4.0 part ii: effect of social-cognitive...

52
100 4.0 PART II: EFFECT OF SOCIAL-COGNITIVE INTERVENTION ON HOSTILE ATTRIBUTION OF INTENT, AGGRESSION, IRRITABILITY, IMPULSIVITY, EMOTIONAL SUSCEPTIBILITY AND NARCISSISM Individuals differ in their way of processing information in social situations. The manner, in which individuals evaluate social situation, is a strong predictor of ones’ reactions to that situation. In this vein, Crick and Dodge’s (1994) social information processing model is one of the most generative models in understanding individual differences in such reactivity process. This model maintains that appropriate reaction to a social situation depends on the accuracy of social information processing. For appropriate response in any situation, social information has to be encoded and interpreted correctly. Inaccurate encoding and interpretation may lead to aggressive behaviour. Social information processing biases are well established correlates of aggression. Aggressive individuals tend to have distorted interpretations of the relevant social interactions especially in negative ambiguous situations. They believe that such aversive situations are caused intentionally by others. This assumption of aggressive individuals leads them to further behave aggressively. Besides, it instigates more problematic interactions and thereby limits pro-social behaviour. Thus, hostile attribution of intent is considered a key element in the development and maintenance of aggressive behaviour. Therefore, aggressive individuals should be trained to interpret negative ambiguous situations without any biases. They can be trained to replace attribution of hostile intent with benign intent. By reducing hostile attribution of intent, behavioural problems of aggressive individuals can be prevented and interpersonal relationships and adjustment can be enhanced.

Upload: dolien

Post on 26-Jun-2018

215 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

100

4.0 PART II: EFFECT OF SOCIAL-COGNITIVE INTERVENTION ON

HOSTILE ATTRIBUTION OF INTENT, AGGRESSION,

IRRITABILITY, IMPULSIVITY, EMOTIONAL SUSCEPTIBILITY

AND NARCISSISM

Individuals differ in their way of processing information in social situations. The

manner, in which individuals evaluate social situation, is a strong predictor of ones’ reactions to

that situation. In this vein, Crick and Dodge’s (1994) social information processing model is

one of the most generative models in understanding individual differences in such reactivity

process. This model maintains that appropriate reaction to a social situation depends on the

accuracy of social information processing. For appropriate response in any situation, social

information has to be encoded and interpreted correctly. Inaccurate encoding and interpretation

may lead to aggressive behaviour. Social information processing biases are well established

correlates of aggression. Aggressive individuals tend to have distorted interpretations of the

relevant social interactions especially in negative ambiguous situations. They believe that such

aversive situations are caused intentionally by others. This assumption of aggressive

individuals leads them to further behave aggressively. Besides, it instigates more problematic

interactions and thereby limits pro-social behaviour. Thus, hostile attribution of intent is

considered a key element in the development and maintenance of aggressive behaviour.

Therefore, aggressive individuals should be trained to interpret negative ambiguous situations

without any biases. They can be trained to replace attribution of hostile intent with benign

intent. By reducing hostile attribution of intent, behavioural problems of aggressive individuals

can be prevented and interpersonal relationships and adjustment can be enhanced.

101

The present study was thus carried out with a purpose of reducing hostile attribution of

intent and aggressive tendencies by applying social cognitive intervention. Social cognitive

intervention was devised following the guidelines from Social information processing model

(Crick & Dodge, 1994) and General Aggression Model (Anderson & Bushman, 2002). The

primary purpose of this research work was to assess the efficacy of social cognitive

intervention on subsequent levels of hostile attribution of intent and aggression. In addition to

hostile attribution of intent and aggression, the effect of social cognitive intervention was also

assessed for other significant predictors of aggression namely, irritability, impulsivity,

emotional susceptibility and narcissism.

4.1 Objectives

The primary objectives were to study the effect of SCI on;

1. Hostile attribution of intent

2. Aggression

3. Irritability

4. Impulsivity

5. Emotional susceptibility

6. Narcissistic features

7. Aggression, hostile attribution of intent, impulsivity, narcissistic features, irritability

& emotional susceptibility for both males and females separately

4.2 Hypotheses

On the basis of review of literature, following hypotheses were formulated.

1. Individuals in experimental group would exhibit less hostile attribution of intent as

compared to individuals in control group

102

2. Individuals in experimental group would exhibit less aggression as compared to

individuals in control group.

3. Post-intervention scores on hostile attribution of intent and aggression among

individuals in experimental group would be significantly less than their pre-intervention

scores.

4. Individuals in experimental group would show less irritability as compared to

individuals in control group.

5. There would be less impulsivity among individuals in experimental group as compared

to individuals in control group.

6. Individuals in experimental group would score less on emotionally susceptibility as

compared to individuals in control group.

7. Individuals in experimental group would show less narcissistic features as compared to

individuals in control group

8. Social cognitive intervention would be effective for both males and females.

4.3 Method

4.3.1 Design

Experimental-control group design was used to examine the effect of SCI on hostile

attribution of intent (HAI), aggression, irritability, impulsivity, narcissism, and emotional

susceptibility. However, pre-post intervention scores on aggression and hostile attribution of

intent were also compared with the help of one way ANOVA. One way Multivariate Analysis

Of Variance (MANOVA) was applied to analyze the significance of difference between

experimental and control group collectively on hostile attribution of intent, aggression,

103

irritability, impulsivity, emotional susceptibility & narcissistic features. Further a series of

univariate ANOVAs with 2 levels of Intervention (experimental and control group) was applied

to analyze the significance of difference between experimental and control group on

aggression, hostile attribution of intent, impulsivity, narcissistic features, irritability &

emotional susceptibility for combined sample. Separate one way ANOVAs for both males and

females were also applied to assess the effect of social cognitive intervention on both males and

females.

4.3.2 Sample

The sample for Part II of the study mostly comprised of those individuals who were

placed in ‘high on aggression’ group with the help of Aggression Questionnaire (Buss & Perry,

1992) and teachers’ classification in first part of the study. From 125 individuals (65 males and

60 females) in ‘high on aggression’ group, five females did not turn up to participate in the

intervention. However, this did not affect the sample size as five males and one female were

referred by school authorities to take part in the social cognitive intervention. Their scores on

Aggression Questionnaire and Hostile Attribution of Intent Explorer were taken before giving

intervention. These participants were found to have high score on both the scales. Thus, sample

in Part II comprised of 126 aggressive individuals (70 males & 56 females) having mean age of

13.40 (SD=0.86). These 126 individuals were then randomly assigned to experimental and

control groups. This assignment was done with the help of slips representing each aggressive

individual. All aggressive individuals were divided into two halves by drawing those slips

randomly and putting them into two blind boxes. After this, one of these boxes was labeled as

experimental group and other one as control group. These two groups were compared on

aggression and hostile attribution of intent scores which were obtained while screening

104

aggressive individuals in Part I of the study and no significant differences were found between

the two groups (Table 2.2). This was done in order to make both groups comparable. There

were 38 males and 25 females in control group and 32 males and 31 females in experimental

group. Demographic characteristics of sample used in Part II are given in Table 2.1

Table 2.1 Number of participants bifurcated into various categories and their mean age

Group Gender Specification

Numbers of individuals

Mean Age (In Years)

SDa (In Years)

Control group Males 38 13.51 0.87

Females 25 13.24 0.66 Total 63 13.40 0.802

Experimental group

Males 32 13.50 0.97 Females 31 13.29 0.86

Total 63 13.39 0.92

Total Sample Males 70 13.51 0.92

Females 56 13.26 0.77 Total 126 13.40 0.86

a- Standard Deviation Table 2.2 Pre-intervention comparison for means scores on aggression and hostile attribution of intent among individuals in control and experimental group

Variables Means scores t-ratio

Groups

Control Experimental

Anger 23.25 25.00 -2.31*

Physical aggression 31.08 31.46 -0.37(ns)

Hostility 30.73 30.76 -0.03(ns)

Verbal aggression 18.22 19.19 -1.78(ns)

Total Aggression 103.29 106.41 -1.52(ns)

Hostile Attribution of Intent 60.79 61.06 -0.14(ns)

* p<.05 ns- Non-significant

105

4.3.3 Dependent Measures

4.3.3.1 Hostile Attribution of Intent

Hostile attribution of intent scores were obtained with the help of the same instrument

as used in Part I of the study.

4.3.3.2 Aggression

As done in Part I of the study, aggression was assessed using Aggression Questionnaire

(Buss & Perry, 1992).

4.3.3.3 Irritability

Irritability of the subjects was measured with the help of Irritability Scale (Caprara &

Renzi, 1981). The final version of the Irritability scale consists of 30 items. Respondents have

to respond to each item on 6- point likert type scale ranging from ‘Completely True for me’ to

‘Completely False for me’. ‘Completely true for me’ is given a score of six and ‘Completely

false for me’ is given a score of one. Item no. 3, 4, 6, 11, 12, 14, 17,19,21,27 are controlled

items that need to be scored reversely. Total score of an individual is calculated by adding these

scores on all the items. Thus higher score represents more irritability. As reported by Caprara et

al. (1983), internal consistency, test-retest reliability and split-half reliability have been

examined and found satisfactory for the scale. Coefficient alpha for the Irritability scale is 0.81

(p<0.001). The test-retest correlation has been found to be 0.83 (p<0.001). The reliability

coefficient by the Spearman-Brown method for the two halves, including the even and the odd

items, is 0.90 (p< 0.001) for the Irritability scale.

