psyc215 chapter 3 notes definitions - amazon...

8
PSYC215 Chapter 3 Notes Definitions: Priming: Activating particular associations in memory. Belief Perseverance: Persistence of one’s initial conceptions, as when the basis for one’s belief is discredited but an explanation of why the belief might be true survives. Misinformation Effect: Incorporating “misinformation” into one’s memory of the event, after witnessing an event and receiving misleading information about it. Overconfidence Phenomenon: The tendency to be more confident than correct-to overestimate the Research reveals the extent to which our assumptions and prejudgments can bias out perceptions, interpretations, and recall. The first group of experiments examine how predispositions and prejudgments affect how we perceive and interpret information. The second group plants a judgment in people’s minds after they have been given information to study how after-the-fact ideas bias recall. The overarching point: We respond not to reality as it is but to reality as we construe it. Our memory system is a web of associations, and priming is the awakening or activating of certain associations. Priming experiments reveal how one thought, even without awareness, can influence another thought, or even an action.

Upload: hoangtram

Post on 06-Jun-2018

213 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: PSYC215 Chapter 3 Notes Definitions - Amazon S3s3.amazonaws.com/prealliance_oneclass_sample/Dq6Xxbp2a6.pdf · PSYC215 Chapter 3 Notes Definitions: ... Attribution Theory: ... Our

PSYC215 Chapter 3 Notes

Definitions:

Priming: Activating particular associations in memory.

Belief Perseverance: Persistence of one’s initial conceptions, as when the basis for one’s belief is discredited but an explanation of why the belief might be true survives.

Misinformation Effect: Incorporating “misinformation” into one’s memory of the event, after witnessing an event and receiving misleading information about it.

Overconfidence Phenomenon: The tendency to be more confident than correct-to overestimate the accuracy of one’s beliefs.

Confirmation Bias: A tendency to search for information that confirms one’s preconceptions.

Heuristics: A thinking strategy that enables quick, efficient judgments.

Representativeness Heuristic: The tendency to presume, sometimes despite contrary offs, that someone or something belongs to a particular group if resembling (representing) a typical member.

Available Heuristics: A cognitive rule that judges the likelihood of things in terms of their availability in memory. If instances of something come readily to mind, we presume it to be commonplace.

Illusory Correlation: Perception of a relationship where none exists, or perception of a stronger relationship than actually exists.

Illusion of Control: Perception of uncontrollable events as subject to one’s control or as more controllable than they are.

Regression Toward The Average: The statistical tendency for extreme scores or extreme behaviour to return toward ones average.

Misattribution: Mistakenly attributing a behaviour to the wrong cause.

Attribution Theory: The theory of how people explain others’ behaviour-for example, by attributing it either to internal dispositions (enduring traits, motives, and attitudes) or to external situations.

Dispositional Attribution: Attributing behaviour to the person’s disposition and traits.

Situational Attribution: Attributing behaviour to the environment.

Fundamental Attribution Error: The tendency for observers to underestimate situational influences and overestimate dispositional influences on others’ behaviour. (Also called correspondence bias, because we so often see behaviour as corresponding to a disposition.)

Self-awareness: A self-conscious state in which attention focuses on oneself. It makes people more sensitive to their own attitudes and dispositions.

Self-fulfilling Prophecy: A belief that leads to its own fulfillment.

Chapter Notes:

Research reveals the extent to which our assumptions and prejudgments can bias out perceptions, interpretations, and recall.

The first group of experiments examine how predispositions and prejudgments affect how we perceive and interpret information. The second group plants a judgment in people’s minds after they have been given information to study how after-the-fact ideas bias recall. The overarching point: We respond not to reality as it is but to reality as we construe it.

Our memory system is a web of associations, and priming is the awakening or activating of certain associations. Priming experiments reveal how one thought, even without awareness, can influence another thought, or even an action.

Page 2: PSYC215 Chapter 3 Notes Definitions - Amazon S3s3.amazonaws.com/prealliance_oneclass_sample/Dq6Xxbp2a6.pdf · PSYC215 Chapter 3 Notes Definitions: ... Attribution Theory: ... Our

Priming effects surface even when the stimuli are presented subliminally-too briefly to be perceived consciously.

Our first impression of one another are more often right than wrong, and the better we know people, the more accurately we can read their minds and feelings.

When social information is subject to multiple interpretations, preconceptions matter.

Ways of thinking or schemas guide not only our interpretations of our self, but also our understanding of others.

People everywhere perceive media and mediators as biased against their position. “There is no subject about which people are less objective than objectivity.”

