a for effort: consumer responses to own and others’ effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort...

73
Sofie Sagfossen A FOR EFFORT CONSUMER RESPONSES TO OWN AND OTHERS’ EFFORT

Upload: others

Post on 10-Jul-2020

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

Sofie Sagfossen A

FOR EFFO

RT

ISBN 978-91-7731-167-6

DOCTORAL DISSERTATION IN BUSINESS ADMINISTRATION STOCKHOLM SCHOOL OF ECONOMICS, SWEDEN 2020

Sofie Sagfossen

A FOR EFFORTCONSUMER RESPONSES TO OWN AND OTHERS’ EFFORT

A FOR EFFORT: CONSUMER RESPONSES TO OWN AND OTHERS’ EFFORT

Despite digitalization, e-commerce, and the fact that people spend more and more of their time in front of screens and devices, people still have phys-ical bodies. This thesis comprises four articles which in different ways show that humans’ bodily activity can have effects on information processing. Researchers in marketing and consumer behavior have paid less attention to the physical and bodily aspects of decision making. The findings from this thesis are more relevant than ever as marketers currently can and do reach consumers as they are active in some way. This thesis focus on the concept of effort and its ability to affect consumers and their evaluations in different ways. Consumers’ effort and physical arousal are both consequences of hu-man bodies being activated in various ways and to different extent.

SOFIE SAGFOSSEN is a researcher at Center for Consumer Marketing at Stockholm School of Economics.

Page 2: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

Sofie Sagfossen A

FOR EFFO

RT

ISBN 978-91-7731-167-6

DOCTORAL DISSERTATION IN BUSINESS ADMINISTRATION STOCKHOLM SCHOOL OF ECONOMICS, SWEDEN 2020

Sofie Sagfossen

A FOR EFFORTCONSUMER RESPONSES TO OWN AND OTHERS’ EFFORT

A FOR EFFORT: CONSUMER RESPONSES TO OWN AND OTHERS’ EFFORT

Despite digitalization, e-commerce, and the fact that people spend more and more of their time in front of screens and devices, people still have phys-ical bodies. This thesis comprises four articles which in different ways show that humans’ bodily activity can have effects on information processing. Researchers in marketing and consumer behavior have paid less attention to the physical and bodily aspects of decision making. The findings from this thesis are more relevant than ever as marketers currently can and do reach consumers as they are active in some way. This thesis focus on the concept of effort and its ability to affect consumers and their evaluations in different ways. Consumers’ effort and physical arousal are both consequences of hu-man bodies being activated in various ways and to different extent.

SOFIE SAGFOSSEN is a researcher at Center for Consumer Marketing at Stockholm School of Economics.

Page 3: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

A for Effort

Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort

Sofie Sagfossen

Akademisk avhandling

som för avläggande av ekonomie doktorsexamen vid Handelshögskolan i Stockholm framläggs för offentlig granskning

fredagen den 5 juni 2020, kl 10.15, sal Ragnar, Handelshögskolan,

Sveavägen 65, Stockholm

Page 4: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

A for Effort

Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort

Page 5: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm
Page 6: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

A for Effort

Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort

Sofie Sagfossen

Page 7: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

i i

Dissertation for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy, Ph.D., in Business Administration Stockholm School of Economics, 2020

A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort © SSE and the author, 2020 ISBN 978-91-7731-167-6 (printed) ISBN 978-91-7731-168-3 (pdf)

Front cover illustration: © Sofie Sagfossen, 2020

Back cover photo: © SSE. Photo by Juliana Wiklund

Printed by: BrandFactory, Gothenburg, 2020

Keywords: Effort, arousal, embodiment, consumer evaluations

Page 8: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

To Saga

Page 9: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm
Page 10: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

Foreword

This volume is the result of a research project carried out at the Department of Marketing and Strategy at the Stockholm School of Economics (SSE).

The volume is submitted as a doctoral thesis at SSE. In keeping with the policies of SSE, the author has been entirely free to conduct and present her research in the manner of her choosing as an expression of her own ideas.

SSE is grateful for the financial support provided by Torsten Söderbergs Stiftelse, Johan och Jakob Söderbergs Stiftelse, Stiftelsen Louis Fraenckels Stipendiefond, KAW:s Jubileumsanslag and C.F. Liljevalch Jr:s donationsfond which have made it possible to carry out the project.

Göran Lindqvist Hans Kjellberg

Director of Research Professor and Head of the Stockholm School of Economics Department of Marketing and Strategy

Page 11: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm
Page 12: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

Acknowledgements

I am eternally grateful to my supervisor, Magnus Söderlund, who is a true scholar. And more specifically, I want to thank him for being an incarnation of Google scholar, for all creative and entertaining discussions in the corridor, of which some actually were realized as projects, and for the conference papers, and published articles that we have produced together. Thanks for always being available, for thoughtful input, patience and careful reading.

I also want to thank Micael Dahlen for being my supervisor in every regard except formally on paper. Your advice and support have been invaluable. After a conversation with you I always walk away with new ideas and inspiration. My huge appreciation for Jonas Colliander for taking me on board fun projects, for accepting the mock opponent task and whose input improved this thesis substantially. I wish to express my deepest gratitude to Carl-Philip Ahlbom, my article-dealer, research co-author, PROCESS-Personal Trainer© and reference-wizard, but foremost a great friend.

Before moving on to present thanks and acknowledgments, I would like to look back and reflect on how I even got to SSE, and eventually the PhD-programme, in the first place. The single most decisive factor to explain this binary variable is the International Baccalaureate-programme. Therefore, I sincerely want to thank all my IB-teachers at Malmö Borgarskola 1999-2002 for fostering a love of learning and for being true role models in teaching.

I am grateful for the financial support provided by Torsten Söderbergs Stiftelse, Johan och Jakob Söderbergs Stiftelse.

Thanks to John Karsberg, co-author and friend, for all the fun, not to say the exciting field data collections we pulled off; Stefan Szugalski the best roomie and coffee maker; Erik Modig, for your inclusive and generous style, always making things happen; Reema Singh for our research project and for

Page 13: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

vii i

being a spirit lifter; Martin Söndergaard for posing the most interesting questions, always sparking intriguing conversations; Hanna Berg and Karina Liljedal for your support and inspiration; Sara Rosengren, for generosity in everything from PhD-seminars to academic advice and involving me in teaching; Angelica Blom, for your thoughtful input; Pär Mårtensson for encouragement and inspiration to continually improving my pedagogical skills; Martin Carlsson-Wall and Robert Larsson for having me on the Centre for Sport and Business team. To all my colleagues at CCM, CFR and SSE: I would like to recognize the invaluable assistance that you all provided during my study: Emelie Fröberg, Nina Åkestam, Rebecca Gruvhammar, Joel Ringbo, Richard Wahlund, Svetlana Kolesova, Wiley Wakeman, Claes-Robert Julander, Fredrik Lange, Karl Strelis, Per Hedberg, Ned Choungprayoon, Ksenia Rundin , Huong Nguyen, Stina Lundgren Högbom and Christofer Laurell. Finally, big kudos and thanks to all staff of SSE assisting me along the journey - in everything from fixing my laptop, to key cards, and not the least editing the PhD Word-template. Thanks to Upplevelseinstitutet and Vasaloppet for generously sharing data and insights.

For my friends: Maria Booth, everyday highlight, since the first day of the programme, always time for a Sosta and a debrief. And Hannah Altmann, we have come a long way from our master thesis, thanks for everything, and for still being around. Gustav Almqvist for adding humour and new perspectives regardless of topic, and thanks for providing valuable input to the theoretical framework. All of you are my allies in course work, academic crisis and happy hours. This journey would not have been as fun without the three of you. Anna von Wachenfelt and Louise Jerneborg of the Politbyrå, the strategic geniuses of my international advisory board, always giving endless support, in good times and bad.

Thanks to my family Tone, Jonny and Mikael for all support, but foremost for never stopping me from doing whatever I set out to do. I also want to express gratitude for my grandmother Vanja. And finally thank you Jocke and Saga, you are everything in the world to me.

Stockholm, April 14, 2020

Sofie Sagfossen

Page 14: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

Contents

CHAPTER 1

Introduction ............................................................................................................ 1

CHAPTER 2

Theorethical framework ........................................................................................ 5 What is Effort? ................................................................................................... 5

Effort in neurological studies ...................................................................... 7 Effort in terms of embodiment .................................................................. 8

Consequences of Effort ................................................................................... 9 Arousal ............................................................................................................ 9 The relationship between Effort and Arousal ........................................ 10 Consequences of Arousal .......................................................................... 11 Effort and value .......................................................................................... 14 Effort as a signal ......................................................................................... 16 Supplier effort .............................................................................................. 17 Effort and meaning .................................................................................... 18

CHAPTER 3

Research Methodology ........................................................................................ 23

CHAPTER 4

Summaries of findings from five research projects relating to effort .......... 35 Research project 1. My army training week ................................................. 35 Research project 2. Consumers’ intrinsic and extrinsic motivation

when consuming high-effort endurance activities and its consequences on future intentions to exert effort again ...................... 37

Page 15: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

x

Research project 3. Can consumer beliefs be strengthened when consumers are engaged in effortful shopping activity while receiving communication? ............................................................... 39

Project 4. The (failed)gym experiment ......................................................... 44 Project 5. Perceived supplier effort in a packaging context ...................... 45

CHAPTER 5

Research article summaries ................................................................................. 47

CHAPTER 6

Article 1. Advertising “On the Go”: Are Consumers In Motion More Influenced by Ads?: Why Advertisers Should Consider Consumers' Physical Activity when Planning Ad Campaigns ........................................ 51

CHAPTER 7

Article 2. The Impact of Stimuli Person’s Arousal Depicted in Video Advertising Clips .............................................................................................. 61

CHAPTER 8

Article 3. The Embodied Retail Consumer: Physical Effort in Shopping-Related Tasks and its Impact on Reactions to Messages .... 73

CHAPTER 9

Article 4. The consumer experience: The impact of supplier effort and consumer effort on customer satisfaction ............................................ 89

CHAPTER 10

Conclusions ......................................................................................................... 101

REFERENCES

References for chapter 1-4 and chapter 10 ..................................................... 107

Page 16: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

Chapter 1

Introduction

Despite digitalization, e-commerce, and the fact that people spend more and more of their time in front of screens and devices, people still have physical bodies. This thesis comprises four articles which in different ways show that humans’ bodily activity can have effects on information processing. However, the traditional type of advertising and marketing research of consumers usually entails no physical activity. The consumer usually sits down and get exposed to some form of marketing activity. It seems that in marketing practice and consumer behavior research, researchers have paid less attention to the physical and bodily aspects of decision making. The findings from this thesis are more relevant than ever as marketers currently can and do reach consumers (e.g. through their mobile devices) as they are active in some way.

Moreover, consumers still need to engage physically in chores and activities related to consumption and marketing to conduct their lives daily. For instance, many people need to commute. The public spaces are full of commercial messages. Another example of ordinary consumer activity includes carrying groceries while being exposed to messages. In our roles as consumers, various kinds of communication messages and information in a ubiquitous ever-increasing rate bombard us. Sometimes consumers pay for convenience; for example, people can take a taxi because they do not want to make an effort to walk a certain distance. At other times, people choose the escalator instead of walking up or down the stairs, because it is less physically demanding. In other situations, consumers pay to be

Page 17: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

2 A FOR EFFORT

inconvenienced. People can pay a relatively large amount of money for a gym membership. As gym members, they can hire a personal trainer in order to make sure the member exerts even more physical effort. Or they can voluntarily choose to partake in a 90 km cross country skiing race, Vasaloppet, which is examined in a project presented in chapter 4.

Several variables are involved in typical marketing situations and phenomena, for example, price, service, advertising, promotions, product features, elements in advertising, and branding. The choice for this thesis was made to focus on the concept of effort and its ability to affect consumers in different ways. Although not entirely overlooked in the literature, effort have received less attention than other variables examined in marketing. The definitions, theoretical foundations and earlier findings about consequences of effort and arousal will follow in chapter 2. Arousal is a consequence of effort. Effort and its consequences play a part in everyday lives and consumer transactions and are therefore worth studying.

