1130204

7
  Wiley and Society for Research in Child Development are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Child Development. http://www.jstor.org Attachment as Regulation: A Commentary Author(s): Sandra Pipp and Robert J. Harmon Source: Child Development, Vol. 58, No. 3 (Jun., 1987), pp. 648-652 Published by: on behalf of the Wiley Society for Research in Child Development Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1130204 Accessed: 10-03-2015 02:40 UTC Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/  info/about/policies/terms.jsp JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Tue, 10 Mar 2015 02:40:05 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Upload: james-gildardo-cuasmayan

Post on 04-Oct-2015

218 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

DESCRIPTION

psychology

TRANSCRIPT

  • Wiley and Society for Research in Child Development are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access toChild Development.

    http://www.jstor.org

    Attachment as Regulation: A Commentary Author(s): Sandra Pipp and Robert J. Harmon Source: Child Development, Vol. 58, No. 3 (Jun., 1987), pp. 648-652Published by: on behalf of the Wiley Society for Research in Child DevelopmentStable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1130204Accessed: 10-03-2015 02:40 UTC

    Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/ info/about/policies/terms.jsp

    JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

    This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Tue, 10 Mar 2015 02:40:05 UTCAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

  • Attachment as Regulation: A Commentary

    Sandra Pipp Lehigh University

    Robert J. Harmon University of Colorado Medical Center

    PIPP, SANDRA, and HARMON, ROBERT J. Attachment as Regulation: A Commentary. CHILD DEVEL- OPMENT, 1987, 58, 648-652. In our commentary, we show how Hofer's work alters the traditional perspective on human attachment in important ways. Hofer describes 2 components of attachment: one that does not develop and one that does. Drawing on work with rodents and primates, Hofer suggests that one component of attachment that does not develop is the sensorimotor or homeostatic regulation between members of a dyad, a stable aspect of all intimate relations throughout the life span. We suggest that this component of attachment in humans may be especially important in the first 6 months of life, a period for which no current theory of attachment provides adequate explana- tion. Second, Hofer has shown that some components of homeostatic regulation change with devel- opment through physiological maturation of organs, internalization of function, and distribution of regulation. We suggest that, in humans, the development of an internal working model can lead to increasing internalization of these functions as well as distribution of regulation among a number of close relationships.

    One major puzzle in attachment theory is how to account for the development of the infant's attachments to others in the first half- year of life (Bretherton, in press). Hofer's con- ceptualization holds the promise of solving this puzzle by suggesting an alternative framework in which to view early attachment (Hofer, 1987, in this issue). In our commen- tary, we will describe how we believe Hofer's approach offers a resolution of the puzzle of early attachment by specifying components of attachment that develop and those that do not.

    The puzzle of how to account for attach- ment in the first 6 months of life was created by Bowlby's (1969) initial description of the four phases of attachment. Bowlby's primary definition of attachment is the infant's ability to seek proximity to a specific caregiver. He described the first two phases of attachment, encompassing the first 6 months of life, as de- voted to the infant's increasing ability to dis- criminate the caregiver from others. These two phases are seen as preparatory to the third phase, proximity seeking, which defines attachment proper and to which most re- search on attachment in humans is devoted.'

    Components necessary to attain the third phase, proximity seeking to a specific care- giver, are thus embedded in the first two phases, discrimination of that specific care- giver.

    This description of the development of attachment, however, fails on at least two grounds. First, it creates a puzzle of determin- ing how an "attached" state develops from a "preattached" state, almost by definition. By specifying attachment as proximity seeking, Bowlby unnecessarily consigns the earlier phases to a preattachment category. Second, by describing the first two phases as being devoted to discrimination between people in the infant's world, Bowlby restricts the de- scription of what does go on between mother and infant.

    Hofer's approach widens the description of attachment to include ways in which mem- bers of a dyad influence each other at the biological level. There are two parts to Hof- er's contribution. Drawing on research with rodents and primates, he offers an exciting ac- count of how infants attach or connect to

    Preparation of this paper was supported by a grant from the MacArthur Foundation Network on the Transition from Infancy to Early Childhood to the second author. Thanks to Linda Crnic and Bruce Pennington for their helpful editorial comments. Requests for reprints should be sent to the first author at the Department of Psychology, Chandler-Ullman Hall 17, Lehigh University, Beth- lehem, PA 18015.

