are evolving paradigms in sociolinguistics turning it into

18
Journal of Applied Linguistics and Language Research Volume 5, Issue 1, 2018, pp. 15-32 Available online at www.jallr.com ISSN: 2376-760X * Correspondence: Ali Bahadoran-Baghbaderani, Email: s.b.faculty gmail.com © 2018 Journal of Applied Linguistics and Language Research Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into a Well-Developed Discipline? Ali Bahadoran-Baghbaderani * Ph.D. Candidate in Applied Linguistics, Department of Foreign Languages, Sheikhbahaee University, Isfahan, Iran Gholam Reza Zarei Associate Professor in Applied Linguistics, English Language Center, Isfahan University of Technology, Isfahan, 8415683111, Iran Abstract The current review aims at highlighting the main theoretical assumptions of historical sociolinguistics associated with its origins with special attention given to how these theoretical assumptions can assist language activists in their endeavor to provide the right conditions for analyzing linguistic variations and changes. Clearly, this review is organized around closely related paragraphs accompanied by commentaries on the historical sociolinguistics. This review provides a clearly written and concise introduction to the origins, philosophical developments, motivations, and main paradigms in historical sociolinguistics with a particular focus on the basic research done in the field over the last four decades with its diverse foundations in sociolinguistics, social history, historical linguistics, histories of individual languages, corpus linguistics, philology, discourse studies, socio-pragmatics, and traditional dialectology. Comparing the first four decades based on the studies conducted in the area, it is clearly observed that various studies have been carried out based on genre, speaker variables, and integration, respectively. This review also demystifies the fundamental distinction between principles and generalizations. Keywords: historical sociolinguistics, origins, paradigms, linguistic variations, traditional dialectology, philosophical developments INTRODUCTION Language is an inevitable phenomenon of the world and plays a momentous role in human life. Nevertheless, there is no denying that majority of people still consider the rigorous scrutiny of language as dispensable and a venue that does not deserve ample consideration. According to Bloomfield (1993), “Perhaps because of its familiarity, we rarely observe it, taking it rather for granted, as we do breathing or walking. The effects of language are remarkable, and include much of what distinguishes man from the animals” (p.3).

Upload: others

Post on 12-May-2022

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into

Journal of Applied Linguistics and Language Research Volume 5, Issue 1, 2018, pp. 15-32 Available online at www.jallr.com ISSN: 2376-760X

* Correspondence: Ali Bahadoran-Baghbaderani, Email: s.b.faculty gmail.com

© 2018 Journal of Applied Linguistics and Language Research

Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into a

Well-Developed Discipline?

Ali Bahadoran-Baghbaderani *

Ph.D. Candidate in Applied Linguistics, Department of Foreign Languages, Sheikhbahaee University, Isfahan, Iran

Gholam Reza Zarei

Associate Professor in Applied Linguistics, English Language Center, Isfahan University of Technology, Isfahan,

8415683111, Iran

Abstract

The current review aims at highlighting the main theoretical assumptions of historical

sociolinguistics associated with its origins with special attention given to how these

theoretical assumptions can assist language activists in their endeavor to provide the right

conditions for analyzing linguistic variations and changes. Clearly, this review is organized

around closely related paragraphs accompanied by commentaries on the historical

sociolinguistics. This review provides a clearly written and concise introduction to the

origins, philosophical developments, motivations, and main paradigms in historical

sociolinguistics with a particular focus on the basic research done in the field over the last

four decades with its diverse foundations in sociolinguistics, social history, historical

linguistics, histories of individual languages, corpus linguistics, philology, discourse studies,

socio-pragmatics, and traditional dialectology. Comparing the first four decades based on

the studies conducted in the area, it is clearly observed that various studies have been

carried out based on genre, speaker variables, and integration, respectively. This review also

demystifies the fundamental distinction between principles and generalizations.

Keywords: historical sociolinguistics, origins, paradigms, linguistic variations, traditional

dialectology, philosophical developments

INTRODUCTION

Language is an inevitable phenomenon of the world and plays a momentous role in

human life. Nevertheless, there is no denying that majority of people still consider the

rigorous scrutiny of language as dispensable and a venue that does not deserve ample

consideration. According to Bloomfield (1993), “Perhaps because of its familiarity, we

rarely observe it, taking it rather for granted, as we do breathing or walking. The effects

of language are remarkable, and include much of what distinguishes man from the

animals” (p.3).

Page 2: Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into

Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into a Well-Developed Discipline? 16

Language is closely intertwined with society; it gets diminished to a set of pure

linguistic details such as sounds, lexis and syntactic forms when factored out from the

social contexts in which it is utilized. The increasing grasp of the interplay between

language and society has led to the advent of the scholarly field known as

'sociolinguistics'. A scrutiny of language and its users undertaken concurrently may be

said to establish the discipline of Sociolinguistics. A person may not constantly utilize

one analogous language variation for verbal communication under all circumstances. He

may be utilizing more than one variety. The variations of language he uses are caused by

socio-cultural determinants. Sociolinguistics, accordingly, studies the verbal demeanor

of the person who is an affiliate of a speech territory. Coulmas (2001) points out that

Sociolinguistics is the empirical study of how language is used in a society. Combining linguistic and sociological theories and methods, it is an interdisciplinary field of research, which attaches great significance both to the variability of language and to the multiplicity of languages, and language forms in a given society (p.563).

In many cases, people appraise others not only from the veritable content of what they

say or write but also from the manner they apply language. What compels people to

resort to the manner they utilize language and what differentiates one individual’s

variety from another's are complicated conundrums dealt with in sociolinguistics. The

close association between language and culture or linguistic construction and social

construction can easily be illustrated with the instance of a boy whose linguistic

utterances differ with the interlocutor and the social setting despite he may be

expressing the identical view. The boy tells his friends, 'Come along with me'. But he

does not utilize the same linguistic structure if the recipient is his father. He uses the

more polite form, 'Father, please come along with me'. The boy turns to the much more

formal and polite usage, 'Sir could you please accompany along with me', when the

recipient is someone socially preeminent to him and a social proximity does not exist

between them.