Researchers have rejected the Descartian view that the mind and matter are strictly separate entities (Damasio, 1994; Schwarz, 2011). This view is consonant with the embodiment literature, which assumes that thoughts, emotions, and behaviors are grounded in bodily states and sensory experiences (Barsalou, 2008; Krischna & Schwartz, 2014). In relation to the embodiment and grounded cognition literature physical effort and arousal constitutes two aspects of bodily states out of many (e.g. temperature, facial muscular activity, movement). The underlying assumption is that humans’ physical bodies and their physical states can affect emotions, decisions, and behavior. Since both effort and arousal are aspects of bodily information, this thesis share the underpinnings of the embodiment and grounded cognition literature, that bodily states has the potential to affect cognition.

Given that consumers’ arousal is always a part of any transaction or encounter because arousal is the level of physiological activation in a given moment, it is valuable to understand arousal’s impact on consumer responses. The same argument exists for effort, which can be exerted by the consumer as well as by the producer of offerings. The fact is that effort is a necessary part of everyday life and an unavoidable consequence of having a physical body. Effort can impact consumers’ perceptions and decisions; thus, it is worth studying. In short, effort can constitute a cost, and it can serve as

Page 18: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

CHAPTER 1 3

a signal. Effort can also transform into value, and it can carry meaning. Earlier research has also established that effort and arousal can impact consumer reactions and information processing (e.g. Reisenzein 1983; Gorn et al., 2001; Inzlicht et al., 2018).

The purpose of this dissertation is to advance the understanding of the influence of effort and arousal on consumer responses. The responses investigated include product-, brand- and advertising evaluations, customer satisfaction, beliefs and behavioral intentions. Moreover, this dissertation is a response to calls for paying more attention to the context when it comes to advertising outcomes and consumer responses (Karsberg, 2016).

Prior to conducting this research, I worked with perception data and examined customer attitudes and satisfaction. Having my corporate experience close in mind, I was inspired by the call from Baumeister et al. (2007), to to include more actual behavior in research, and I decided to move beyond perception data and include behavior in the research projects. The intial idea was to explain behavior as a dependent variable, and to operationalize behavior as physical effort by making people chose between stairs or elvators. However, after discussions with colleagues and after conducting some pre-tests, I decided it was more interesting to use the behavior of physical effort and arousal as independent variables in the research designs.

The four research articles included in this thesis focus on how effort and arousal of consumers can affect consumer behavior, and especially evaluations. Consumers’ effort and arousal are both consequences of human bodies being activated in various ways and to different extent. However, to be specific, the articles also encompass depicted arousal and perceived supplier effort. Understanding what affects consumers’ evaluations is central for understanding consumer decision making, because these evaluations often inform subsequent purchase decisions or recommendations.

The findings of this thesis add to the understanding of consumer behavior. The independent variable of effort constitute an inherent part of consumers’ conditions. These conditions affect dependent variables related to the consumers’ decision making, including beliefs, evaluations, and behavioral intentions, essential parts of understanding consumer behavior. Moreover, one boundary condition, perceived relatedness between

Page 19: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

4 A FOR EFFORT

advertising and context is identified (Article 1). The thesis also examines different underlying mechanisms including: subjective message comprehension, perceived supplier effort, perceived quality and perceived value of offering.

These findings contribute to the research on effort by specifically testing theories in various consumer contexts and in the field, and thereby also contributing to the marketing and advertising literature. Few researchers have focused on consumers own physical effort, compared with for example cognitive effort. Concerning the gap in advertising literature, no one had investigated the link between consumers own movement and subsequent advertising evaluation. Furthermore, the arousal of depicted persons in video ads and its effect on evaluations is to my knowledge also a novel combination, not previously investigated in advertising.

This dissertation includes four articles with eight empirical studies. A theoretical framework relating to the overarching theme of effort and arousal (Chapter 2) will be followed by a methodological discussion (Chapter 3), a summary of five effort related research projects (Chapter 4), introduction to the articles (Chapter 5) followed by the four articles (Chapter 6-9). Article 1 examines how moving consumers evaluate advertising messages. Article 2 investigates the effect of depicted arousal in video clips on advertising and product evaluation. Article 3 examines how the physical effort of consumers affects evaluations of commercial messages. Article 4 studies the effect of the consumer’s effort and the perceived producer effort on the evaluations of an experiential offering. Finally, conclusions from the research will be presented (Chapter 10). .

Page 20: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

Chapter 2

Theorethical framework

The purpose of this chapter is to present a theoretical framework and the literature relating to the concept of effort. Since consumers are frequently moving and engaging in different types of physically demanding activities this can be expected to have several consequences on information processing. This chapter will cover literature from early psychology, social psychology, consumer behavior, advertising, and neuroscience. The ambition is to provide a much wider understandning of the literature relating to effort compared to the theoretical overviews in the four articles. The outline of this chapter is made up by two main sections: In the first section, I describe what effort is; the definition for the concept of effort followed by effort’s characherteristics as being a cost, effort in neurological studies, and finally effort as embodied information. The second section discusses the consequences of effort, which encompasses an overview of research findings on how effort and arousal affect human thinking and consumer behavior.

What is Effort?

A general definition of effort is that it is the “subjective intensification of mental and/or physical activity in the service of meeting some goal” (Inzlicht et al., 2018, p 2). As such, it is separated from, but related to the concept of motivation, which has a direction and a level of intensity, as effort refers to intensity or amplitude of behavior of an agent, but it does not relate to a

Page 21: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

6 A FOR EFFORT

specific goal. Furthermore, effort has been described as a volitional and intentional process, something which people apply intentionally (Inzlicht et al., 2018). As such, it refers to what people actively do and not what is passively happening to them. The main focus of this thesis is on physical effort, but other facets including social and cognitive effort is also briefly included in article 4.

The basic intuition when it comes to effort is that it is costly and should be avoided. Humans and animals are effort averse (Botvinick et al., 2009). We should only exert effort if we are rewarded accordingly. Therefore, in general, if given a choice, both humans and animals, in most situations, tend to avoid effort (Kurzban, 2016). Hull (1943) viewed effort as an aversive stimulus and formed “the least effort hypothesis”,

If two or more behavioral sequences, each involving a different amount of energy consumption of work, have been equally well reinforced an equal number of times, the organism will gradually learn to choose the less laborious sequence leading to the attainment of the reinforcing state of affairs (p. 294).

Furthermore,

A principal law of survival is that energy consumption must exceed energy expenditure. This law implies an economy of action in which energy must be conserved over time expenditure must not exceed consumption. For example, a predator cannot expend more energy in catching its prey than it acquires by eating them. All actions expend energy and this energy must be managed effectively. (Proffitt, 2006, p.111)

Marx (1887) also reasoned about human effort but in terms of labor: “By labour-power or capacity for labour is to be understood the aggregate of those mental and physical capabilities existing in a human being, which he exercises whenever he produces a use-value of any description” (p. 119). Here, we should note that Marx discussed labor only from the production side, and did not consider the workers in a role as consumers. In Das Kapital, Marx discusses how to determine the value of human effort (i.e., labor). He describes how labor is used to create capital. He concludes that labor effort is a resource which is spent and thus needs to be replenished, specifically through rest, food, heat, and shelter, “labour-power, however, becomes a

Page 22: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

CHAPTER 2 7

reality only by its exercise; it sets itself in action only by working. But thereby a definite quantity of human muscle, nerve, brain is wasted, and these require to be restored” (Marx, 1887, p. 121). In other words, the reward needed to cover the workers’ expenses such that tomorrow, the worker will be able to provide the same amount of labor again. To Marx, the efforts exerted by laborers were a cost, and over time value created was transferred to the capitalists.

Effort in neurological studies

Researchers have studied effort in neurological terms. Neuroscience has made advances in understanding how people value a course of action in terms of costs and benefits for the options. The anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) region of the brain processes effort aversiveness (McGuire & Botvinick, 2010). Walton et al. (2006) used rodents and monkeys to investigate brain activity to calculate effort. They identified the ACC and mesolimbic dopamine pathways (Walton et al., 2006) as responsible for processing effort aversion. They also showed that impairment of those areas led to changes in how the test subjects valued low-effort and low-reward versus high-effort and high-reward in relation to each other.

A task faced in everyday life is to decide the amount of effort to exert to obtain a particular reward. Effort exists both in the cognitive domain and in the physical domain. However, little was known before about the neural basis for how people calculate cognitive versus physical efforts. More specifically: are physical and cognitive effort costs processed in the same way, by the same regions of the brain? Chong et al. (2017) examined these questions by using fMRI. The findings showed that those effort calculations were connected to an extensive network of overlapping brain areas across the parietal-prefrontal cortex and insula. Notably, the researchers found that the amygdala plays a specific role in judgments about cognitive effort (Chong et al., 2017). The study provided another exciting finding: when the researchers examined the profiles of the participants, they discovered that individual’s dispositions affected their preferences for mental over physical effort; in other words, individuals can attach different subjective values and inclination towards mental and physical effort, respectively. This finding implies that some

Page 23: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

8 A FOR EFFORT

individuals are more prone to engage in cognitive effort than physical effort and vice versa.

Effort in terms of embodiment

The notion that the physical body influences cognition is nothing new; this phenomenon has been the basis for the literature on embodiment and grounded cognition. Embodiment refers to the notion that feelings, thoughts, and behaviors are grounded in bodily interaction with the surrounding environment (Meier et al., 2012; Tversky, 2019). Grounded cognition researchers argue that bodily states affect cognition (for reviews see Barsalou, 2008; Krishna & Schwartz, 2014). Therefore, physical actions can affect human thoughts and responses in different ways. In social psychology, the concept of embodiment describes the assumption that thoughts, emotions and behaviors are grounded in bodily states and sensory experiences (Barsalou, 2008; Krischna & Schwartz, 2014). In one embodiment study, for example, people who were nodding were more likely to agree with a message than the ones who were shaking their heads (Wells & Petty, 1980). In another study, people rated cartoons more entertaining when holding a pen with their teeth (facilitating smiling) as opposed to their lips (impairing smiling) (Strack et al., 1988).

Findings from the embodiment literature have sometimes been remarkable and spread widely outside academia. However, researchers have been unable to replicate some studies (Ioannidis, 2005), for example, a famous study by Williams and Bargh (2008) of how feeling physical warmth transfers to perceiving interpersonal warmth and pro-social behavior. Embodiment plays a role in how people estimate and calculate the costs of effort. In a series of experiments Proffitt (2006) showed that human perception is embodied and malleable. The participants’ effort in different ways. For one study, the treatment group wore heavy backpacks, while the control group carried lighter backpacks. Each group estimated the slants of hills. To the participants with the heavier bags, the hills appeared steeper as their physiological capability was lower than the participants with the lighter backpacks. The findings indicate that human and specifically visual perception is affected by embodied information. The results also show that

Page 24: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

CHAPTER 2 9

human perception of the spatial environment is not always stable, and people can, for example, perceive a distance as longer when tired or if they have relatively weaker physical capabilities. It seems that the energetic cost of effort which is associated with the actions, such as walking up a hill, is incorporated from the body and into the judgment of humans (Proffitt, 2006).

Consequences of Effort

This section will provide a definition of arousal then subsequently discuss it in terms of its relationship to effort. The reason why arousal is situated under consequences of effort is due to the way arousal is operationalized in the articles (e.g walking stairs, visting gym). Hence, in the articles arousal can be claimed to be a consequence of physical effort which has been exterted either by participants or by stimuli persons.

Arousal

Arousal refers to humans’ physiological state along a continuum ranging from asleep on one end to extremely aroused and excited on the other end. Furthermore, arousal refers to either physiological states encompassing levels of energy, activation, inner tension, and alertness, or psychological states of wakefulness or readiness for taking action (Singh & Hitchon, 1989). The concept has also been described as feeling hyperactive, wide awake and alerted on one end of a spectrum to feeling relaxed, sleepy or sluggish on the other end (Galentino et al., 2017). There has been some confusion in the literature with regards to the use of the terms arousal, activation, and anxiety. Arousal has been used synonymously with the term “activation” when referring to a nondirectional generalized bodily activation, which also has the function of connecting the body’s resources for forceful activity (Sage, 1984). Anxiety, on the other hand, should be regarded as separate from arousal, even if they may correlate, since anxiety additionally refers to a state which has an emotional dimension, or reaction which is often characterized by

Page 25: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

10 A FOR EFFORT

unpleasant feelings of intensity, apprehension, and disturbance (Arent & Landers, 2003; Spielberger, 1975).