    1 The fourth phase is labeled "goal-corrected partnership" and concerns the mother and in- fant's abilities to work together toward some goal.

    [Child Development, 1987, 58, 648-652. @ 1987 by the Society for Research in Child Development, Inc. All rights reserved. 0009-3920/87/5803-0024$01.00]

    This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Tue, 10 Mar 2015 02:40:05 UTCAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

  • Pipp and Harmon 649 others throughout the life span by presenting evidence emphasizing the importance of biological regulation as a component of all close relationships. One component of attach- ment, then, does not "grow up" but maintains itself at all ages. He hypothesizes that the biological component of attachment serves a similar function for all mammals. A second component of attachment does "grow up" and is transformed with development. It is in this context that the notion of "internal working models," defined as a dynamic representation of the relationship (Bretherton, 1985; Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985), becomes important. We will hypothesize that attachment relations which change with development do so by vir- tue of changes in structure of the internal working model. We will treat each point in turn.

    Attachment Functions That Do Not Grow Up

    Bowlby (1969, 1973) suggested that at- tachment systems serve to maintain proximity seeking. Yet, attachment is something much more profound than simple proximity seek- ing. As Bretherton (1985) pointed out, an ob- server external to the behaviors will perceive the infant's attempts to stay close to the care- giver. From the perspective of the organism involved in the behaviors, proximity seeking represents a feeling of security when close to the caregiver. In the very young, "preat- tached" organism, felt security may result from biological regulation, or homeostasis, and Hofer's central finding is that mother- infant interactions in lower animals serve physiological regulatory functions for the in- fant.

    Bowlby (1973) distinguished between an "inner ring" that maintains physiological ho- meostasis and the "outer ring" of life-main- taining systems between the organism and the environment. Hofer's work with infant rats suggests that the "inner ring" that main- tains physiological homeostasis is the "outer ring." As Hofer points out, the prolongation of immaturity in altricial species allows the in- fant to maintain an open homeostatic system, with the regulation of the "inner core" or "in- ternal milieu" being delegated to the interac- tions between self and caregiver. One ques- tion this approach raises is whether the relation between delegated homeostasis and internal homeostasis changes with develop- ment.

    Hofer (1984; 1987, in this issue) suggests that biological homeostasis appears to be a function of all close relationships, not just be-

    tween mother and infant. That is, attachment serves the function of homeostatic regulation throughout the life span. For example, behav- ioral and physiological responses to bereave- ment in adult humans bear striking similarity to those obtained during infants' separation from their caregiver. In making this point, Hofer underscores the "connectedness" of our species. At the most basic level, the biological one, we are connected and embed- ded in each others' biological rhythms, not only in utero, at birth, or in early infancy, but throughout the life span.

    There may be different forms of biolog- ical connectedness, however. Hofer presents intriguing evidence suggesting that the acute, or protest, and chronic, or despair, responses to separation from this biological connect- edness are characterized by different struc- tures. The structure revealed by the im- mediate response to separation is additive, in that each additional cue that represents char- acteristics of the mother decreases protest be- havior in a linear fashion. Thus, littermates, with the greatest number of cues, reduce vocalization the most. This finding finds an analogue in humans. Siblings have been re- ported to attenuate separation distress and to use each other as secure bases (Heinicke & Westheimer, 1966).

    "Despair," the response to long-term separation, is a result of severing of biological homeostasis of the infant's internal function- ing. Specifically, Hofer suggests that the "slow developing changes in the rat are clearly the result of . . . withdrawal of the regulators previously supplied by the mother- infant interaction" (p. 645). The mechanisms underlying behaviors emerging as a function of longer-term separations appear to be rela- tively independent of each other, with the mother providing both "up and down" regu- lation of independent neurochemical and be- havioral systems.

    One question not addressed by Hofer is how each species recovers homeostatic regu- lation after separation. In primates, for ex- ample, some physiological functions, most notably heart rate and body temperature, approached baseline during longer phases of separation (Reite, Short, Seiler, & Pauley, 1981). When homeostatic regulation is sev- ered by separation, how does each species re- cover physiological regulation in time, and what are the mechanisms of recovery? Find- ings of recovery-or lack of recovery-of ho- meostasis might enable us to determine whether there are physiological concomitants

    This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Tue, 10 Mar 2015 02:40:05 UTCAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

  • 650 Child Development that differentiate between the phases of de- spair and detachment.