People’s perceptions of bias can be used to assess their attitudes. Tell me where you see bias, and I will see your attitudes.

Both proponents and opponents of a topic show that the two sides of an identical body of mixed evidence had not lessened their disagreement, but increased it.

The “Kulechov effect” skilfully guided viewers’ inferences by manipulating their assumptions.

Spontaneous trait transference: If we go around talking about others being gossipy, people may then unconsciously associate “gossip” with us. Call someone a jerk and folks may later construe you as one.

There is an objective reality out there, but we view it through the spectacles of our beliefs, attitudes, and values. This is one reason our beliefs and schemas are so important; they shape our interpretation of everything else.

Belief perseverance shows that beliefs can take on a life of their own and survive the discrediting of the evidence that inspired them.

The more we examine our theories and explain how they might be true, the more closed we become to information that challenges our belief.

The remedy of belief perseverance is to simply explain the opposite. Indeed, explaining any alternative outcome, not just the opposite, drives people to ponder various possibilities.

In constructing memories, we reconstruct our distant past by using our current feelings and expectations to combine fragments of information. In its search for truth, the mind sometimes constructs a falsehood.

People will incorporate misleading information into their memories when they receive the misleading information about it.

People whose attitudes have changed often insist that they have always felt much as they now feel. George Vaillant notes: “It is all too common for caterpillars to become butterflies and then to maintain that in their youth they had been little butterflies. Maturation makes liars of us all.”

The construction of positive memories does brighten our recollections. We tend to later recall experiences as even more fond, minimizing the unpleasant or boring aspects and remembering the high points.

With any positive experience, some of the pleasure resides in the anticipation, some in the actual experience, and some in the rosy retrospection.

We also revise our recollections of other people as our relationships with them change. University students in steady relationships rated their partners. Two months later, those more in love saw their partner as love at first sight while those who broke up saw their partners originally as selfish and bad-tempered.

In reconstructing past behaviour, it is necessary for humans to remember that events happened in a desired manner. We tend to underreport bad behaviours and overreport good behaviours.

Page 3: PSYC215 Chapter 3 Notes Definitions - Amazon S3s3.amazonaws.com/prealliance_oneclass_sample/Dq6Xxbp2a6.pdf · PSYC215 Chapter 3 Notes Definitions: ... Attribution Theory: ... Our

While our cognitive systems process a vast amount of information efficiently and automatically, our adaptive efficiency has a trade-off; as we interpret our experiences and construct memories, our automatic intuitions often err.

We are unaware of our flaws and the “intellectual conceit” evident in judgments of past knowledge (“I knew it all along”) extends to estimates of current knowledge and predictions of future behaviour and this tends to be positive.

The most confident people were most likely to be overconfident. Studies reveal a similar correlation between self-confidence and accuracy in discerning whether someone is telling the truth.

Ironically, incompetence feeds overconfidence. Students who score at the bottom on tests of grammar, humour, and logic are most prone to overestimating their gifts as such.

Our ignorance of our ignorance sustains our self-confidence. In follow up studies, the “ignorance of one’s incompetence” occurs mostly on relatively easy-seeming tasks, but on hard tasks, poor performers more often appreciate their lack of skill.

Dunning concludes: “what others see in us…tends to be more highly correlated with objective outcomes than what we see in ourselves.”

People may often give too much weight to their current intentions when predicting their future behaviour.

People often tend to recall their mistaken judgments as times when they were almost right.

In confirmation bias, people tend to not seek information that might disprove what they believe. We are eager to verify our beliefs but less inclined to seek evidence that might disprove them.

Out preference for confirming information helps explain why our self-images are so remarkably stable. William Swann and Stephen Read discovered that students see, elicit, and recall feedback that confirms their beliefs about themselves.

People seek friends and spouse those who bolster their own self views-even if the think poorly of themselves. This self-verification to how someone with a domineering self-image might behave at a party.

You need to be careful about other peoples’ dogmatic statements. Even when people seem sure they are right, they may be wrong. Confidence and competence need not coincide.

Three techniques to reduce the overconfidence bias includes: o Prompt feedback from experts. o To reduce “planning fallacy” overconfidence, people can be asked to “unpack” a task-to

break it into its subcomponents-and estimate the time required for each. When people think about why an idea might be true, it begins to seem true.

o Get people to think of one good reason why their judgments might be wrong: force them to consider disconfirming information.

We should be careful not to undermine people’s self-confidence to a point where they spend too much time in self-analysis or where self-doubts begin to cripple decisiveness. In times when their wisdom is needed, those lacking self-confidence may shrink from speaking up or making tough decisions.

