article evidence for selective executive function deficits...

30
Article Evidence for selective executive function deficits in ecstasy/polydrug users Fisk, John and Montgomery, C. Available at http://clok.uclan.ac.uk/1191/ Fisk, John and Montgomery, C. (2009) Evidence for selective executive function deficits in ecstasy/polydrug users. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 23 (1). pp. 40-50. ISSN 0269-8811  It is advisable to refer to the publisher’s version if you intend to cite from the work. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0269881108089815 For more information about UCLan’s research in this area go to http://www.uclan.ac.uk/researchgroups/ and search for <name of research Group>. For information about Research generally at UCLan please go to http://www.uclan.ac.uk/research/ All outputs in CLoK are protected by Intellectual Property Rights law, including Copyright law. Copyright, IPR and Moral Rights for the works on this site are retained by the individual authors and/or other copyright owners. Terms and conditions for use of this material are defined in the http://clok.uclan.ac.uk/policies/ CLoK Central Lancashire online Knowledge www.clok.uclan.ac.uk

Upload: ngomien

Post on 01-May-2018

219 views

Category:

Documents


3 download

TRANSCRIPT

Article

Evidence for selective executive function deficits in ecstasy/polydrug users

Fisk, John and Montgomery, C.

Available at http://clok.uclan.ac.uk/1191/

Fisk, John and Montgomery, C. (2009) Evidence for selective executive function deficits in ecstasy/polydrug users. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 23 (1). pp. 40­50. ISSN 0269­8811  

It is advisable to refer to the publisher’s version if you intend to cite from the work.http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0269881108089815

For more information about UCLan’s research in this area go to http://www.uclan.ac.uk/researchgroups/ and search for <name of research Group>.

For information about Research generally at UCLan please go to http://www.uclan.ac.uk/research/

All outputs in CLoK are protected by Intellectual Property Rights law, includingCopyright law. Copyright, IPR and Moral Rights for the works on this site are retained by the individual authors and/or other copyright owners. Terms and conditions for use of this material are defined in the http://clok.uclan.ac.uk/policies/

CLoKCentral Lancashire online Knowledgewww.clok.uclan.ac.uk

John E Fisk

1

Evidence for selective executive function deficits in ecstasy/polydrug users.

John E. Fisk, PhD

University of Central Lancashire,

Catharine Montgomery, PhD

Liverpool John Moores University

Running head: selective executive function deficits

Corresponding author:

Dr John E Fisk

Department of Psychology

University of Central Lancashire

Preston PR1 2HE

United Kingdom

Tel 44 (0) 1772 894465

Fax 44 (0) 1772 892925

e-mail: [email protected]

John E Fisk

2

Abstract

Previous research has suggested that the separate aspects of executive functioning

are differentially affected by ecstasy use. While the inhibition component executive

process appears to be unaffected by ecstasy use, it is unclear whether this is true of heavy

users under conditions of high demand. Tasks loading on the updating component

executive process have been shown to be adversely affected by ecstasy use. However, it

remains unclear whether the deficits observed reflect the executive aspects of the tasks

and whether they are domain general in nature affecting both verbal and visuo-spatial

updating. Fourteen heavy ecstasy users, 39 light ecstasy users and 28 non users were

tested on tasks loading on the inhibition executive process (random letter generation) and

the updating component process (letter updating, visuo-spatial updating and computation

span). Heavy users were not impaired in random letter generation even under conditions

of high demand. Ecstasy related deficits were observed on all updating tasks. These

deficits remained statistically significant following controls for various aspects of

cannabis use. It was concluded that the inhibition component executive process is

unaffected by ecstasy use even among heavy users. By way of contrast, the updating

component process appears to be impaired in ecstasy users with the deficit apparently

domain general in nature.

Keywords: ecstasy (MDMA), inhibition, updating, executive, random generation,

visuo-spatial.

John E Fisk

3

There is cause to believe that among the many illicit drugs commonly in use, ecstasy in

particular causes long term impairment to cognitive processes through its effects on the

serotonin system (Gouzoulis-Mayfrank & Daumann 2006; Morgan, 2000; Reneman et al

2006). Morgan (2000, page 234) has noted that ‘it has been proposed that it [serotonin]

may play an orchestrating role in cognition’. More specifically, the serotonin system is

believed to underpin working memory processes through its modulation of the

dopaminergic systems that support prefrontal executive processes (Luciana, Collins, &

Depue, 1998; Robbins 2000).

Recent investigations of executive functioning suggest that the central executive

is fractionated. For example, Miyake et al. (2000) studied the separability of three

supposed executive functions: mental set shifting (“shifting”), information updating and

monitoring (“updating”), and inhibition of pre-potent responses (“inhibition”). Structural

equation modelling revealed that the three executive functions were clearly separate and

appeared to contribute differentially to performance on higher level tasks that are known

to be reliant on prefrontal cortical resources. For example, the Wisconsin Cart Sort Task

(WCST) was linked to the shifting component, the Tower of Hanoi to the inhibition

component, random number generation to both the inhibition and updating components,

and operation span to the updating component.

Using factor analysis, Fisk and Sharp (2004) provided further support for Miyake

et al’s model, finding that reading span and computation span (analogous to Miyake et

al’s operation span task), letter updating (Morris & Jones, 1990) and a visuo-spatial serial

recall task (Brooks, 1967) all loaded on a single factor corresponding to Miyake et al’s

updating executive process. Aspects of random letter generation loaded on a separate

John E Fisk

4

factor corresponding to Miyake et al’s inhibition executive process, while WCST

measures loaded on a third factor (equivalent to Miyake et al’s switching executive

process). Additionally, a fourth factor emerged which Fisk and Sharp termed access to

long-term memory and on which verbal fluency tasks loaded.