How individuals deviate from each other in the manner they speak and what leads to

such variations shape a provocative field of study. Some of the variations in speech may

be idiosyncratic, but beyond that there are diversities associated with group or class,

gender, context and many such determinants. Males vary from females in their pitch

and further in the way they use language. Correspondingly, an adult never

communicates like a teen-aged individual. These two are biologically attributed to

diversities of language use. Other than these two, there are spectrums of social variation

in language influenced by other factors. Discrepancies in social status, cultivation,

affluence, and power do make significant impact on the mode of speech. In the words of

Guy (1988):

Bankers clearly do not talk the same as bus boys, and professors don't sound like plumbers. They signal the social differences between them by features of their phonology, grammar and lexical choice, just as they do extra-linguistically by their choices in clothing, cars, and so on ( p.37).

Page 3: Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into

Journal of Applied Linguistics and Language Research, 2018, 5(1) 17

As commented by Trudgill (1995):

Language is very much a social phenomenon. A study of language totally without reference to its social context inevitably leads to the omission of some of the more complex and interesting aspects of language and to the loss of opportunities for further theoretical progress. One of the main factors that have led to the growth of sociolinguistic research has been the recognition of the importance of the fact that language is a very variable phenomenon, and that this variability may have as much to do with society as with language (p.20).

The territory of sociolinguistics has broadened further. Some of the issues covered

under sociolinguistics include: language and variationism, identity, ethnicity, gender,

nationality, religion, globalization, relativity, and dialogic meaning.

LINGUISTIC VARIATION AND LINGUISTIC CHANGE

This section addresses the evolution of two seemingly uncompromising sides of the

same coin in the study of language variation and change (i.e. diachrony and synchrony)

which the scope of historical sociolinguistics tries to hybridize in a kind of supportive

companion piece. Differently stated, this part has been built upon the controversy

surrounding the relationship between diachrony and synchrony that may be attributed

to the apparent differences in the essence of each one. The uncompromising approach

to the issue of diachrony and synchrony is that used by Saussure (1959) to distinguish

two approaches to the study of language: diachronic as opposed to synchronic. He

demystifies the distinction as follows: “[e]everything that relates to the static side of our

science is synchronic; everything that has to do with evolution is diachronic”.

Correspondingly, synchrony and diachrony designate respectively a language-state and

an evolutionary phase. To clarify this point, Janda and Joseph (2003) point out that

[t]he utilization of all reasonable means to extend our knowledge of what might have been going on in the past, even though it is not directly observable. Normally, this will involve a heavy concentration on the immediate present, but it is in fact more realistic just to say we wish to gain a maximum of information from a maximum of potential sources: different times and different places – and, in the case of language, also different regional and social dialects, different contexts, different styles, different topics, and so on (p.37).

Nevertheless, contrary to scholars who try to show that diachrony and synchrony are

irreconcilable, Jakobson (1972) says about the impossibility of separating diachrony

from synchrony. In other words, he believes that integration of the static with the

dynamic is one of the most fundamental dialectic paradoxes that determine the sprite of

the language. This statement by Jakobson is supported by many others. As a case in

point, Labov (1972) supports such integration by admitting: “ [t]he same mechanisms

which operated to produce the large scale changes of the past may be observed

operating in the current changes taking place around us”. However, it would be a

mistake to reach a compromise in all diachronic/synchronic hurdles (e.g. layering,

polysemy).

Page 4: Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into

Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into a Well-Developed Discipline? 18

PHILOSOPHICAL FOUNDATIONS AND HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT

Interest in languages has had a long history and it can be traced back as far as the

ancient Greeks when great philosophers of the time sought to discover the most

effective sources of rhetorics in order to support polemical discourse underlying their

philosophical views about life and universe (Howatt, 1999). Since then, attempts to

describe what language is, how it is learned and how it is used for solving language

related problems in real life have gained momentum and have established a voluminous

body of discourse whose main goal has been to demystify the complexities of human

language.

Unsurprisingly, a substantial part of long discourse has been focused on the nature of

language and how it works. Under such perspectivization, linguistics – the study of

language scientifically – has created a wealth of useful information revolutionizing the

development of an insightful technical dialectic, which has promoted our understanding

of the role of language in human affairs. Language is mainly used for a myriad of

purposes in the real civic ecology, and here is where sociolinguistics steps in – trying to

solve the language problems influencing human life (Schmitt, 2002). This is in fact the

main objective of sociolinguistics in a narrow sense. However, Wilkins (1996) defines

sociolinguistics in a broad sense stating that:

…sociolinguistics is concerned with increasing understanding of the major function of language in human affairs and thereby with providing the knowledge necessary for those who are one responsible for taking language – related decisions whether the need for those arises in the classroom, the workplace, the law court, or the laboratory (p.7).

In the course of the past four and half decenniums, studies of the interplays between

language and society have amalgamated to constitute the field of scholarly study known

as sociolinguistics. Differently stated, it is in the twentieth century that the field of

sociolinguistics elevated to the position of an independent discipline; but it is observed

that systematized examination of human speech commenced with the nineteenth

century itself. Even from the end of the eighteenth century, taking inspiration from

Rousseau’s ideas, many scholarly figures and practitioners initiated to manifest much

enthusiasm in the study of native dialect and promptly they were accompanied by the

linguists who were keen to indicate that dialects along with literary languages had

creditable pedigrees in received philosophy. A scholarly event that gave a voice to this

movement was the publication of the Lyrical Ballads in 1798 that was the result of a

collaborative effort by Wordsworth and Coleridge. It included poems divergent from

those that had composed in English language until that time.

