dr. richard sisley - anzam

22
Cognitive Facets of Intrinsic Motivation as Predictors of Academic Performance Dr. Richard Sisley Department of Management, Business Faculty, Auckland University of Technology Auckland, New Zealand [email protected]

Upload: others

Post on 18-Dec-2021

7 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Dr. Richard Sisley - ANZAM

Cognitive Facets of Intrinsic Motivation as Predictors of Academic Performance

Dr. Richard Sisley

Department of Management, Business Faculty,

Auckland University of Technology

Auckland, New Zealand

[email protected]

Page 2: Dr. Richard Sisley - ANZAM

Cognitive Facets of Intrinsic Motivation as Predictors of Academic Performance

ABSTRACT

This paper reports a study that examined the intrinsic and extrinsic motivational profiles of a sample (N =

280) of business students in a New Zealand university, and tested their cognitive ability (N = 158). As

expected, cognitive ability strongly predicted academic performance (grade point average). Of the four

facets of motivation, the two cognitive as opposed to affective facets, challenge (intrinsic) and being

motivated by ones goals for grades (extrinsic), were correlated with performance, but only the latter

significantly predicted it. Women participants had equivalent levels of cognitive ability and intrinsic

motivation, but significantly higher extrinsic grade-goal motivation and significantly higher GPA scores

than did the men. Possible explanations, including parallels with Achievement Goal theory, are suggested,

and implications for tertiary teaching strategy briefly discussed.

Keywords: motivation, business education, learning environment/climate

This paper reports an empirical study of different facets of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation and their

relationship to performance in a sample of students in a university business school. Student motivation is

of obvious concern to educators in any discipline, and the contrast between intrinsic and extrinsic reasons

for choosing particular subjects and for tertiary study in general has wide implications for classroom

teaching practice, approaches to assessment, and for the characteristics of graduates who emerge to take

up careers in business. One element of this concern is the possibility of there being contrasting

relationships between different aspects or facets of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation and student

performance, as reflected in grades, and this possibility was the focus of the present study.

Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation

At their simplest, intrinsic motivation refers to behaviour engaged in for interest and enjoyment, and

extrinsic motivation refers to behaviour performed to attain contingent outcomes separate from the activity

itself (Vallerand & Ratelle, 2002).

Page 3: Dr. Richard Sisley - ANZAM

For the purposes of this study the definitions given by Amabile, Hennessey and Tighe (1994, p.950) were

used:

intrinsic motivation: the motivation to engage in work primarily for its own sake, because the

work itself is interesting, engaging, or in some way satisfying.

extrinsic motivation: the motivation to work primarily in response to something apart from the

work itself, such as reward or recognition or the dictates of other people.

Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation and Academic Performance

From the 1970s onwards a large number of studies explored the relationship between these two forms of

motivation, initially as induced states and later as more enduring motivational traits, and their correlations

with a variety of other educationally relevant variables.

For example, with tertiary students, trait intrinsic motivation has been associated with more meaningful

cognitive engagement (Walker, Greene and Mansell, 2006), with greater commitment to careers

(Brownlow, Gilbert and Reasinger, 1997), and with greater persistence in the face of an academic

challenge (Vallerand and Bissonnette, 1992).

Other studies have used intrinsic motivation and performance as outcome variables, often with autonomy-

promoting vs. controlling teacher strategies or student autonomy orientation as the independent variable.

Thill, Mailhot and Mouanda (1998) found that student autonomy orientation was associated with more

state intrinsic motivation and higher performance, and Benware and Deci (1984) showed that an active

learning orientation was linked to state intrinsic motivation and higher conceptual learning. Teachers’

autonomy-supporting strategies in a maths class were associated with improved self-perceived

Page 4: Dr. Richard Sisley - ANZAM

competence, with higher levels of state intrinsic motivation, and with better performance, among

Norwegian junior high school students (Valas and Sovik, 1993).

Autonomy support from parents and family has also been shown to play a part in building intrinsic

motivational orientation and better academic performance (Ginsburg and Bronstein, 1993). Similarly

Wong, Wiest and Cusick (2002) found both teacher and parental autonomy support, as perceived by ninth

grade students, predicted both trait intrinsic motivation (preference for challenge, and independent

mastery) and academic achievement. A study by Conti (2000) found greater autonomy of students’ goal-

setting was associated with more intrinsic and less extrinsic trait motivation and higher GPA scores. Thus

intrinsic motivation and academic performance often co-vary as dependent variables, where both are

predicted by a third variable such as autonomy-support or autonomy orientation.