Previous research from our laboratory (Fisk et al, 2004; Montgomery et al, 2005;

Wareing et al 2004) utilising Miyake et al’s conceptual framework, revealed ecstasy-

related deficits in memory updating (computation span and letter updating), and access to

long-term memory (verbal fluency). No differences were observed on tasks assessing the

switching (the plus-minus and number letter task) and inhibition (random letter

generation) component processes. However inconsistencies in this area of research have

emerged. From our own laboratory, while Wareing, et al (2000) found ecstasy users to be

impaired in random letter generation, we have failed to obtain this outcome in subsequent

studies (Fisk, et al, 2004; Montgomery, et al, 2005). Similarly the ecstasy-related deficits

in relation to the updating executive component process that emerged from our own

research (Montgomery et al, 2005) have not have been replicated by Dafters (2005) using

the keeping track task.

Clearly these inconsistencies require explanation and warrant further research.

Wareing et al’s (2000) participants had an atypically high estimated lifetime ecstasy dose

exceeding 1000 tablets and the random letter generation task used, restricted participants

to producing only consonants. In our later study (Fisk et al, 2004) those we tested had an

appreciably lower lifetime exposure to the drug and we administered the original version

of the task (Baddeley 1966) in which any letter of the alphabet may be produced. It is

possible therefore that ecstasy might impair the inhibition component process but only at

John E Fisk

5

high does and only when the processing load is substantial. To resolve this ambiguity, it

would be desirable to repeat the consonants only random generation task on a group of

high dosage users in order to replicate our previous findings.

Questions have also emerged concerning our previous results which demonstrated

ecstasy-related updating component executive deficits. First at six letters, the

maintenance element of the updating task exceeded the letter span of the majority of

participants that we tested. Consequently many participants may have adopted a free

recall recency based strategy negating the need for updating (Collette et al, 2006; Smith-

Spark et al, 2003). It would be desirable to repeat our original experiment ensuring that

the maintenance component of the task does not exceed the letter span of our participants.

Second, we have recently demonstrated ecstasy-related deficits in visuo-spatial working

memory tasks (Wareing et al, 2004; 2005) although it is unclear which executive

component processes may be implicated in this regard. Recent conceptualisations of

executive function tasks suggest the processing component of these is domain general

(e.g., Bayliss et al 2003; Kane et al , 2004). If the updating component does reflect some

domain general process it would be expected that the impairments that we have observed

in verbal updating would also manifested in visuo-spatial updating .

Method

Design and Analysis.

For the random letter generation task, a mixed design was used with rate of letter

generation (with three levels; 4 seconds, 2 seconds, and 1 second)) within participants

John E Fisk

6

and ecstasy users group (with three levels; heavy, light, and nonuser) between

participants. Dependent variables were the number of alphabetically ordered pairs,

number of repeated pairs, redundancy, and the number of vowel intrusions. With regard

to the updating executive component tasks, verbal and spatial updating, and the

computation span task, a multivariate design was used with user group (3 levels) between

participants and the three measures of executive functioning as dependent variables.

Orthogonal contrasts were employed in which heavy ecstasy users were compared to

light users and the two ecstasy user groups combined compared with nonusers.

Orthogonal contrasts were used as they allow inter-group comparisons to be made while

controlling the Type 1 error rate without the need to adjust the alpha level per comparison

(Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001).

Participants.

Fourteen heavy ecstasy users (mean age 22.86; 9 male), 39 light ecstasy users

(mean age 21.41; 19 male) and 28 non-user controls (mean age 20.71; 7 male) took part

in the study. The heavy users group comprised all of those users with an estimated

lifetime dose exceeding 400 tablets, (mean 1000.21, s.d., 786.41). Light users were those

with an estimated lifetime dose of less than 400 tablets, (mean 149.69, s.d., 96.91). The

cut off point of four hundred tablets was determined by trial and error so as to produce a

high use group with a mean lifetime dose of 1000 tablets as this was the level of exposure

characterising the ecstasy users in our previous study (Wareing et al 2000). Nonusers

were those who indicated that they had never used ecstasy. Participants were recruited via

direct approach to university students, and the snowball technique (Solowij et al, 1992).

With 14 ecstasy users, the present sample is sufficient to detect a difference of 1.25 for α

John E Fisk

7

= .05 and β =.20 (Hinkle et al, 1994). Participants were requested to refrain from ecstasy

use for at least 7 days and ideally 10 days prior to testing. The mean period of abstinence

was actually 22 and 27 weeks for heavy users and light users respectively; median

abstinence period 5.5 and 4 weeks respectively. Participants were also requested not to

use any other illicit drugs for at least 24 hours and ideally for 7 days prior to testing.

Measures.

Patterns of drug use and other relevant lifestyle variables were investigated via

means of a background questionnaire. The questionnaire gauged the use of ecstasy and

other drugs, as well as age, years of education, general health and other relevant lifestyle

variables. In relation to other drugs, participants were asked a range of questions

including frequency and duration of use and the last time that they had used each drug.