    Attachment Functions That Do Grow Up

    Not only does attachment serve homeo- static regulatory functions throughout the life span, but the mechanisms of regulation may change with development. We believe that his speculations about the transformation in regulation are quite exciting, especially in the light of the concept of "internal working mod- els."

    Internal working models are defined as dynamic representations of the relationship (Bowlby, 1969, 1973, 1980; Bretherton, 1985; Main et al., 1985). A major focus of this con- cept is the infant's affective and cognitive rep- resentational schemas of the attachment rela- tionship. Main et al. (1985) hypothesized that the internal working model changes as cogni- tive and affective development increases. While they focused on representational thought, sensorimotor components of the in- ternal working model are possible, and, in- deed, Main et al. speculated that an internal working model first appears at 3 to 4 months of life in the human.

    The most basic level of an internal work- ing model may be the sensorimotor one that concerns homeostatic regulation. Sander (1975), for example, specifies regulation as the first issue to be resolved between infant and mother. Those aspects of the internal working model that are most resistant to change may be those that relate to sensorimotor codings of the reliability of homeostatic regulation. At the most basic, "security of attachment" re- lates to a physiological coding that the uni- verse is benign and need-satisfying, that is, homeostatic disruptions will be set right.'

    Maternal upset over inability to restore homeostatic regulation may have a biological basis. Hofer points out that the intricate "dance" or "attunement" (Stern, 1984) be- tween mother and infant may be recast as biological symbiosis in which both partners are dependent on the other to carry out their role. Hofer's major contribution to this notion of symbiosis is to underscore the importance of the infant's behavior for the mother's sym- biotic dependence on her infant for mainte- nance of her mothering role. At the biological level, then, we return to the notion of the im- portance of the child on the parenting func- tions of the caregiver (Bell, 1968).

    Within this framework, sensorimotor components of the internal working model

    may account for many of the puzzles of "un- conscious" or "primary process" influences on later behavior. Those aspects of the inter- nal working model that are most resistant to change may be those sensorimotor codings about the reliability of the attachment figure for meeting the self's needs. These codings are relatively resistant to change because they are sensorimotor and not under conscious control. Some tentative support for this posi- tion is obtained from studies showing that the original Ainsworth classification derived for 1-year-old infants' security of attachment is also relevant for classification at older ages, such as preschool (Main et al., 1985), adoles- cence (Kobak & Sceery, in press) and adult- hood (Hazan & Shaver, in press). Stronger evidence for this claim will require longitudi- nal research examining the stability of attach- ment classification throughout the life span.

    One of the most fascinating aspects of Hofer's article is his speculation on the pro- cess by which biological regulation may be- come internalized. Internalization of biolog- ical regulation in all probability requires little cognitive ability in the rat. In humans, how- ever, internalization of biological regulation should be a function of (1) maturational changes in organ systems, (2) internalization of affective and cognitive components of the relationship (internal working models), and (3) the lifelong influence of social relation- ships. While point number 1 is beyond the scope of this paper, points 2 and 3 will be considered in turn.

    Hofer's appeal to increased higher-order associative and symbolic functions to control homeostatic functioning may be one compo- nent of the internal working model. One way in which this may occur is through the pro- cess broadly described by neo-Piagetians of a social cognitive orientation (Case, Hayward, Lewis, & Hurst, in press; Fischer & Pipp, 1984). A child's affective and cognitive under- standings of relationships will change in structure with development, and this devel- opment will influence a person's biological responses to separation. To make an obvious point, an explanation to a 12-month-old that Mommy will be right back will have less in- fluence on physiological responses than it will have on a 4-year-old, presumably be- cause the older child has a representational structure that allows for a more stable under- standing of the reliability of the mother.

    Equally interesting is Hofer's hypothesis that the shift from external to internal regula- tion may also involve differences in response to social interactions, specifically, a move-

    This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Tue, 10 Mar 2015 02:40:05 UTCAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

  • Pipp and Harmon 651

    ment "from a dependence on interaction with the mother to a more flexible dependence on a variety of different social interactions" (p. 20). That is, increased internalization may come about not only by increased affective and cognitive structures but by distributing biological homeostasis and "attachments" to more than one close relationship. Or, greater predictability in a greater number of situa- tions and/or people will be obtained when in- fants or children can generate larger numbers of appropriately specific internal working models.