In the prologue to the Lyrical Ballads, the authors picked holes in the manipulated

diction of the poems composed in the eighteenth century and upheld that poetry should

be composed in the undoctored language of people. In fact they developed the

momentum to form the language of poetry, notably dialogue to consider current diction

and to make the speech of its characters less stilted.

Page 5: Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into

Journal of Applied Linguistics and Language Research, 2018, 5(1) 19

The major characters dealt with in poetry to that time had been largely members of

upper echelon urbanites. But Wordsworth's (1978) concern deviated strongly from

such people and contemplated on the peasants who lived in close proximity and

communion to nature. Differently stated, for him, real people comprised of ordinary

people from the proletariat and members of classes other than the upper-class

urbanites. In this way Wordsworth (1978) notified the death of the Augustan era and

the commencement of Romanticism which placed emphasis on the spirit of the folk in

language, markedly as depicted in dialect.

TRADITIONAL DIALECTOLOGY AND SOCIOLINGUISTICS

It is very enlightening to catch a fleeting glimpse of how dialect studies of the late

nineteenth century and early twentieth century have paved way to sociolinguistics of

the present. Nowadays, sociolinguistics is much seen as quantitative sociolinguistics

and/or urban dialectology since it occupies a good portion of sociolinguistics despite

the fact it covers other issues as well. The dialectologists of the second half of the 19th

century focused attention on aggregating details about geographically dispersed and

mostly rustic variations of language. Their plan was mainly tracing the evolution of

modern languages from prior varieties. Scholars of urban dialectology, on the other

hand, have been endeavoring to evaluate the features of urban diversities with a

perspective to associating them with social determinants. They perceive of their study

as one of the numerous lines of doing linguistics. As Trudgill (1995) puts it,

contemporary inquiry on the subject:

[…] is concerned to learn more about language, and to investigate topics such as the mechanisms of linguistic change, the nature of linguistic variability, and the structure of linguistic systems. All work in this category is aimed ultimately at improving linguistic theory and at developing our understanding of the nature of language (p.11).

The effort made by dialectologists helped contend the social implication of language;

but apart from that not much work carried out in this area. It took some more time

before the subject came out of its immaturity and grew in to a well-developed discipline.

Plenty of progression in sociolinguistics has been made since the late 1960s. In the

recent decades there has emerged a far-reaching consensus in the discipline due to the

apprehension that sociolinguistics can shed further light on the nature of language and

the nature of society concurrently. The discipline is proceeding along the course of

development and it is interested more in the study of languages of urban societies.

Accordingly, many of the traditional notions have been redefined in accord with the

developing language usage.

Many of the nineteenth century linguistic practitioners were especially involved with

the documentation of historical transformations leading to the evolution of languages

and they diverged in the source materials they employed in their works. One group of

philogists made use of document remains of dead languages or classical scripts,

inscriptions or documents shedding light on the earlier levels of modern literary

Page 6: Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into

Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into a Well-Developed Discipline? 20

languages. Their procedure of tracking the advancement of modern languages from

earlier prehistoric varieties of protolanguages greatly resembled the process executed

by biological scientists in scrutinizing the evolution of animal beings.

A second group of philogists became involved in the categorical study of oral practices

of rural populace and ethnic groups who, according to them, provided living proof of

evolutionary sequences. They turned away from written documents for the empirical

study of authentic diction. Practitioners of dialectology inclined in this direction

launched in to a series of field studies in which rustic speech was analyzed either

through mail questionnaires or directly through fieldwork by inquirers who frequently

walked many miles in order to amass dialect samples by means of direct methods.

These designs of inquiry had many pitfalls; but they served to demystify that language is

fundamentally a social construction and that social constituents are much complicated

in language variation, thus denouncing earlier ideas of biological or geographical

determinism. According to Lehrnann (1973), a significant contribution of dialect

geography to the present lies in making us cognizant of the great variety in language.

Through the variation and wealth of forms it has uncovered, dialect geography has

widened considerably our views on language. Historical grammars no longer treat

languages as single strata, but rather as complexes composed of numerous data. A given

speaker masters some of these. But the complete language is discernible only as one

collects material from a variety of speakers. Lehrnann (1973) adds:

But even the early dialect geographers went on to scrutinize the geographically differing layers of language. Subsequent study caused concern for strata determined also by social, functional and occupational discrepancies. In this way a view of language more complex than that of the founders of historical linguistics has been contributed to historical linguistics by the study of dialect geography. Contemporary study of language variation according to geographical, occupational, and social groups is conducted in the field of sociolinguistics (p.127).

Thus, it can be noticed that the plotting of dialect distributions based on territorial

variation has been traditionally recognized as dialect geography and it has engaged

many methods and assumptions from another ramification of linguistics known as

historical/diachronic linguistics. According to dialect geographers, the dialects of a

language are developed as the users segregate themselves from each other over time

and space. Clearly, the diversification of a language in to dialects comes to such an

extent that the users are no longer intelligible to each other; this contributes to the

derivation of new languages from the dialects. This is how, Latin, over a long amount of

time transformed into many languages like French, Spanish, and Italian. This kind of

diversification takes in to consideration only the two determinants, time and space. As

an illustration, the British and American varieties of English are set apart by a few

centuries of political independence and by the Atlantic Ocean. In a like manner,

Northumbrian and Cockney English are separated by approximately three hundred

miles and many centuries apart.