Additionally, arousal constitutes one of the two general dimensions in the circumplex model of affect in which discrete emotions (e.g., happiness, sadness, anger, boredom) are plotted in terms of degree of activation (i.e., arousal) on one axis and in terms of pleasantness on the other axis (Russell, 1980; Russell & Barrett, 1999). Early research in psychology tried to disentangle the relationships of physical arousal, cognition, and emotional state (Schachter, 1964). The visceral states of the body could be identical for a variety of emotional states, and variety of emotions, moods and feelings, but the emotional states did not correspond with the same variety of visceral states, which suggested that “an emotional state may be considered a function of a state of physiological arousal, and of a cognition appropriate to this state of arousal” (Schachter & Singer, 1962, p. 380). The researchers concluded that human cognitions that arise in a context would make up the framework within which we understand and label our emotions. This finding means that it is cognition that determines if a state of physiological arousal will be understood and labeled as “joy,” “fear,” “anger,” or something else. This outcome implies that both the information of arousal and cognition are needed to experience an emotion (Schachter & Singer, 1962).

The relationship between Effort and Arousal

Given the constructs used in the research articles which concern arousal (Article 1 and 2) and effort (Article 1 - 4), it is essential to explain how the concepts of effort and arousal relate to each other. Also for an overview of the articles, see Table 1, p 21. As stated earlier, both effort and arousal are consequences of human bodies which are being activated. The concept of arousal describes a certain level of physiological activation along a continuum at a specific point in time. Effort is conceptually different from arousal as effort can refer to some cumulative amount of energy, which often means an expenditure of energy over time, as discussed in the previous section. Effort can, of course, also be used to describe how much subjective effort is exerted at a given point in time. However, arousal is practically never used to describe a sum of energy levels cumulatively. In some sense, arousal refers to an

Page 26: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

CHAPTER 2 11

absolute value along a continuum, and at a specific point in time. Higher levels of arousal are an inevitable effect of exerting physical effort. Effort can entail certain levels of arousal, but arousal, especially at low levels, is not always accompanied by a notion of effort. And finally, after some time has passed since high effort has been exerted, it is to be expected that people can experience low arousal such as tiredness, fatigue or sluggishness etc.

In general, I assume that the concepts are positively associated with each other such that: the higher the physical effort, the higher the level of arousal. However, after a certain amount of time has passed, high effort levels arousal is expected to be lowered, meaning that people get tired. Effort usually refers to a cumulative amount of energy which has been exerted in a time interval concerning some goal. The fact that effort as a concept has a cumulative characteristic, distinguishes effort from the concept of arousal, which refers to a certain level of tension, and which occurs at a certain point in time. To use an analogy, arousal is the balance sheet, while effort is the income statement.

Consequences of Arousal

The Yerkes-Dodson Law posits that optimal learning and decision making takes place within a critical range of negative stimulation (Brown & Herrnstein, 1975). “This law states that a relationship between arousal and behavioral task performance exists, such that there is an optimal level of arousal for an optimal performance. Over- or under-arousal reduces task performance” (Cohen, 2011, p. 429)

Researchers have written many articles about Yerkes-Dodson’s Law. In its more modern versions, it has come to refer to the relationship between physical arousal and cognitive performance (cf. Anderson, 1990; Levitt & Gutin, 1971). The curve derived from the Yerkes-Dodson law essentially shows a span of optimal levels of arousal in subjects when it comes to cognitive processing capacity. Contemporary references to Yerkes and Dodson’s work do not convey the fact that in its original form, Yerkes and Dodson (1908) explored the rate of learning in mice when inducing a negative stimulus in the form of electric shocks when mice failed to discriminate between visuals of black and white. The punishment intensity

Page 27: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

12 A FOR EFFORT

and task difficulty varied, resulting in inverted U shaped patterns, where the ability to handle a task well increased with greater electric shocks, while at challenging levels of the task as and at even higher levels of unpleasant stimulation the ability to learn diminished (Yerkes & Dodson, 1908). Furthermore, Yerkes and Dodson never explicitly assumed that their findings applied to humans, but suggested researchers test the findings on other animals.

In a neatly performed study by Arent and Landers (2003), the levels of arousal in subjects were manipulated from 20% to 90% of heart rate to investigate the relationship with performance on a quick response task (low complexity). They found support for the inverted U-shaped relationship posited by the Yerkes Dodson Law. In the study, Arent and Landers (2003) found that the optimal level of arousal for the task was at 60% to 70% of maximum arousal. Somatic anxiety also significantly explained a smaller part of the variance in performance (Arent & Landers, 2003).

A study by Davranche and Audiffren (2004) confirmed the pattern. The study focused on the facilitating effect of submaximal physical exercise on cognitive processes, including reaction times, decision errors. The results showed that moderate-intensity exercise (at 50% of maximal aerobic power) improved cognitive performance when measured as response time (Davranche & Audiffren, 2004). Dietrich and Sparling (2004) found evidence that endurance exercise impairs cognition, and more specifically, the pre-frontal dependent cognition, which includes the higher-order cognitions including working memory, sustained and directed attention, temporal integration, and response inhibition.

Researchers need to consider many factors when investigating the relationship between arousal (and effort) and cognitive ability. The intensity and the duration of the effort or arousal is one, but also the nature of the cognitive task, as well as when the person performs the cognitive task in relation to the effort (Davranche & Audiffen, 2004).

In two experiments, the participants performed a rather strenuous kind of endurance training (at around 70-80 percent of maximum heart rate), either cycling or running for 25 to 40 minutes before the researchers introduced various cognitive tests while the participants continued the endurance training. The result showed that the exercising group performed

Page 28: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

CHAPTER 2 13

significantly worse, as measured in terms of the number of errors compared to the control group. The underlying hypothesis, the transient hypo-frontality hypothesis, suggested that there is a temporary inhibition of the brain regions that are not essential to performing the exercise, such as areas of the frontal lobe involved in higher cognitive functions (Browne, 2019). More specifically, the reason this occurs is that cerebral blood flow is constant during exercise, and thereby no additional metabolic resources are available during exercise; the sustained effort of motor and sensory systems during exercise takes place at the expense of activity in other parts of the brain (Dietrich & Sparling, 2004; Ide & Secher, 2000). In summary, humans’ capacity to think can be affected both positively and negatively depending on the level of arousal and the task complexity. The implication for marketing and specifically in an advertising context is that consumers’ ability to process stimuli will depend on their level of arousal.

Increased arousal stemming from physical activity likely guides stimuli evaluation in positive ways. The affect-as-information framework posits that affect is embodied information about value and importance (Clore & Storbeck, 2006). Extensions of this framework argue that arousal not only intensifies evaluative judgments but also serves as information by signaling importance and urgency (Storbeck & Clore, 2008). In the advertising literature, researchers have recognized arousal as influencing study participants’ evaluation of advertising, through the mechanism of (mis)attribution of the arousal, i.e., excitation transfer, caused by movement in the ad (Duff & Sar, 2015), or ads placed after highly arousing television programs (McGrath & Mahood, 2004). In the same study though, the recall of advertising was negatively influenced by the arousing television programs (McGrath & Mahood, 2004). Arousal in subjects has been shown to positively affect evaluations of advertising (Gorn et al., 2001; Shapiro & MacInnis, 2002).

Furthermore, arousal has been found to affect decision making, and higher arousal increases risk-taking (Galentino et al., 2017). Arousal can also impact consumer preferences (Di Muro & Murray, 2012) and intertemporal choice (Van den Bergh et al., 2008). A considerable amount of research supports the notion that arousal can heighten subsequent affective responses and evaluations (for review, see Reisenzein, 1983). Researchers have

Page 29: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

14 A FOR EFFORT

emphasized two underlying explanations for the effects of arousal. The first explanation concerns the interference of cognitive capacity. Highly elevated arousal is associated with a reduction in the cognitive capacity, which in turn impairs information processing and can lead consumers to rely more on peripheral cues (Sanbonmatsu & Kardes, 1988). The second mechanism is referred to as misattribution. The residual arousal following a specific event subdues relatively slowly and can be misattributed to the current stimuli (Schachter & Singer, 1962; White et al., 1981). The work on misattribution later developed into the excitation transfer hypothesis (Zillmann, 1971). These mechanisms are the theoretical underpinnings of the effects of arousal on the evaluation of advertising and products in articles 1 and 2.

Effort and value

In this section, I discuss different types of value-enhancing mechanisms of effort. Temporally the value of effort can arise either simultaneously as the effort is exerted or afterward. The fact that effort is sometimes valued in itself, even if no tangible rewards follow, and no extrinsic motives exist can seem counterintuitive. In line with basic microeconomic assumptions, humans are effort averse since effort represents a costly resource. Nevertheless, effort can lead to perceptions of value and even be valued in itself, both in the present and afterward. One example where effort is valued is when people experience flow, which is a highly pleasant state where effort and challenge are in equilibrium (Nakamura & Csikszentmihalyi, 2014). Studies of flow have found that people can experience flow in for different activities from Skydiving (Celsi et al., 1993) to playing the piano (de Manzano et al., 2010). This finding aligns with the findings that people sometimes value effort in its own right, for example, mountain climbing, an extremely effortful activity (Loewenstein, 1999). When discussing this activity, Lowenstein concluded that regular consumption motives and utility theory did not convincingly explain the findings. Lowenstein (1999) discusses four non-consumption-related motives where self-signaling, goal-completion, mastery, and meaning help explain effortful consumption such as mountaineering. Two of these, signaling and meaning, will be further elaborated on later in this chapter. Perhaps the reason people engage in effort

Page 30: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

CHAPTER 2 15

sometimes is in order to develop a state of higher arousal if that state is forecasted to be more pleasant than the current state. Perhaps that is the explanation behind the reports from early psychology studies about a drive for an activity for its own sake (Brown and Herrnstein, 1975). Rodents were found to have a fundamental inclination to move around after having been kept physically still for some time. Depriving a rat of its possibility to run for some time, later led to increased activity when an opportunity for moving occurred (Skinner, 1933). More recently, Hsee et al. (2010) showed that people try to avoid being idle and choose to get busy and thereby engage in effort as long as some kind of justification was present, even if the justification was specious.

Learned industriousness is another way in which effort has come to be valued in its own right (Eisenberger, 1992). The process behind this valuation can be understood in terms of conditioned association since effort usually is followed by a reward, and the greater the effort, the greater the reward. If this pattern repeats, effort can take the role of a secondary reinforcer, which signifies that a reward is pending. Thereby through such a learning process, effort is paired with value and thus becomes less aversive (Inzlicht et al., 2018). Another potential factor that links effort to (moral) value is the surrounding culture, which places value on hard work, for example, the Protestant work ethics (Weber, 1920).

Often effort has been found to increase the perceived value of something in retrospect, that is when the effort has already been exerted. Lewis pointed out that “effort does seem to enhance the value of a stimulus associated with the expenditure of effort” (1965, p. 183). The mechanisms behind the effort-enhancing effect have been pointed out as cognitive dissonance (Festinger, 1962) and sunk cost (Arkes & Blumer, 1985). Effort as an input variable from the consumer has also been studied by Cardozo (1965), who found that the effort customers put into the transaction in terms of search and evaluation time affected both the evaluation of the product and the shopping experience. The study showed that product satisfaction increased as when consumers undertook a higher amount of effort, and the effect was moderated by met or unmet product expectations. The mechanism can work in two directions, either through cognitive dissonance, where in order to justify the amount of effort expended, the evaluation of the product increases

Page 31: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

16 A FOR EFFORT

or through the contrast theory, implying that when getting less than expected, the customer will magnify the difference between the product received and expectation.