4.3.3.4 Impulsivity

Impulsivity was measured with the help of Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11,

Patton et al., 1995). It consists of 30 statements of personal characteristics. Respondents are

106

asked to indicate the extent to which the statements apply to them using a four-point scale

ranging from ‘rarely/never’ to ‘always/almost always’. Each item is rated on 1 (rarely/never) to

4 (always/almost always) scale. The scoring on items no. 1, 4, 7, 8, 9, 10, 12, 13, 15, 20, 23, 29

and 30 is done in reverse order i.e. 4 (rarely/never) to 1 (always/almost always). The raw

impulsiveness measure is the sum of the scores of these responses (the larger the sum, the more

impulsive is the respondent). The BIS-11 has been used with a variety of populations (Crean et

al., 2000; Kirby et al., 1999) and has demonstrated good reliability and validity (Fossati et al.,

2002; Patton et al., 1995). Internal consistency coefficients for the BIS-11 total score have been

reported (Patton et al., 1995) to range from 0.79 to 0.83.

4.3.3.5 Emotional Susceptibility

Emotional Susceptibility Scale (Caprara, et al., 1985) was used in present research

work to assess emotional susceptibility of the individuals. For constructing this scale, authors

analyzed the Buss and Durkee’s inventory for an investigation of the possible connections

between offensive components and defensive components of the aggressive reactions. The final

version of the Emotional Susceptibility scale consists of 40 items. Respondents have to respond

to each item on 6- point likert type scale ranging from “Completely True for me” to

“Completely False for me”. ‘Completely true for me’ is given a score of 6 and ‘Completely

false for me’ is given one score. Items no. 2, 6, 8, 14, 15, 20, 26, 29, 33 and 37 are scored in

reverse order. The total score of an individual is calculated by adding these scores on all the

items. Thus higher score represents more emotional susceptibility. As reported by Caprara et al.

(1983), internal consistency, test-retest reliability and split-half reliability have been examined

and found satisfactory for the scale. Coefficient alpha for the Emotional Susceptibility scale has

been found to be 0.88 (p < 0.001). The test-retest correlation has been found to be 0.84 (p<

107

0.001). The reliability coefficient by the Spearman-Brown method for the two halves, including

the even and the odd items, is 0.94 (p < 0.001) for the Emotional Susceptibility scale.

4.3.3.6 Narcissism

The Revised Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI, Raskin & Terry, 1988) was

used to assess narcissistic features in present research work. The NPI is the most common

measure used by social and personality researchers to assess narcissism in normal populations.

The most recent review on narcissism, for example, focused almost exclusively on empirical

data collected with the NPI (Morf & Rhodewalt, 2001). The Revised Narcissistic Personality

Inventory (NPI) is a 40- item forced choice self-report questionnaire. Each item has two

optional statements. The respondents have to select one statement, which is true about them or

which they feel more suitable to themselves. The inventory evolved through a 54-item, four-

factor version to its current 40-item, seven-factor version. It has demonstrated ample reliability

and construct validity (e.g., Emmons, 1987; Raskin & Terry, 1988). The NPI correlates with

observer rating of narcissistic acts as well as inflated self-beliefs compared to accurate criteria

(e.g., John & Robins, 1994). The 40- item NPI has been shown to have good validity in the

sense that NPI scores have been found to be positively correlated with the use of first-person-

singular pronouns (Raskin, 1981; Raskin & Terry, 1988). The internal consistency estimate for

the revised total scale has been found to be 0.83 (Raskin and Terry 1988). The reliability, when

tested for alternate forms eight weeks apart, has been found to be 0.72 (Raskin & Hall, 1981).

In present research work, only total scores were calculated to assess the extent of narcissistic

features in participants. Total score comprised of the total number of narcissism-loaded items

selected by participants.

108

4.3.4 Procedure

Social Cognitive Intervention was applied on 63 ‘high on aggression’ individuals. The

126 individuals were randomly assigned to experimental and control group with the procedure

mentioned above in Sample section. Experimental group was given a 6-session social cognitive

intervention designed specifically to reduce aggression, hostile attribution of intent and other

aggressive tendencies. Social-Cognitive intervention would be described in detail in the

following separate section. These sessions were conducted in the school premises after

obtaining permission from the respective Principals. All the sessions were conducted in a

separate classroom of each school in which chairs were arranged in a semi-circular manner so

that each participant could easily look at others as well as at the experimenter. With this seating

arrangement, activities could be done easily in the middle space. A standard curriculum

(described in the next section) of social cognitive intervention was applied to all the

participants in experimental groups in each school. There were 10 to 12 participants in each

group. Each session was conducted once a week with duration of 70 to 75 minutes. Since the

intervention sessions were planned according to the availability of participants, no absentees in

any of the sessions were recorded. This whole exercise for one group took around 50 days. To

avoid unethical practice, neutral or unrelated issues like study habits, time management &

career selection were discussed with the participants in control group. After six sessions of

social cognitive intervention, all the aggressive individuals, both in experimental and control

groups were administered seven questionnaires, six of dependent measures namely, aggression,

hostile attribution of intent, irritability, impulsivity, emotional susceptibility & narcissism and

one Post experimental questionnaire. Scores on aggression and hostile attribution of intent of

participants were available from Part I of the study. Pre-Post intervention comparison for both

109

the variables was also done for both control and experimental group using one way ANOVA.

However, for other four dependent variables, experimental and control groups were compared

by applying one way MANOVAs and a series of univariate ANOVAs with two levels of

Intervention i.e. intervention (experimental group) and no intervention (control group).

Separate analysis for both males and females was done using same statistics.

4.3.5 Social Cognitive intervention

A six session Social-Cognitive Intervention was designed specifically for the present

research work with material and activities appropriate for the age group of 12-15 years. After

reviewing the related literature, a standard curriculum for all six sessions of the social cognitive

intervention was developed. The detailed session-wise curriculum of intervention is presented

below.

Session I

First session of the intervention started with the rapport building. The researcher

introduced himself and a round of general introduction of all the participants was carried out.

The participants were instructed as follows:

“Before we start with the exercises and activities, I would request each of you to

introduce yourself. Also tell everyone about your hobbies and general likes and dislikes. This is

being done so that we all are comfortable and at ease with each other”.

The rapport building session proceeded with a general introduction of the purpose,

procedure and benefits of the intervention program. The participants were made positively

oriented towards the intervention as follows:

110

“This programs aims at helping you all develop a healthy personality which would be

further beneficial for your future success. As session proceeds with the activities, you are

requested to maintain proper decorum. If you find anything at any step of the program unclear,

you should ask it there and then. This is because if you continue doing some exercises or

activities with a doubt in your mind, the whole program can prove to be a futile exercise.”

These instructions were followed by discussion of:

• Ellis’s ABC model of behaviour. The significance of an individual’s beliefs and

thoughts in his behaviour was highlighted.

• the role of perception in subsequent behaviour. It was ensured that each

participant understood as to how one’s perception can guide as well as misguide

one’s subsequent behaviour. This was done with the help of examples from day

to day life with which the participants could identify.

It was vital that the participants understood how accurate perception of one’s own as

well as others’ behaviour could enhance interpersonal relationships. On contrary to this, how

inaccurate perception could hamper interpersonal relationships.

The above mentioned detailed discussion on the role of perception was followed by an

activity named “Attitude Matters” wherein each participant had to quote an incident from

his/her life to confirm the role of perception and attitude in behaviour. With such incidents

participants were made aware of the self fulfilling prophecy of the attitudes.

The session was concluded with a general discussion where every participant got a

chance to speak his/her mind. In the end, a home assignment was given to the participants as

part of which they had to collect instances of inappropriate behaviour created by inaccurate

perception of a social situation.

111

Session II

The purpose of second session was to make the participants understand the negative

consequences of aggressive behaviour. This was followed by the role of cognitions in initiating

and maintaining aggressive behaviour. The session began with discussion of home assignment

wherein instances of inappropriate behaviour were explained and described as a function of

inaccurate processing of social information. A brief overview about aggression, its causes &

consequences was done. An emphasis was given in its role in intrapersonal and interpersonal

relationship and its negative influences on success. By this, participants were motivated to feel

importance and urgency to reduce their tendencies to behave aggressively.

The activity of this session was related to the role of cognitions in aggressive behaviour.

For this the participants were instructed as follows:

“Now that you all have an idea about the negative effect of misperception on behaviour,

I would like you to think of a situation of inappropriate retaliation and aggression caused by

an individual who behaved aggressively without understanding the context or by incorrectly

attributing harmful intent to a peer”.

The activity was followed by the introduction of the concept of intent. Participants were

made aware of the difference between accidental, pro-social and hostile intent. The session was

concluded with a discussion on what kind of mistakes people make in negative social

encounters e.g. wrongly attributing negative intent to others’ behaviour with no clear evidence

of the same. The causes of such mistakes were also discussed in which role of ambiguous

situation and inattention to relevant social cues present at the time of encounter was primarily

highlighted.