Participants were also questioned concerning their history of drug use, and using a

technique employed by Montgomery, et al (2005), these data were used to estimate total

lifetime use for each drug. Average weekly dose and the amount of each drug consumed

within the previous 30 days were also assessed. Fluid intelligence was measured via

Raven’s Progressive Matrices (Raven et al, 1998), and premorbid intelligence was

assessed via the National Adult Reading Test (NART, Nelson, 1982).

Letter Span: Consonants were presented sequentially on a computer screen for

1.25 seconds. Participants were then required to recall the letters in the order in which

they were presented. The task commences with three sets of two letters, and is then

increased to three sets of three, four, five etc., until the individual fails on at least two out

of three trials. Digit span was administered in a similar manner except that the letters

were replaced with digits.

John E Fisk

8

Updating. Updating has been used extensively as a measure of prefrontal

executive functioning. (see for example, Fisk & Sharp, 2003; 2004; Miyake et al, 2000;

Morris & Jones, 1990; Smith-Spark & Fisk, in press; van der Linden et al 1999). The

participant’s letter span, ‘n’, was determined. In the consonant updating task the

participant was presented with a random sequence of between n and n+6 consonants on a

computer screen. Twenty-four such lists were presented, and in each case, the participant

was unaware of the number of consonants to be presented. The task was always to recall

the most recent ‘n’ consonants in the order in which they were presented. (Thus the

maintenance element of the task was limited to the individual’s actual span. This

contrasts favourably with our previous study where at six letters the maintenance

requirement exceeded the span of most of our participants.) The participant experienced

six trials at each of the four list lengths: n, n+2, n+4, and n+6 items. The order in which

the lists were presented was randomised. A single composite score of updating was

calculated by computing the average number correct for each serial position over the 6

trials at each list length. The resulting figures were then averaged over list length and

serial position.

The spatial updating task was analogous to consonant updating except that it

involved the serial recall of cells that were highlighted sequentially, one at a time, in a

Corsi style display. The participant’s spatial span, ‘n’, was determined. In the updating

task the participant was presented with a random sequence of between n and n+6 cells

highlighted on a computer screen in a Corsi style configuration. Twenty-four such

sequences were presented, and in each case, the participant was unaware of the number of

cells that were highlighted. The task was always to recall the most recent ‘n’ cells in the

John E Fisk

9

order in which they were presented. As in the consonant task, the participant experienced

six trials at each of the sequence lengths. The order in which the lists were presented was

randomised. A single composite score of updating was calculated using the same

procedure as outlined for the consonant updating task.

Computation Span. Computation span has been used extensively as an indicator

of working memory functioning in the cognitive ageing literature (Fisk & Warr, 1996;

Salthouse & Babcock, 1991) and it is similar to the operation span measure used by

Miyake, et al (2000) in their investigation of executive processes. Participants were

required to solve a number of arithmetic problems (e.g., 4+7 = ?) by circling one of three

multiple-choice answers as each problem was presented. They were also required to

simultaneously remember the second digit of each presented problem. At the end of each

set of problems the second digits had to be recalled in the order in which they were

presented. The number of arithmetic problems that the participant had to solve, while at

the same time remembering each second digit, gradually increased as the test proceeded.

For each of the first three trials only a single problem was presented. For the next three

trials, two problems were presented. Subsequently, the number of problems presented per

trial increased by one every third trial. In order to proceed, the participant was required to

be correct in at least two of the three trials at the current level. Computation span was

defined as the maximum number of end digits recalled in serial order, with the added

requirement that the corresponding arithmetic problems had been solved correctly. In

order to take account of individual differences in the non-executive maintenance

component of the task, the load on executive resources was computed as the percentage

John E Fisk

10

difference between the computation and digit span scores. Large percentage differences

are indicative of poor executive functioning.

Random letter generation. A computer display and concurrent auditory signal was

used to pace responses. Participants were asked to speak aloud a letter every time the

signal was presented. They were told to produce only consonants (i.e., to avoid the letters

a, e, i, o, and u); to avoid repeating the same sequence of letters; to avoid producing

alphabetically ordered sequences; and to try to speak each letter with the same overall

frequency. Individuals attempted to produce three sets of 100 letters; one set at a rate of

one letter every 4 s, a second set at one letter every 2 s, and a third at one letter every 1 s.

The order in which the sets were generated was randomised. The experimenter recorded

the responses on an answer sheet. The test yields four scores. First, the number of

alphabetically ordered pairs; second, a repeat sequences score corresponding to the

number of times that the same letter pair is repeated; third, a “redundancy” score, which

measures the extent to which all 26 letters of the alphabet are produced equally often (0%

being truly random); and fourth, the number of vowel intrusions. In all cases, higher

scores are indicative of poor performance.

Procedure.

Participants were informed of the general purpose of the experiment, and written

informed consent was obtained. The tests were administered under laboratory conditions,

and a computer running MS-DOS was used for the computer based tasks. The tests were

administered in the following order: background questionnaire, random letter generation,

digit, letter, and spatial span, computation span, letter and spatial updating, Raven’s

progressive matrices, and the NART. Participants were fully debriefed, paid £15 in store

John E Fisk

11

vouchers, and given drugs education leaflets. The study was approved by the Ethics

Committee of Liverpool John Moores University, and was administered in accordance

with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Results

There were no significant differences between the groups on the Raven’s and

NART measures and the number of years of education. Inspection of Table 1 reveals that

age differed significantly between the groups. Tukey’s test revealed that heavy users were

significantly older than the other two groups, p<.01 and p<.05 in relation to nonusers and

light users respectively. Light users and nonusers did not differ significantly from each

other. Table 1 also contains mean simple span scores (digit, letter and spatial) for each of

the three groups. Ecstasy users were unimpaired on these measures in fact scoring

marginally higher compared to non ecstasy users.