Effort has also been studied in terms of consumers’ effort as an input to the actual product. This situation is the basis for the so-called IKEA-effect, demonstrated in a study where consumers who assembled furniture from IKEA, pieces of Lego, and folded origami themselves valued the product higher and were willing to pay a higher price compared to the consumers who got the same products ready-made and without investing personal effort (Norton et al., 2012). Researchers have explained the underlying mechanism of the IKEA-effect through people’s need to signal competence to themselves and others, and the feelings of competence and control which arise in connection with self-made products cause their increased value (Mochon et al., 2012).

Effort as a signal

Another role of effort is the fact that it can act as a signal to oneself and to others. Researchers in evolutionary biology and evolutionary psychology developed the concept of signaling theory. Signaling theory explains communication between individuals or between species (Barett et al., 2002). Costly signaling can explain behaviors or even the development of certain physical features and behaviors in animals, which otherwise would appear wasteful and illogical, such as altruism (Zahavi, 1977a; Zahavi, 1977b). The peacock represents the classic example. The peacock sports a beautiful, yet extremely inconvenient tail. Carrying around a colorful tail is a handicap as it makes the peacock more vulnerable to predators in terms of its visibility and flight capacity. However, because the tail is costly, it signals, in a credible way, that the individual has a desirable quality, namely strength (Barett et al., 2002).

A signal is credible only if it is costly, hard to fake, and corresponds to some phenotypic quality, such as physical strength in the case of the peacock male (Zahavi, 1997). Kirmani and Rao (2000) extended the logic of signal theory to the context of marketing. They suggested that signaling is most efficient for conditions where consumers have some level of risk-aversion and have imperfect information regarding quality (Kirmani & Rao, 2000).

Page 32: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

CHAPTER 2 17

Supplier effort

Up until now, the focus has been on the consumers’ own effort (or arousal). Nevertheless, effort can also be exerted by the producer of an offering and then be perceived by consumers. This type of effort, originating from the producer of an offering or message, has been shown to serve as a heuristic of quality, especially when the quality of the object is difficult to establish (Kruger et al., 2004). The effect called “the effort heuristic” was demonstrated across three experiments where subjects rated the quality of poems, a painting, and a suit of armor while manipulating the level of effort claimed to have been involved in making the product (Kruger et al., 2004). The results showed that the more effort which was perceived to be behind the product, the higher the quality perceptions, and the level of object ambiguity moderated the effect. This finding is in line with the much earlier research on cognitive dissonance and self-perception research, which explains how the perceived discrepancy between the subjects’ invested effort and the evaluation of the outcomes is reduced (Aronson & Mills, 1959; Bem, 1972; Festinger, 1962). The same effect was conceptually replicated in research about producer effort, as conveyed by the packaging of a product (Söderlund et al., 2017).

As described in the previous section, perceived effort can work as a signal for unobservable qualities such as commitment and confidence (Kirmani & Wright, 1989;), or as summarized by Rory Sutherland, vice chairman at Ogilvy, who claimed that advertising is the costly signaling of faith in your futurity:

In my view, much advertising expenditure probably works this way. Since advertising is expensive — and difficult to do well — the cost of advertising is also a virtual engagement ring proffered to the potential consumer; the upfront expense entailed being proof of long-term commitment to the product, the brand and the relationship. Advertising sometimes conveys information, of course. But much of it ostensibly conveys really very little that is new or compelling. But the act of advertising, especially in expensive media, is a form of information in itself. Since it takes time to recoup the cost of an advertising campaign, it only pays to run one when the advertiser has reasonable expectations of the long-term, widespread popularity of the product being advertised. The act of advertising your product is hence, a valuable signal that

Page 33: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

18 A FOR EFFORT

the manufacturer has faith in its own product — equivalent to a racehorse owner betting heavily on his own horse. (Sutherland, 2003)

In summary, since effort is costly, effort has the potential to act as a signal. Consumers notice and care about the perceived effort of a supplier or offering. Perceived supplier effort can thus act as a signal to consumers and demonstrate an association with higher perceived quality, higher perceived value, and higher satisfaction (Kirmani & Rao, 2000; Mohr & Bitner, 1995; Söderlund & Sagfossen, 2017). Companies’ efforts in different domains such as advertising spending and advertising repetition can act as a signal to consumers and influence the perceptions of firms, brands, and quality (Dahlen et al., 2018; Kirmani, 1997). Moreover, Dahlen et al. (2008) found that consumers believe that creative advertising results from greater sender effort, which signals confidence, and consequently, consumer perceptions about the brand quality increase.

Effort and meaning

Another aspect of effort apart from efforts dimensions as a cost is that effort acts as a creator of value and as a signal. Exerted effort can also convey meaning. Effort is a part of many religious rituals. For example, in religious rituals, such meaning can be created as effortful activities are made, and the subsequent meaning is positively connotated. One example from Buddhism is chora, which essentially is a sacred lap around a holy place. One famous, and indeed very effortful, chora is to complete a full lap of 52 km at an altitude between 4600 and 5500 meters around Mount Kailash, a mountain in Tibetan Himalaya. The most extreme version of the chora is to prostrate, which means to lie flat, face down on the ground to show reverence, or submission, all the way around the mountain, a feat which allegedly takes approximately four weeks to complete. This feat serves as an extreme example where effort becomes valuable as effort becomes intertwined with religious meaning. However, while effort can convey and add meaning, effort can also lack meaning, and in that case, be used as a punishment.

Ariely et al. (2008) investigated the connection between labor, and meaning and found that people’s willingness to work (i.e., to exert effort),

Page 34: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

CHAPTER 2 19

increased when the work was recognized as opposed to when the work was clearly ignored or even undone. The same study used another experiment to test this theory. Participants finished an administrative task to detect specific letters on pages full of text. In one of the conditions, the work was ignored and then destroyed with a paper shredder in front of the participants. The groups which lacked meaning in their effort produced less work and were rewarded less. The results showed that willingness to exert effort, when applied without recognition, or even explicitly devoid of any meaning, was diminished, which means the same task but without meaning was experienced as being subjectively aversive (Ariely et al., 2008). Effort, which is pointless, is a means to exercise power for its own sake.

Meaning, at least in part, derives from the connection between work and some purpose, however insignificant or irrelevant that purpose may be to the worker’s personal goals. When that connection is severed, when there is no purpose, work becomes absurd, alienating, or even demeaning. (Ariely et al. 2008, p. 676)

The Greek myth of Sisyphus can serve as an example. According to the myth, Sisyphus received the punishment to everlastingly roll a large rock toward the top of a mountain, but to never reach the top. Indeed, since then, and probably before that, penal labor has been widely used in many parts of the world. In the Victorian era, hard labor prisoners had physical labor explicitly designed as a punishment. Sometimes the labor was slightly productive, but in its most extreme forms, the labor was entirely meaningless. In some cases, prisoners lifted and moved cannonballs around in piles all day or worked the treadmill. The GULAG system in the Soviet Union represented a large scale example of penal labor. The work was part of the punishment, and although meant actually to render output, the ways the work was operated made it on the whole extremely inefficient (Applebaum, 2007). Given the findings on effort justification, researchers have shown that exerting physical effort without reason and purpose would be additionally hard, even beyond the actual physical effort exerted (Ariely et al., 2008).

Page 35: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

20 A FOR EFFORT

To further summarize this theoretical overview: effort and arousal are distinct but related concepts. Arousal being the physiological state at a specific time ranging from highly aroused, awake and excited to low levels of arousal described as sleepy or relaxed. Effort was defined as the experience of intensification of physical activity in relation to some goal. A number of different studies described the effects from different levels of arousal on subjects’ information processing, performance on cognitive tasks, and evaluation of information. The effects are mixed depending on the levels, but many of the studies which have used objective measures such as heart rate confirm the inverted U-shape pattern suggested by the Yerkes-Dodsons law. The resource of energy is wasted as effort, the other concept of this chapter, is exerted, incurring a cost to the individual. This resource must then be replenished. As effort is exerted, it becomes a cost to the individual exerting it, and since effort is costly, effort can act as a credible signal, to the self, and others. The effort in itself can be rewarding and valuable, or the consequences or results of the effort can be valued higher or perceived to have higher quality. Finally, meaning represents an additional dimension of effort. Effort can be a part of meaningful experiences. However, when effort is exerted without meaning, effort is experienced as subjectively worse compared to than when effort is coupled with some purpose. The research summarized in this chapter establishes that both effort and arousal have effects on human thinking and evaluations.

Page 36: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

Table 1. Effort in the articles

Article 1 Article 2 Article 3 Article 4 S1 S2 S3 S1 S2 S1 S1 S2 Own effort /arousal IV IV IV mediator mediator IV IV IV

Depicted arousal IV IV

Supplier effort mediator IV IV

Dependent variable

evaluation of ad & brand

evaluation of ad & brand

evaluation of ad & brand

evaluation of product & ad

evaluation of product & ad

evaluation of ad satisfaction satisfaction

Data source & study design

field observation

field experiment

field experiment

Mturk experiment

Mturk experiment

(F2F) experiment

Scenario experiment

Scenario experiment

IV= Independent variable

Page 37: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm
Page 38: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

Chapter 3

Research Methodology

Scientific Perspective

The subject of marketing has been described as transactions, the exchange of values between parties, and the process of transactions (Hunt, 1976, 1996a). Since effort is a costly resource, as described in chapter 2, effort plays a role in consumer transactions. The part of Hunt’s definition that comprises “exchange of values,” encompasses the focus of this thesis, which is concerned with understanding a set of consumer reactions to different forms of effort (1976). Hunt (1976, 1996b) argued that marketing constitutes a science since it fulfills certain criteria. A science must have a subject matter from the real world which is distinct, there must be “underlying uniformities and regularities interrelating the subject matter,” and finally a scientific field has to use “inter-subjectively certifiable procedures for studying the subject matter” (Hunt, 1976, p. 27); thus, it must use a scientific method. The current thesis is also aligned with the Buzzell’s (1963) notion that a science can be described as having a centralized body of knowledge, with one or more central theories and general principles, and it has knowledge which enables prediction of future events.

The most dominant theories used in the subfield called consumer behavior stem directly from psychology, or more specifically from consumer psychology. This group of theories is usually labeled positivistic, implying the aim to “reveal causal regularities that underlie reality” (Tsoukas & Knudsen, 2005, p. 41), and the explanations are the causes that determine the effects;

Page 39: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

24 A FOR EFFORT

the formal logic of an explanation is that an independent variable, X, causes the dependent variable, Y. Nevertheless, the term positivism is unclear and has come to signify many different views (Hunt, 1994a). In its original meaning, positivism refers to the views of, for example, Auguste Comte and Emile Durkheim. Another early version of positivism was logical positivism by the First Vienna Circle around the 1920s. However, the assumptions of these groups are not the ones underpinning this thesis. In order avoid adding to the confusion, the research of this dissertation was conducted in the deductive tradition and adhered to the empirical realism described by McMullin (1984), which claim that: “the long run success of a scientific theory gives reason to believe that something like the entities and structure postulated by the theory actually exists “ (p. 26). Importantly, positive science contains positive statements about the world and its causation. Positive science is also said to be value-free, in the sense that it explains what factors cause a phenomenon but does not make prescriptions (thereby values) to decide what should be done.

As most consumer behavior research draws heavily on psychology, the ontological and epistemological assumptions are to a large extent shared with the kind of psychology research which utilizes experiment based methods. The ontological assumption of this thesis is empirical realism, which has the natural science model as a role model, and the aim is to explain what occurs. A key focus is to find causal relationships between variables and to find effects and mechanisms which are generalizable. The primary method for establishing causal effects is through randomized controlled experiments, which are described more in detail below.

The mode of explanation of this thesis, and in the consumer behavior field, in general, is deductive nomological, which is adapted from the natural sciences and adopts the subject-object model of research sharing the epistemology of the natural sciences. The model includes the underlying ontological assumption that a reality exists independently of our knowledge of it, and that the social world is made up of general laws (Tsoukas & Knudsen, 2005). The epistemological assumption is that knowledge can be acquired through systematic observations. Hypotheses are formed before observation and subsequently tested. The main objective of the consumer

Page 40: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

CHAPTER 3 25

behavior field is to find the causal explanations for consumer behavior, and this practice is generally assumed to be value-free by the research community.