112

The participants were given home assignment to keep a log of instances where

misattributions in their as well as others’ behaviour were noticed.

Session III

In third session, concept of ambiguous situation was discussed in detail with main

emphasis on informing that negative ambiguous situation may be an accidental one. The idea

that ambiguous situations are difficult to understand and inadequate understanding of such

situations can be detrimental for an individual’s adjustment was also focused upon. Discussion

about the role of uncontrolled and unintentional situational factors in negative social ambiguous

situation was also the primary concern of the session.

For the activity of the third session, the participants were instructed as follows:

“From your own lives, think of some negative social situation in which you retaliated

inappropriately and why you did so”. In most of the instances, the participants were not clear

about the situations and thought that other might have done it intentionally. Thus with the help

of this activity, the participants were trained to consider ambiguous situation (in which cues are

inadequate to infer causal intent) as accidental, unintended and uncontrollable.

The activity was followed by a discussion on the concept of ‘Thinking Time’ . The

participants were trained to take appropriate time in analyzing different social situations

especially negative ones.

The importance of understanding the role of one’s own feeling in behaviour was

discussed in the end of the session. This was done by showing vicious cycle depicting

relationship between negative feelings and maladaptive behaviour (Figure D). Participants were

trained to differentiate between feeling and thinking about the social situation.

113

As a home assignment, the participants were given negative outcome scenarios and they had to

complete unfinished sentences to:

• identify their own feelings

• determine, on the basis of description, what they actually know about the actor’s

intent.

Session IV

Fourth session was basically related to responses after attribution and what should be

kept in mind while reacting to negative ambiguous social situation. The session started with the

discussion on three kinds of intent introduced in second session. The effects of each type of

intent on one’s subsequent reactions in social situation were discussed with the help of

examples from day to day life.

The activity in this session aimed at training the participants as to how to decide upon

an appropriate response in ambiguous situations. For the purpose following instructions were

given to them:

• Think about whether you have enough information to classify the situation and

try to collect the same including non-verbal cues and the situational context.

• In an ambiguous situation, choose a verbal response that seems to fit & describe

the situation, ask yourself quickly a question regarding what should be done in

that situation.

• Remember to use a calm tone of voice.

• Try several responses for a single situation.

• Don’t start your conversation with “Did you” but with “why, what, how etc. to

understand others’ understanding of the same situation.

• Don’t jump to conclusion at once

• Try to be relaxed while responding to any kind of situation.

• Always be objective in all situations to respond appropriately.

114

• Remember that your aroused state of mind would lead you to irrational and

inappropriate understanding of the situation. Thus make a clear distinction

between your feeling and knowledge about the situation.

For the home assignment, the participants were instructed as follows:

“Record three instances of peer interaction when things did not turn out the way you

wanted. Describe what the peer & you did”.

Session V

In fifth session, other factors that predispose individual to misattribute intentions and

behave aggressively were discussed. Hypothetical scenarios were used to practice all the steps

that were taught to them in previous sessions i.e. from negative encounter to appropriate

response in which getting information about situation, attributing intent & finally choosing

appropriate responses were included. A detailed discussion on the following factors affecting

responses in social situations was carried out

• Frustration

• Anger

• Irritability

• Impulsivity

• Emotional susceptibility

• Narcissism.

• Modelling

As a part of activity the participants were to discuss the issues of deciding upon an

appropriate course of action when responding to ambiguous situation. For this purpose, the

researcher read a story depicting a negative social situation and asked for alternative courses of

action in that situation. Participants were to provide three alternatives with pros and cons of

each and choose the best alternative. The reasons of choosing that particular alternative were

also discussed.

115

Participants were instructed to keep following things in mind while responding in a

social situation:

• Situational context

• Knowledge about the person

• Feelings at that time

• Non-verbal cues

• Tone of voice

For the home assignment, the participants were instructed as follows:

“Write one situation which really happens between you and a peer in the coming days

when something happens that you don’t like much. Try the skills discussed here. How did it

work? What else did you do? How did the situation finally end up? What were your feelings?”

Session VI

The last session started with discussion about home assignment wherein feedback

regarding application of skills learned, problems faced while practically applying and

effectiveness of skills were sought. This was followed by review of all the skills discussed in

the whole program. Session was concluded with the administration of all dependent measures.

In the end all participants were thanked and some refreshment was also arranged for them.

The standard procedure mentioned above was followed for each group in all schools.

Besides home assignments, participants were asked to perform certain relaxation exercises for

which they were trained briefly during each session. After fifteen days of last session, one

session of general interaction was scheduled with the participants. The purpose of this session

was to get feedback from them. The feedback provided by the participants was very

satisfactory.

116

4.4 Results

Findings of Part II of the present study are presented in Table 2.1 to 2.21 and graphical

representation of the same is depicted in Figure 2.1 to 2.7. In Part II of the study, Social

Cognitive Intervention (SCI) was conducted to reduce hostile attribution of intent, aggression

and other aggression tendencies namely, irritability, emotional susceptibility, impulsivity and

narcissism among adolescents. Results show that SCI was effective in reducing hostile

attribution of intent, aggression, irritability, emotional susceptibility, impulsivity and

narcissism among aggressive adolescents.

After fifteen days of the intervention, all the participants were asked some questions in

the form of Post-Experimental Questionnaire (PEQ) to assess their perception of change

directly at cognitive and behavioural levels. Participants had to rate the extent of change they

experienced in themselves on some aspects depicted in seven questions on a 5 point rating scale

ranging from “Not at all” to “Very Much”. “Not al all” response was given a score of one and

“Very Much” was given a score of five. Thus, higher score on PEQ represented more

perception of change. After scoring, control and experimental group were compared on PEQ to

assess the effect of intervention on their perception of change. Results of PEQ are mentioned in

Table 2.3 to Table 2.5. Table 2.3 is showing means and SDs for Question no. 1 (Q1) to

Question no. 7 (Q7) & total scores of PEQ obtained by individuals in control & experimental

groups. Summaries of ANOVA and MANOVA for the effect of Intervention (Experimental &

Control Group) on seven questions of Post-Experimental Questionnaire are presented in Table

2.4 and Table 2.5 respectively.

When asked “to what extent you believe that now you think about other possible

reasons for negative encounters than blaming other person directly”(Q1), participants in the

117

control group showed less extent of change they experienced (M=1.84, SD= 1.00) as compared

to participants in experimental group (M=3.20, SD= 1.28). Both groups differed significantly

on Q1 {F (1,124) = 44.18, p<.001}. After intervention, individuals in experimental group

believed that they considered other possible reasons for negative encounters than blaming other

person directly. While responding to second question(Q2) i.e. “To what extent you believe that

now you have more realistic viewpoint of your self than before” participants in experimental

group showed significantly greater change (M=3.25, SD= 1.19) as compared to those in control

group (M=1.90, SD= 1.07). Difference between both group was found to be highly significant

on this question {F (1,124) = 44.61, p<.001}. On third question (Q3), (To what extent you

believe that now you take more time before reacting in most social encounters than before)

individuals in control group scored significantly less (M=1.85, SD= 0.94) than those in

experimental group (M=3.46, SD= 1.26). Highly significant difference was found for this

question between control and experimental group {F (1,124) = 64.61, p<.001}. “To what extent

you believe that you have more tolerance than before” was the fourth question (Q4) and

intervention was effective to make the participants in experimental group score higher

(M=3.09, SD=1.11) as compared to those in control group (M=1.71, SD=0.81). Significant

difference was observed between both groups {F (1,124) = 62.97, p<.001}. In response to

question number five (Q5) which asked “To what extent you believe that trivial issues bother

you less than before”, control group participants showed less extent of change (M=1.77, SD=

0.94) as compared to experimental group participants (M=3.14, SD= 1.26) and difference

between the two was found to be statistically significant {F (1,124) = 47.07, p<.001}.

Experimental group participants showed more scores (M=3.25, SD= 1.21) on question number

six (Q6) which asked “To what extent you believe that you are more relaxed than before” as

118

compared to control group participants (M=1.68, SD= 0.91). Significant difference was

observed between both the groups {F (1,124) = 67.17, p<.001}. On the last question, in which

both the groups were asked directly “To what extent you believe that now you behave less

aggressively than before” individuals in experimental group showed greater extent of change as

they scored more (M=3.04, SD= 1.26) than individuals in control group (M=1.68, SD= 0.87).

Intervention might have made individuals in experimental group behave less aggressively as

the difference between both groups on last question was found to be statistically significant {F

(1,124) = 49.68, p<.001}. MANOVA results also support the findings obtained by univariate

analysis. Significant difference between experimental and control groups was observed {F

(MANOVA; 7,118) = 16.358 (exact statistic), p<.01; Table 2.5} for all the questions of PEQ.

