effects of dextromethorphan on zebrafish larval development

1
tions of executive control processing including performance on an auditory continuous performance task (CPT). The CPT was a computerized task consisting of 80 randomly-ordered tones representing two different frequen- cies (highand low); 20 of the trials were target stimuli to which the child was instructed to respond by pressing the mouse button. Prior to the task, each child was given practice to an acquisition criterion of 5 consecutive correct responses. Preliminary analyses of response accuracy, reaction time (RT), and errors of commission (false alarms) revealed no significant differences in accuracy or reaction time, although reaction times were slower in the exposed children in each drug group compared to the non-exposed. All exposed groups showed elevations in the number of false alarms with a significant increase in the CBZ group (F (1, 76) = 7.35; p = 0.008) and a marginally significant increase in the PHT group (F (1, 68) = 3.37; p = 0.07) when compared to the respective non-exposed groups. Additional data from other measures of attention and working memory will also be presented. doi:10.1016/j.ntt.2009.04.055 NBTS52 The use of developmental neurotoxicity data in pesticide risk assessments Kathleen Raffaele a , Jess Rowland b , Brenda May b , Susan Makris a , Kelly Schumacher d , Louis Scarano c a Office of Research and Development, U.S. Environmental Protection Agency, Washington, DC, USA b Office of Pesticide Programs, U.S. Environmental Protection Agency, Washington, DC, USA c Office of Pollution Prevention and Toxics, U.S. Environmental Protection Agency, Washington, DC, USA d Region 7, U.S. Environmental Protection Agency, Kansas City, KS, USA Following passage of the Food Quality Protection Act, which increased focus on potential toxicity of pesticides to children, the number of guideline developmental neurotoxicity (DNT) studies (OPPTS 870.6300) submitted to the US Environmental Protection Agency Office of Pesticide Programs (OPP) was greatly increased. To evaluate the impact of available DNT studies on individual chemical risk assessments, the ways in which data from these studies are being used in pesticide risk assessment was investigated. In addition, neurobehavioral and neuropathological parameters affected at the lowest observed adverse effect level (LOAEL) for each study were evaluated to ascertain whether some types of endpoints were consistently more sensitive than others. As of 12/2008, final OPP reviews of DNT studies for 70 pesticide chemicals were available: 15 had been used to determine the point of departure for at least one risk assessment scenario; 15 more were determined to have the potential for use as points of departure for future risk assessments. Analysis of parameters affected at study LOAELs found no single endpoint consistently more sensitive than another. Early assessment time points (e.g. PND 1121) tended to be more sensitive than later time points (PND 60). These results demonstrate that DNT data, when available, can provide endpoints for risk assessment. Data from these studies affirm the importance of evaluating a spectrum of behavioral and neuropathological endpoints, in young and adult animals, to improve detection of the potential for a chemical to cause developmental neurotoxicity. (This abstract does not necessarily reflect the policy of the USEPA.) doi:10.1016/j.ntt.2009.04.056 NBTS53 Effects of dextromethorphan on zebrafish larval development Frank Scalzo, Mike Levine Bard College, Annandale-on-Hudson, New York, USA Dextromethorphan (DXM) is a noncompetitive NMDA receptor antago- nist used in many over-the-counter medications as a cough suppressant. While it has been shown that DXM has teratogenic effects in chick embryos, evidence suggests that developmental exposure to DXM in other species is not deleterious. In an attempt to develop experimental parameters for neurobehavioral testing of DXM in the zebrafish model, we conducted studies to identify concentrations of DXM that could be used in acute and long-term exposure experiments. Fertilized zebrafish eggs were treated with either 0, 200 mg/L or 400 mg/L DXM from 4 h post-fertilization to 172 h post-fertilization. Observations were made for structural and functional development. At 200 mg/L, embryos showed signs of malformations that included edema and scoliosis. At 400 mg/L, a significant number of embryos did not survive; surviving embryos displayed edema and scoliosis. In both groups, edema and scoliosis were almost always paired. The results suggest that prolonged exposure to DXM does alter developmental processes in zebrafish. These results may have implications for future studies on the structural and functional development of the zebrafish nervous system and behavior. doi:10.1016/j.ntt.2009.04.057 NBTS54 Monkey models for studies of maternal and child health: When and why? Deborah Rice Maine Center for Disease Control and Prevention, Augusta, ME, USA Because of financial considerations, most teratology and developmental neurotoxicology studies use rodents as the experimental model. However, there are important differences in physiology and reproductive strategy between rodents and simian primates, including humans. Rats and mice have a reproductive strategy that is characterized by producing a large number of offspring in a short time with little investment in each individual. Primates typically deliver one offspring after a long gestation period and invest substantial resources in each offspring. These differences may influence the response of the maternal-fetal unit. Laboratory rodents are born less mature than primates. Humans are typical primates in most respects of reproduction and fetal development. An important exception is that the brains of human infants are born less mature than other primates (secondarily altricial). The brain structure of primates and rodents is different in a number of important ways, which has important implications for the pattern of effects produced by toxicants. Additionally, monkeys are capable of much more complicated behaviors than rodents; in some cases, identical procedures can be used in monkeys and children. Primates, including humans, use vision as a primary way of perceiving the world, and a large portion of the primate brain is devoted to visual processing. Rodents rely on olfaction for much of their information about the environment, which makes behavioral testing more challenging and interpretation less straightforward. The use of monkeys is appropriate when investigation of specific endpoints that differ in a critical dimension between rodents and primates is desirable. doi:10.1016/j.ntt.2009.04.058 NBTS55 Nonhuman primate models for pediatric AIDS Nancy Haigwood a,b , Koen Van Rompay c a Oregon National Primate Research Center, Beaverton, OR, USA b Oregon Health and Sciences University, Beaverton, OR, USA c California National Primate Research Center, Davis, California, USA Nonhuman primate (NHP) models for HIV infection and AIDS have been developed and refined over the last 20 years in an effort to investigate key questions in lentiviral transmission and pathogenesis. There are an estimated half million HIV-infected children who are born to infected mothers each year. Vertical transmission of HIV has been studied extensively, in an effort to understand factors contributing to the timing and route of infection, which can be in utero, intrapartum, or postpartum via breastmilk. Progress in the NBTS 2009 Abstracts 250