Research Design

The studies in this dissertation used a deductive method, where theory is used to develop hypotheses that are subsequently tested in an empirical setting. The deductive method is a common consumer behavior research approach. The design of the studies is experimental, which is the method which is well suited to handle causality (Söderlund, 2018), and thereby to answer the research question(s) or hypotheses. There are three requisites for causality: the temporal aspect that X should occur before Y, X and no other variable affect Y, and X must covary with Y (Söderlund, 2018). In addition to being able to identify causal effects between two variables, e.g., X causes Y, sometimes referred to as the main effect, experimental designs can also identify mediators and moderators. Identifying the processes which mediate variables illuminates how or why an effect occurs (Pieters, 2017) represents one way of contributing to theory. Mediators have been included in some of the studies of this thesis. For example, in article 3, message comprehension was identified as a mediator of the relationship between high- and low-effort and message evaluation.

Moderating variables can either be manipulated factors as a part of the experiment design acting as an independent variable or moderators can be measured (e.g., age, gender) and, have the capability of modifying the strength or form of the relationship between the independent and dependent variables (Söderlund, 2018). Moderators are therefore also discussed in terms of being boundary conditions of a theory. For example, in study 3 in article 1 a moderating variable was identified in the perceived relatedness of context and advertisement, which was moderating the effect of physical activity (measured as arousal) on advertising and brand attitude.

In psychology-based research, such as studies focused on consumer behavior, researchers assume the relationships between variables and their existence, strength, and boundary conditions can be verified and tested through experiments. Researchers conduct many studies in labs in order to tease out the noise. However, people do not live their lives in labs, and

Page 41: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

26 A FOR EFFORT

consequently, in understanding such a complex phenomenon as consumption, the amount of variance explained is usually quite modest. The term labs can mean different settings and do not necessarily represent the archetypical chemistry lab with a minimalistic interior and boiling test tubes. In psychology, an ordinary classroom can be referred to as a lab, in the sense that it is a controlled setting.

In a sizeable meta-meta-analysis of the subfields of marketing, Eisend (2015) found that the amount of variance explained in consumer behavior is estimated to be 8% (expressed as r2), which might not come across as impressive. Nonetheless, this value constitutes the second highest share of explanation among the subfields of marketing, after pricing but ahead of, distribution channels, sales, strategy, and advertising which all had lower amounts of variance explained. The levels should not be that surprising since most behaviors, attitudes, intentions and beliefs can have multiple causes (stimuli, environments, personality traits), and these can interact in so many ways that the number of effect interaction combinations becomes immense. The field of psychology and consumer behavior is about understanding human behavior, and it is clear that human behavior is complex at best or eternally chaotic at worst.

Sapolsky, a professor of biology and neurology at Stanford, wrote about how complicated the question of behavior (which should be understood here to include consumer responses), can be in terms of what level of explanation you seek. Consider any human behavior that we can observe and ask: why did this behaviour just happen? Sapolsky (2017) claims that through that question we are in fact asking several temporally separated questions. One question is: What did just occur in the brain a second ago? Another is: What sensory cues were around in the environment a minute ago, and what neurons fired? Another question is: what were the hormone levels this morning? And furthermore: In terms of neuroplasticity over the years, back to childhood, what experiences have sensitized the subject? And also, which culture are you raised in? and to take an extremely long-term perspective: what did the ancestors do? These questions are all interconnected and can all be a part of the answer to the question of why a particular behaviour just happened (Sapolsky, 2017). And consumer responses, seen as a subset of human behavior, can be argued to be just as complex to explain. Just looking

Page 42: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

CHAPTER 3 27

at the sheer number of things that influence behavior, it is not surprising to see that even a seemingly simple behavior is not so easy to explain. The point is that marketing and consumer behavior, and the findings in this thesis are just one level of analysis, and it is one piece of a giant puzzle of the variations of human behavior.

The psychologist Skinner, inspired by Pavlov, is mostly famous for the development of behaviorism and theories on operant conditioning. He argued that you should: “control your conditions, and you will see order” (Skinner, 1956, p. 223). By designing an experiment carefully, the researcher controls the environmental conditions, and ideally only changes the tested variable. While a lab environment has the advantage of keeping conditions constant, ruling out confounding factors, and the situation is characterized by more “order,” the drawbacks are an artificial situation different from the reality of consumers, which can lead to overestimating the effects. On the one hand, order, less noise, and the ruling out of confounding factors are desirable for providing clarity and certainty.

On the other hand, field studies can cause difficulties for the opposite reasons. Field studies can be noisy, include confounding factors, and the possibility to study underlying mechanisms can be minimal (e.g., the emotional states experienced by the subjects). A few of the studies in this thesis are field studies or a mix between field study and a controlled setting in the sense that the participants were gathered in a natural environment (for example, the central train station), but were then approached by researchers and asked to participate in a study and answer questions.

So, while researchers have conducted studies on consumers in controlled settings, the reality is that humans do not live in labs. Researchers often fail to replicate study findings from lab and field studies (Open Science Collaboration, 2015). In consumer psychology journals, a substantial focus has been on looking at the internal processes of the mind, preceding behavior (also discussed as mediators above). These processes are sometimes easier to study in controlled settings, such as a lab. This focus on internal processes has come to dominate publication in such a way that very few studies in the top social and personality psychology journals include actual behavior in their studies. This condition has been criticized (Baumeister et al., 2007). In this thesis, classical behavior has not been investigated as a dependent variable.

Page 43: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

28 A FOR EFFORT

However, in the field studies and the scenario-based experiment, behavior has been the independent variable of the experiments, for example, holding heavy items (Article 3), while the psychological “inner state” responses have been the dependent variables.

Another type of experiment technique is to use scenarios where the subjects imagine themselves in the situation described by the researcher. One drawback of using scenario-based treatment is, similarly to lab experiments, that they risk being artificial. The main advantage of using scenarios is that they are a way of holding conditions constant across participants (Söderlund, 2018). Researchers used scenarios in Article 4, where a context manipulating both the consumer’s effort and the producers’ effort was described.

An essential feature of the experiments conducted for this thesis is that the treatments have been randomly allocated to the participants in all of the studies except the first study in article 1. In that particular study the subjects self-selected either stairs or the escalator as means of transportation, which makes the study more of a quasi-experiment or observational study. Another potential problem when conducting experiments with humans is the risk of demand effects.

Demand effects are any effect that arises as a part of being in an experiment that is not intended by the researcher, but which alters the behavior and the results. Demand effects occur when participants form an idea about what the experiment is about and consciously or unconsciously alters his or her behavior in any direction. This alteration can mean trying to please the researcher by answering in accordance with their perceived aim of a study, or the opposite response, where participants try to sabotage the research. Also, the mere knowledge of being a part of a study can alter responses in ways such that the results are not reliable or replicable outside the lab.

There are different ways to mitigate the risks of demand effects; one way is not to reveal the true purpose or have a cover story about the real purpose of the experiment. Another way to handle demand effects is to minimize interaction between the experimenter and the subject, keeping to strict protocols, and using different researchers to control for experimenter effects. Nonetheless, demand effects pose a potentially serious problem and must always be considered in the design and execution of a research study. The

Page 44: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

CHAPTER 3 29

measures which have been taken in this thesis work to minimize demand effects include: using different researchers and interviewers in face-to-face studies, to hide the real purpose of the study, controlling for participant’s understanding of the study and excluding those who have correctly guessed or understood the purpose.

Another criticism of experiments includes the fact that that they only capture the very immediate effects directly after an experimental treatment. That is perhaps not a valid criticism of the methods per se, but it is nonetheless a limitation of the generalizability of the research findings.

Measurement

Contrary to Skinner, who had a narrow focus on behavior and disregarded the mental processes and cognitions preceding behavior, this thesis investigates the mental processes including emotions and cognitions in consumers, such as attitudes, beliefs, intentions, satisfaction, all of which are typical variables of interest in consumer behavior and marketing. Admittedly, to objectively observe and measure what happens in a consumer’s mind (i.e., the cognitions and emotions) is difficult.

The simplest method has been to ask people to self-report this data by describing their inner states. This method presents several problems, including self-serving bias, social desirability, demand effects, measurement errors, vagueness and ambiguity of concepts. New methods do not rely on subjective self-reporting. These more modern, and in some senses, more objective methods of measuring human cognition and emotions include fMRI, facial recognition, and eye-tracking. A facial recognition technology called Facereader analyzes emotions from human faces, and this was used to pre-testing the arousal levels and emotional valence of the stimuli used in article 2.

These “objective” methods all have separate merits, and they constitute an excellent complement with the potential to add reliability to studies. After the shift from radical behaviorism into the cognitive revolution and what has also been called peering into the black box, i.e., the human mind, introspection again became the standard (Buss, 2016). Introspection was the focus in the era of Freud and James before being replaced by the behaviorism

Page 45: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

30 A FOR EFFORT

paradigm of Watson, Pavlov, and Skinner (Buss, 2016). The criticism from Baumeister et al. (2007) is that actual meaningful behavior is lacking since behavior in current studies is typically represented by filling out questionnaires, measurement of finger movements, and response times, and consequently, the authors called for the inclusion of more meaningful behavior. The position of this thesis is that both behavior and cognition are important, and introspection into the black box, even if not measured perfectly, at least approximates human information processing, which is an undeniable part of understanding behavior.

In defense of the current universally accepted focus on cognitive processes, it seems that people can report on their inner states in a way that is highly correlated with these other more objective measurements. One example is the study where participants evaluated red wine both subjectively through self-reports and simultaneously through fMRI scanning. The results showed that the subjective and objective measurements were positively associated (Plassmann et al., 2008). In other words, people seem to be reasonably good at reporting emotions and thoughts. Given this, it is cheaper and less invasive to ask people what they think and what they feel instead of, for example, scanning and analyzing the blood flow in their brains.

Data Collection

Different sources provided the data of the studies in this dissertation. Data sources include classroom surveys on university students, face-to-face interviews at university and industry fairs, riders in the Stockholm metro system, the central train station, and at a gym. Finally, data was through Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk), which is an online panel frequently used in research.

Criticism has arisen in the wake of the frequent use of MTurk participants, as researchers found MTurk participants to differ in some respects from other populations (Goodman et al., 2013). There has been criticism of using student samples in research, with the argument that university students are not representative of other populations (Ferber, 1977). However, in defense of using MTurk and university students is the argument that if researchers are testing general theories about human

Page 46: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

CHAPTER 3 31

behaviors related to consumption, there is no reason why a student should differ with regards to basic human psychological reactions. Calder et al. (1982) claimed that the sample does not matter when the goal is to test a theory. A final note to sampling is that Fisher himself (i.e. the founder of statistical testing) was concerned with allocating treatments randomly, not the idea that participants should be randomly sampled (Söderlund, 2018).

Furthermore, online panel data quality concerns include the fact that participants might not process stimuli properly; they fail to read instructions and do not carefully read the questions and the response alternatives (Goodman et al., 2013). In response to these concerns, researchers have developed measures and tactics to counterbalance these behaviors. The tactics included writing explicit instructions combined with different types of attention checks in the beginning and throughout the survey. Also, programming techniques were used to ensure that stimuli, such as a video, could not be fast-forwarded, but had to play to the end before the participant could move forward in the survey. These measures, more thoroughly described in article 1, were found to improve the online panel participants’ response quality considerably.

Ethics

All participants in the studies gave written or oral consent and were informed before participating in the studies that the purpose was research in consumer behavior, marketing, or advertising conducted by researchers at the Stockholm School of Economics. In a few of the studies, when possible, interested respondents were later informed about the research findings of the study. The topics of the studies were non-controversial and constituted typical contemporary consumer situations. Also, in the cases where students were respondents, they were not offered course credits, historically a common practice in psychology experiments, as this condition put the students into a non-voluntary participation condition.

Replication Crisis and Methodological Malpractices

In recent years researchers raised different issues regarding the rigor and quality of research in social sciences. As mentioned above, many findings in

Page 47: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

32 A FOR EFFORT

social psychology have been hard to replicate. In a large-scale project which repeated 100 experiments from well-cited psychology journals, only 39% were successfully replicated (Open Science Collaboration 2015). These studies included findings from primary and embodiment literature. There are several ways to try to improve the replication quality. One way is to replicate the main effect in several different studies and also to try to replicate other, previously shown effects, and to disclose as clearly as possible how the studies were designed and conducted.

