013). SPSS survival manual: A step by step guide to data analysis using IBM

SPSS (5th ed. ed.). Maidenhead: McGraw-Hill. Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1993). Friendship and friendship quality in middle childhood:

Links with peer group acceptance and feelings of loneliness and social dissatisfaction. Developmental Psychology, 29(4), 611-621. doi:10.1037//0012-1649.29.4.611

Parkhurst, J., & Hopmeyer, A. (1998). Sociometric popularity and peer-perceived popularity:

Two distinct dimensions of peer status. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 18(2), 125-144. doi:10.1177/0272431698018002001

Perren, S., & Alsaker, F. D. (2006). Social behavior and peer relationships of victims, bully-

victims, and bullies in kindergarten. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(1), 45-57. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01445.x

Piaget J. (1932). The moral judgment of the child. Glencoe, IL: Free Press. Pinquart, M., Sörensen, S., & Light, L. L. (2000). Influences of socioeconomic status, social

network, and competence on subjective well-being in later life: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 15(2), 187-224. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.15.2.187

Proctor, C., Linley, P. A., & Maltby, J. (2010). Very happy youths: Benefits of very high life

satisfaction among adolescents. Social Indicators Research, 98(3), 519-532. doi:10.1007/s11205-009-9562-2

Page 57: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

45

Rojas, M. (2011). Happiness, income, and beyond. The Official Journal of the International Society for Quality-of-Life Studies, 6(3), 265-276. doi:10.1007/s11482-011-9153-7

Rose, A. J. (2002). Co-Rumination in the Friendships of Girls and Boys. Child Development,

73(6), 1830-1843. Retreived from http://www.jstor.org/stable/3696420?seq=1#page_scan_tab_contents

Rubin, K. H., Bowker, J. C., McDonald, K. L., & Menzer, M. (2013). Peer relationships in

childhood. In P. D. Zelazo (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Developmental Psychology, Vol. 2: Self and Other, Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199958474.013.0011

Rubin, K., Bukowski W., & Parker J. (2006). Peer interactions, relationships, and groups. In

W. Damon, R. Lerner, & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (571–645). New York: Wiley. doi:10.1002/9780470147658.chpsy0310

Sandstrom, M. J., & Cillessen, A. H. N. (2006). Likable versus popular: Distinct implications

for adolescent adjustment. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30, 305–314. doi:10.1177/0165025406072789

Schei, J., Jozefiak, T., Nøvik, T. S., Lydersen, S., & Indredavik, M. S. (2016). The impact of

coexisting emotional and conduct problems on family functioning and quality of life among adolescents with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 20(5), 424-433. doi:10.1177/1087054713507976

Sijtsema, J. J. (2016). Preadolescents and their friends: Similarity in aggression and

depressive problems as a function of social status and friendship reciprocity. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 40(6), 565-576. doi:10.1177/0165025415607380

Sirgy, M. J. (2012). The psychology of quality of life: Hedonic well-being, life satisfaction,

and eudaimonia. Retrieved from https://link.springer.com/book/10.1007/978-94-007-4405-9/page/1

Sullivan H. S. (1953). The interpersonal theory of psychiatry. New York: Norton. Svavarsdottir, E.K., & Olysdottir, B. (2006). Health-related quality of life in Icelandic school

children. Scandinavian Journal of Caring Science, 20(2), 209-215. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6712.2006.00397.x

Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2013). Using multivariate statistics (6th ed., International

ed. ed.). Boston: Pearson. The Children’s Society (2012). The good childhood report 2012. A summary of our findings.

Retreived from https://www.childrenssociety.org.uk/sites/default/files/tcs/good_childhood_research_summary_final_0.pdf

Page 58: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

46

Urberg, K. (1992). Locus of peer influence: Social crowd and best friend. A Multidisciplinary Research Publication, 21(4), 439-450. doi:10.1007/BF01537896

Uusitalo-Malmivaara, L., & Lehto, J. E. (2013). Social factors explaining children's

subjective happiness and depressive symptoms. Social Indicators Research, 111(2), 603-615. doi:10.1007/s11205-012-0022-z

Vygotskij, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes.

Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press. Wentzel, K. R., & Asher, S. R. (1995). The academic lives of neglected, rejected, popular,

and controversial children. Child Development, 66(3), 754-763. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00903.x

Page 59: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

47

Appendices Appendix I: Flowchart of sample selection

7 schools from Horten 1 school from Akershus 1 school from Bærum

957 students from 6th and 7th grades

802 elegible students with informed consent

691 students included in the current study;

response rate: 72.3%

students from 2nd to 5th grades were

excluded due to project design

155 students did not obtain parent consent for study participation

111 students were absent during the collection of data

Page 60: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

48

Appendix II: The Inventory of Life Quality in Children and Adolescents (ILC, Norwegian

variant)

1. Skole

På skolen har du jo mange fag. Du må lære mye i disse, skriving, regning osv. Hvor godt får

du dette til? Hvordan klarer du det?

2. Familie

Hvor godt kommer du overens med moren din, faren din, og dine søsken og hvordan er de

mot deg? Hvordan går det med deg i din familie?

3. Andre barn

Når du gjør noe sammen med andre barn. Hvordan er de andre barna mot deg og hvor godt

kommer du overens med andre barn?

4. Alene

Når du er for deg selv, for eksempel leker eller gjør noe annet, hvordan føler du deg da?

Page 61: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

49

5. Helse

Er du for tiden frisk og i god form eller føler du deg syk? Hvor godt er altså helsen din?

6. Humør

Er du for det meste irritert, trist osv. eller er du for det meste i godt humør? Hvor bra er altså

humøret ditt?

7. Alt sammen

Hvis vi tenker på alle spørsmålene som ble still til deg; alt sett under ett, hvordan har du det?

Page 62: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

50

Appendix III: Tests of normality: Quality of life variable

Figure A1. Histogram

Figure A2. Normal Q-Q plot

Page 63: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

51

Appendix IV: One-way ANOVA post hoc tables

Table A1. One-Way ANOVA Comparison of Quality of Life and Children’s Popularity

Bonferroni Comparisons/

Mean Difference

Number of

nominations n Mean SD 0 1 2 3 4 5

6 and

more

0 62 4.22 .47 - .19* .16 .24* .28* .31* .30*

1 108 4.41 .38 - -.03 .05 .09 .12 .11

2 115 4.38 .47 - .08 .12 .15 .14

3 137 4.46 .35 - .04 .07 .06

4 113 4.50 .39 - .03 .02

5 77 4.53 .34 - -.01

6 and more 79 4.52 .39 -

Note. bold type with * means p<.05.

Table A2. One-Way ANOVA Comparison of Quality of Life and Children’s Reciprocal

Friendship

Bonferroni Comparisons/

Mean Difference

Number of reciprocal

nominations n Mean SD 0 1 2 3 4

0 113 4.30 .48 - .07 .17* .19* .37*

1 180 4.37 .41 - .10 .12* .30*

2 181 4.47 .38 - .02 .20*

3 159 4.49 .35 - .18*

4 58 4.67 .32 -

Note. bold type with * means p<.05.

Page 64: Associations between popularity, reciprocal friendship and

52

Table A3. One-Way ANOVA Comparison of Quality of Life and Paternal Education

Bonferroni Comparisons/

Mean Difference

Father’s education n Mean SD 1 2 3

1. High school 145 4.34 .46 - .16* .16*

2. University degree (up to 3 years) 198 4.50 .37 - .00

3. University degree (4 years and more) 131 4.50 .38 -

Note. bold type with * means p<.05.