<Insert Table 1 about here>

The vast majority of all of the groups had previous exposure to cannabis. Ninety-

three percent of heavy ecstasy users, 87% of light users and 75% of non ecstasy users

indicated that they had used cannabis. Cocaine use was also common among ecstasy

users. All heavy ecstasy users and 72% of light ecstasy users indicated that they had

previously used cocaine. However, only 11% of nonusers had had any exposure to this

drug. Other drugs that had been previously used included amphetamine and LSD but use

of these was less prevalent and limited to the ecstasy user groups. Sixty-four percent of

heavy ecstasy users and 33% of light ecstasy users indicated that they had previously

used amphetamine. The equivalent figures for LSD were respectively, 64% and 13%. For

John E Fisk

12

those drugs where it was possible to quantify the level of use a number of relevant

measures are set out in Table 2. Heavy ecstasy users also consumed substantial quantities

of cocaine and cannabis. Light ecstasy users also used these drugs but to a lesser extent.

Among non ecstasy users the only illicit drug that was used was cannabis but the

exposure to this drug was of a lower order compared to the other two groups. (Three non

ecstasy users indicated that they had used cocaine on one or two previous occasions but

were unable to quantify the amount.) For the most part the measures of drug use were not

normally distributed. In many instances the median response was zero and more generally

the distributions were positively skewed. In addition the group variances were generally

heterogeneous. As a consequence nonparametric analyses were used with the results

revealing that most of the indicators of drug use set out in Table 2 differed significantly

between the groups. With regard to differences between the two ecstasy user groups,

heavy users scored significantly higher than light users in terms of total lifetime and

average weekly consumption of both cannabis and cocaine. Heavy users had used ecstasy

and cocaine significantly longer than light users although length of cannabis user did not

differ significantly between the two ecstasy user groups. Also there were no significant

differences between heavy and light ecstasy users in terms of their use of illicit drugs in

the previous 10 and 30 days.

<Insert Table 2 about here>

Outcomes for the different aspects of executive functioning are set out in Table 3. In all

aspects of random letter generation the scores were similar between the groups. Mixed

ANOVA was used with group between participants and rate of letter generation within.

Consistent with the trends evident in the means, none of the group effects were

John E Fisk

13

statistically significant, for each of the four analyses (redundancy, repeat sequences,

alphabetical sequences and vowel intrusions), F < 1. Also in each case the interaction

between group and rate of letter generation was non significant, again in each of the four

analyses, F < 1. The number of letters produced was close to ceiling (i.e., 100 letters) for

all groups at the four and two second generation rates. Although performance declined at

the one second rate this was equally apparent for all groups. The maximal number of

letters generated by most participants at the four and two second rates prevents

meaningful analysis. However, ANOVA with group between participants and the number

of letters generated at the one second rate as the dependent variable revealed no

significant difference between the groups, F < 1. Thus to summarise the present results

provide no evidence of ecstasy-related differences in the inhibition component executive

process on which random letter generation is known to load.

<Insert Table 3 about here>

The percentage reduction in capacity associated with the concurrent processing

component of the computation span task is set out in Table 3 along with the outcomes for

the spatial and verbal (consonant) updating tasks. All three measures are believed to load

on the updating executive component process. For each of these three indicators of

executive functioning non ecstasy users outperform both ecstasy user groups. Non users

exhibit substantially lower costs in relation to the processing component of the

computation span task. They also exhibit more efficient updating both in relation to

verbal and visuo-spatial stimuli. MANOVA with user group between participants and the

three measures of the updating component executive process as dependent variables

revealed a significant multivariate group effect, Wilks’ lambda = .836, F(6,150)=2.34,

John E Fisk

14

p<.05. Regarding the univariate outcomes, the group differences were statistically

significant for both computation span and spatial updating, with F values of 3.23 and 3.97

respectively on 2,77 degrees of freedom, p<.05 in both cases. The group difference for

the verbal updating task failed to reach significance, F(2,77) = 2.23, p>.05. Difference

contrasts revealed that nonusers performed significantly better than the combined user

groups on the computation span and spatial updating measures, p<.05 and p<.01

respectively, the difference approached significance for the verbal updating task, p=.091.

However, the contrasts revealed that the two user groups did not differ from each other

on any of the updating measures, p>.05 in all cases.

As inspection of Table 2 revealed, ecstasy users (especially heavy users)

consumed considerably more cannabis compared to non ecstasy users both in terms of

long term and recent use. It is possible therefore that various aspects of cannabis use may

have accounted for the ecstasy-related group differences that were obtained on the

updating executive component measures. To control for this possibility, the MANOVA

was repeated with the two long term (total lifetime consumption and average weekly

consumption) and two short term measures of cannabis use (amount used in the last 10

and 30 days) as covariates. The multivariate group effect remained significant, Wilks’

lambda = .793, F(6,118)=2.42, p<.05. Regarding the univariate outcomes, the group

differences were statistically significant for verbal updating and spatial updating with F

values of 3.86 and 3.62 respectively on 2,61 degrees of freedom, p<.05 in both cases. The

group difference for the computation task approached significance, F(2,61) = 3.02,

p=.056. Difference contrasts revealed that nonusers performed significantly better than

the combined user groups on all three measures, p<.05 in all cases, while the two user

John E Fisk

15

groups did not differ significantly from each other on any of the measures, p>.05 in all

cases. Tests for homogeneity of regression were conducted for each covariate with

respect to each of the dependent variables yielding a total of 12 analyses. In 11 of the 12

cases, homogeneity of regression was obtained, p>.05, for the covariate by group

interaction in all cases. In one case homogeneity of regression was not obtained. This was

in relation to average weekly cannabis consumption and the computation span measure

where the covariate by group interaction yielded F(2,62) = 4.81, p<.05.