Upload: frank-scalzo

Post on 11-Sep-2016

219 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Effects of dextromethorphan on zebrafish larval development

tions of executive control processing including performance on an auditorycontinuous performance task (CPT). The CPT was a computerized taskconsisting of 80 randomly-ordered tones representing two different frequen-cies (“high” and “low”); 20 of the trials were target stimuli to which the childwas instructed to respond by pressing themouse button. Prior to the task, eachchild was given practice to an acquisition criterion of 5 consecutive correctresponses. Preliminary analyses of response accuracy, reaction time (RT), anderrors of commission (false alarms) revealed no significant differences inaccuracy or reaction time, although reaction times were slower in the exposedchildren in each drug group compared to the non-exposed. All exposed groupsshowed elevations in the number of false alarms with a significant increase inthe CBZ group (F(1, 76)=7.35; p=0.008) and a marginally significant increasein the PHT group (F(1, 68)=3.37; p=0.07) when compared to the respectivenon-exposed groups. Additional data from other measures of attention andworking memory will also be presented.

doi:10.1016/j.ntt.2009.04.055

NBTS52

The use of developmental neurotoxicity data in pesticiderisk assessments

Kathleen Raffaelea, Jess Rowlandb, Brenda Mayb, Susan Makrisa,Kelly Schumacherd, Louis ScaranocaOffice of Research and Development, U.S. Environmental Protection Agency,Washington, DC, USAbOffice of Pesticide Programs, U.S. Environmental Protection Agency, Washington,DC, USAcOffice of Pollution Prevention and Toxics, U.S. Environmental Protection Agency,Washington, DC, USAdRegion 7, U.S. Environmental Protection Agency, Kansas City, KS, USA