Comparison between control group and experimental group on total scores of PEQ

reveals that experimental group participants showed more extent of change (M=22.46, SD=

5.95) as compared to control group participants (M=12.46, SD= 4.23). This difference was

statistically significant {F (1,124) = 117.96, p<.001}

Table 2.6 shows comparison between control & experimental group on hostile

attribution of intent, aggression, irritability, impulsivity, emotional susceptibility and

narcissism. As evident, individuals in control group scored higher on all the aggressive

tendencies i.e. hostile attribution of intent, aggression, irritability, impulsivity, emotional

susceptibility and narcissism, as compared to those in experimental group. The difference

between control group and experimental group on all the variables has been found to be

statistically significant {F (MANOVA; 6,119) = 28.511(exact statistic), p<.01}. The obtained

values of all the tests, F-statistics and MANOVA summary are presented in Table 2.8. Values

for Pillai’s trace test, Wilks’ Lambda, Hotelling’s Trace and Roy’s Largest Root test are found

119

to be 0.545, 0.455, 1.199 and 1.199 respectively. All these values are found to be statistically

significant, proving the efficiency of social Cognitive intervention. After significant multiple

analysis of variance (MANOVA), a series of univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) was

applied to assess the effectiveness of SCI on each dependent measure separately. Findings of

the same are presented in Table 2.6 and summary of the one way ANOVAs is given in Table

2.7.

As shown in Table 2.6, individuals in experimental group showed less hostile

attribution of intent (M=50.84, SD=11.98) as compared to those who did not receive any such

treatment i.e. individuals in control group (M=61.5, SD=9.08). Significant difference was

observed between the two groups {F (1,124) = 31.707, p<.01}.

The scores of aggression are also presented in Table 2.6. Individuals in experimental

group showed less aggression (M=88.55, SD=12.22) as compared to individuals in control

group (M=101.73, SD=15.24). Significant difference was observed between both the groups {F

(1,124) = 28.63, p<.01}.

More irritability (M=96.67, SD=13.34) was observed among individuals in control

group as compared to individuals in experimental group (M=64.73, SD=16.94). For irritability,

both the groups differed significantly {F (1,124) = 138.078, p<.01}. The same trend was

observed for impulsivity scores. Control group participants scored higher (M=75.14,

SD=10.22) on impulsivity as compared to experimental group participants (M=62.25,

SD=12.47). The effect of intervention was found to be significant for impulsivity as the

difference between experimental and control group came out to be statistically significant {F

(1,124) = 40.239, p<.01}. For emotional susceptibility also, individuals in experimental group

scored less (M=86.78, SD=23.9) after receiving intervention, whereas individuals in control

120

group scored higher on the same (M=120.95, SD=14.84) and significant difference was

observed between the two groups on emotional susceptibility {F (1,124) = 92.932, p<.01}. On

narcissistic features, individuals in experimental group scored lower (M=16.55, SD=5.26) than

those in control group (M=20.60, SD=4.61) and the difference between these two group

reached the significance level {F (1,124) = 7.680, p<.01}.

Thus it can be stated that all the proposed hypotheses proved to be true in the present

study. Social cognitive intervention was found to be effective in reducing all the aggressive

tendencies measured in the form of six dependent variables i.e. hostile attribution of intent,

aggression, irritability, impulsivity, emotional susceptibility and narcissism.

As the individuals who participated in Intervention program, were screened in with the

help of Aggression Questionnaire and Hostile Attribution of Intent questionnaire, their pre-

intervention scores on these two variables were available which were then compared with post

intervention scores. This analysis was done in order to enhance the efficacy of intervention.

Finding of this Pre-Post comparison is presented in Table 2.9. It can be observed that in control

group no significant difference was observed between pre and post scores on aggression and

hostile attribution of intent. However, Post intervention scores in experimental group showed

marked decline for aggression scores (M=88.56) as compared to pre intervention scores

(M=106.41) and this difference was found to be highly significant {t (62) = 8.78, p<.01}.

Similar findings were observed for sub-dimensions of Aggression namely, anger, physical

aggression, hostility and verbal aggression. Same trend was observed for hostile attribution of

intent. When pre intervention scores were compared with post intervention scores, the

difference was found to be statistically {t (62) = 5.56, p<.01}. Before intervention, Individuals

were higher on hostile attribution of intent (M=61.06) than post intervention (M=50.84). It can

121

be stated that intervention significantly reduced participants’ tendency to attribute hostile intent

in negative social encounter.

Scores on all dependent variables obtained by control group and experimental group

were also analyzed for males and females separately to examine the significance of intervention

for both the genders.

The results are shown in Table 2.10 to Table 2.15. Males in experimental group differed

significantly from males in control group on all the dependent variables {F (MANOVA; 6, 63)

= 16.617(exact statistic), p<.01}. Females in experimental group also differ significantly from

females in control group on all the dependent variable except narcissism. However MANOVA

results came out be significant for females {F (MANOVA; 6, 49) = 13.368 (exact statistic),

p<.01}.

Data from Tables 2.10 and 2.13 were combined in Tables 2.16 to 2.21 that are

presenting mean scores of all dependent variables bifurcated for each gender in both control

and experimental group. The graphical representation of the same has been given in Figures 2.2

to 2.7.

As shown in Table 2.16, Aggression scores have been much higher in control group for

both males and females {M (males) =105.18; M (females) =96.48} as compared to those in

experimental group {M (males) =91.71; M (females) =85.29}. Significant difference has been

observed on aggression in control group and experimental group for both males {F (1, 68) =

17.76, p<.01, Table 2.10} and females {F (1, 54) = 9.52, p<.01, Table 2.13}. As shown,

aggressive behaviours were reduced among aggressive males and females adolescents after

intervention.

122

It can be observed from Table 2.17 that both males and females scored higher on hostile

attribution of intent {M (males) =62.36 & M (females) =60.20} in control group as compared

to those in experimental group {M (males) =51.06; M (females) =50.61}. The social cognitive

intervention given to experimental group has been found effective in reducing hostile

attribution of intent among both males and females but it showed greater change among males

{F (1, 68) = 24.74, p<.01, Table 2.10} as compared to females {F (1, 54) = 8.76, p<.01; Table

2.13}, however the difference between control and experimental group was large enough

among both males and females to reach to the significance level.

For irritability also, both males and females in control group scored higher {M (males)

=97.63; M (females) =95.2} than those in experimental group {M (males) =64.87; M (females)

=64.58} (Table 2.18). Significant difference has been found between control group and

experimental group for both males {F (1, 68) = 74.64, p<.01, Table 2.10} and females {F (1,

54) = 59.48, p<.01, Table 2.13}. It shows that intervention given to experimental group was

effective to reduce this aggressive tendency i.e. irritability, in both males and female aggressive

adolescents. Same trend has been observed for other four variables for both males and females.

Table 2.19 shows that impulsivity scores are much higher in control group for both

males and females {M (males) =76.55; M (females) =73.00} as compared to those in

experimental group {M (males) =63.28; M (females) =61.19}. Significant difference has been

observed on impulsivity between control group and experimental group for both males {F (1,

68) = 19.11, p<.01, Table 2.10} and females {F (1, 54) = 20.91, p<.01, Table 2.13}. After

intervention, impulsivity scores were reduced among both aggressive males and females

adolescents.

123

It can be observed from Table 2.20 that individuals in experimental group, both males

and females showed less emotional susceptibility {M (males) =83.63; M (females) =90.03} as

compared to those in control group {M (males) =118.31; M (females) =124.96}. The effect of

intervention on emotional susceptibility has also been found to be statistically significant for

both males {F (1, 68) = 46.39, p<.01, Table 2.10} and females {F (1, 54) = 53.21, p<.01; Table

2.13}.

The intervention proved to be effective for males as there was significant difference {F

(1, 68) = 13.41, p<.01, Table 2.10} between narcissism levels of males in experimental group

(M=16.93) and control group (M=21.44) (Table 2.21). Same trend has been there for females

also as they scored less in experimental group (M=16.16) as compared to those in control group

(M=19.32), however the difference between both groups did not reach the significance level {F

(1, 54) = 3.062, p=.082}.

It can be concluded here that social cognitive intervention proved to be effective in

reducing aggressive tendencies among both aggressive males and aggressive females.

Aggressive adolescents, both males and females who were given social cognitive intervention

showed significant reduction in aggressive tendencies measured by six dependent variables i.e.

hostile attribution of intent, aggression, irritability, impulsivity, emotional susceptibility, and

narcissism (only males).