Further measures include reporting hypothesis before analyzing data and disclosing the total number of studies done, not just the successful studies. An inherent by-product of the existing publication paradigm is that non-significant results are systematically not published. This situation could mean that many researchers are doing the same studies, yielding non-significant results, and not publishing them, which on a macro level, leads to substantial inefficiencies, as researchers waste resources on attaining the same (non-significant) results. This point is something that this thesis tried to amend by including a chapter of related studies and some studies which did not yield the expected results (see chapter 4 on effort-related empirical studies).

Some criticized analysis malpractices include forking, (p-hacking) and data mining. Data mining refers to the analysis phase of a project when researchers analyze a dataset, basically looking for any significant differences or covariations between variables and then framing hypothesis post-testing, and adapt the hypothesis to fit the data (Gelman & Loken, 2013; Ioannidis 2005). Several scholars have criticized the current methodological paradigm for the improper use of statistical testing (Camerer et al., 2016; Gelman & Loken, 2013; Taleb 2007). Recently there have been voices advocating the abolishment of the practice of reporting p-values (Amrheim et al., 2019). In the studies of this thesis the hypotheses have been made before collecting the data thus avoiding data mining for results. Nonetheless, in many journals, p-values make up the standard reporting format of results, and this work has conformed to that practice.

Page 48: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

CHAPTER 3 33

Intended users of results

This thesis deals with consumer responses. Both consumers and marketers could use the findings. Consumers could for example be careful to make important evaluations of offerings when engaging in in certain effortful activities and be wary of how an aroused state can spill-over on judgement of different offerings and marketing activities. However, it is more likely that marketers, who are professionals and want to influence consumers, will use the findings. Also, it is uncertain how informed consumers would be able to mitigate the effects of depicted arousal (Article 2). This mitigation would require a considerable amount of conscious focus in their daily lives as the consumer is exposed to various communication. A parallel example would be the cognitive biases from Tversky and Kahneman’s research in Behavioral Economics (Kahneman, 2011), which are extremely hard to mitigate even when the subject or experts were made aware of the cognitive biases; simply put: “it’s difficult to fix errors we can’t see” (Kahneman et al., 2011, p.52).

Page 49: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm
Page 50: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

Chapter 4

Summaries of findings from five research projects relating to effort

In addition to the articles in this dissertation, a number of other projects relating to effort has been conducted by me and colleagues, ranging from a single experiment to conference papers, a research film and a published article. In this chapter, I will summarize five such projects, including the primary findings of each project and some reflections. These projects are valuable since they further illuminate consumer responses relating to effort and inform the overall conclusions in chapter 10.

Research project 1: My army training week

Szugalski S, Söderlund M, Sagfossen S & Colliander J, My army training week, research film presented at ACR, North American conference, October 1 -4, New Orleans. In this project, we explored a different medium to present research, namely by producing a film. The purpose of the study was to look at the phenomena of why consumers voluntarily choose to get out of bed at 5 am in the middle of the dark and cold winter in Sweden to expose themselves to physical exercise called ‘army training’. The workout takes place outside and labels

Page 51: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

36 A FOR EFFORT

itself with associations to the military and the army. It is supposed to be very hard, and thereby highly efficient training.

The project consisted of four researchers, where one researcher participated in the training during one week. While not participating in the training, the others were interviewing army training participants, the coaches, and officers responsible for the real physical training programs of the Swedish army. The video material from the training and the concept were showed to two high-ranked commanders in charge of physical development in the Swedish military. In the interview, they gave a contrasting perspective as they established that the training did not resemble military training as it is conducted nowadays. Only one aspect shared some similarity to the military, and that was the formation of ranks. This was accomplished through division of the participants into skill levels by giving them different colored vests.

The researchers’ conclusions from the analysis were that the label ‘military’ or ‘army’ was used as a co-brand to add a sense of efficacy to the training, as well as justify the training occurring outside and the rough exercises involving body contact. Some of the participants also said it added flavor and “edge” to their otherwise mundane life as an office worker. This co-brand was used in spite of the fact that, apart from the ranks, there was nothing specifically military about the training. The military staff emphasized that there is nowadays more focus on individual training and capabilities than group exercising. Similarly, to the participants in Vasaloppet, physical exercise is not only a means to being healthy but also a status signaling form of consumption. The clearly visible ranks of the participants also offered a clear path of increasing competence and in-group status. Currently, militarization in the training industry is the trend five years later, with many actors using words such as “bootcamp” in their marketing, and the same type of tough training exists in a number of different varieties. Both companies that were used when making the film are still operating today.

Page 52: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

CHAPTER 4 37

Research project 2: Consumers’ intrinsic and extrinsic motivation when consuming high-effort endurance activities and its consequences on future intentions to exert effort again

Sagfossen S, 2015, “Understanding consumption of active experiences using Self-determination theory”, paper presented at the 23rd Nordic Academy of Management Conference, Copenhagen Business School, 12-14 August. The first project on the theme of effort was conducted on the popular annual cross-country skiing endurance race in Sweden called Vasaloppet, which has more than 50,000 participants. The project had two main purposes: the first was to establish the nature of the participants’ motivations for participating (i.e. exerting a lot of effort), and the second purpose was to investigate if different types of motivation could explain future intentions with regards to training harder, participating in the same race or other races, sharing the experience on social media, and purchasing ski equipment. The consumer effort in this instance is relatively substantive, not only in terms of the physical effort on the actual race day, but also in terms of additional activities including training, planning, and travelling. The major drawback of this study was the fact that it is based on questionnaire data and not experimental data. The data consisted of 502 participants, who answered a survey by e-mail within a week after the race was completed.

The main findings were that participants were both intrinsically and extrinsically motivated. In other words, effort was invested not solely for the joy of the activity itself, but for the consequences, in this case, the status earned by performing and finishing in such an effortful activity, such as a long-distance endurance race. One way in which this manifested itself was by the fact that the vast majority, 92%, of the respondents had shared their participation through social media. In the Self-Determination literature, the two different kinds of motivation seem to be mutually exclusive, but intrinsic

Page 53: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

38 A FOR EFFORT

motivation would explain the desire to do it again, while extrinsic motivation would not drive propensity to do it again.

Furthermore, a regression analysis showed a positive association between intrinsic motivation and the intention to participate in the same race again, to exercise more and to spend more money on ski-related equipment in the near future. On the other hand, the level of extrinsic motivation was only found to positively explain future intentions to eat unhealthier. These outcomes suggest that for managers of events that require high physical efforts as inputs from their participants, it is important for the consumers to be able to signal their efforts to others. Perhaps more importantly, it is the intrinsically motivated participants who will more likely become repeat customers, while the extrinsically motivated participants have lower intentions to participate again. This means that in order to attain more repeat customers, the organizers of high-effort experiences can try to strengthen the participants’ intrinsic motivations, and also try to target intrinsically motivated participants.

In relation to the whole theme of the thesis, this project is an example of a setting in which consumers’ efforts act as a potent way of signaling unobservable qualities in a credible way. It also implies that when designing extraordinary experiences that require large investments in effort, it is important to make it easy for the consumers to display this status in different ways.

Page 54: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

CHAPTER 4 39

Research project 3: Can consumer beliefs be strengthened when consumers are engaged in effortful shopping activity while receiving communication?

Project with Magnus Söderlund and Carl-Philip Ahlbom. Sagfossen S, 2019, “Convincing the general public about the benefits for a state monopoly and restricted accessibility of alcohol – understanding one way of affecting consumers’ beliefs by communication of public policy while they are engaged in effortful activity”, paper presented at the AMA Public Policy Conference, Washington DC, 6-9 June 2019. In this project, we wanted to investigate if consumer beliefs about factual claims in commercial messages was affected by the consumer’s own effort while receiving the message. The project was the next step based on the findings from article 3 and 4, where the consumer’s own effort was found to impact evaluations of an ad and an experiential offering respectively.

This research is intended to contribute to understanding more about the contextual variables in a physical setting, which affect how consumers respond to communication. More specifically, the two variables in focus of this project was the physical effort of consumers and their subsequent beliefs related to the content of a commercial message. This study also contributes to understanding more about how consumer beliefs can be affected in general, an important but sometimes overlooked variable. A lot of advertising and brand literature focuses of how evaluations are shaped, such as perceived value, brand attitude, or ad attitude, while considerably fewer studies focus on understanding consumers’ beliefs. Consumer beliefs are important to understand, both in themselves, and for understanding formation of attitudes and consecutive behavior (Ajzen, 1991). Beliefs are to some extent precursors to decisions and evaluations; therefore, how beliefs are validated or inferred, strengthened, or weakened will be important for understanding consumer decision making. Therefore, the main purpose of this study was to understand if consumers’ beliefs in factual statements can be affected by communication, and if a contextual variable—in this case, consumers’ own efforts—can play a role. Earlier research has shown how physical effort can be a causally potent antecedent variable in information

Page 55: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

40 A FOR EFFORT

processing (Davranche et al., 2005; Dietrich & Sparling, 2004; PAAS & Adam, 1991).

Theoretical Framework and Hypothesis

Despite the fact that a lot of shopping nowadays takes place online and offers home delivery, a substantial amount of shopping is still conducted in brick and mortar stores, and not online (Turow, 2017). A main theme here is that our bodily activities will have implications for how information is processed. Previous research has shown that thoughts, emotions, evaluations, attitudes and behaviors are affected by bodily states (Meier et al., 2012). Other research has examined how people receive messages in more passive bodily states, like watching TV or browsing the internet, or looking at printed ads.

Given that people nowadays are targeted by an ever-increasing number of messages and spend more and more time online with portable devices and online shopping, our attention remains limited. It is important to understand how the physical aspects of a shopper experience can play a role. Perhaps it is physically in the store, or actual customers meeting where organizations have a comparably favorable opportunity to reach consumers and gain their attention. It is probable that a lot of communication is in fact not efficient; in fact, it is probable that the communication is not even understood properly. Jacoby and Hoyer showed in a large scale study of print communication that on average 21,4% of the material was miscomprehended, in addition to the 15,5% of the respondents who stated they did not notice the material (1989).

Based on previous studies of message comprehension, people find it easier to believe in statements than doubting them (Gilbert, 1991). When in a state of high physical effort, the ability to critically evaluate propositions diminishes, leading to a stronger belief in the information. The fact that consumers are still largely shopping in physical stores and exerting effort as a part of doing so, and the antecedents of consumer beliefs are worth investigating further. This paper asks the question: can consumer beliefs be strengthened when consumers are engaged in effortful shopping activity while receiving communication?

Page 56: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

CHAPTER 4 41

This study investigates if higher levels of exerted effort, which were manipulated and operationalized by carrying heavy products, impact the degree of agreeing with the propositions communicated in the message. The first two statements were asserting the information which was given in the message, while the others are more interpretive inferences which did not exactly have a corresponding part in the message, but would be reasonable inferential beliefs that could be logically deduced from the content of the message. The two first statements can, in terms of Alba & Hutchinson’s framework of consumer expertise, be classified as mostly descriptive inferences, and the remaining statements could be classified as interpretive inferences (1987).

Given that physical effort can cause temporary inhibition of the brain functions which are not essential for undertaking effort, it can be expected that higher-order functions are impaired under conditions of high physical effort. That is, sustained activation of the motor and sensory systems involved in high physical effort can reduce the capacity to engage in higher order cognitive processing (Dietrich & Sparling, 2004). This, in turn, could imply two different outcomes in the case of how beliefs are affected. One potential way is that message comprehension is reduced, and the beliefs will not be affected, since the information is not processed due to the high effort. The alternative is that the message will be received but not critically scrutinized due to the impairment of higher order cognitive brain functions while exerting high effort, which would lead to a strengthening of the beliefs. Therefore, the hypothesis is:

H1: Consumers exerting high levels of effort while receiving a message with factual statements will believe more strongly in the descriptions stated in the message.