    This does not mean that the internal working model of each relationship must fol- low the same classification. One of the puz- zles of data deriving from attachment theory is that an infant's attachment to one caregiver, for example, the mother, is not necessarily predictive of other attachment relationships, for example, the father (Lamb, 1977, 1978; Main et al., 1985). While an infant may gener- alize attachment behavior from a good mother who is no longer available to another individ- ual, it is also true that security of attachment changes as a function of the quality of interac- tions and so may vary as a function of the life stress of the family or with different relation- ships. And, it should not be presumed that multiple problematic attachment relation- ships predispose a child to be unable to de- velop more secure relationships in appropri- ate parenting situations (Harmon, Wagonfeld, & Emde, 1982).

    In summary, we have suggested that Hof- er's contribution alters the traditional per- spective on attachment in profound ways. By suggesting a language to understand attach- ment in the first 6 months of life, we no longer must puzzle how the "preattached" infant be- comes "attached." Instead, Hofer's emphasis on biological connectedness suggests an im- portant component of attachment in the first 6 months of life: Infant and mother are con- nected at the level of biological symbiosis. The symbiosis serves an attachment function that manifests itself as proximity seeking after approximately 6 months.

    Our treatment of Hofer suggests that at- tachment can be conceptualized in terms of those components that develop and those that do not. In this, we follow the tradition of Freud (1911/1958) and Piaget (1970), who suggest that some structures of thought maintain themselves throughout develop- ment (e.g., primary process and sensorimotor thought), while others develop from the trans- formation of the earliest structures (e.g., sec- ondary process and operational thought). In

    attachment theory, it may be that throughout the life span we are biologically connected to those with whom we have close relationships. Sensorimotor or homeostatic regulation be- tween members of a dyad is a stable aspect of all intimate relationships throughout the life span. This component does not develop, but instead may serve as a sensorimotor version of the internal working model and may account for the unconscious components of attach- ment relationships (Bretherton, in press; Fischer & Pipp, 1984; Main et al., 1985). In addition, some components of homeostatic regulation change with development through physiological maturation of organs and devel- opment of the internal working model. The development of the internal working model leads to increasing internalization of these functions as well as distribution of regulation among a number of close relationships. References Bell, R. Q. (1968). A reinterpretation of the direc-

    tion of effects in studies of socialization. Psy- chological Review, 75, 63-72.

    Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. At- tachment. New York: Basic.

    Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Sep- aration. New York: Basic.

    Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Loss. New York: Basic.

    Bretherton, I. (1985). Attachment theory: Retro- spect and prospect. In I. Bretherton & E. Wa- ters (Eds.), Growing points of attachment the- ory and research. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50(1-2, Serial No. 209).

    Bretherton, I. (in press). New perspectives on at- tachment relations: Security, communication and internal working models. In J. Osofsky (Ed.), Handbook of infant psychology. New York: Wiley.

    Case, R., Hayward, S., Lewis, M., & Hurst, P. (in press). Toward a neo-Piagetian theory of affec- tive and cognitive development. Develop- mental Review.

    Fischer, K. W., & Pipp, S. L. (1984). The develop- ment of structures of unconscious thought. In D. Meichenbaum & K. Bowers (Eds.), The un- conscious reconsidered (pp. 88-148). New York: Wiley.

    Freud, S. (1958). Formulation on the two principles of mental functioning. The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 12). London: Hogarth. (Original work published 1911)

    Harmon, R. J., Wagonfeld, S., & Emde, R. N. (1982). Anaclitic depression: A follow-up from infancy to puberty. The Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 37.

    This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Tue, 10 Mar 2015 02:40:05 UTCAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

  • 652 Child Development Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (in press). Romantic love

    conceptualized as an attachment process. Jour- nal of Personality and Social Psychology.

    Heinicke, C. M., & Westheimer, I. J. (1966). Brief separations. New York: International Univer- sities Press.

    Hofer, M. A. (1984). Relationships as regulators: A psychobiologic perspective on bereavement. Psychosomatic Medicine, 46, 183-197.

