salt and blood pressure: the triangular hypothesis

4
SALT ANJ BLOOD PRESSURE: THE TRIANGULAR HYPOTHESIS D.A. Booth and A.L. Thompson, Department of Psychology, University of Birmingham,Birmingham, B15 2TT, U.K. R. Shepherd and D.G. Land, AFRC Institute for Food Research, Norwich Laboratory and R.P. Griffiths, RHM Research Ltd., High Wycombe. ABSTRACT If anunselected sample includes individuals whose blood pressure is sensitive to their salt intake and individuals whose blood pressure is not sensitive, then the superposition of these two sub-populations in a scatterplot of individuals' blood pressures against their salt intakes could give a triangular distribution. The non-correlation in the insensitives would obscure the correlation expected in the sensitives. This hypothesis justifies truncation of such data to test for correlation between blood pressure and salt intake among only the individuals in the higher range of blood pressures observed. No criterion of salt sensitivity is needed. The analysis should succeed if salt intake makes a major contribution to hypertension and would be improved if other putative causes were factored out. INTRODUCTION Correlations between salt intake (SI) and blood pressure (BP) are evident across populations but are seldom observed within populations (1). We suggest that one reason for this may well be that the analyses have not been appropriate to current working hypotheses conerning 'the effects of salt on the development of hypertension, for these imply a triangular distribution of SI/BP data in an unselected sample. THE HYPOTHESIS It is generally accepted that only some in a population are likely to be susceptible to chronically hypertensive effects of high salt intake (2). Various 325

Upload: rp

Post on 05-Jan-2017

213 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

SALT ANJ BLOOD PRESSURE: THE TRIANGULAR HYPOTHESIS

D.A. Booth and A.L. Thompson, Department of Psychology, University of Birmingham,Birmingham, B15 2TT, U.K.

R. Shepherd and D.G. Land, AFRC Institute for Food Research, Norwich Laboratory

and R.P. Griffiths, RHM Research Ltd., High Wycombe.

ABSTRACT

If anunselected sample includes individuals whose blood pressure is sensitive to their salt intake and individuals whose blood pressure is not sensitive, then the superposition of these two sub-populations in a scatterplot of individuals' blood pressures against their salt intakes could give a triangular distribution. The non-correlation in the insensitives would obscure the correlation expected in the sensitives. This hypothesis justifies truncation of such data to test for correlation between blood pressure and salt intake among only the individuals in the higher range of blood pressures observed. No criterion of salt sensitivity is needed. The analysis should succeed if salt intake makes a major contribution to hypertension and would be improved if other putative causes were factored out.

INTRODUCTION

Correlations between salt intake (SI) and blood pressure (BP) are evident across populations but are seldom observed within populations (1). We suggest that

one reason for this may well be that the analyses have not been appropriate to current working hypotheses conerning 'the effects of salt on the development of hypertension, for these imply a triangular distribution of SI/BP data in

an unselected sample.

THE HYPOTHESIS

It is generally accepted that only some in a population are likely to be susceptible to chronically hypertensive effects of high salt intake (2). Various

325

rates of increase in BP with age among salt-susceptible individuals are thought to yield the well-established population rise of average BP with age (3), which in turn yields the higher average BP in populations with higher average SI, when general populations with similar age profiles are compared.

It follows from such hypotheses that BP is most unlikely to correlate substantially with SI across a non- clinical sample - especially if young. The lack of correlation hypothesised in the insusceptible will mask any correlation in the susceptible. Nonetheless, high BPS attributable to and correlated with chronically high SIs,

when superimposed on a lower range of BPS unrelated to SI (high or low) could yield a triangular scatterplot of BPS against SIs. That is, the insusceptibles would largely provide a base to the triangle, consisting of the unsloping (non-correlated) distribution of their blood pressures in the normal range across all salt intakes. The susceptibles would provide a right-skewed apex to the triangle, consisting of the partial correlation of the highest pressures with higher intakes.