124

Table 2.3 Post Experimental Questionnaire (PEQ)

Questions of Post Experimental Questionnaire

Control Group

Experimental Group F

(ANOVA) F

(MANOVA) N Means SD N Means SD

To what extent you believe that now you think about other possible reasons for negative encounters than blaming other person directly? (Q1)

63 1.84 1.00 63 3.20 1.28 44.18*

16.358*

To what extent you believe that now you have more realistic viewpoint of your self than before. (Q2)

63 1.90 1.07 63 3.25 1.19 44.61*

To what extent you believe that now you take more time before reacting in most social encounters than before. (Q3)

63 1.85 0.94 63 3.46 1.26 64.61*

To what extent you believe that you have more tolerance than before. (Q4)

63 1.71 0.81 63 3.09 1.11 62.97*

To what extent you believe that trivial issues bother you less than before. (Q5)

63 1.77 0.94 63 3.14 1.26 47.07*

To what extent you believe that you are more relaxed than before. (Q6)

63 1.68 0.91 63 3.25 1.21 67.17*

To what extent you believe that now you behave less aggressively than before. (Q7)

63 1.68 0.87 63 3.04 1.26 49.68*

TOTAL 63 12.46 4.23 63 22.46 5.95 117.96*

*p<.01

125

PEQ SCORES OF CONTROL AND EXPERIMENTAL GROUPS

Q1 Q2 Q3 Q4 Q5 Q6 Q7

Q1 Q2Q3

Q4 Q5 Q6Q7

1

2

2

3

3

4

4

5

5

Q1 Q2 Q3 Q4 Q5 Q6 Q7PEQ QUESTIONS

PE

Q S

CO

RE

S

MEAN CON

MEAN EXP

Figure 2.1

Control & Experimental group comparison for mean scores on Post Experimental Questionnaire (PEQ)

Table 2.4

ANOVA Summary for the effect of Intervention (Experimental & Control Group) on scores of Post-Experimental Questionnaire

Variables SS

Effect

df

Effect

MS

Effect

SS

Error

df

Error

MS

Error F-ratios

Q1 58.70 1.00 58.70 164.73 124.00 1.33 44.19*

Q2 57.34 1.00 57.34 159.37 124.00 1.29 44.62*

Q3 80.96 1.00 80.96 155.37 124.00 1.25 64.62*

Q4 60.07 1.00 60.07 118.29 124.00 0.95 62.97*

Q5 58.70 1.00 58.70 154.60 124.00 1.25 47.08*

Q6 77.79 1.00 77.79 143.59 124.00 1.16 67.17*

Q7 58.70 1.00 58.70 146.51 124.00 1.18 49.68*

Total PEQ 3150.00 1.00 3150.00 3311.30 124.00 26.70 117.960*

*p<.01 SS-Sum of Square

Df-Degree of Freedom MS-Mean Square

126

Table 2.5

MANOVA Summary for the effect of Intervention (Experimental & Control Group) on scores of Post-Experimental Questionnaire

Effect Test Value F Hypothesis df Error df

Intervention

Pillai's Trace .492 16.358a 7 118

Wilks' Lambda .508 16.358a 7 118

Hotelling's Trace .970 16.358a 7 118

Roy's Largest Root .970 16.358a 7 118

a- exact statistics & p<.01

Table 2.6

Control & Experimental group comparison on Hostile Attribution of Intent, Aggression, Irritability, Impulsivity, Emotional Susceptibility and Narcissism

Variables Control Group Experimental Group F

(ANOVA) F

(MANOVA) Means SD N Means SD N Hostile Attribution of Intent

61.5 9.08 63 50.84 11.98 63 31.707*

28.511*

Aggression 101.73 15.24 63 88.555 12.22 63 28.638*

Irritability 96.67 13.34 63 64.73 16.94 63 138.078*

Impulsivity 75.14 10.22 63 62.25 12.47 63 40.239* Emotional Susceptibility

120.95 14.84 63 86.78 23.9 63 92.932*

Narcissism 20.6 4.61 63 16.55 5.26 63 7.680* *p<.01

127

Table 2.7

ANOVA Summary for the effect of Intervention (Experimental & Control Group) on Hostile Attribution of Intent, Aggression, Irritability, Impulsivity, Emotional Susceptibility and Narcissism

Variables Source of variance Sum of Squares df Mean Square F

Hostile Attribution of

Intent

Between Groups 3584.00 1 3584.00

31.707* Within Groups 14016.16 124 113.03

Total 17600.16 125

Aggression

Between Groups 5467.46 1 5467.46

28.638* Within Groups 23673.97 124 190.92

Total 29141.43 125

Irritability

Between Groups 32128.13 1 32128.13

138.078* Within Groups 28852.4 124 232.68

Total 60980.54 125

Impulsivity

Between Groups 5232.89 1 5232.89

40.239* Within Groups 16125.65 124 130.05

Total 21358.54 125

Emotional Susceptibility

Between Groups 36482.03 1 36482.03

92.302* Within Groups 49010.60 124 395.25

Total 85492.64 125

Narcissism

Between Groups 1176.39 1 1176.39

7.680* Within Groups 18993.56 124 153.17

Total 20169.94 125 *p<.01

Table 2.8

MANOVA Summary for the effect of Intervention (Experimental & Control Group) on Hostile Attribution of Intent, Aggression, Irritability, Impulsivity, Emotional Susceptibility and Narcissism

Effect Value F Hypothesis df Error df

Intervention

Pillai's Trace .590 28.511a 6 119

Wilks' Lambda .410 28.511a 6 119

Hotelling's Trace 1.438 28.511a 6 119

Roy's Largest Root 1.438 28.511a 6 119 a. Exact statistic & p<0.01

128

Table 2.9

Pre and Post intervention mean scores on aggression and hostile attribution of intent for control and experimental group

Groups Variables Means scores t-ratio

Pre-Intervention

Post-Intervention

Control Group

Anger 23.25 22.76 0.58(ns)

Physical aggression 31.08 31.95 0.79(ns)

Hostility 30.73 29.62 1.10(ns)

Verbal aggression 18.22 17.40 1.44(ns)

Total Aggression 103.29 101.73 0.63(ns) Hostile Attribution of

Intent 60.79 61.51 0.37(ns)

Experimental Group

Anger 25.00 20.06 5.97**

Physical aggression 31.46 25.33 5.64**

Hostility 30.76 28.37 2.31*

Verbal aggression 19.19 14.79 6.79**

Total Aggression 106.41 88.56 8.78** Hostile Attribution of

Intent 61.06 50.84 5.56**

* p<0.05 **p<0.01

ns- Non-significant Table 2.10

Control & Experimental group comparison on Hostile Attribution of Intent, Aggression, Irritability, Impulsivity, Emotional Susceptibility and Narcissism for males

Variables Control Group

Experimental Group F

(ANOVA) F

(MANOVA) N Means (SD) N Means (SD)

Hostile Attribution of Intent

38 62.36 9.59 32 51.06 9.32 24.74*

16.617*

Aggression 38 105.18 14.53 32 91.71 11.69 17.76*

Irritability 38 97.63 14.23 32 64.87 17.49 74.64*

Impulsivity 38 76.55 11.02 32 63.28 14.35 19.11* Emotional

Susceptibility 38 118.31 14.26 32 83.63 27.3 45.36*

Narcissism 38 21.44 4.37 32 16.93 5.91 13.41* *p<.01

129

Table 2.11

ANOVA Summary for the effect of Intervention (Experimental & Control Group) on Hostile Attribution of Intent, Aggression, Irritability, Impulsivity, Emotional Susceptibility and Narcissism for males

Variables Source of variance

Sum of Squares df Mean Square F

Hostile Attribution of

Intent

Between Groups 2220.48 1 2220.483

24.742* Within Groups 6102.72 68 89.746

Total 8323.20 69

Aggression

Between Groups 3149.76 1 3149.764

17.760* Within Groups 12060.178 68 177.356

Total 15209.94 69

Irritability

Between Groups 18639.43 1 18639.429

74.635* Within Groups 16982.34 68 249.740

Total 35621.77 69

Impulsivity

Between Groups 3059.62 1 3059.622

19.116* Within Groups 10883.86 68 160.057

Total 13943.49 69

Emotional Susceptibility

Between Groups 20307.37 1 20307.375

45.369* Within Groups 30437.21 68 447.606

Total 50744.59 69

Narcissism

Between Groups 353.32 1 353.316

13.413* Within Groups 1791.27 68 26.342

Total 2144.59 69 *p<.01

Table 2.12

MANOVA Summary for the effect of Intervention (Experimental & Control Group) on Hostile Attribution of Intent, Aggression, Irritability, Impulsivity, Emotional Susceptibility and Narcissism for males

Effect Value F Hypothesis df Error df

Intervention

Pillai's Trace .613 16.617a 6 63

Wilks' Lambda .387 16.617a 6 63

Hotelling's Trace 1.583 16.617a 6 63

Roy's Largest Root 1.583 16.617a 6 63 a. Exact statistic & p<0.01

130

Table 2.13

Control & Experimental group comparison on Hostile Attribution of Intent, Aggression, Irritability, Impulsivity, Emotional Susceptibility and Narcissism for females

Variables Control Group Experimental Group F

(ANOVA) F

(MANOVA) N Means SD N Means SD Hostile

Attribution of Intent

25 60.20 8.24 31 50.61 14.38 8.76*

13.368*

Aggression 25 96.48 15.06 31 85.29 12.07 9.52*

Irritability 25 95.20 12.00 31 64.58 16.65 59.48*

Impulsivity 25 73.00 8.65 31 61.19 10.30 20.91* Emotional

Susceptibility 25 124.96 15.09 31 90.03 19.71 53.21*

Narcissism 25 19.32 4.77 31 16.16 4.56 3.062(ns) *p<.01

ns- non-significant

131

Table 2.14

ANOVA Summary for the effect of Intervention (Experimental & Control Group) on of Hostile Attribution of Intent, Aggression, Irritability, Impulsivity, Emotional Susceptibility and Narcissism for females