There is also some evidence of a direct relationship between intrinsic motivation and performance.

Vansteenkiste, Simons, Lens, Sheldon and Deci (2004) found with both high-school and university

students that, when goals for learning or physical exercise were framed by instructors as intrinsic (i.e. an

experimentally induced intrinsic state), test performance, depth of processing, and persistence were all

significantly higher than for extrinsically framed goals. Similarly significant associations have been found

between intrinsic motivation and academic performance for British secondary school students (Whitehead,

1984), Austrian high-school leavers (Brandstatter and Farthoffer, 2002), Italian high-school students

(Mason and Arcaomo, 2001) and British (Baker, 2003) and American (Mitchell, 1992) tertiary students.

Looking more specifically at intrinsic intellectual motivation, defined as ‘an emotional response to the

content and process of intellectual learning’ (p. 646), Lloyd and Barenblatt (1984) found this form of trait

intrinsic motivation predicted academic performance independently of cognitive ability. With an all-

female sample Li, Lee, and Solmon (2005) found a similar relationship between intrinsic motivation and

both persistence at a physical activity and scores on an associated skills test.

Page 5: Dr. Richard Sisley - ANZAM

However not every study supports these findings. Garcia and Pintrich (1996) found the state-intrinsic goal

orientation induced by an experience of classroom autonomy did not directly predict performance. Baker

(2004) failed to replicate her earlier findings (Baker, 2003) - none of three possible trait-motivational

orientations (intrinsic, extrinsic, amotivational) was associated with significantly better or worse

performance, and Mason and Arcaoma (2001) found both intrinsic and extrinsic motivational traits

predicted performance on more complex tasks. Metofe (2002) reported that trait-intrinsically motivated

black university students performed no differently than their more extrinsically motivated colleagues, the

best predictor of university GPA being their previous high-school GPA. Wong and Csikszentmihalyi

(1991) found that while intrinsically motivated students tended to enrol in more difficult classes, it was the

personality factor “work orientation” that best predicted academic performance (controlling for ability)

rather than intrinsic motivation.

While these studies all failed to replicate the findings of a positive relationship between intrinsic

motivation and academic performance, two studies actually found contrary results. Moneta and Siu (2002)

measured trait extrinsic and intrinsic motivation in a sample of first-year Hong Kong university students

and found that while intrinsic motivation was associated with greater creativity, it correlated with lower

GPA scores than those of the extrinsically motivated participants; and Burton, Lydon, D’Asselandro and

Koestner (2006) found that while intrinsic motivation predicted psychological well-being, it was a highly

internalised form of extrinsic motivation that best predicted academic performance.

Overall, although there appears to be clear and consistent evidence linking an intrinsic motivational

orientation to a variety of positive outcomes, the relationship with direct measures of performance,

especially academic grades, is less well-established. A possible reason for this may be that the great

majority of studies have considered intrinsic and extrinsic motivation as unitary variables and have failed

to examine the differential effects of different sub-types or facets of each. This is despite the fact that

several of the scales most often used to measure them are composed of sub-scales based on distinct

Page 6: Dr. Richard Sisley - ANZAM

aspects of the two major types. For example the Academic Motivation Scale (Vallerand et al, 1992, 1993)

has three extrinsic and three intrinsic scales, and the Work Preference Inventory (Amabile et al, 1994) has

two of each. The latter scale was used in the present study.

Page 7: Dr. Richard Sisley - ANZAM

Gender Differences in Motivation and Academic Performance

Previous research has found women to be significantly higher on autonomy orientation and lower on

control orientation than are men (Deci & Ryan, 1985b; Pelletier, Fortier, Vallerand, Tuson, Briere &

Blais, 1995; Vallerand & Bissonette, 1992). Thus they might be expected to exhibit greater intrinsic

motivation and, if there is a positive relationship between intrinsic motivation and performance, to

perform at higher levels. There is some evidence for both halves of this assertion. Covington and Dray

(2002) found that women were more intrinsically motivated than were men, as did Vallerand and

Bissonette (1992). In the latter study women also showed higher persistence in the face of academic

challenges, and Baker in two separate studies (2003, 2004) found her female participants exhibited higher

levels of academic performance, as did Furnham, Chamorro-Premuzic and McDougall (2003). However

Pintrich and DeGroot (1990) found no motivational differences between seventh-grade boys and girls.

Thus in general there appears to be some evidence of greater intrinsic motivation among women than men,

and some evidence of better academic performance by women.