Of course, if a criterion of susceptibility (such as family history) is known for members of the sample, we can try to correlate BP with SI in putative susceptibles, separated out from putative insusceptibles. However, it follows from the triangular implication of currently accepted theory that we can test the hypothesis of a correlation between BP and SI with individual data, even when we lack any information on which to identify people who may be susceptible to hypertensive action of high salt

intake. Such data can legitimately be truncated, omitting people in the lower range of BPS. If salt is an important

influences on blood pressure, a BP/S1 correlation should be more readily detected amongst the higher blood

pressures than over the whole sample.

Such an outcome is unlikely 'to be a statistical artefact, for elimination of data at an extreme (in this case the lowest BPS) is bound generally to lower the value of the correlation coefficient where there is a

homogeneous bivariate relationship. If, however, there is a relationship that has been masked at one extreme, then truncating the data at that extreme should increase the correlation value. Indeed, if any other information

relevant to BP is available, then a relationship between SI and BP may be tested with greater sensitivity, either by multivariate analysis of the truncated sample or by simple correlation among the people with the higher blood pressures after further elimination of data to allow for the other influence(s) on BP. An improved correlation on still fewer data would be more evidence that an underlying relationship exists.

326

Pilot tests of the triangular analysis ---

This triangular analysis has proved productive in the

two small studies to which we have been able to apply it, one on 30 healthy young men (4) and the other on 36

healthy women. Higher BPS correlated with SI, even though no-one in either sample had especially high blood

pressure.

We averaged the last two of three seated BP measurements by random-zero sphygmomanometer, taking disappearance and appearance of Korotkoff sounds as DBP

and SBP respectively, on one occasion in the men and on

two separate occasions in the women. Salt intake was

estimated by validated questionnaire (5) in the men and by urinary Na over 7 days in the women. Two food-salt

preferences, salting habits, opinions on salt, body

weight, height and smoking were also assessed.

In the 30 men, only the amount of salt preferred in tomato soup was related to total SI in discriminant analysis (similarly to other recent observations), and

only total salt intake predicted BP (SBP only) in multiple

regression. For a SI range of 9.1 - 14.7 g NaCl/day and SBPS and DBPs up to 124 and 92 mm Hg respectively, the simple correlations between SI and BP were, for SBP, r - 0.46 (p < 0.05), DBP r = 0.02, and average BP r - 0.08.

The significant correlation appeared not to be an accidental feature but an attribute of the sample's homogeneity - for, when12 smokers were excluded on the grounds that smoking may decrease BP (6), the SI/SBP r rose to 0.69 (p < 0.05). Then, truncating the base of the

theoretical SI/BP triangle, the 8 non-smokers at or above

SBP of 115 mm Hg gave a still higher correlation coefficient of 0.84 (p < 0.01).

Truncation in the female sample gave results that weigh strongly against those effects in the male sample being fortuitous to that set of data. In a BP range of 95/54 - 169/94 and an intake range of 2.5 NaCl/day,

- 11.4 g an overall SI/SBP correlation of 0.29 rose to

0.54 (p < 0.05) in the 18 women having the higher range of SBPs. Also, the overall SI/DBP r = 0.22 rose to 0.47 (p < 0.06) in the higher half of the DBP range.

CONCLUSION

We therefore suggest that this method of data analysis be applied to healthy and clinical samples, both homogeneous and inhomogeneous. A variety of experience with analysis for a triangular distribution in the scatterplot between salt intake and blood pressure would help ascertain to what extent the puzzling discrepancy between within- and between-population correlations arises

327

\IH B

from failure to direct the statistical testing to the relevant hypothesis.

REFERENCES

Simpson FO. Salt and hypertension: a sceptical review of the evidence. Clin. Sci. 57: 463S-480s.

Dahl LK. Salt and hypertension. Am. J. Clin. Nutr.

25: 231-244, 1979.

Tobian L. The relationship of salt to hypertension. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 32: 2739-2748, 1979.

Booth DA, Thompson AL, Shahedian B. A robust, brief measure of an individual's most preferred level of

salt in an ordinary foodstuff. Appetite 4: 301-312,

1983:

Shepherd R, Farleigh CA, Land DG. Estimation of salt intake by questionnaire. Appetite 6: 219-233, 1985.

US DHEW. The health consequences of smoking: a

report of the Surgeon-General. US Government

Printing Office, 1972.

328