Page 7: Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into

Journal of Applied Linguistics and Language Research, 2018, 5(1) 21

20TH CENTURY AND THE ADVENT OF NEW IDEAS

In America, since the first decades of the twentieth century, up till the advent of

Chomsky's generative grammar, the accepted linguistics of the time was become heavily

involved in the prompt and detailed description of a vast number of dying native

American languages. The descriptive linguist considered language as a single entity

standing alone outside the domain of the social structure. The descriptive analysis of a

language provided insights based on its structure stated categorically in terms of

linguistic features. In such formalistic description, the addresser, the addressee, the

theme, and the scope and situations of its use were not considered pertinent to

structural description. The adoption of various styles of communication in case of a

monolingual and use of different language varieties and styles in case of multilingual

was not regarded something to be discerned by a linguist. The descriptive linguist was

fulfilled with providing an account of the language variety a person used; it was none of

his interest as to why a person utilized various speech styles or language varieties. It did

not happen to the linguist that employment of a language or divergent registers or

styles of it constituted a system, which could be unwinded by dint of the social

structure. The scrutiny of semantics was also factored out from the scope of linguistics.

The second half of the twentieth century is characterized by a refreshed enthusiasm in

the study of language in its socio-cultural context triggered by a controversy over

Chomskian principles associated with the autonomy of language. Chomskian linguistics

was erected on the autonomy of the syntax and it had the competence-performance

binary as one of its strong foundations. Lavandera (1988) in her article entitled, "The

Study of Language in its Socio-cultural Context" appraises the situation thus: "It does

not seem far-fetched to hold Chomsky indirectly responsible for the accelerated

development of sociolinguistics and ethnolinguistics at the end of the 1960 and for the

emphasis laid upon pragmatics and discourse analysis in the mid 1970" (p.1).

Classifications like langue-parole and competence-performance as developed by

Saussure and Chomsky respectively, require a little more details, as they are pivotal to

fathoming how historical/descriptive linguistics increasingly smoothed the path for

'socially realistic linguistics' (Labov, 1966). During the period of times when linguists

have been in pursuit of the pure, optimum form of language amongst the wide

variations utilized by individuals. De Saussure, the Swiss linguist postulated that the

homogeneous, pure system of language kept in the 'collective mind' of the society and

for Chomsky it was roughly theoretical - it existed in the mind of the ideal speaker-

interlocutor. Chomsky refuses to accept Saussure's ideas of langue as a frame of

material or a repertoire of ideas from which the individual locutor selects his parole. On

the contrary, Chomsky observes language as comprising of the locutor, or recipient’s

innate knowledge of his language (competence) and his actual application of language

in plausible contexts (performance).

According to Chomsky (1965), competence is a fundamental generative process, which

will find ever more potential acts of performance. Albeit very limited is really known of

the process and how it is operated, a case study of the five-year-old forming sentences

Page 8: Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into

Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into a Well-Developed Discipline? 22

he has not heard before theorizes the likelihood of an internal grammar, which

authorizes the formation of new material. In the same vein, Bernstein (1972) believes

that:

Chomsky neatly severs the study of the rule system of language from the study of the social rules, which determine their contextual use. He does this by drawing an absolute and principled distinction between competence and performance. Competence refers to the child's tacit understanding of the rule system, performance relates to the essential social use to which the rule system is put. Competence refers to man abstracted from contextual constraints. Performance refers to man in the grip of the contextual constraints, which determine his speech acts (p.160).

Saussure, who early in the 20th century viewed language as a 'social fact', suggested

that a language is a social product and that every society manifests itself through

language. Nevertheless, Saussure never troubled to scrutinize the social side of

language; differently stead, he was intrigued in studying language as a constructural

phenomenon with its own inherent set of rules.

Saussurean idea was that parole or language in actual application by various individuals

was too altered to authorize inquiry and only langue could be analyzed. However,

empirical hindrance emerged when descriptive linguists commenced to examine langue

because langue was not accessible to close observation by the senses. Langue was

existed in the 'collective awareness' of the speech populace and what could be inspected

was the parole only. An empirical approach meant intense scrutiny of language in actual

use and so a tough adoption of Saussure's doctrine became a difficulty for the

descriptive/historical linguist who found performance, parole, or usage fluctuating to

be defined. The sociologists who regarded language as part of society and culture were

specifically concerned with language in actual application and so they disintegrated

from any study or theory that did not give due importance to performance or parole.

Lavandera (1988) remarks upon the situation thus:

The reaction to Chomsky's position that the systematicity of language is confined to competence took a number of different forms. Some, seeing systematicity outside of competence in Chomsky's narrow use of the term, attempted to extend the notion of competence to cover most of the aspects that Chomsky ascribed to performance. An example is Hymes's 'communicative competence' (Models.. .) which he defined as the knowledge of the abstract rules of a language required to produce sound / meaning correspondences and the ability to use those correspondences between sound, meaning and form in socially and culturally appropriate ways. On the other hand some saw system in performance as well and began to develop theories specifically of the former. But whichever path was taken, a growing core of investigators was united in the conviction that the Chomskian paradigm was too narrow to accommodate most of the interesting questions about language (p.1).

Page 9: Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into

Journal of Applied Linguistics and Language Research, 2018, 5(1) 23

ORIGINS AND MAIN FIGURES

Over a span of time, many linguists came forward with the assertion that an asocial

linguistics is not beneficial and that clear understandings in to language can be acquired

only if such matters as diversity and application are involved as part of the data which

must be clarified in an adequate linguistic theory; an adequate theory of language must

have something to suggest about the applications of language. An understanding and

appreciation of linguistic variation connotes that language is not just an abstract system

of rules or object of study. It is something that people utilize for all sorts of

conversational encounters. Accordingly, there provided fertile grounds of studying

language hinged on its use and not as a mere object.

Some of the preeminent researchers and practitioners that contributed to the

tremendous development of sociolinguistics as a discipline in its formative stages on a

parallel with other disciplines (such as linguistics, sociology, philosophy, anthropology

and others) are: Humboldt, Boas, Sapir, Whorf, Bloomfield, Malinowsky, and Firth.