Following passage of the Food Quality Protection Act, which increasedfocus on potential toxicity of pesticides to children, the number of guidelinedevelopmental neurotoxicity (DNT) studies (OPPTS 870.6300) submitted tothe US Environmental Protection Agency Office of Pesticide Programs (OPP)was greatly increased. To evaluate the impact of available DNT studies onindividual chemical risk assessments, the ways in which data from thesestudies are being used in pesticide risk assessment was investigated. Inaddition, neurobehavioral and neuropathological parameters affected at thelowest observed adverse effect level (LOAEL) for each study were evaluatedto ascertain whether some types of endpoints were consistently moresensitive than others. As of 12/2008, final OPP reviews of DNT studies for 70pesticide chemicals were available: 15 had been used to determine the pointof departure for at least one risk assessment scenario; 15 more weredetermined to have the potential for use as points of departure for future riskassessments. Analysis of parameters affected at study LOAELs found no singleendpoint consistently more sensitive than another. Early assessment timepoints (e.g. PND 11–21) tended to be more sensitive than later time points(PND 60). These results demonstrate that DNT data, when available, canprovide endpoints for risk assessment. Data from these studies affirm theimportance of evaluating a spectrum of behavioral and neuropathologicalendpoints, in young and adult animals, to improve detection of the potentialfor a chemical to cause developmental neurotoxicity. (This abstract does notnecessarily reflect the policy of the USEPA.)

doi:10.1016/j.ntt.2009.04.056

NBTS53

Effects of dextromethorphan on zebrafish larval development

Frank Scalzo, Mike LevineBard College, Annandale-on-Hudson, New York, USA

Dextromethorphan (DXM) is a noncompetitive NMDA receptor antago-nist used in many over-the-counter medications as a cough suppressant.

While it has been shown that DXM has teratogenic effects in chick embryos,evidence suggests that developmental exposure to DXM in other species isnot deleterious. In an attempt to develop experimental parameters forneurobehavioral testing of DXM in the zebrafish model, we conductedstudies to identify concentrations of DXM that could be used in acute andlong-term exposure experiments. Fertilized zebrafish eggs were treated witheither 0, 200 mg/L or 400 mg/L DXM from 4 h post-fertilization to 172 hpost-fertilization. Observations were made for structural and functionaldevelopment. At 200 mg/L, embryos showed signs of malformations thatincluded edema and scoliosis. At 400 mg/L, a significant number of embryosdid not survive; surviving embryos displayed edema and scoliosis. In bothgroups, edema and scoliosis were almost always paired. The results suggestthat prolonged exposure to DXM does alter developmental processes inzebrafish. These results may have implications for future studies on thestructural and functional development of the zebrafish nervous system andbehavior.

doi:10.1016/j.ntt.2009.04.057

NBTS54

Monkey models for studies of maternal and child health: When and why?

Deborah RiceMaine Center for Disease Control and Prevention, Augusta, ME, USA

Because of financial considerations, most teratology and developmentalneurotoxicology studies use rodents as the experimental model. However,there are important differences in physiology and reproductive strategybetween rodents and simian primates, including humans. Rats and mice havea reproductive strategy that is characterized by producing a large number ofoffspring in a short time with little investment in each individual. Primatestypically deliver one offspring after a long gestation period and investsubstantial resources in each offspring. These differences may influence theresponse of the maternal-fetal unit. Laboratory rodents are born less maturethan primates. Humans are typical primates in most respects of reproductionand fetal development. An important exception is that the brains of humaninfants are born less mature than other primates (secondarily altricial). Thebrain structure of primates and rodents is different in a number of importantways, which has important implications for the pattern of effects producedby toxicants. Additionally, monkeys are capable of much more complicatedbehaviors than rodents; in some cases, identical procedures can be used inmonkeys and children. Primates, including humans, use vision as a primaryway of perceiving the world, and a large portion of the primate brain isdevoted to visual processing. Rodents rely on olfaction for much of theirinformation about the environment, which makes behavioral testing morechallenging and interpretation less straightforward. The use of monkeys isappropriate when investigation of specific endpoints that differ in a criticaldimension between rodents and primates is desirable.

doi:10.1016/j.ntt.2009.04.058

NBTS55

Nonhuman primate models for pediatric AIDS

Nancy Haigwooda,b, Koen Van RompaycaOregon National Primate Research Center, Beaverton, OR, USAbOregon Health and Sciences University, Beaverton, OR, USAcCalifornia National Primate Research Center, Davis, California, USA

Nonhuman primate (NHP) models for HIV infection and AIDS have beendeveloped and refined over the last 20 years in an effort to investigate keyquestions in lentiviral transmission and pathogenesis. There are an estimatedhalf million HIV-infected children who are born to infected mothers eachyear. Vertical transmission of HIV has been studied extensively, in an effort tounderstand factors contributing to the timing and route of infection, whichcan be in utero, intrapartum, or postpartum via breastmilk. Progress in the

NBTS 2009 Abstracts250