Method

An experiment with two conditions was conducted where the participants (N=86, 56% female, age 20-76) attended a fair. The participants were interviewed individually and were asked to pick up and hold two shopping bags with the wine bottles from the retailer. In order to manipulate different

Page 57: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

42 A FOR EFFORT

levels of physical effort, two pairs of identical-looking bags were used: relatively light bags (1.5 kg per bag, 3 kg per pair) were used in order to induce a relatively low level of physical effort, while relatively heavy bags (9 kg per bag, 18 kg per pair) were used to induce a high level of physical effort. The participants were asked to hold the bags until all questions had been asked. In both conditions, and while the participant was still holding the bags, a researcher read an authentic piece of information (103 words) from Systembolaget explaining how they take social responsibility while selling alcohol. After having listened to the message, the researcher asked questions from a questionnaire and recorded the responses.

Measures

The questionnaire had 7 filler questions at the beginning about their last visit to a store in order to make sure the level of effort would be felt for all participants. The level of effort and duration of effort was pre-tested in a separate pre-study. Six beliefs were measured, where two were the dependent variables which were directly related to the message: “Systembolaget takes responsibility so people don’t drink too much” and “Systembolaget wants to limit the overall consumption of alcohol.” The scale was 1-10 (Do not agree at all – agree completely). The remaining beliefs were related to overall strategy but were not directly addressed in the message, e.g. Systembolaget stands for thoughtfulness, makes customers feel welcome, and wants people to enjoy alcohol. Subjective message comprehension, attitude towards the message and mood were measured and controlled for.

Analysis & Results

The manipulation of effort was successful. The participants perceived the high effort condition as more effortful (M=6.34 SD= 2.25) than the low effort condition (M=3.68 SD 2.31), t(84)= -5.4, p=0.001. An independent sample t-test showed that the descriptive beliefs directly related to the message were more strongly supported by the participants in the high effort condition (M=7.65 SD=2.19), compared to the low effort condition (M=6.67 SD=2.57 ): t(84)= -1.9, p=0.03. These results suggest that the level of consumers’ effort does have an effect on how strongly their beliefs are

Page 58: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

CHAPTER 4 43

affected by communication. Specifically, the higher the effort consumers are involved in, the more strongly they believe the statements from the message. H1 was thus supported.

Discussion

In order to explain consumer beliefs, it is essential to understand what situations and what contextual variables can enhance or hinder communication’s effectiveness. This can be said to be more relevant than ever, in an era where false information, so-called fake news, and rumors can go viral and strongly influence consumers’ beliefs. The findings of this study suggest that important beliefs should be addressed directly in communication. It also indicated that communication can become extra potent in strengthening beliefs when consumers are engaged in physical effort, as this tends to enhance the strength of a consumer’s beliefs of the message further. It was also found that the manipulation did not affect consumers’ inferential beliefs, suggesting a limit for the effect of physical effort on beliefs to only apply for the descriptive and explicitly stated statements, but not on inferential beliefs.

Limitations and further research

In this study, effort was manipulated by one specific activity (holding heavy bags), but several other activities can produce physical effort. Further research could uncover if different ways of experiencing effort in a shopping setting will have the same effect on belief formation as in this study. The stimuli used here was only one, and the type was listening to a brief text. Further research is needed on other formats of communication. Future studies could also look at the role of previous consumer knowledge, as the effect might be different when it concerns an area where there are no strong beliefs or schemas already in place.

Page 59: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

44 A FOR EFFORT

Project 4: The (failed)gym experiment

Unpublished results from an experiment by Jonas Colliander, Sofie Sagfossen and Stefan Szugalski 2015 In an attempt to isolate and control the level of physical effort, we conducted a study in a gym. We investigated information processing in terms of attitude formation and behavioral intentions towards a range of different items, including service and brand evaluations of the gym and other unrelated products. The experiment had two conditions: high effort (which was about 70 percent of maximum heart rate) and low effort (below 30 percent of maximum heart rate). 52 students were recruited and scheduled for a short session to be riding a cardio bicycle at a local gym. They were given 200 SEK in compensation for their participation.

After a short session on the bikes using pulse meters to ensure physical effort level, a questionnaire was handed out and completed directly after the exercise. The results were analyzed with a t-test and showed no significant differences between the groups for any of our attitudes and behavioral intentions.

At the time, we did not know exactly why the experiment “failed” in the sense that no significant differences in our dependent variables were found between the high and low effort group. However, the results of another experiment in a different project, namely study 3 in article 1, which was conducted on people exiting the gym, illustrated that the setting of a gym designed for exerting physical effort, and the manipulation of pulse levels, made salient the source of arousal in an obtrusive way. This in turn was making it impossible to misattribute the level of arousal in a way that would influence the evaluations and behavioral intentions.

Page 60: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

CHAPTER 4 45

Project 5: Perceived supplier effort in a packaging context

Söderlund, M., Colliander, J., Karsberg, J., Liljedal, K. T., Modig, E., Rosengren, S., Sagfossen, S., Szugalski, S. & Åkestam, N. (2017). The allure of the bottle as a package: an assessment of perceived effort in a packaging context. Journal of Product & Brand Management, 26(1), 91-100. This article investigated whether perceived effort behind the packaging of a soft drink can affect product evaluations. The purpose of the article was to investigate if the perceived supplier effort behind the packaging of a soft drink affected product evaluations. The perceived effort was defined as a consumer’s perception of the amount of manufacturer effort entailed in the product. The perceived supplier effort was suggested to affect evaluations positively by signaling unobservable product qualities.

This project was a joint effort of many researchers owing to the time- and personnel-consuming data collection. The interview process included providing taste samples and interviewing each participant face-to-face. Three between-subject experiments were conducted, where perceived effort was manipulated through a high-versus-low effort condition. More specifically, two different bottles were used containing the same soft drink. In three experiments, the packaging of a soft drink was manipulated in terms of different amounts of perceived effort from the manufacturer. Perceived supplier effort was found to be associated with higher overall evaluations of the product, and this effect was mediated by perceptions of quality. The findings are consonant with the research on effort’s capacity to signal and to increase perceived value due to improved quality perceptions, as discussed in chapter 2.

Page 61: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm
Page 62: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

Chapter 5

Research articles

This chapter contains the abstracts of the four research articles.

Article 1. Advertising “On the Go”: Are Consumers In Motion More Influenced by Ads?: Why Advertisers Should Consider Consumers' Physical Activity when Planning Ad Campaigns

Authors Micael Dahlen, Stockholm School of Economics John Karsberg, Stockholm School of Economics Sofie Sagfossen, Stockholm School of Economics Helge Thorbjørnsen, Norwegian School of Economics Fredrik Lange, Stockholm School of Economics Status Published in the Journal of Advertising Research, 59(4); 2019

Summary

As people are increasingly moving when exposed to advertising, this paper asks the question “are moving consumers more influenced by advertising?”

Page 63: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

48 A FOR EFFORT

Building on grounded cognition (the body influences the brain) this paper shows that effort in the form of a physical activity such as walking up stairs or cardio exercise in a gym, has positive effects on the attitudes and intentions towards an ad and the brand, and that these effects are caused by an increased state of physical arousal. This effect is shown to be attenuated when the ad is less related to the arousing context, since the person is then not reminded of the source of the arousal. The implications for researchers and advertisers are discussed.

Article 2. The Impact of Stimuli Person’s Arousal depicted in Video Advertising Clips

Author Sofie Sagfossen, Stockholm School of Economics Status Submitted for potential publication in Advances in Consumer Research 2020 proceedings, and presented at Association for Consumer Research (ACR) conference 2020

Summary

This article investigates human arousal depicted in short advertising video clips. Two studies show a main effect of stimulus persons’ arousal on product evaluation. Moreover, depicting arousal in videos affect the viewers through two different processes: firstly through affecting the respondents’ own experienced arousal, and secondly through signaling supplier effort.

Page 64: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

CHAPTER 5 49

Article 3. The Embodied Retail Consumer: Physical Effort in Shopping-Related Tasks and its Impact on Reactions to Messages

Authors Sofie Sagfossen, Stockholm School of Economics Magnus Söderlund, Stockholm School of Economics Carl-Philip Ahlbom, Stockholm School of Economics, University of Bath Status Published in Advances in Advertising Research IX (pp. 303-316), 2018

Summary

This study examines if shoppers’ bodily activities have implication for their information processing. The results from an experimental study showed that the consumers which were exposed to a commercial message (a print ad), while doing a shopping related activity involving high physical effort evaluated the ad less positively compared to consumers in the low effort condition. The results also showed that the shopping-induced physical effort compromised subjective message comprehension, which in turn affected the evaluation of the ad negatively.

Page 65: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

50 A FOR EFFORT

Article 4. The consumer experience: The impact of supplier effort and consumer effort on customer satisfaction

Authors Magnus Söderlund, Stockholm School of Economics Sofie Sagfossen, Stockholm School of Economics Status Published in Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 39, (219-229), 2017

Summary

Existing research on experiential offers often examines the impact of such offers on consumers’ evaluations (e.g. customer satisfaction). Yet existing research has neglected that experiential offers typically involve effort from both the supplier and the consumer – and neglected that effort can influence evaluations. To address this gap, the present study examines the impact of supplier effort and the consumer's own effort on the consumer's evaluation of experiential offers in terms of customer satisfaction. Two experiments, comprising two different experiential offers, were carried out. In both experiments, supplier effort (low vs. high) and consumer effort (low vs. high) were manipulated. Customer satisfaction was the dependent variable. The results show that high supplier effort boosts customer satisfaction, and that the effects of consumer effort are either absent or indirect with a negative impact. Moreover, the results indicate that a supplier effort-consumer effort gap (i.e., the consumer perceives that the supplier has expended more effort than the consumer) contributes positively to customer satisfaction.

Page 66: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

References

References for chapter 1-4 and chapter 10

Amrhein, V., Greenland, S., & McShane, B. (2019). Scientists rise up against statistical significance. Nature, 567(7748), 305-307.

Anderson, K. (1990). Arousal and the inverted-U hypothesis: A critique of Neiss's "Reconceptualizing arousal.". Psychological Bulletin, 107(1), 96-100.

Applebaum, A. (2007). Gulag: A History. Anchor. Arent, S. M., & Landers, D. M. (2003). Arousal, anxiety, and performance: A

reexamination of the inverted-U hypothesis. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 74(4), 436-444.

Ariely, D., Kamenica, E., & Prelec, D. (2008). Man’s search for meaning: The case of Legos. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 67(3-4), 671-677.

Arkes, H. R., & Blumer, C. (1985). The psychology of sunk cost. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 35(1), 124-140.

Aronson, E., & Mills, J. (1959). The effect of severity of initiation on liking for a group. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 59(2), 177.

Barrett, L., Dunbar, R., & Lycett, J. (2002). Human evolutionary psychology. Princeton University Press.

Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617-645.

Bem, D. J. (1972). Self-perception theory. Advances in experimental social psychology, 6(1), 1-62. New York: Academic Press.

Botvinick, M. M., Huffstetler, S., & McGuire, J. T. (2009). Effort discounting in human nucleus accumbens. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 9(1), 16-27.

Brown, R. & Herrnstein, R. J. (1975). Psychology. Little, Brown.

Page 67: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

108 A FOR EFFORT

Browne, S. (2019). The effect of strenuous exercise on cognitive function, mood, energy, and fatigue states in trained sporting individuals (Doctoral dissertation, Northumbria University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom). Retrieved from http://nrl.northumbria.ac.uk/39714/

Buzzell, R. D. (1963). Is marketing a science?. Harvard Business Review, 41(1), 32-40.

Calder, B. J., Phillips, L. W., & Tybout, A. M. (1982). The concept of external validity. Journal of Consumer Research, 9(3), 240-244.

Cardozo, R. N. (1965). An experimental study of customer effort, expectation, and satisfaction. Journal of Marketing Research, 2(3), 244-249.

Celsi, R. L., Rose, R. L., & Leigh, T. W. (1993). An exploration of high-risk leisure consumption through skydiving. Journal of Consumer Research, 20(1), 1-23.

Chong, T. T. J., Apps, M., Giehl, K., Sillence, A., Grima, L. L., & Husain, M. (2017). Neurocomputational mechanisms underlying subjective valuation of effort costs. PLoS Biology, 15(2), e1002598.

Clore, G. L., & Storbeck, J. (2006). Affect as information about liking, efficacy, and importance. New York: Psychology Press.