The tantalizing notion that language has a powerful impact on the speakers’ thinking

and cultural world view has long been one of the most significant discussions in

psycholinguistic and sociolinguistic research. Building on Humboldt’s (1836) polemical

views about language, Sapir and his student Whorf claimed that language and culture

are interrelated. Edward Sapir is probably the first linguist of the century to declare that

an individual's view of his or her own immediate surrounding is influenced by the

native language s/he applies. Sapir's ideas on language were backed up by Benjamin Lee

Whorf, whose analogous study of American Indian language Hopi and the European

languages equipped him with adequate information in support of his ideas.

Whorf (1956) maintained that language is not simply a vehicle for expressing ideas but

rather it serves as “the shaper of ideas, the program and guide for the individual’s

mental activity, for his analysis of impressions, for his synthesis of his mental stock of

trade….” (pp. 212–214). As such, different speech communities possess typical and

unique codified patterns of language by which they can view the realities of the world in

particular ways.

The strong version of Sapir-Whorf hypothesis known as linguistic determinism holds

that members of a given speech community think differently because their background

linguistic system by its very nature makes them perceive the world through the lenses

that are exclusively cut and shaped for them. Although most sociolinguists oppose the

theoretical claims of the strong version, the majority adopt the weak version of

Whorfian hypothesis labeled as linguistic relativity, asserting that perceptions,

thoughts, and even behaviors of individuals belonging to a particular speech community

are influenced by the language they speak.

Clearly, they used this analogy not because of the abstract meaning of truth relativism

demystified above, but as a remark to Einstein’s theory of relativity, which inform us

that diversities in one’s perceptions are the outcome of discrepancies in his speed and

Page 10: Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into

Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into a Well-Developed Discipline? 24

position. As such, Sapir contends that a divergence in language, like in speed and

position, indicates a difference in perspective or point of view, and must accordingly be

taken into consideration. This difference is the product of the impact language has on a

person’s conceptual system. Differently stated, the grammar of a language shapes our

knowledge of the world and leads the speaker to different ways of understanding.

In this perspectivization, words and structural patterns in every language not only differ

from other languages but they also ascribe culturally defined significances and

meanings to different conceptual categories in the environment surrounding them.

Accordingly, the words and structural features comprising the linguistic system used by

the speakers of a specific language are completely unique to that system and can be

found in no other languages. This is how Whorf (1956) provided his perspective on the

interplay between language and thought:

[---l the background linguistic system (in other words the grammar) of each language is not merely a reproducing instrument for voicing ideas but rather is itself the shaper of ideas, the programme and guide for the individual's mental activity, for his analysis of impressions, for his synthesis of his mental stock in trade. Formulation of ideas is not an independent process, strictly rational in the old sense, but is part of a particular grammar and differs, from slightly to greatly, between different grammars. We dissect nature along lines laid down by our native language. The categories and types that we isolate from the world of phenomena we do not find there because they stare every observer in the face; on the contrary the world is presented in a kaleidoscopic flux of impressions which has to be organized by our minds - and this means largely by the linguistic system in our minds [. . .l. We are thus introduced to a new principle of relativity, which holds that all observers are not led by the same physical evidence to the same picture of the universe, unless their linguistic backgrounds are similar, or can in some way be calibrated (p.26).

Whorf here enunciates about how our thought is influenced by the grammar of our

language. The footprint of lexis on thought is possibly more evident; but it is difficult to

digest the contention of Sapir and Whorf that linguistic experience is the sole type of

experience that affects our thought patterns. However, it is out of expectation that the

Whorfian hypothesis will be confirmed in the strong form. For one thing, all humans

enjoy similar conceptualization potentialities so much so the likelihood of universalities

across different cultures with regard to conceptual capacities of human beings is very

strong. As a consequence, it is logical to assume that human beings have certain

common conceptual processes which make them observe certain realities of the world

on a universal basis. The interesting point is that human beings may also differ in their

conceptualization of the world because certain cognitive abilities have been shaped and

influenced by language specific features. Not surprisingly, such overlapping basis makes

people of the world conceive thought patterns in both similar and different ways.

Joseph (2005), an illustrious Keralian writer, accentuates the interplay between

language and culture thus: “when a new word emerges in a language, it will introduce a

Page 11: Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into

Journal of Applied Linguistics and Language Research, 2018, 5(1) 25

new culture and life-style into that social territory” (p.15). Not surprisingly, the

codablity of object realities is not consistently and/or uniformly realized since culture

has a direct bearing on linguistic and sociopragmatic aspects of language which act as a

medium for the symbolic manifestation of culture. As a consequence, understanding

particular speakers’ knowledge about categorization is prerequisite to the

demystification of the factors motivating their interaction with the world (Biria &

Bahadoran-Baghbaderani, 2016; Gilbert et al. 2008). As an illustration, when we see a

sparrow, a car, or a priest, we use our knowledge of categorization to make inferences

about their functional utility by making a rapid analysis of typical characteristics

defining their actions and behaviors. Overall, categorical thinking helps reduce our

cognitive efforts when interacting with the world.

Malinowsky and Firth, as an anthropologist and linguist respectively, have been highly

authoritative in the rise of sociolinguistics in Britain. Firth's (1957) assertion was that

he and his colleagues studied language as part of the social dynamic process and that a

'key idea' to their method was the idea of the 'context of situation'—a description of the

contextual factors influencing an utterance —provided by Malinowsky. They discussed

meaning in terms of function in context and repudiated all approaches to the study of

language, which sought to leave out the study of meaning. The primary communication

theories had confined languages to mere conveyance of information while in the

functionalist perspective of linguists such as Firth and Malinowsky, the concept of

‘context of situation’ is indispensable and must incorporate the participants in speech

situations, the action taking place and other pertinent determinants. Building on this

perspective, a given language proposition is noticed as felicitous to a peculiar

application within a specific cultural context; in a diverse linguistic and cultural

environment, slight changes have to be addressed. Firth contend that this context of

situation would be evaluated not as a vague and impetuous breach of faith , shifting

background to a speech event, but more zealously in terms of relevant classifications at

a different level from syntactic classifications but rather of the identical abstract feature

structures. He did not enlarge upon it but did propose that such classifications could be:

I. Interior Relations

a) The relevant features of participants, persons, and personalities.