HYPOTHESES

In accordance with the majority findings of the relatively small literature that directly addresses this

question, the first hypothesis was:

Hypothesis 1: High Work Preference Inventory (WPI) intrinsic motivation scale scores

(Challenge and Enjoyment) will predict high GPA scores, after controlling for cognitive

ability.

Page 8: Dr. Richard Sisley - ANZAM

The second hypothesis was based on a very large body of previous research (Schmidt, 2002), and

the third and fourth on more modest but largely consistent findings in the literature alluded to

above.

Hypothesis Two: High cognitive ability (WPT) scores will predict high GPA (Grade Point

Average) scores.

Hypothesis Three: Female student participants will average higher GPA scores than male

student participants, after controlling for cognitive ability.

Hypothesis Four: Female student participants will exhibit higher levels of trait-intrinsic

motivation, as measured by WPI scales, than will male student participants.

Given the strong focus on grades observable among today’s business students, a further hypothesis was:

Hypothesis5: WPI Grade Goal Motivation subscale scores will predict GPA scores better than

will scores on the other extrinsic motivation subscale, Outward Orientation.

METHOD

Over a four year period 280 participants were recruited from second and third year Bachelor of Business

students in the Business Faculty of Auckland University of Technology (AUT). The research project was

explained to classes of students, (omitting the hypotheses), and they were invited to participate. Thus the

sample was self-selected rather than random. Each student volunteer completed the full 30-item WPI and

a measure of cognitive ability, the Wonderlic Personnel Test. With their permission, each student

participant’s GPA at the end of the semester in which they completed these measures was accessed from

AUT academic records.

Page 9: Dr. Richard Sisley - ANZAM

The WPI was factor analysed (using a maximum-likelihood factor analysis with oblimin rotation) and a

shorter version with cleaner and more robust factors was developed (Table 2). Details of the analysis are

given under Findings. Descriptive statistics were calculated for all measures used, both for the whole

sample and by gender (Table 3), and significant trends identified by one-way Analysis of Variance (Table

4). Cross-correlations were calculated between each of the cognitive (Wonderlic) and motivational (WPI)

variables, and between these variables and GPA (Table 5). Those variables that correlated significantly

with GPA were entered as independent variables in a multiple regression analysis with GPA as the

dependent variable.

FINDINGS

The factor analysis produced a 12-item four-factor version and a 15-item five-factor alternative. These

were compared, using maximum likelihood factor analysis on randomly split half-samples. Only the four-

factor version was consistently replicated in both half-samples. Within the time constraints of the study it

was not practical to gather another independent sample of comparable size with which to validate this

new version of the WPI. Accordingly, as recommended by Hair et al (2006), in order to provide

some confirmation of the factor structure and to identify any inconsistencies the original sample

was again randomly split and the first half-sample subjected to a maximum likelihood factor

analysis. This clearly replicated the model derived from the full sample. The second half-sample

was then used in a confirmatory factor analysis of the model. Goodness of fit indices (GFIs) for

this analysis are displayed in Table 1. Nothing was found in the modification indices and

standardised residual covariances to indicate a serious level of model misspecification, and the

GFIs were judged to indicate a moderately good fit for the model, certainly better than was found

for the original 30-item WPI by Amabile et al (1994). The four three-item subscales in the new

instrument each retained a strong conceptual similarity with the corresponding subscales in the original

Page 10: Dr. Richard Sisley - ANZAM

WPI and were given the same names, with the exception of the Compensation scale which was re-named

Grade Goal Motivation to better reflect its content. The items making up the four subscales, and the

subscale reliabilities, are shown in Table 2.

As can be seen in Table 3, levels of intrinsic motivation did not differ between genders, but female

students had stronger extrinsic grade-goal motivation and higher GPA scores, despite equivalent cognitive

ability to the males. One–way analysis of variance indicated these differences were statistically

significant (p < 0.001) (Table 4).

Cognitive ability and one extrinsic sub-scale of the WPI, grade-goal motivation, were moderately

positively correlated with GPA, the intrinsic sub-scale Challenge was weakly positively correlated with

GPA, and the other intrinsic sub-scale, Enjoyment was weakly negatively correlated with GPA (see Table

5). Results of the regression analysis, displayed in Table 6, showed cognitive ability and grade-point

motivation were significant predictors of GPA, but scores on the two intrinsic motivation scales were not,

each producing a non-significant ∆ R2 (change in the coefficient of determination) of only 0.01. The

conclusions regarding the hypotheses are shown in Table 7.