Overconfidence can cost use, but realistic self-confidence is adaptive.

Our cognitive system specializes in mental shortcuts, which we use to form impressions, make judgments, and invent explanations with lightning speed. Utilization of heuristics promotes our survival. The biological purpose of thinking is less to make us right than to keep us alive. However, in some situations, haste makes errors.

Page 4: PSYC215 Chapter 3 Notes Definitions - Amazon S3s3.amazonaws.com/prealliance_oneclass_sample/Dq6Xxbp2a6.pdf · PSYC215 Chapter 3 Notes Definitions: ... Attribution Theory: ... Our

Consider the question: Do more people live in Iraq or in Tanzania. Based on examples readily available in our memory-as Iraqis tend to be-then we presume that the event is commonplace. This cognitive rule is called available heuristic.

Sometimes availability heuristic deludes us. The more absorbed and “transported” the reader (“I could easily picture the events”), the more the story affects the reader’s later beliefs.

One use of availability heuristics highlights a basic principle of social thinking: People are slow to deduce particular instances from a general truth, but they are remarkably quick to infer general truth from a vivid instance.

The availability heuristic explains why powerful anecdotes are often more compelling than statistical information and why perceived risk is therefore often badly out of join with real risks.

Easily imagined (cognitively available) events also influence our experiences of guilt, regret, frustration, and relief. Imagining worse alternatives helps us feel better. Imagining better alternatives, and pondering what we might do differently next time, helps us prepare to do better in the future.

The more significant the event, the more intense the counterfactual thinking.

It’s easy to see a correlation where none exists. When we expect significant relationships, we easily associate random events, perceiving an illusory correlation.

If we believe a correlation exists, we are more likely to notice and recall confirming instances. If we believe the premonitions correlate with events, we notice and remember the joint occurrence of the premonition and the events later occurrence. We seldom notice or remember all the times unusual events do not coincide.

Experiments on gambling illustrate the illusion of control, where more than 50 experiments have consistently found people acting as if they can predict or control chance events.

Gamblers tend to attribute wins to their skill and foresight. Losses become “near misses” or “flukes” –perhaps (for the sports gambler) a bad call by the referee or a freakish bounce of the ball.

We fail to recognize the statistical phenomenon of regression toward the average. Because exam scores fluctuate partly by chance, most students who get extremely high scores on an exam will get lower scores on the next exam.

Experience has taught us that when everything is going great, something will go wrong, and that when life is dealing us terrible blows, we can usually look forward to things getting better. Often, though, we fail to recognize this regression effect.

Nature operates in such a way that we often feel punished for rewarding others and rewarded for punishing them. In actuality, as every student of psychology knows, positive reinforcement for doing things right is usually more effective and has fewer negative side effects.

Social judgment involves efficient, though fallible, information processing. It also involves our feelings: our moods infuse out judgments.

Unhappy people-especially those bereaved or depressed-tend to be more self-focused and brooding. A depressed mood motivates intense thinking-a search for information that makes one’s environment more understandable and controllable.

Happy people, by contrast, are more trusting, more loving, more responsive. In a happy mood, the world seems friendlier; decisions are easier, good news more readily comes to mind.

Let the mood turn gloomy, however, and thoughts switch onto a different track. Off comes the rose-coloured glasses; on come the dark glasses. Bad mood primes our recollections of negative events. Our relationships seem to sour. Our self-image takes a dive. Our hopes for the future dim. Other people’s behaviour seems more sinister.

Page 5: PSYC215 Chapter 3 Notes Definitions - Amazon S3s3.amazonaws.com/prealliance_oneclass_sample/Dq6Xxbp2a6.pdf · PSYC215 Chapter 3 Notes Definitions: ... Attribution Theory: ... Our

Michael Ross and Garth Fletcher notes that we don’t attribute our changing perceptions to our mood shifts. Rather, the world really seems different.

Our moods colour how we see our worlds partly by brining to mind past experiences associated with the mood.

Mood-related thoughts may distract us from complex thinking about something else. Thus, when emotionally aroused-when angry or even in a very good mood-we become more likely to make snap judgments and evaluate others based on stereotypes. But if our attention is drawn to our moods, we may “correct” our judgments.

People in a foul mood had less flattering views of another person than did people in a happy mood, unless they first attended to their moods. In that case mood had little impact on impressions of the other person. It seems that if we acknowledge our moods, we can keep them from biasing our judgments.

Our judgment of people depends on their behaviours. Depending on our explanation, we may judge killing as murder, manslaughter, self-defence, or heroism.