1) The verbal action of the participants.

2) The non-verbal action of the participants.

b) The relevant objects.

c) The effect of the verbal action

II. Exterior Relations

a) Economic, religious, social structures to which participants belong.

b) Types of discourse-monologue, narrative.

c) Personal Interchanges-age, sex of participants.

d) Types of speech-social flattery, cursing.

Page 12: Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into

Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into a Well-Developed Discipline? 26

Bloomfield (1933) in his book "Language" brought up the topic of various speech

communities and in it he elucidates our perception of the mechanisms by which social

variables exert influence on language variation. As Bloomfield said later, linguistic

diversification in human communities is closely allied to the amount of verbal

production or bulk of communication among interactants. Precursory practitioners had

made efforts to find direct correlations between language and different context-

sensitive factors; but he hypothesizes an intervening level of human communication,

which reconciles between linguistic and nonlinguistic phenomena. According to him,

financial, political, and even regional facets are not overtly manifested in speech. They

influence language only to the extent that they can be shown to direct verbal

communication among interactants, triggering certain individuals to have more verbal

interaction directly with some than with others and thereby affecting the rate at which

innovations diffuse innovations diffuse and are adopted at the individual level. In the

words of Bloomfield (1933):

Envisage a chart with a dot for every speaker in the society, and envisage that every time any speaker uttered a sentence, an arrow was drawn in to the chart printing from his dot to the dot representing each one of his interlocutors. At the end of a given period of time, say 70 years, that chart would depict us the density of communication in the community....we believe that the differences in communication are not only personal and individual but that the community is divided into various systems of subgroups, such that persons within a subgroup speak much more to each other than to persons outside their subgroup…. sub groups are separated by lines of weakness in this net of oral communication (pp.46-47).

All the linguists cited above contributed to an increased significance of social

determinants in language and language inquiry; but they could not form any coalition in

linguistics provoking the acceptance of linguistic diversity as an influential factor. Even

Sapir was not geared up to include the element of variation in to his study of language.

Sapir and the scholars that came after him did not set up any flow devoted to

incorporating the diversity factor of language. Sapir who had come up with the pithy

remark, "Unfortunately or luckily, no grammar is tyrannically consistent. All grammars

leak” (p.38) adopted in practice a very strong form of the axiom of categoricity.

Chambers (2003) describes the axiom of categoricity as “the simplifying assumption

that data for linguistic analysis must be regularized to eliminate real-world variability

(p.l2).

When linguists detected the presence of variations in language use, they viewed them

either as belonging to different synchronal/coexistent linguistic systems or as dormant

free alternates. The various forms employed by community members were never

debated or investigated seriously and were considered simply as haphazard

alternations between varieties. Those who strongly favored the idea of

synchronal/coexistent linguistic units took the view that interactants applied distinctive

lexis, grammar, and phonology that gave them availability to more than one code but

shifting over to the varieties should not be intermittent. All in all, fluctuating between

Page 13: Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into

Journal of Applied Linguistics and Language Research, 2018, 5(1) 27

the spectrums of variations should not take place to the extent that the conditions

surrounding the speech situation remain untouched. But, in practice, many interactants

are observed to be shifting over from one variety to another in the same sentence itself.

A vast group of speakers resort to this sort of code-mixing in alike contexts. The

linguistic demeanor of such group of people provides testimony for a singular system in

which varieties coexist rather than the coexistence of various linguistic systems.

In the opinion of Coulmas (2001), categoricity has superseded by frequency, i.e. the

frequency of occurrence of variant features of language use in a given speech

community. He observes thus:

Instead of categorizing a certain pronunciation or a certain construction as either belonging to or not belonging to a language L, sociolinguistics would measure the frequencies with which such features and constructions occurred in variety X compared with variety Y. The fact that speech communities and individual speakers had a wide range of possible speech forms at their command was recognized as an essential rather than a haphazard condition of the social functioning of language. Discovering systematic patterns underlying the actual occurrences of variant speech forms and relating them to social characteristics of speakers and speech situations turned in to be the major challenge that sociolinguistics set out to confront (p.565).

LINGUISTIC VARIABLES

Hudson (2001) characterizes a variable as a "collection of alternatives which have

something in common "(p.l69). A linguistic variable is a linguistic form with two or

more identifiable alternatives employed in covariation with other social and/or

linguistic variables (Chambers & Trudgill). A much discussed example is the use of

variant pronunciations for the suffix -ing which are symbolized as (ng): [g] and (ng): [n].

Variables of this kind require to be inspected in sociolinguistic terms because other

sorts of explanations are beside the point here. This fact becomes more evident when

we notice that the variation between the same two sounds at the end of sin and sing

does not fall under the domain of sociolinguistics because they can be categorized as

lexical items. An individual who is familiar with the meaning and application of these

words recognizes accurately the reason why it is sin on one situation and sing on

another (Biria & Bahadoran-Baghbaderani, 2015). Similarly, the selection between the

words sing and sings comes under the rules of grammar. The social determinant has

utterly no pertinence in the study of the selection of these sounds. Linguistic scholars

expound these facts without regarding at the social context or the social variables.

Consequently, it has to be accentuated that linguistic variables that call for a

sociolinguistic explanation only can be called as 'sociolinguistic variables.'

Hudson identifies different types of sociolinguistic variables based on the level of

language (phonetics, phonology, morphology, lexicon, syntax) at which the variants are

different.