Variables Source of variance

Sum of Squares df Mean Square F

Hostile Attribution of

Intent

Between Groups 1272.00 1 1272.002

8.762* Within Groups 7839.35 54 145.173

Total 9111.36 55

Aggression

Between Groups 1732.80 1 1732.801

9.528* Within Groups 9820.63 54 181.863

Total 11553.43 55

Irritability

Between Groups 12974.95 1 12974.952

59.480* Within Groups 11779.55 54 218.140

Total 24754.50 55

Impulsivity

Between Groups 1929.09 1 1929.090

20.906* Within Groups 4982.84 54 92.275

Total 6911.93 55

Emotional Susceptibility

Between Groups 17272.11 1 17272.112

54.643* Within Groups 17068.73 54 316.088

Total 34340.84 55

Narcissism

Between Groups 966.92 1 966.924

3.062(ns) Within Groups 17050.43 54 315.749

Total 18017.36 55 *p<0.01

ns- non-significant Table 2.15

MANOVA Summary for the effect of Intervention (Experimental & Control Group) on of Hostile Attribution of Intent, Aggression, Irritability, Impulsivity, Emotional Susceptibility and Narcissism for females

Effect Value F Hypothesis df Error df Intervention Pillai's Trace .621 13.368a 6.000 49.000

Wilks' Lambda .379 13.368a 6.000 49.000

Hotelling's Trace 1.637 13.368a 6.000 49.000

Roy's Largest Root 1.637 13.368a 6.000 49.000 a. Exact statistic & p<0.01

132

Table 2.16

Mean scores of Aggression bifurcated for gender in both control and experimental group

Intervention

Control Group Experimental Group

Gender Males 105.18 91.71

Females 96.48 85.29

Intervention X Gender

105.18

91.7196.48

85.29

60

70

80

90

100

110

120

Control Group Experimental Group

Intervention

Ag

gre

ssio

n S

core

s

Males

Females

Figure 2.2 Comparison of mean aggression scores bifurcated for gender in both control and experimental group Table 2.17

Mean scores of Hostile Attribution of Intent bifurcated for gender in both control and experimental group

Intervention

Control Group Experimental Group

Gender Males 62.36 51.06

Females 60.2 50.61

133

Intervention X Gender

62.36

51.06

60.2

50.61

40

45

50

55

60

65

Control Group Experimental GroupIntervention

Ho

stile

Att

rib

uti

on

of

Inte

nt Males

Females

Figure 2.3

Comparison of mean scores on hostile attribution of intent bifurcated for gender in both control and experimental group

Table 2.18

Mean scores of Irritability bifurcated for gender in both control and experimental group

Intervention

Control Group Experimental Group

Gender Males 97.63 64.87

Females 95.2 64.58

134

Intervention X Gender

97.63

64.87

95.2

64.58

40

50

60

70

80

90

100

110

120

Control Group Experimental Group

Intervention

Irri

tab

ility

Sco

res

Males

Females

Figure 2.4 Comparison of mean irritability scores bifurcated for gender in both control and experimental group Table 2.19

Mean scores of Impulsivity bifurcated for gender in both control and experimental group

Intervention

Control Group Experimental Group

Gender Males 76.55 63.28

Females 73 61.19

Intervention X Gender

76.55

63.28

73

61.19

50

55

60

65

70

75

80

Control Group Experimental Group

Intervention

Imp

uls

ivit

y S

core

s

Males

Females

Figure 2.5 Comparison of mean impulsivity scores bifurcated for gender in both control and experimental group

135

Table 2.20

Mean scores of Emotional Susceptibility bifurcated for gender in both control and experimental group

Intervention

Control Group Experimental Group

Gender Males 118.32 83.63

Females 124.96 90.03

Intervention X Gender

118.32

83.63

124.96

90.03

60

70

80

90

100

110

120

130

Control Group Experimental Group

Intervention

Em

oti

on

al S

usc

epti

bili

ty

Sco

res

Males

Females

Figure 2.6

Comparison of mean scores on emotional susceptibility bifurcated for gender in both control and experimental group

Table 2.21

Mean scores of Narcissism bifurcated for gender in both control and experimental group

Intervention

Control Group Experimental Group

Gender Males 21.44 16.93

Females 19.32 16.16

136

Intervention X Gender

21.44

16.93

19.32

16.16

10

12

14

16

18

20

22

24

Control Group Experimental Group

Intervention

Nar

ciss

ism

Sco

res

Males

Females

Figure 2.7

Comparison of mean scores on narcissism bifurcated for gender in both control and experimental group

137

4.5 Discussion

Part II of the present study followed the basic tenants of social information processing

model (Crick and Dodge, 1994) and general aggression model (Anderson & Bushman, 2002)

in providing social cognitive intervention to aggressive adolescents. The central assumption of

these models is that deficits like encoding social cues and interpreting the intention of others

increase the risk for aggressive retaliation. Empirical research has demonstrated that highly-

aggressive individuals are more likely to interpret others’ intent as hostile in negative

ambiguous situation. Therefore, in social cognition intervention, aggressive adolescents were

trained to process social situation accurately and implement learned skills in non-threatening

group setting which further can enhance their confidence in applying new skills in real negative

social encounters. Findings of the study highlighted the success and significance of social

cognitive intervention in dealing with aggression and other aggressive tendencies namely

irritability, impulsivity, emotional susceptibility, and narcissism by reducing hostile attribution

of intent. All the major hypotheses of the study were proved. The differences between

participants in control and experimental group on all the dependent variables has been found to

be statistically significant {f (MANOVA; 6,119) = 28.511(exact statistic), p<.01).

The major hypothesis of the study that social cognitive intervention would reduce

hostile attribution of intent has been proved. As depicted in Table 2.6, aggressive adolescents in

experimental group showed significantly less hostile attribution of intent as compared to those

in control group{F (1,124) = 31.707, p<.01,}. Post-intervention scores on hostile attribution of

intent among aggressive adolescents in experimental group were also found to be significantly

less {F (1,124) = 30.88, p<.01, Table 2.9) than their pre-intervention scores. The findings can

be explained within the framework of social information processing model (Crick & Dodge,

138

1994). The social cognitive intervention used in this study trained the aggressive participants in

processing social information accurately. Such individuals, in negative ambiguous situation,

tend to process social information inaccurately and infer that the situation is caused

intentionally by other individuals present in the situation (Crick & Dodge, 1994). However they

do not have any evidence as there are no clear social cues depicting causal relationship of

malicious intent and negative social situation. The focus of the intervention was thus to train

the aggressive adolescents to process social information in a biased free manner.

Since social information processing deficits like encoding social cues and interpreting

the intention of others increase the risk for aggressive retaliation (Gifford-Smith & Rabiner,

2004; Hanish et al., 2004), it was ensured in the intervention to correct the mistakes aggressive

adolescents make at these stages of social information processing. The participants were trained

to consider all available information present in the situation that can help in its better

understanding. The exercises and activities enabled the participants in experimental group to

overcome the deficits at the stage of encoding social cues which if not taken care of can

increase aggressive tendencies (Dodge, 2003). For instance, efforts were made to incorporate

among aggressive adolescents the concept of ‘thinking time’ that is to be taken before

responding in negative situation. This exercise enabled aggressive adolescents to reappraise

entire situation which may help them find some non-hostile, unintentional and uncontrolled

factors that may further prevent their aggressive response. Participants were also helped to

understand how by perceiving and encoding all possible causes of a negative situation can

affect an individuals’ evaluation of that situation can be affected and how by focusing on

partial or irrelevant information, the evaluation of that situation can be faulty. The participants

were further sensitized that the faulty evaluation of a negative social situation can lead to

139

maladaptive behavioural patterns or inappropriate expression of behaviour which can even be

detrimental for social relationships.

Another explanation for the present findings is that the interpretational style of the

participants that has been significantly accounted for aggressive behaviour was corrected.

Interpretation of social information is an important predictor of one’s’ reaction in social

situation because misinterpretation or hostile interpretation of social situation increases the

probability that the individual would behave aggressively (Lengua, 2003). Therefore the

participants in the experimental group were trained to apply appropriate social skills which

helped them to modify hostile interpretation style. The hostile attribution of intent of the

participants reduced after intervention as they were informed about the general mistakes people

make in negative social encounter i.e. wrongly interpreting or attributing hostile intent to

other’s behaviour, even with no clear evidence of the same. The concept of intent was also

reviewed with special emphasis on distinguishing between accidental, pro-social & hostile

intent. Besides, discussion about characteristics of ambiguous situation, and the reactions to

ambiguous situation might have changed their approach to interpret the situation, resulting in

less hostile attribution of intent.

The findings can also be explained on the basis of assertions made by Bierman (2004)

and Leve et al., (2002). Social cognitive intervention might have enhanced the participants’

ability to navigate social situations. The exercises focused on helping the participants to

reappraise entire situation which they thought hostile and to choose appropriate responses

accordingly. Thus, intervention in the present study helped the subjects to master the cognitive

skills and enhanced their ability to differentiate hostile intent from benign intent.