The absence of cocaine users among the non-ecstasy users makes it impossible to

control statistically for the potentially confounding effects of cocaine use since it is not

possible to test for homogeneity of regression. To evaluate the extent to which cocaine

use (as well as ecstasy and cannabis use) is associated with performance on the updating

executive component measures we examined the correlations (Spearman’s rho) between

the different aspects of drug use and the outcomes on the updating measures. The results

are set out in Table 4. It is clear that only aspects of ecstasy use are significantly

correlated with the updating executive component measures. None of the aspects of

cocaine use are associated with statistically significant correlations.

<Insert Table 4 about here>

Discussion

No significant ecstasy-related differences were observed in the random letter

generation task. The consonants only version used here was the same as that used in our

early research and as noted above this was associated with a significant group difference

in our initial study (Wareing et al, 2000). Mean lifetime consumption of ecstasy tablets

exceeded 1000 in that study which is appreciably higher than was apparent in our

John E Fisk

16

subsequent research. Furthermore there is clear evidence that restricting the letter set to

consonants only, caused particular problems for the ecstasy users groups in Wareing et

al’s (2000) study as they produced significantly more vowel intrusions under all

generation rates. Thus it had been conjectured that deficits might be found in the present

study among heavy chronic users on the consonants only version of the task. However,

the results presented here demonstrated that this was not the case. The basis for the

significant group difference in our original study (Wareing et al, 2000) remains unclear.

With just 10 participants in each group, the sample sizes were small. Furthermore in our

previous study, we did not assess participants on measures of intelligence. Thus it

remains possible that the differences we observed might have been due to some

premorbid factor other than drug use. The fact that the present study along with other

recent results from our laboratory (Fisk et al 2004; Montgomery et al 2005) have not

revealed ecstasy related deficits in random letter generation suggest that this aspect of

cognitive functioning is unimpaired. Since random letter generation is an established

measure of the inhibition component executive process, it would seem reasonable to

conclude that ecstasy use does not adversely affect this aspect of cognition.

Research from our own laboratory has suggested that ecstasy use may be

associated with deficits in the updating executive component process. Two measures of

the updating function appear to be subject to ecstasy-related impairment, computation

span and letter updating (Montgomery et al, 2005). However, with regard to the latter, in

our previous study, participants were required to maintain a load of six letters while

concurrently performing the updating task. It appears that this load exceeded the letter

span of the majority of participants and for these individuals it would have been

John E Fisk

17

impossible to perform the maintenance element of the task. This raises the question of

whether any serial rehearsal element and concurrent updating activity was actually

occurring. It is possible that participants may have adopted a free recall strategy with a

reliance on the recency component of the process. Baddeley and Hitch (1993) have

argued that the recency phenomenon is distinct from the maintenance and processing

functions of working memory. Thus while we have demonstrated an ecstasy related

deficit in letter updating, this may not in fact reflect an executive function deficit. In an

important early study of the updating process Morris and Jones (1990) addressed this

problem by running two experiments, the first with a load of six letters and the second

with a load of four letters. However, reducing the load to four letters is not without

problems as it makes it possible for those individuals with large letter spans to avoid

updating all together by encoding and serially rehearsing the entire sequence where the

presented sequence length allows this. In order to address this problem, in the present

study participants were required to maintain a load that was equivalent to their letter span.

The orthogonal difference contrasts revealed that following control for group differences

in cannabis consumption ecstasy users performed worse than nonusers on all three

measures of the updating component executive process, including the letter updating task,

computation span, and visuo-spatial updating. Thus it can be argued with some degree of

confidence that ecstasy users are impaired on this executive component function.

It has been argued that the storage aspects of the working memory system are

domain specific while processing is domain-general in nature (e.g., Bayliss et al 2003;

Kane et al , 2004). Thus verbal and visuo spatial information would be stored by

functionally separate systems but the processing component of tasks utilising this

John E Fisk

18

information would be domain-general in nature. Given that ecstasy users have been found

to exhibit deficits in verbal updating, the domain general nature of the process would

imply that deficits should also be apparent in updating visuo-spatial information. In our

previous research we have found ecstasy users to be impaired in a visuo-spatial complex

span task (Wareing et al, 2004; 2005). While analogous verbal tasks have been found to

load on the updating component executive process (Miyake et al, 2000; Fisk & Sharp,

2004), it is unclear whether this applies to the visuo-spatial working memory task that we

employed previously. The present study has used an analogue of the verbal updating task,

in which individuals were required to maintain and update a spatial sequence and it was

established that ecstasy users were significantly impaired on this task relative to nonusers.

The presence of deficits on both the verbal and visuo-spatial updating tasks is consistent

with Baylis et al (2003) and Kane et al’s (2004) view of working memory and it may be

that the deficits observed reflect an ecstasy-related impairment in this domain general

updating process.