Page 14: Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into

Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into a Well-Developed Discipline? 28

PARADIGMS

The macro/micro distinction

Sociolinguistics is a fertile ground for social practitioners and linguists, some of whom

strive to uncover the social facets of language while others are mainly interested in the

linguistic manifestations of society. Accordingly, there have arisen two centers of

gravity, recognized as microsociolinguistics and macrosociolinguistics, respectively.

These delineate diverse inquiry-based agendas and theoretical frameworks,

macrosociolinguistics deals with the sociolinguistics of society, issues such as societal

multilingualism, language policy and standardization, whereas microsociolinguistics

puts premium on the sociolinguistics of language, the influence of social interaction in

language use. The broad perspective concerning the divergence between macro-

sociolinguistics or sociology of language and microsociolinguistics or sociolinguistics is

this: Sociolinguistics is the inspection of language touching on society while sociology of

language is the scrutiny of society dealing with language. The discrepancy between the

two lies principally in the point of emphasis, whether the researcher is interested more

in society or language.

Wardhaugh (2010) demarcates the two by noting that sociolinguistics deals with

observing the symbiotic interplays between society and language with the purpose

being a better awareness of the structural properties of language and of how languages

operate in communicative discourse; the parallel aim in the sociology of language is

endeavoring to discern how civic structure can be better fathomed through the study of

language, e.g., how particular linguistic constituents function to specify certain social

adjustment.

Micro-sociolinguistics examines how social construct affects the way inhabitants of a

language ecology speak and how social peculiarities like gender, age, and class are

pertained to language diversities. Motifs like language policy, language attitude,

language planning, language birth, maintenance, death or to put it in a nutshell what

civic colonies do with their language comes within the realm of microsociolinguistics.

Wardhaugh, albeit he acknowledges this sort of dichotomy, is of the opinion that it is not

feasible to make a clear separation between the two because they imbricate in several

strata. In his perspective adamant micro-macro dichotomy seems highly dubious and

futile in the current state of knowledge concerning the manifold interplays between

societal and linguistic patterns.

Gendered Power Structures and Sociolinguistics

The general propensity is a relocation from language as structure towards language use

with references to those of Wittgenstein’s meaning-as-use thesis and Austin's speech act

theory (how to do things with words). Foucault's poststructuralist perspective

associates discourse with power structures. Scholars coming from different disciplines

take different angles.

Page 15: Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into

Journal of Applied Linguistics and Language Research, 2018, 5(1) 29

Some confusion exists over the distinction between sex and gender. Individuals often

confuse sex with gender. Gender performativity implies the ways in which a person

expresses his/her gender identity through gendered behavior. Gender construction

suggests the way that socialization envisages appropriate gendered behavior to be the

hegemonic account of what it actually signifies to be a man or a woman.

According to Butler (1990), gender performativity is how an individual performs

his/her preferred gender identity. Gender is not permanent and fixed. It does not

physically connect to our sexuality or our biological sex category. As long as gender is a

construction, a person can construct what gender to play and how to play it. Drag

queens are served as an instance of how gender is not a permanent absolute. There is no

truly existing male or female gender (gender dichotomy) they are constructions, and as

such can be shaped and a diversity of genders can be performed.

Differently stated, Butler (1990) contends that gender can be acted out in ways that

deepen sociocultural understandings of what gender (a social category) is and ought to

be. It also can destabilize social constructions and recognitions of what gender is -

Butler’s use of drag performance to problematize these assumptions and provide the

likelihood of transgressing gender categories and sex/gender binaries has become the

focus of a variety of critiques.

The crux of the matter about the Foucault’s approach to power is that it goes beyond

politics and considers power as a socialized and embodied phenomenon. Foucault’s

theory of power is evasive and lifted from the agency that there seems to be little room

for practical action. But he has been authoritative in manifesting the ways that norms

can be so entrenched as to be beyond our perception. According to Foucault (1998,

p.93), “…power is everywhere and comes from everywhere. It is always already there

and one is never outside it”. Apparently, in this sense is neither a structure nor an

agency. Foucault utilizes the term power to indicate that power is created through

authorized forms of knowledge and truth.

The power and discourse foundation provokes practitioners to reflect about the

likelihood of freedom for the subjects they scrutinize. Power and freedom are

prerequisites of each other. To be free is the absence of pressure or obligation; to have

power is to be able to coerce or oblige another. Clearly, freedom is the absence of

others' power over us; power is the absence of others' freedom from us.

CONCLUSION

Over the past four decades, developments in social issues have heightened the need for

cross-disciplinary interactions which satisfy the pressing demands of the avid

sociocultural-linguistic researchers throughout the globe. To satisfy such demanding

goals and to provide a more effective solution, many practitioners are recently taking

advantage of collaboration opportunities involving diverse methodological and

conceptual tools in order to expand the realm of research in contemporary research on

language, culture, and society.

Page 16: Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into

Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into a Well-Developed Discipline? 30

Indubitably, the nature of sociolinguistics studies can greatly be influenced by the views

reported by Hymes (1964), Pride and Holmes (1972), and Duranti (2003). These

writers make deliberate links between linguistics and anthropology in which the

production and selection of diverse methodological and conceptual tools are managed

differently.

A considerable amount of literature has been published on the necessity of

interdisciplinary connections in all fields of study, and sociolinguistics is not by no

means an exception. The effective coalition of anthropological and linguistic approaches

to sociolinguistics issues, as an important professional endeavor, has widely been

recognized in the research contexts (Duranti 2001, 2003; Gumperz 1972, 1982, 1999;

Koerner 1991; Lerner, 2004; Murray 1998; Paulston & Tucker, 1997; Shuy 1990). As

Knoblauch and Kotthoff (2001) put it, the intellectual coaliaton of these and many other

strands of scholarship toward interdisciplinary research indicates that there are

problem areas and sets of issues that previous methods of analysis overlooked.