We endlessly analyze and discuss why things happen as they do, especially when we experience something negative or unexpected.

Amy Holtzworth-Munroe and Neil Jacobson report that married people often analyze their partners’ behaviours. Cold hostility is more likely than a warm hug to leave the partner wondering “why?”

Those in unhappy relationships typically offer distress-maintaining explanations for negative acts (“she was late because she doesn’t care about me”). Happy couples more often externalize (“She was late because of heavy traffic”).

Antonia Abbey have repeatedly found that men are more likely than women to attribute a woman’s friendliness to mild sexual interest.

Misattribution is also the reason why 23% of American women who say they have been forced into unwanted sexual behaviour is eight times the 3% of American men who say they have ever forced a woman into a sexual act.

The variation of attribution theory shares some common assumptions. “Each construes the human skin as a special boundary that separates one set of ‘causal forces’ from another. On the sunny side of the epidermis are the external or situational forces that press inward upon the person, and on the meaty side are the internal or personal forces that exert pressure outward. Sometimes these forces press in conjunction, sometimes in opposition, and their dynamic interplay manifests itself as observable behaviour.”

Some people are more inclined to attribute behaviour to stable personality; others tend more to attribute behaviour to situations.

Edward Jones and Keith Davis noted that we often infer that other people’s actions are indicative of their intentions and dispositions.

Jones and Davis’ “theory of correspondent inferences” specifies the conditions under which such attributions are most likely. For example, normal or expected behaviour tells us less about the person than does unusual behaviour.

Attributions are often rational. Harold Kelley described how we use information about “consistency,” “distinctiveness,” and “consensus”.

o Example: Edgar is having trouble with his computer. Is Edgar usually unable to get his computer to work? (Consistency) Does Edgar have trouble with other computers, or only this one?

(Distinctiveness)

Page 6: PSYC215 Chapter 3 Notes Definitions - Amazon S3s3.amazonaws.com/prealliance_oneclass_sample/Dq6Xxbp2a6.pdf · PSYC215 Chapter 3 Notes Definitions: ... Attribution Theory: ... Our

Do other people have similar problems with this make of computer? (Consensus)

Based on whether the problem resides with Edgar (internal) or the actual computer itself (external), we can find out the problem at hand.

Our common sense psychology often explains behaviour logically. Kelley also found people often discount a contributing cause of behaviour if other plausible causes are already known. If we can specify one or two reasons a student might have done poorly on an exam, we may ignore of discount other possibilities.

Social psychology’s most important lesson concerns the influence of our social environment. At any moment, our internal state, and therefore what we say and do, depends on the situation, as well as on what we bring to the situation. In experiments, a slight difference between two situations sometimes greatly affects how people respond.

Attribution researchers have found that we often fail to appreciate this important lesson. When explaining someone’s behaviour, we underestimate the impact of the situation and overestimate the extent to which it reflects the individual’s traits and attitudes.

The fundamental attribution error is so irresistible that even when people know they are causing someone else’s behaviour, they still underestimate external influences. If subjects dictate an opinion that someone else must then express, they still tend to see the person as actually holding that opinion.

In short, we tend to presume that others are the way they act. In fact, intelligent and socially competent people are more likely to make the attribution error.

Attribution theorists point out that we have a different perspective when observing others than when acting. When we act, the environment commands our attention. When we watch another person act, that person occupies the center of our attention and the situation becomes relatively invisible.

Using the perceptual analogy of figure and ground, the person is the figure that stands out from the surrounding environment ground. So what would happen if it was reversed and we could see ourselves as others see us and if we saw the world through their eyes?

Remembering an experience from an observer’s perspective-by “seeing” oneself from the outside-has the same effect.

Bertram Malle concludes that the actor-observer difference is often minimal. People typically exhibit empathy when they observe someone after explaining their own behaviour in the same situation. It’s when one person misbehaves while another observes that the two will offer strikingly different attributions.

The camera perspective bias influences people’s guilt judgments even when the judge instructed them not to allow it to. That’s what such tapes yield a nearly 10% conviction rate when played by prosecutors when the camera is focused on the detective, they perceive the confession was coerced.

Perspectives change with time. As the once-visible person recedes in their memory, observers often give more and more credit to the situation. Immediately after hearing someone argue an assignment position, people assume that’s how the person really felt. A week later they are much more likely to credit the situational constraints.

When recalling our past, we become like observers of someone else. For most of us, the “old you” is someone other than today’s “real you.”