140

Support for the present findings come from other researches (Guerra and Slaby, 1990:

CPPRG, 2002a; 2002b, Sukhodolsky, Golub, Stone, and Orban, 2005, Bugental et al., 2002)

that have reported positive effects of social cognitive interventions in reducing hostile

attributional biases among aggressive individuals.

To sum up, the difference between experimental and control group on hostile attribution

of intent can be attributed to specific intervention in which aggressive adolescents were trained

in processing social information correctly. The intervention mainly focused on modifying

cognitions and information processing style of aggressive adolescents in social situation

specifically in negative ambiguous one and with this modification their hostile attribution of

intent was reduced. Therefore, for adolescents at risk, social cognitive intervention may hold a

promise to change information processing patterns and aggressive tendencies.

The next major hypothesis of the present study was that individuals in intervention

group would exhibit less aggression as compared to individuals in control group (Table 2.6).

Findings support this hypothesis as experimental group showed significantly less aggression as

compared to control group{F (1,124) = 28.63, p<.01}. Besides, while comparing pre-post

intervention scores, significant reduction in aggression level was also observed in post

intervention scores of participants in experimental group{F (1,124) = 77.03, p<.01, Table 2.9}.

These findings can be explained with the assertions made by attribution and appraisal

theories (e.g., Weiner, 1985, 1986; Lazarus, 1991), Social information processing model (Crick

& Dodge, 1994), and the General aggression model (Anderson & Bushman, 2002). The present

finding i.e. remarkable reduction in aggression as an outcome of modified tendency to attribute

other’s intent as benign or accidental is in line with attributions theories (e.g. Weiner’s 1985,

1986) which state that attribution precedes emotions and emotions further guide our behaviour.

141

By modifying faulty attributional style, social cognitive intervention might have reduced

aggression among adolescents. The content of the intervention was aimed at making

attributional patterns of the aggressive participants more functional as aggressive individuals

have a dysfunctional attributional style (Dodge, 1980, 1986; Weiner, 1986). Attributions which

aggressive individuals make for different events in their lives may serve to justify and

perpetuate their aggression (Bell-Dolan & Anderson, 1999), and thus need to be rectified.

Therefore, by making use of structured activities, the participants in the experimental group

were trained not to over-interpret hostility and to attribute non-hostile intent in negative

ambiguous social situation. Because their attributional biases were rectified, the participants in

the experimental group thus showed less aggression after intervention. Other support for the

findings comes from appraisal theories (e.g. Lazarus, 1991). The training provided in the

intervention might have enabled them to look for all available reasons that might have caused

the negative situation. Appropriate appraisal of the situation might be the reason for reduced

aggression among aggressive adolescents.

The finding can also be explained on the basis of social information processing models

(Dodge, 1986; Crick & Dodge, 1994) which state that inappropriate processing at the

interpretation step may lead to maladaptive behaviour like aggression (Gifford-Smith &

Rabiner, 2004). Because hostile intent of attribution predispose an individual to behave

aggressively (e.g., Beck, 1999; Beck & Fernandez, 1998; Deffenbacher, Dahlen, Lynch,

Morris, & Gowensmith, 2000), it was all the more important to train participants with

appropriate activities to interpret social information in objective and accurate manner. This

could be the reason that aggression level of the participants in experimental group reduced after

social cognitive intervention.

142

Another important explanation of the findings come from the general aggression model

(GAM; Anderson & Bushman, 2002; Anderson & Carnagey, 2004) in which the relationship

between social cognitions and aggression has been highlighted. The intervention in the present

study focused on altering social cognition of the participants in the experimental group. An

attempt was made to modify Hostile thoughts and aggressive scripts of the aggressive

participants with the help of different exercises. This had a significant effect on participants’

aggression level. Since cognitions, affect and behaviour are related (Anderson & Bushman,

2002), modifying faulty cognitions probably led to a change in subsequent inappropriate affect,

arousal and the participants’ level of aggression.

Aggression in the present intervention was targeted with a social cognitive intervention

as the social-psychological (e.g., Anderson & Bushman, 2002; Berkowitz, 1990) and

developmental (e.g., Crick & Dodge, 1994) frameworks for understanding aggression have

primarily adopted a cognitive perspective and it is maintained that if the social cognitive

mechanism can be modified, aggression level can also be reduced (Dodge & Frame, 1982;

Orobio de Castro, Veerman, Koops, Bosch, & Monshouwer, 2002).

Support for the present findings is provided by previous researches carried out with

similar objectives. For example, Guerra and Slaby (1990) designed a treatment program to

correct a variety of cognitive biases linked to aggression, including the hostile attribution bias

that successfully reduced aggression. The Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC)

(Thornton, Craft, Dahlberg, Lynch, & Baer, 2000) identified social-cognitive interventions as

best-practice strategies for reducing and preventing aggressive behaviour in youth.

Another purpose of the present research work was to see whether social cognitive

intervention is equally effective for both males and females. It was hypothesized that both

143

males and females would benefit equally from the intervention because females who have

social information processing biases are at a comparable risk for the same overt aggressive

outcome as their male counterparts (Dodge et al., 2003). The findings reveal (Table 2.10 &

2.13) that both males and females in experimental group showed significantly reduced hostile

attribution of intent, aggression and other aggressive tendencies namely, irritability,

impulsivity, emotional susceptibility and narcissism(only males). Same findings are graphically

presented in Figure 2.2 to 2.7. The finding is in line with previous researches (Fraser et al.,

2005; Lansford et al., 2006; Zelli et al., 1999) which have found that faulty social cognitive

patterns are amenable to change among both males and females. Thus, results are supporting

the applicability of social cognitive intervention to both males and females.

Social cognitive intervention has also been proved to reduce other aggressive tendencies

namely, irritability, impulsivity, emotional susceptibility and narcissism.

Since irritability as a key in the manifestation of aggression, it needs intervention

(Caprara et al., 2007), and hence was targeted for change in the present study. As for other

affective and behavioural responses, irritability may also be the outcome of irrational cognitive

processes. Due to potential link between faulty cognitions and irritability, it was hypothesized

that when faulty cognitions would be modified in social cognitive intervention, the irritability

of aggressive individuals would also be reduced. Findings support this hypothesis as

individuals in experimental group showed significantly less irritability {F (1,124) = 138.078,

p<.01} as compared to those in control group (Table 2.6). Aggressive individuals due to their

irrational cognitions misinterpret social situation as provocative and threatening even in the

absence of any clear cues. This misperception and misinterpretation was when corrected in the

intervention provided might have led aggressive participants in experimental group to start

144

evaluating negative social situation accurately rather than misinterpreting it as provocative.

This transition might be the reason for reduced irritability among aggressive individuals.

This finding can also be explained within the framework of cognitive neo-

associationistic model (Berkowitz, 1993). The intervention focused on rectifying maladaptive

cognitions which are further linked to the affective states of the individuals including irritability

which leads to aggression. This modification at cognitive level might have changed

participants’ affective response resulting in less irritability. As the experience of negative affect

(i.e., irritability) further activates aggression-related memories, emotions, physiological

responses, and motor patterns, thus, it predisposes an individual toward an aggressive response

to perceived provocation (Berkowitz, 1993). In the present social cognitive intervention,

training was provided to aggressive participants to control the initial experience of negative

affect by perceiving ambiguous social situation accurately and non-provocative, which further

prevented activation of other negative associations that leads to aggressive behaviours. With

such training irritability level of aggressive participants reduced. By reducing irritability, the

tendency of aggressive adolescents to retaliate aggressively can also be expected to diminish as

irritability and aggression have been found to be significantly related to each other (Bettencourt

et al., 2006; Caprara et al., 2007).

The intervention had a significant effect on the impulsivity of aggressive participants in

the experimental group. The difference between the impulsivity of the participants in

experimental and control group was statistically significant {F (1,124) = 40.239, p<.01, Table

2.6}. Impulsivity has been described as the tendency to act without thinking or to respond

quickly to a given stimulus, without deliberation or evaluation of consequences (White et al.,

1994). Since impulsive individuals are characterized as thinking less of future consequences

145

before responding to the situation, they end up acting aggressively. Therefore, impulsivity has

been considered a target in this intervention.

Social information processing patterns (Crick & dodge, 1994) also provide explanations

for the findings on impulsivity. Aggressive individuals are less likely to integrate peripheral

social information and do not consider long-term future consequences of their actions in a

particular social situation. Thus when faced with a potentially conflictual situation in real life,

individuals who are impulsive are not likely to consider the unique elements of the social

situation at hand and deliberate on the future consequences of their aggressive retaliation. The

tendency of aggressive individuals to act without considering the unique elements of a social

situation could be due to their default, biased or non-objective way of processing and

responding. Furthermore, once the impulsive adolescent has enacted a response, he may not

possess the effortful control skills necessary to reevaluate or change a response once it has been

enacted (Newman and Wallace, 1993). The present intervention equipped the participants in the

experimental group with appropriate skills to evaluate responses and their consequences and

not to have predetermined mental set while responding in negative encounter and have non-

impulsive and fresh appraisal of the situation. Such kinds of strategies have been proved to

lower down the impulsiveness of aggressive individual in experimental group supporting

assumed hypothesis that individuals in experimental group would score less on impulsivity as

compared to individuals in control group.