In the new global professionalism in the domain of sociolinguistics and linguistic

anthropology, appropriate methodology has long been at the heart of scholarly inquiry

about culture and society. What makes the research significant is largely dependent

upon the on points of interdisciplinary (and intradisciplinary) intersection. The

coalition between sociolinguistics and anthropological linguistics leads to affective and

innovative problem-solving engagement. This type of involvement creates a

methodological readiness which is an important facilitator for implementing the

sociocultural linguistic studies.

All in all, despite the existing discrepancies in their theoretical and methodological

commitments, the cooperative and complementary work between sociolinguistics and

associated fields (e.g. linguistic anthropology, conversation analysis, critical discourse

analysis, language, gender, etc.) amplify a dialogue across intellectual divides and

moving sociocultural linguistic inquiry in fresh ways.

REFERENCES

Bernstein, B. (1972). Class, codes and control: Theoretical studies toward a sociology of language. (vol. 1). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

Biria, R. & Bahadoran-Baghbaderani, A. (2015). The interplay between topic familiarity and passage sight vocabulary: Focusing on its impact on EFL learners' lexical inferencing and recall. Journal of Applied Linguistics and Language Research, 2(4), 179-195.

Biria, R. & Bahadoran-Baghbaderani, A. (2016). Cross-cultural analysis of prototypicality norms used by male and female Persian and American speakers. Journal of psycholinguistic Research, 45(6), 1301-1314.

Bloomfield, L. (1933). Language, New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

Page 17: Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into

Journal of Applied Linguistics and Language Research, 2018, 5(1) 31

Butler, J. (1990). Gender trouble: Feminism and the subversion of identity. New York: Routledge.

Chambers, J. K. (2003). Sociolinguistic theory: Linguistic variation and its social implications. Oxford: Blackwell.

Chambers, J. K. and Trudglll, P. (1980). Dialectology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press

Coulmas, F. (2001). Formulaic language. In R. Mesthrie, & R. E. Asher (Eds.), Concise encyclopedia of sociolinguistics (pp. 233-235). Oxford: Elsevier Science Ltd.

Duranti, A. (2001). Linguistic anthropology: History, ideas, and issues. In A. Duranti (Ed.), Linguistic anthropology: A reader (pp. 1-38). Malden, MA: Blackwell.

Duranti, A. (2003). Language as culture in U.S. anthropology, Current Anthropology 44(3), 323–347.

Firth, J. R. (1957). Papers in linguistics 1934–1951. London: Oxford University Press.

Foucault, M. (1998). The history of sexuality: The will to knowledge. London, Penguin.

Gilbert, A. L., Regier, T., Kay, P., & Ivry, R. B. (2008). Support for lateralization of the Whorf effect beyond the realm of color discrimination. Brain and Language, 105(2), 91–98.

Gumperz, J. (1982). Discourse strategies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Gumperz, J. (1999). Sociocultural knowledge in conversational inference. In A. Jaworski & N. Coupland (Eds.), The discourse reader (pp. 98-106). London: Routledge.

Gumperz, J. (1972). Introduction. In John J. Gumperz and Dell Hymes (Eds.) Directions in sociolinguistics: The ethnography of communication (pp.1–25). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

Guy, G. R. (1988). Language and social class. In F. J. Newmeyer (Ed.), Language: The socio-cultural context. Linguistics: The Cambridge survey (pp. 37-63). Cambridge, Eng: Cambridge University Press.

Howatt, W. A. (1999). Journaling to self-evaluation: A tool for the adult learner. International Journal of Reality Therapy, 18(2), 32-34.

Hudson, R. (2001). Sociolinguistics. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Humboldt, W. V. (1836). On language: The diversity of human language-structure and its influence on the mental development of mankind. Translated by Peter Heath. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Hymes, D. (1972). Models of the interaction of language and social life. In J. J. Gumperz & D. Hymes (Eds.), Directions in sociolinguistics: Ethnography of communication (pp. 35-71). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

Janda, R. D., & Joseph, B. D. (2003). On Language, change and language change. In B. D. Joseph & R. D. Janda (Eds.), The handbook of historical linguistics (pp. 3–180). Wiley-Blackwell.

Knoblauch , H. , & Kotthoff , H. (2001) . Verbal art across cultures. Tübingen : Gunter Narr.

Page 18: Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into

Are Evolving Paradigms in Sociolinguistics Turning It into a Well-Developed Discipline? 32

Koerner, K. (1991). Towards a history of modern sociolinguistics. American Speech, 66, 57-70.

Labov, W. (1966). The social stratification of English in New York City. Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics.

Lee, Joseph, J. L. (2005). The native speaker: An achievable model?,. The Asian EFL Journal Quarterly, 7(2), 152-163.

Lerner, G. H. (2004). Conversation Analysis. Studies from the First Generation. Amsterdam

Philadelphia: John Benjamins Pub.

Murray, S. O. (1998). American sociolinguistics: Theorists and theory groups . Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

Paulston, C. B. & Tucker, G. R. (1997): The Early Days of Sociolinguistics: Memories and Reflections. Dallas, TX: The Summer Institute of Linguistics.

Pride, J. B. and Holmes, J. (1972). Sociolinguistics. Harmondsworth, Mx: Penguin.

Schmitt, N. (2002). An introduction to applied linguistics. London, Britain: Arnold

Shuy, R. (1990). Evidence of cooperation in conversation: Topic-type in a solicitation to murder case. In R. Rieber & W. Stewart (Eds.), The Language scientist as expert in the legal setting: Issues in forensic linguistics (pp. 85-106). New York: The New York Academy of Sciences.

Wardhaugh, R. (2001). An introduction to sociolinguistics. Beijing: Foreign Language Teaching and Research Press.

Whorf, B. L. (1956). Language, thought and reality: Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Wilkins, D. P. (1996). Morphology. In H. Goebl, P. Neide, Z. Stary, & W. Wölck (Eds.), International handbook of contact linguistics (pp. 109-117). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.