Circumstances can also shift our perspective on ourselves. Seeing ourselves on television redirects our attention to ourselves. Seeing ourselves in a mirror, hearing our tape-recorded

Page 7: PSYC215 Chapter 3 Notes Definitions - Amazon S3s3.amazonaws.com/prealliance_oneclass_sample/Dq6Xxbp2a6.pdf · PSYC215 Chapter 3 Notes Definitions: ... Attribution Theory: ... Our

voices, having our picture taken, or filling out biographical questionnaires similarly focus our attention inward, making us self-conscious instead of situation-conscious.

When our attention focuses on ourselves, we attribute more responsibility to ourselves.

People whose attention focuses on themselves-either briefly during an experiment or because they are self-conscious persons-view themselves more as observers typically do; they attribute their behaviour more to internal factors and less to the situation.

We find causes where we can look for them. Because we are acutely aware of how our behaviour varies with the situation, we see ourselves as more variable than other people.

It’s an overstatement to say that at all times and in all settings observers underestimate situational influences. For this reason, many social psychologist follow Edward Jones in referring to the fundamental attribution error-seeing behaviour as corresponding to an inner disposition-as the correspondence bias.

Experiments reveal that the bias occurs even when we are aware of the situational forces-when we know that an assigned debate position is not a good basis for inferring someone’s real attitudes.

It is sobering to think that we can know about a social process that distorts our thinking and still be susceptible to it. Perhaps that’s because it takes more mental effort to assess social effects on someone’s behaviour than merely to attribute it to a disposition.

Culture also has a strong influence on the fundamental attribution error. In cultures that emphasize collectivism as opposed to individualism, the fundamental attribution error is less likely to emerge and more easily overcome.

Psychologists generally assume that even our biases serve a purpose, or else nature would have rejected rather than selected those who exhibit them.

Attributing behaviour to dispositions rather than to situations is not only efficient, it often does little hard since our dispositions often lead us to choose our situations.

When we experience a person only in a single role, we can predict their behaviour equally well whether attributing it to their role or to their disposition. It’s only when we experience a person in a new situation that our disposition-based predictions may go astray.

The attribution error is fundamental because it colours our explanations in basic and important ways. Researchers in Britain, Indian, Australia, and the United States have found, for example, that people’s attributions predict their attitudes toward the poor and unemployed.

Illusory thinking is often a by-product of our mind’s strategies for simplifying complex information. It parallels our perceptual mechanisms, which generally give us a useful image of the world, but sometimes lead us astray.

A second reason for focusing on biases such as the fundamental attribution error is humanitarian. Thomas Gilovich and Richard Eibach says that “people should not always be blamed for their problems. Failure, disability, and misfortune are more often than people are willing to acknowledge the product of real environmental causes.”

A third reason to focus on the biases is that we are mostly unaware of them and can benefit from greater awareness. As with other biases, such as the self-serving biases, people see themselves as less susceptible than others to attribution errors.

Our social beliefs and judgments matter because they influence how we feel and act, and by doing so may generate their own reality. When our ideas lead us to act in ways that produce their apparent confirmation, they become self-fulfilling prophecy.

Idealization helped buffer conflict, bolster satisfaction, and turn self-perceived frogs into princes or princesses. When someone loves and admires us, it helps us become more the person he or she imagines us to be.

Page 8: PSYC215 Chapter 3 Notes Definitions - Amazon S3s3.amazonaws.com/prealliance_oneclass_sample/Dq6Xxbp2a6.pdf · PSYC215 Chapter 3 Notes Definitions: ... Attribution Theory: ... Our

Among married couples, those who worry that their partner doesn’t love and accept them interpret slight hurts as rejections, which motivate them to devalue the partner and distance themselves. Those who presume their partner’s love and acceptance respond less defensively and even may be closer to their partner. Love helps create its presumed reality.

Mark Snyder experiments show how, once formed, erroneous beliefs about the social world can induce others to confirm those beliefs, a phenomenon called behavioural confirmation.

Behavioural confirmation also occurs as people interact with partners holding mistaken beliefs. People who are believed lonely behave less sociably. Men who are believed sexist behave les favourably toward women.

Expectations influence children’s behaviour. Repeatedly tell children they are hard-working and kind (rather than lazy and mean), and they may live up to their label.

Experiments help us understand how social beliefs, such as stereotypes about people with disabilities or about people of a particular race or sex, may be self-confirming. We help construct our own social realities. How others treat us reflects how we and others have treated them.

As with every social phenomenon, the tendency to confirm others’ expectations has its limits. Expectations often predict behaviour simply because they are accurate.