The finding can also be explained within the framework of research by Dodge and

Newman (1981) that has high highlighted the role of speed of decision-making in the

association between hostile attribution and aggression. They maintained that aggressive boys

who respond quickly attribute hostile behaviours to others in unwarranted circumstances and

146

aggressive boys who do not respond quickly make decisions that are not biased or different

from those of non-aggressive boys. The irrational quickness and deficit in paying attention to

future consequences was corrected in present intervention at the response evaluation step of

social information processing model, in which participants were trained to consider both

immediate and long-term future consequences of an action while responding in a social

situation specifically in the negative one. Such kind of the training may be the reason for

reduced impulsivity among aggressive adolescents in experimental group. Reduction in

impulsivity may further help aggressive adolescents in controlling their aggressive behaviour.

Emotional susceptibility was also taken as one the dependant measures as it has been

found to be significantly associated with aggression. Emotional susceptibility is the inclination

to experience negative affect and a tendency to become upset and defensive when confronted

with personal attacks and insults, to experience states of sadness, anxiety, frustration, and

anger. If it can somehow be reduced, subsequent reduction in aggressive behaviour may also be

expected (Shoal & Giancola 2003). As irritability, this tendency to experience negative affect

and easily become upset may be related to ones’ maladaptive social information processing as

this internal state is the interaction between cognitions, affect, and arousal (Anderson and

Bushman, 2002). With this assertion, it was hypothesized that individuals in social cognitive

intervention group would score less on emotionally susceptibility as compared to individuals in

control group and the hypothesis was proved{F (1,124) = 92.932, p<.01, Table 2.6}.

Important explanation for reduced emotional susceptibility comes from research by

Graham et al., (1992) in which they maintained that cognitions influence ones’ tendency and

intensity to feel negative emotions, particularly anger which mediates the link between

interpretational biases and aggression. In present research work, participants were informed

147

about their own limitation in negative social situation which may lead to negative behavioural

outcome including aggression. They were trained that it is ones’ sensitivity to provocation and

not always the real provocation that makes some issues either too big or trivial. Sometimes the

negative encounter is not very significant in ones’ life but he/she tend to perceive it otherwise

resulting in negative affective responses, which once activated determine related subsequent

interactions (Berkowitz, 1993; Mischel & Shoda, 1995). Thus, besides altering faulty cognition

of the participants in experimental group, they were made to understand the role of negative

affects or anger in developing and maintaining aggressive behaviour as discussed in Figure D.

such exercises and discussions might have led to reduced emotional susceptibility.

The finding can also be explained with Huesmann’s (1997) argument that negative

affect results from the cognitive evaluation that an external stimulus is provocative and that

emotional susceptibility, marked by the propensity to experience negative affect or to perceive

situations as provoking is associated with higher levels of aggressive behaviour. However, in

the present study, when aggressive individuals’ tendency to evaluate negative social situation

inaccurately was modified, their level of emotional susceptibility reduced that might have

further led to less inclination to behave aggressively(Shoal & Giancola 2003). Thus, social

cognitive intervention has the potential to alter ones pattern of evaluating stimuli present in

negative social situations (Huesmann, 1997).

The last dependent measure assessed in the present study was narcissism. It was

hypothesized that participants in experimental group would show less narcissistic features as

compared to individuals in control group. As depicted in Table 2.6, aggressive individuals in

experimental group demonstrated significant less narcissistic features as compared to their

counterparts in control group{F (1,124) = 7.680, p<.01}. The intervention used in the present

148

study helped the participants to have an accurate evaluation of the self, thus altering their

biases in self perceptions. For instance, in one session, with the help of related activities,

adolescents were trained to be objective in evaluating the situation as well as their self and

informed that if they were biased about their self, had irrational beliefs about certain situations

and the self, their behaviour was bound to be biased and maladaptive. Such kind of training

might have resulted in less narcissistic features among aggressive individuals after the

intervention.

The findings can be explained on the basis of various researches (Mohan et al., 2009;

Garrison, Earls, and Kindlon, 1983; Patterson et al., 1990; David & Kistner, 2000; Baumeister

et al., 1996). Individuals classified as positively biased in their self-perceptions of acceptance

and competences are rated by clinicians and teachers as exhibiting more behaviour problems

(e.g., aggression) than individuals with accurate or negatively biased self-view. If their

inaccurate evaluation of the self are left unattended, individuals’ tendency to have more

narcissistic features and aggressive behaviour increases. Thus, the intervention in the present

study aimed at making these self perceptions more accurate. When the aggressive individuals’

inflated but unstable self-esteem was modified by changing their self evaluation style in social

cognition intervention, it resulted in decreased narcissistic features. As individual’s

relationships with others depend on their abilities to both understand themselves and interpret

the perspectives of others’ accurately (Crick and Dodge, 1996; Ladd & Troop-Gordon, 2003;

Moretti et al., 2001), certain activities were conducted in the present research work, in which

how to interpret the social situation specially negative ambiguous one and how to think

objectively about the self, was discussed. As mentioned earlier, these positive exercises might

have decreased the inflated self esteem of aggressive adolescents in experimental group as

149

proposed in the hypothesis. Reduced narcissistic features may further lessen their tendency to

behave aggressively in negative social situation. This finding can be attributed to the assertion

followed in this intervention that if one wishes to improve his/her social interactions and be

successful in interpersonal relationship, one has to evaluate oneself objectively and modify

ones’ biases in processing information about the social situation and the self.

Aggressive individuals with high narcissism become more aggressive when they are

criticized or rejected by others (e.g., Bushman & Baumeister, 2002; Campbell, et al., 2004;

Stucke & Sporer, 2002). Their tendency to get easily upset in negative social situation, in which

one is criticized or rejected, was reduced as mentioned above. This might have led to reduced

level of narcissism further resulting in less aggression.

In this intervention, guidelines by Kohut (1972) were also followed. Participants’ sense

of grandiosity was first affirmed then they were then guided gently and supportively toward an

acceptance of their limitations and realistic self worth. With this inclusion, the efficacy of

social cognitive intervention in reducing narcissistic features among aggressive adolescents has

been proved.

Hughes et al., (1997) and Webster-Stratton & Lindsay (1999) supported the notion of

considering narcissism in any intervention designed to reduce aggression. They stated that an

unrealistic and inflated view of one's competence and support is a liability rather than an asset

because it may reflect distorted social reasoning processes that interfere with the ability to learn

from past experiences or the motivation to change maladaptive behaviour. Rather than serving

a protective function, unrealistic and inflated self-perceptions increase the risk status for

aggressive children. Therefore, it needs to be incorporated in the intervention meant for

reducing aggression.

150

The findings of the present study were strengthened by the administration of post

experimental questionnaire. The subjects in the experimental group were interviewed after 15

days of termination of the intervention and administered a Post Experimental Questionnaire to

assess the change in perception about their aggressive tendencies. In post experimental

questionnaire, participants in experimental group perceived more change in their approach to

process social information as compared to participants in control group (Table 2.3). For

example, participants in experimental group believed that after intervention, they considered

other possible reasons for negative encounters than blaming other person directly, had realistic

viewpoint of their self, and were more tolerant than before. They verbally reported that the

exercises that were a part of intervention helped them to see things in a new perspective. It

helped them to be relaxed, re-focused & to re-think on certain important issues of social

interactions in general and negative ones in specific. They felt changed. When asked, school

authorities and teacher also reported positive changes in participants’ behaviour. Change in

their perception about their aggressiveness can be attributed to the intervention as in control

group no such change was observed. In the present intervention, providing the participants with

knowledge about aggression and its correlates, role of cognitions in behaviours especially

aggression, concept of intent and ambiguous situation, role of uncontrolled and unintentional

situational factors in negative social ambiguous situation etc. was found to be effective in

changing aggressive adolescents’ perception about aggressive behaviour which reduced their

aggressive behaviour also.

Thus, success of the present social cognitive intervention might be due to close fit

between the content of the intervention and the social deficiencies of the intervention sample

(Coie, 1985).

151

Researchers have been paying attention to develop effective strategies for reducing

excessive displays of aggression in school settings. One familiar strategy used for reducing

aggression in school is to simply suspend or expel highly aggressive students (Reed, 1988).

Although removing troubled and troubling students may provide symptomatic relief for

schools, tragically, such strategies ignore the root causes of aggressive behaviour and banish

those students who are in most need of the benefit of a strong academic foundation, a caring

school environment, and positive peer relations. Thus some sort of in-school intervention is the

need of the hour and the present intervention provides an alternative to traditional solution of

aggressive behaviour problems.

The present research has implications for adolescents’ adaptive social functioning and

adjustment. Humans can train themselves to change or eliminate their faulty cognitions. It has

been said that people have vast untapped resources for actualizing their personal and social

destiny, but because of their dysfunctional social information processing, aggressive

individuals are not able to assess different situations accurately resulting in personal and social

maladjustment. However, by altering this faulty social information processing with the help of

social cognitive intervention, aggressive individuals can be helped to have better adjustment

and healthy interpersonal relationship. To conclude social cognitive intervention used in the

present research resulted in reduced hostile attribution of intent and aggressive tendencies

among aggressive adolescents.