The present findings may be viewed in the context of recent neuroscience

evidence. It is known that MDMA affects both serotonergic and dopaminergic systems

(e.g. Kish et al, 2002). Functional neuroimaging studies indicating that ecstasy/polydrug-

related neurotransmitter changes may be concentrated in the dorsolateral and parietal

regions of the prefrontal cortex (Cohen et al, 1996), and in addition may give rise to

significantly lower grey matter concentrations in multiple brain regions (bilateral BA 18

and cerebellum, left BA 21 and left BA 45, as well as the midline brainstem; Cowan et al.

2003). Memory updating has been particularly linked to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex

(Goldman-Rakic, 1996) while performance on the letter-updating task is most strongly

John E Fisk

19

associated with the left fronto-polar cortex (Collette et al, 2006; Van-der-Linden et al,

1999). So it is likely that the deficits observed in the present study reflect reduced

serotonergic/dopaminergic functioning in specific regions of the prefrontal cortex.

As with most studies in this area, a number of limitations need to be

acknowledged. Due to the quasi-experimental design of the study, it is possible that the

groups in each study may have differed on some variable other than ecstasy use. The

groups differed significantly in age and although the tests administered are subject to age-

related decline, this does not typically occur until old age. None of the participants tested

in the present study were more than 27 years old. Group differences in other variables

such as general health, nutrition, or some premorbid condition predating drug use

(Verheul, 2001) cannot be ruled out. Furthermore, due to limited resources we were

unable to provide an objective measure of recent drug use (e.g. from hair or urine

samples). However, most published studies testing cognitive deficits among ecstasy users

have not used these techniques (e.g. Fox et al, 2002; Morgan, 1998; Rodgers, 2000). We

were able to statistically control for group differences in both recent and longer term

aspects of cannabis use. Furthermore, aspects of ecstasy use were more closely correlated

with the cognitive outcomes compared to the equivalent correlations with cocaine use.

However it must be acknowledged that a minority of the ecstasy users had in the past

used amphetamine and a small number LSD. We cannot therefore entirely exclude the

possibility that these drugs may have played some role in the results that were obtained.

John E Fisk

20

References

Baddeley AD (1966). The capacity for generating information by randomization. Q J

Exp Psychol 18: 119-129.

Baddeley AD, & Hitch, GJ (1993). The recency effect: Implicit learning with explicit

retrieval. Memory & Cognition 21: 146-155.

Bayliss DM, Jarrold C, Gunn DM, & Baddeley AD (2003). The complexities of complex

span: Explaining individual differences in working memory in children and

adults. J Exp Psychol Gen 132: 71-92.

Brooks LR (1967). The suppression of visualisation by reading. Q J Exp Psychol 19:

289-299.

Cohen Z, Bonvento G, Lacombe P, Hamel E (1996). Serotonin in the regulation of brain

microcirculation. Prog Neurobiol 50: 335-362.

Collette F, Hogge M, Salmon E, & M. Van der Linden, M (2006). Exploration of the

neural substrates of executive functioning by functional neuroimaging.

Neuroscience 139: 209-221.

Cowan RL, Lyoo IK, Sung SM, Ahn KH, Kim MJ, Hwang J, Haga E, Vimal RLP, Lukas

SE, Renshaw PF (2003). Reduced cortical gray matter density in human MDMA

(ecstasy) users: a voxel-based morphometry study. Drug Alcohol Depend 72: 225-

235.

Dafters RI (2006). Chronic ecstasy (MDMA) use is associated with deficits in, task-

switching but not inhibition or memory updating executive functions. Drug

Alcohol Depend 83: 181-184.

John E Fisk

21

Fisk JE, Montgomery C, Murphy P, & Wareing M (2004). Evidence of executive deficits

among users of MDMA (Ecstasy). Br J Psychol 95: 457-466.

Fisk JE, & Sharp CA (2003). The role of the executive system in visuo-spatial memory

functioning. BrainCogn 52: 364-381.

Fisk JE, & Sharp CA (2004). Age-related impairment in executive functioning: Updating,

inhibition, shifting, and access. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 26: 874-890

Fisk JE, Warr P (1996) Age and Working memory: the role of perceptual speed, the

Central Executive and the phonological loop. Psychol Aging 11: 316-323

Fox HC, McLean A, Turner JJD, Parrott AC, Rogers R, Sahakian BJ (2002)

Neuropsychological evidence of a relatively selective profile of temporal

dysfunction in drug-free MDMA (“ecstasy”) polydrug users.

Psychopharmacology 162: 203-214

Goldman-Rakic PS (1996) The prefrontal landscape: Implications of functional

architecture for understanding human mentation and the central executive. Philos

Trans R Soc Lond 351: 1445-1453

Gouzoulis-Mayfrank E, Daumann J (2006). Neurotoxicity of

methylenedioxyamphetamines (MDMA; ecstasy) in humans: how strong is the

evidence for persistent brain damage? Addiction 101:348-361.

Hinkle DE, Wiersma W, Jurs SG (1994) Applied Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences

(3rd ed.). Boston MA: Houghton Mifflin Company

Kane MJ, Hambrick DZ, Tuholski SW, Wilhelm O, Payne TW, & Engle RW (2004). The

generality of working-memory capacity: A latent-variable approach to verbal and

visuo-spatial memory span and reasoning. J Exp Psychol Gen 133: 189-217.

John E Fisk

22

Kish SJ (2002) How strong is the evidence that brain serotonin neurons are damaged in

human users of ecstasy? Pharmacol Biochem Behav 71: 845-855

Luciana M, Collins PF, Depue RA (1998). Opposing roles for dopamine and serotonin in

the modulation of human spatial working memory functions. Cereb Cortex 8:

218-226.

Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD (2000) The

unity and Diversity of executive functions, and their contributions to complex

“frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognit Psychol 41: 49-100.

Montgomery C, Fisk JE, Newcombe R, & Murphy PN (2005). The differential effects of

ecstasy/polydrug use on executive components: Shifting, inhibition, updating and

access to semantic memory. Psychopharmacology 182: 262-276.

Morgan MJ (1998) Recreational use of “ecstasy” (MDMA) is associated with elevated

impulsivity. Neuropsychopharmacology 19: 252-264

Morgan MJ (2000) Ecstasy (MDMA): A review of its possible persistent

psychological effects. Psychopharmacology 152: 230-248

Morris N, & Jones DM (1990). Memory updating in working memory: The role of the

central executive. Br J Psychol 81, 111-121.

Nelson HE (1982). National Adult Reading Test (NART) Test Manual. Windsor,

Berkshire, UK: NFER-Nelson

Raven J, Raven JC, Court JH (1998). Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and

Vocabulary Scales. Oxford, UK: Oxford Psychologists Press

John E Fisk

23

Reneman L, Schilt T, de Win MM, Booij J, Schmand B, van den Brink W, Bakker O

(2006). Memory function and serotonin transporter promoter gene polymorphism

in ecstasy (MDMA) users. J Psychopharmacol 20: 389-399.

Robbins TW (2000). Chemical neuromodulation of frontal-executive functions in humans

and other animals. Exp Brain Res 133: 130-138.

Rodgers J (2000). Cognitive performance amongst recreational users of “ecstasy”.

Psychopharmacology 151: 19-24

Salthouse TA, Babcock RL (1991) Decomposing adult age differences in working

memory. Dev Psychol 27: 763-776

Smith-Spark JH, & Fisk JE (in press). Central executive functioning in developmental

dyslexia. Memory.

Smith-Spark JH, Fisk JE, Fawcett AJ, & Nicolson RI (2003). Investigating the central

executive in adult dyslexics: Evidence from phonological and visuospatial

working memory performance. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 15:

567-587.

Solowij N, Hall W, Lee N (1992). Recreational MDMA use in Sydney: a profile of

'Ecstacy' users and their experiences with the drug. Br J Addict 87: 1161-72

Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using Multivariate Statistics, (4th Ed.). Boston,

MA, USA: Allyn and Bacon.

Van der Linden M, Collette F, Salmon E, Delfiore G, Delgueldre C, Luxen A, Franck G

(1999). The neural correlates of updating information in verbal working memory.

Memory 7: 549-560.

Verheul R (2001). Co-morbidity of personality disorders in individuals with substance

John E Fisk

24

use disorders. Eur Psychiatry 16: 274-282

Wareing M, Fisk JE, & Murphy PN (2000). Working memory deficits in current and

previous users of MDMA (“Ecstasy”). Br J Psychol 91: 181-188.

Wareing M, Fisk JE, Murphy P, & Montgomery C (2005). Visuo-spatial working

memory deficits in current and former users of MDMA (‘Ecstasy’). Hum

Psychopharmacol 20: 115-123.

Wareing M, Fisk JE, Murphy P, & Montgomery C (2004). Verbal working memory

deficits in current and previous users of MDMA. Hum Psychopharmacol 19: 225-

234.

Wareing M, Fisk JE, Murphy P, & Montgomery C (2005). Visuo-spatial working

memory deficits in current and former users of MDMA (‘Ecstasy’). Hum

Psychopharmacol 20: 115-123.

Wareing M, Murphy P, & Fisk JE (2004). Visuospatial memory impairments in users of

MDMA ('ecstasy'). Psychopharmacology 173: 391-397.

John E Fisk

25

Table 1: Background Variables

Heavy Ecstasy users

Light Ecstasy Users

Non Ecstasy Users

F(2,78)

Mean S.D. Mean S.D. Mean S.D.

Age (years)

22.86 2.38 21.41 2.05 20.71 1.37 5.91**

Years of Education

15.11 2.66 14.87 3.11 15.55 2.24 < 1

Raven’s Progressive Matrices (maximum 60)

45.86 7.19 46.74 5.96 49.36 5.06 2.25

NART (maximum 50)

27.86 8.39 28.72 5.67 29.14 5.02 < 1

Digit Span

6.86 1.03 6.89 1.17 6.60 1.47 < 1

Letter Span 5.21 1.05 5.46 0.94 5.04 0.84 1.75

Spatial Span 4.57 0.85 4.92 0.77 4.57 0.96 1.71

** p<.01

John E Fisk

26

Table 2: Indicators of Illicit Drug Use

Heavy Ecstasy Users Light Ecstasy Users Non Ecstasy Users

Median Mean S.D. n Median Mean S.D. n Median Mean S.D. n Total Use Ecstasy (Tablets) 6.8.00 1000.21 786.41 14 142.00 149.69 96.91 39 0.00 0.00 0.00 28 Cannabis (joints) 5200.00 6383.27 5830.32 11 320.00 1779.51 2971.07 37 22.00 262.13 507.44 23 Cocaine (grams) 75.75 127.52 144.64 6 0.00 17.51 35.84 21 0.00 0.00 0.00 25 Average Weekly Dose Ecstasy (tablets) 2.87 3.49 2.05 14 0.73 0.99 0.68 38 0.00 0.00 0.00 28 Cannabis (joints) 15.08 15.59 12.76 11 2.40 5.96 9.97 35 0.17 1.60 2.84 23 Cocaine (grams) 0.44 0.52 0.47 6 0.00 0.14 0.29 21 0.00 0.00 0.00 25 Length of use (weeks) Ecstasy 271.00 300.82 136.12 14 148.00 176.29 108.55 39 - - - - Cannabis 260.00 342.14 184.14 13 268.00 283.56 145.16 33 172.00 172.83 106.63 21 Cocaine 217.22 240.63 136.35 14 121.00 137.65 79.50 27 - - - - Drugs Used During the 30 days Prior to Testing