    Hofer, M. A. (1987). Early social relationships: A psychobiologist's view. Child Development, 58, 633-647.

    Kobak, R., & Sceery, A. (in press). Attachment in adolescence: Working models, affect regula- tion and perceptions of self and others. Child Development.

    Lamb, M. E. (1977). The development of mother- infant and father-infant attachments in the sec- ond year of life. Developmental Psychology, 13, 637-648.

    Lamb, M. E. (1978). Qualitative aspects of mother- and father-infant attachments. Infant Behavior and Development, 1, 265-275.

    Main, M., Kaplan, K., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, childhood and adulthood: A move to the level of representation. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), Growing points of attach- ment theory and research, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50(1-2, Serial No. 209).

    Piaget, J. (1970). Piaget's theory. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael's manual of child psychol- ogy (pp. 703-732). New York: Wiley.

    Reite, M., Short, R., Seiler, C., & Pauley, J. D. (1981). Attachment, loss, and depression. Jour- nal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 22, 141-169.

    Sander, L. W. (1975). Infant and caretaking environ- ment: Investigation and conceptualization of adaptive behavior in systems of increas- ing complexity. In E. J. Anthony (Ed.), Ex- plorations in child psychiatry. New York: Plenum.

    Stem, D. N. (1984). Affect attunement. In J. D. Call, E. Galenson, & R. L. Tyson (Eds.), Frontiers in infant psychiatry (pp. 3-14). New York: Basic.

    This content downloaded from 181.118.153.129 on Tue, 10 Mar 2015 02:40:05 UTCAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    Article Contentsp. [648]p. 649p. 650p. 651p. 652

    Issue Table of ContentsChild Development, Vol. 58, No. 3, Jun., 1987Front MatterSpecial Section -- Developmental Psychology and the Neurosciences: Building a BridgeDevelopmental Psychology and the Neurosciences: An Introduction [pp. 533 - 538]Experience and Brain Development [pp. 539 - 559]New Directions in the Study of Early Experience [pp. 560 - 567]Basic Concepts of CNS Development [pp. 568 - 595]Some Additional Influences Shaping the Development of Behavior [pp. 596 - 600]Development of Cortical Circuitry and Cognitive Function [pp. 601 - 622]Relations between Brain and Cognitive Development [pp. 623 - 632]Early Social Relationships: A Psychobiologist's View [pp. 633 - 647]Attachment as Regulation: A Commentary [pp. 648 - 652]Neurobiological Aspects of Language in Children [pp. 653 - 688]Reconciling Stable Asymmetry with Recovery of Function: An Adaptive Systems Perspective on Functional Plasticity [pp. 689 - 697]Neuronal Plasticity in the Mammalian Brain: Relevance to Behavioral Learning and Memory [pp. 698 - 712]Memory Development and Neurophysiology: Accomplishments and Limitations [pp. 713 - 717]

    Left-Hemisphere Language Specialization: Consistency in Hand Preference and Sex Differences [pp. 718 - 724]Developmental Patterns of Spatial Ability: An Early Sex Difference [pp. 725 - 740]Organizational Processes in Infants' Perception of Auditory Patterns [pp. 741 - 749]Categorization and Memory Retrieval by Three-Month-Olds [pp. 750 - 767]Behavior of Young Children with Down Syndrome before the Mirror: Exploration [pp. 768 - 778]Developmental Outcome of Preterm Infants with Intraventricular Hemorrhage at One and Two Years of Age [pp. 779 - 786]Temperament and Attachment Security in the Strange Situation: An Empirical Rapprochement [pp. 787 - 795]Short-Term Stability of Aggregated and Nonaggregated Measures of Parental Behavior [pp. 796 - 797]Attentional Inertia Reduces Distractibility during Young Children's TV Viewing [pp. 798 - 806]Memory for Pictures: A Life-Span Study of the Role of Visual Detail [pp. 807 - 815]Peer Perceptions of Hyperactivity and Medication Effects [pp. 816 - 828]Validity of Self-Report Measures of Girls' Pubertal Status [pp. 829 - 841]Moral Stages and Moral Orientations in Real-Life and Hypothetical Dilemmas [pp. 842 - 858]Childhood Correlates of Adult Ego Development [pp. 859 - 869]ReviewChildren and Metaphors [pp. 870 - 885]

    Back Matter