Cohen R.A. (2011) Yerkes–Dodson Law. In J. S. Kreutzer, J. DeLuca J., & B. Caplan (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology. Springer.

Dahlen, M., Rosengren, S., & Karsberg, J. (2018). The effects of signaling monetary and creative effort in ads: Advertising effort can go a long way influencing B2B clients, employees, and investors. Journal of Advertising Research, 58(4), 433-442.

Dahlen, M., Rosengren, S., & Törn, F. (2008). Advertising creativity matters. Journal of Advertising Research, 48(3), 392-403.

Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, rationality, and the human brain. New York: Putnam.

Davranche, K., & Audiffren, M. (2004). Facilitating effects of exercise on information processing. Journal of Sports Sciences, 22(5), 419-428.

De Manzano, Ö., Theorell, T., Harmat, L., & Ullén, F. (2010). The psychophysiology of flow during piano playing. Emotion, 10(3), 301.

Di Muro, F., & Murray, K. B. (2012). An arousal regulation explanation of mood effects on consumer choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 39(3), 574-584.

Page 68: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

REFERENCES 109

Dietrich, A., & Sparling, P. B. (2004). Endurance exercise selectively impairs prefrontal-dependent cognition. Brain and Cognition, 55(3), 516-524.

Duff, B. R. L., & Sar, S. (2015). Seeing the big picture: Multitasking and perceptual processing influences on ad recognition. Journal of Advertising, 44(3), 173-184.

Eisenberger, R. (1992). Learned industriousness. Psychological Review, 99(2), 248.

Eisend, M. (2015). Have we progressed marketing knowledge? A meta-meta-analysis of effect sizes in marketing research. Journal of Marketing, 79(3), 23-40.

Ferber, R. (1977). Research by Convenience. Journal of Consumer Research, 4(1), 57-58.

Festinger, L. (1962). A theory of cognitive dissonance. London: Tavistock. Galentino, A. (2015). The arousing risk: Influences of positive and negative arousal on

preferences for economic risk (Doctoral dissertation, University of Trento, Trento, Italy). Retrieved from http://eprints-phd.biblio.unitn.it/1607/

Galentino, A., Bonini, N., & Savadori, L. (2017). Positive arousal increases individuals’ preferences for risk. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2142.

Gelman, A., & Loken, E. (2013). The garden of forking paths: Why multiple comparisons can be a problem, even when there is no “fishing expedition” or “p-hacking,” and the research hypothesis was posited ahead of time. Unpublished Manuscript. Department of Statistics, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA.

Gilbert, D. T. (1991). How mental systems believe. American psychologist, 46(2), 107.

Goodman, J. K., Cryder, C. E., & Cheema, A. (2013). Data collection in a flat world: The strengths and weaknesses of Mechanical Turk samples. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 26(3), 213-224.

Gorn, G., Tuan Pham, M., & Yatming Sin, L. (2001). When arousal influences ad evaluation and valence does not (and vice versa). Journal of Consumer Psychology, 11(1), 43-55.

Hsee, C. K., Yang, A. X., & Wang, L. (2010). Idleness aversion and the need for justifiable busyness. Psychological Science, 21(7), 926-930.

Hull, C. L . (1943). Principles of Behavior. Appleton-Century.

Page 69: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

110 A FOR EFFORT

Hunt, S. D. (1976). The nature and scope of marketing. Journal of Marketing, 40(3), 17-28.

Hunt, S.D. (1994a). A realist theory of empirical testing resolving the theory-ladenness/objectivity debate. Philosophy of the Social Sciences, 24(2), 133-158.

Hunt, S. D. (1994b). On rethinking marketing: our discipline, our practice, our methods. European Journal of Marketing, 28(3), 13-25.

Ide, K., & Secher, N. H. (2000). Cerebral blood flow and metabolism during exercise. Progress in Neurobiology, 61(4), 397-414.

Inzlicht, M., Shenhav, A., & Olivola, C. Y. (2018). The effort paradox: Effort is both costly and valued. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(4), 337-349.

Ioannidis, J. P. (2005). Why most published research findings are false. PLoS Medicine, 2(8), e124.

Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. Macmillan. Kahneman, D., Lovallo, D., & Sibony, O. (2011). Before you make that big

decision. Harvard Business Review, 89(6), 50-60. Karsberg, J. (2016) “Reception, reception, reception: the effects of receiver

context on advertising effectiveness,” PhD Dissertation, Stockholm School of Economics

Kirmani, A. (1997). Advertising repetition as a signal of quality: If it’s advertised so much, something must be wrong. Journal of Advertising, 26(3), 77-86.

Kirmani, A., & Rao, A. R. (2000). No pain, no gain: A critical review of the literature on signaling unobservable product quality. Journal of Marketing, 64(2), 66-79.

Kirmani, A., & Wright, P. (1989). Money talks: Perceived advertising expense and expected product quality. Journal of Consumer Research, 16(3), 344-353.

Krishna, A., & Schwarz, N. (2014). Sensory marketing, embodiment, and grounded cognition: A review and introduction. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 24(2), 159-168.

Kruger, J., Wirtz, D., Van Boven, L., & Altermatt, T. W. (2004). The effort heuristic. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40(1), 91-98.

Kurzban, R. (2016). The sense of effort. Current Opinion in Psychology, 7, 67-70.

Page 70: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

REFERENCES 111

Levitt, S., & Gutin, B. (1971). Multiple choice reaction time and movement time during physical exertion. Research Quarterly. American Association for Health, Physical Education and Recreation, 42(4), 405-410.

Lewis, M. (1965). Psychological effect of effort. Psychological Bulletin, 64(3), 183.

Loewenstein, G. (1999). Because it is there: The challenge of mountaineering … for utility theory. Kyklos, 52(3), 315-343.

McMullin, E. (1984). A case for scientific realism. In J. Leplin, (Ed), Scientific realism: 8-40. Berkeley: University of California Press.

Marx, K. (1887). Capital: A critique of political economy. Vol. 1, The process of production of capital. Progress Publishers

McGrath, J. M., & Mahood, C. (2004). The impact of arousing programming and product involvement on advertising effectiveness. Journal of Current Issues & Research in Advertising, 26(2), 41-52.

McGuire, J. T., & Botvinick, M. M. (2010). Prefrontal cortex, cognitive control, and the registration of decision costs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(17), 7922-7926.

Meier, B. P., Schnall, S., Schwarz, N., & Bargh, J. A. (2012). Embodiment in social psychology. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(4), 705-716.

Mochon, D., Norton, M. I., & Ariely, D. (2012). Bolstering and restoring feelings of competence via the IKEA effect. International Journal Of Research in Marketing, 29(4), 363-369.

Mohr, L. A., & Bitner, M. J. (1995). The role of employee effort in satisfaction with service transactions. Journal of Business Research, 32(3), 239-252.

Nakamura, J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2014). The Concept of Flow. In M. Csikszentmihalyi, Flow and the Foundations of Positive Psychology (pp. 89-105). Dordrecht: Springer.

Norton, M. I., Mochon, D., & Ariely, D. (2012). The IKEA effect: When labor leads to love. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22(3), 453-460.

Open Science Collaboration. (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science, 349(6251), aac4716.

Pieters, R. (2017). Meaningful Mediation Analysis: Plausible Causal Inference and Informative Communication. Journal of Consumer Research, 44(3), 692-716.

Page 71: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

112 A FOR EFFORT

Plassmann, H., O’Doherty, J., Shiv, B., & Rangel, A. (2008). Marketing actions can modulate neural representations of experienced pleasantness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(3), 1050-1054.

Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(2), 110-122.

Reisenzein, R. (1983). The Schachter theory of emotion: Two decades later. Psychological Bulletin, 94(2), 239.

Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(6), 1161.

Russell, J. A., & Barrett, L. F. (1999). Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: Dissecting the elephant. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(5), 805.

Sage, G. H. (1984). Motor learning and control: A neuropsychological approach. Brown.

Sanbonmatsu, D. M., & Kardes, F. R. (1988). The effects of physiological arousal on information processing and persuasion. Journal of Consumer Research, 15(3), 379-385.

Sapolsky, R. M. (2017). Behave: The biology of humans at our best and worst. Penguin.

Schachter, S. (1964). The interaction of cognitive and physiological determinants of emotional state. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 49-80). New York: Academic Press.

Schachter, S., & Singer, J. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69(5), 379.

Schwarz, N. (2011). Feelings-as-information theory. In Handbook of Theories of Social Psychology, 1, 289-308. London: Sage.

Shapiro, S., & MacInnis, D. J. (2002). Understanding program-induced mood effects: Decoupling arousal from valence. Journal of Advertising, 31(4), 15-26.

Singh, S. N., & Hitchon, J. C. (1989). The intensifying effects of exciting television programs on the reception of subsequent commercials. Psychology & Marketing, 6(1), 1-31.

Skinner, B. F. (1933). The measurement of “spontaneous activity.” The Journal of General Psychology, 9(1), 3-23.

Page 72: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

REFERENCES 113

Skinner, B. F. (1956). A case history in scientific method. American Psychologist, 11(5), 221-233.

Söderlund, M. (2018). Experiments in marketing. Studentlitteratur. Söderlund, M., & Sagfossen, S. (2017). The consumer experience: The impact

of supplier effort and consumer effort on customer satisfaction. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 39, 219-229.

Söderlund, M., Colliander, J., Karsberg, J., Liljedal, K. T., Modig, E., Rosengren, S., Sagfossen, S., Szugalski, S. & Åkestam, N. (2017). The allure of the bottle as a package: an assessment of perceived effort in a packaging context. Journal of Product & Brand Management, 26(1), 91-100.

Spielberger, C. D. (1975). The measurement of state and trait anxiety: conceptual and methodological issues. In L. Levi (Ed). Emotions-their parameters and measurement ((pp. 713–725). New York: Raven Press.

Storbeck, J., & Clore, G. L. (2008). Affective arousal as information: How affective arousal influences judgments, learning, and memory. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(5), 1824-1843.

Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(5), 768.

Sutherland, R. (2003, June 3). Rory Sutherland Knows How to Save Marketing. WIRED. https://www.wired.co.uk/article/rory-sutherland-knows-how-to-save-marketing

Taleb, N. N. (2007). Black swans and the domains of statistics. The American Statistician, 61(3), 198-200.

Tsoukas, H., & Knudsen, C. (Eds.). (2005). The Oxford Handbook of Organization Theory. Oxford University Press.

Tversky, B. (2019). Mind in motion: How action shapes thought. Hachette UK. Van den Bergh, B., Dewitte, S., & Warlop, L. (2008). Bikinis instigate

generalized impatience in intertemporal choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 35(1), 85-97.

Walton, M. E., Kennerley, S. W., Bannerman, D. M., Phillips, P. E. M., & Rushworth, M. F. (2006). Weighing up the benefits of work: behavioral and neural analyses of effort-related decision making. Neural networks, 19(8), 1302-1314.

Weber, M. (1920). The Protestant ethic and the spirit of capitalism. Scribner.

Page 73: A for Effort: Consumer Responses to Own and Others’ Effort€¦ · sofie sagfossen a for effort isbn 978-91-7731-167-6 doctoral dissertation in business administration stockholm

114 A FOR EFFORT

Wells, G. L., & Petty, R. E. (1980). The effects of overhead movements on persuasion: Compatibility and incompatibility of responses. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 1(3), 219-230.

White, G. L., Fishbein, S., & Rutsein, J. (1981). Passionate love and the misattribution of arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41(1), 56.

Williams, L. E., & Bargh, J. A. (2008). Experiencing physical warmth promotes interpersonal warmth. Science, 322(5901), 606-607.

Yerkes, R. M., & Dodson, J. D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit‐formation. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology, 18(5), 459-482.

Zahavi, A. (1977a). “The cost of honesty (Further remarks on the handicap principle).” Journal of Theoretical Biology. 67(3). 603–605.

Zahavi, A. (1977b). Reliability in communication systems and the evolution of altruism. In B. Stonehouse & C.M. Perrins (Eds.). Evolutionary Ecology. pp. 253–259. Macmillan.

Zahavi, A. (1997). The Handicap Principle. Oxford University Press. Zillmann, D. (1971). Excitation transfer in communication-mediated

aggressive behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 7(4), 419-434.