Ecstasy 0.00 2.61 4.09 14 0.50 1.73 2.60 39 0.00 0.00 0.00 28 Cannabis 24.00 56.31 75.15 13 3.00 22.80 45.00 38 0.00 7.04 29.30 26 Cocaine 0.25 0.45 0.55 10 0.00 0.25 0.66 30 0.00 0.00 0.00 27 Drugs Used During the 10 days Prior to Testing

Ecstasy 0.00 0.68 1.49 14 0.00 0.32 0.86 39 0.00 0.00 0.00 28 Cannabis 0.50 6.75 12.50 14 0.00 2.79 6.03 39 0.00 1.14 3.57 28 Cocaine 0.00 0.10 0.18 14 0.00 0.08 0.24 39 0.00 0.00 0.00 28

John E Fisk

27

Table 2 continued

Overall Group Effect: Kruskal-Wallace (χ)

Heavy Ecstasy Users versus Light Users: Mann-Whitney U value

Total Use Ecstasy (Tablets) χ(df=2, N=81) = 70.26*** 0.00*** Cannabis (joints) χ(df=2, N=71 )= 16.65*** 113.50* Cocaine (grams) χ(df=2, N=52) = 28.37*** 16.50** Average Weekly Dose Ecstasy (tablets) χ(df=2, N=80) = 65.49*** 46.00*** Cannabis (joints) χ(df=2, N=69) = 13.37** 108.50* Cocaine (grams) χ(df=2, N=52) = 27.43*** 21.00* Length of use (weeks) Ecstasy 115.50** Cannabis χ(df=2, N=67) = 10.29** 178.50 Cocaine 91.00** Drugs Used During the 30 days Prior to Testing

Ecstasy χ(df=2, N=81) = 19.07*** 272.00 Cannabis χ(df=2, N=77) = 9.77** 205.50 Cocaine χ(df=2, N=67) = 13.65** 109.00 Drugs Used During the 10 days Prior to Testing

Ecstasy χ(df=2, N=81) = 5.80 251.50 Cannabis χ(df=2, N=81) = 6.25* 235.00 Cocaine χ(df=2, N=81) = 7.48* 246.00

John E Fisk

28

Table 3 Performance on Executive Measures

Heavy Ecstasy users

Light Ecstasy Users

Non Ecstasy Users

Mean S.D. Mean S.D. Mean S.D.

Random Generation Redundancy 4 second 4.78 2.53 4.00 1.72 4.49 2.14 2 second 5.95 1.94 5.01 2.05 5.11 2.26 1 second 7.05 3.00 7.17 3.02 7.37 2.15 Repeat Sequences 4 second 16.00 3.26 14.05 4.76 14.14 4.28 2 second 16.86 3.61 16.72 6.41 16.54 4.77 1 second 19.07 8.40 18.23 5.89 18.39 6.15 Alphabetic Sequences 4 second 5.07 2.50 4.62 3.88 3.79 3.37 2 second 6.79 3.58 7.10 5.08 6.96 4.62 1 second 10.86 5.55 11.87 6.51 12.11 6.44 Vowel Intrusions 4 second 0.71 0.83 1.23 1.86 1.18 1.49 2 second 1.29 1.49 1.49 2.76 1.61 2.06 1 second 2.79 3.02 3.18 3.43 3.04 2.25 Number of Letters Generated 4 second 100.14 0.36 100.15 0.43 100.11 0.50 2 second 99.93 1.00 99.15 3.08 98.79 3.08 1 second 90.00 11.73 89.74 9.51 88.43 10.15 Computation Span (% cost)

49.04 28.75 41.32 25.21 29.43 23.00

Consonant Updating

3.99 0.71 3.83 0.83 4.22 0.61

Spatial Updating 4.01 0.56 4.03 0.66 4.48 0.75

John E Fisk

29

Table 4. Correlations between Aspects of Illicit Drug Use and Updating Executive

Component Measures

Computation Span

Verbal Updating

Spatial Updating

n

Total Use Ecstasy (Tablets) .295** -.234* -.235* 80 Amphetamine Cannabis (joints) .040 .035 -.020 70 Cocaine (grams) .246 .014 -.183 51 Average Weekly Dose Ecstasy (tablets) .316** -.279* -.288** 79 Amphetamine Cannabis (joints) .011 .036 -.020 68 Cocaine (grams) .251 .013 -.188 51 Length of use (weeks) Ecstasy .075 .147 .081 53 Amphetamine Cannabis -.121 -.127 -.031 43 Cocaine .183 .079 -.017 66 Drugs Used During the 30 days Prior to Testing

Ecstasy .117 -.125 -.305** 80 Amphetamine Cannabis .106 .039 -.055 76 Cocaine .024 .069 -.166 66 Drugs Used During the 10 days Prior to Testing

Ecstasy -.084 .008 .036 80 Amphetamine Cannabis .123 -.093 -.194 80 Cocaine .032 .009 -.169 80

** p<.01; * p<.05