DISCUSSION

Cognitive and Affective Facets of Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation

That cognitive ability was the best predictor of academic performance is, of course, not surprising and is

in accord with a vast amount of previous research (Schmidt, 2002). That strong grade-goal motivation

should also lead to better grades may, at one level, seem equally unsurprising and almost a trivial finding.

However when the correlations of the two intrinsic motivation scales with GPA are taken into account a

more interesting pattern emerges. Amabile et al (1994) classified the intrinsic Challenge scale and the

Page 11: Dr. Richard Sisley - ANZAM

extrinsic Compensation scale (which was the parent scale from which the grade-goal scale was derived) as

cognitive facets and the intrinsic Enjoyment scale and the extrinsic Outward Orientation scale (that taps a

concern with the opinions of other people) as affective facets. Hence it was the two cognitive facets of

intrinsic and extrinsic motivation that positively correlated with GPA and not the affective facets1.

Attaining good grades obviously presents some degree of challenge to all students, greater or lesser

according to their ability. It would appear that, with this sample at least, regardless of whether they enjoy

either the material studied or the process of learning (intrinsic affective), or how much they care what

others think of their efforts (extrinsic affective), it is the extent to which they are “fired up” by both the

challenge of succeeding, in grade terms, (intrinsic cognitive) and, more strongly, the value they place on

grades as desirable goals (extrinsic cognitive), that determines the performance outcome.

While largely contrary to the hypotheses proposed under Research Questions above, this result does echo

some findings in closely related fields. Recent research on salesperson motivation, using the Adult version

of the WPI, demonstrated that challenge and compensation (both cognitive facets), but not enjoyment and

recognition-seeking (both affective facets), served as main predictors of sales performance (Miao, Evans

& Zhou, 2006). In the field of achievement goal theory the distinction is made between mastery goals and

performance goals. The motivation for pursuing mastery goals is to develop competence in the activity

concerned, whereas performance goals are pursued in order to demonstrate that competence in

comparison to that of other people (Barron & Harackiewicz, 2000). Mastery and performance goals have

sometimes been referred to respectively as intrinsic and extrinsic goals (Pintrich & Garcia, 1991) and

appear closely related to the constructs of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. Certainly the motivation to

attain high grades appears to be primarily a performance rather than a mastery goal. Although mastery

1 The weak negative correlation of the affective intrinsic scale Enjoyment with GPA appears likely to have been a

consequence of the wording of the three items in the scale, especially item 22 that explicitly frames the valuing of

enjoyment as being at the expense of being focused on grade goals (see Table 2). Consideration was given to

dropping this item, however as Factors with fewer than three items are considered weak and unstable (Costello &

Osborne, 2005) the item was retained in the analysis.

Page 12: Dr. Richard Sisley - ANZAM

goals are associated with a number of positive consequences similar to those associated with intrinsic

motivation (Barron & Harackiewicz, 2000), a series of studies (Barron, Schwab & Harackiewicz, 1999;

Harackiewicz, Barron, Carter, Lehto & Elliot, 1997; Harackiewicz, Barron, Tauer, Carter & Elliot, 2000)

have demonstrated that while students with mastery goals develop greater interest in the subjects studied,

it is performance goals that predict final grades.

What are the implications of all this for business educators? It should not be concluded that calls for

greater efforts to develop students’ intrinsic interest in the subjects studied and to encourage autonomous

learning (e.g. Reeve, 2002; Sisley, 2004) are misplaced. The wide range of benefits associated with

intrinsic motivation are well documented (Sheldon, Ryan, Deci & Kasser, 2004) and educators who

engage in teaching practices designed to promote student autonomy can feel confident that they are aiding

and strengthening their students’ learning (Reeve, 2002). However it appears these benefits may not

include improved performance as measured by course grades, which, judging by the results of this study,

requires a narrowly focused cognitive facet of extrinsic motivation.

Gender Differences in Motivation and Performance

Female students in the sample possessed this form of motivation more strongly than males. In other

words, women obtained significantly better overall grades than did men, despite having similar levels of

intellectual ability, simply because they were significantly more motivated to do so. This motivational

difference is contrary to previous research findings that women tend to be more autonomy oriented (e.g.

Deci & Ryan, 1985b, Pelletier et al, 1995) and more intrinsically motivated than were men (Covington &

Dray, 2002; Vallerand & Bissonnette, 1992). However it is noteworthy that Covington and Dray found

their female participants were both more intrinsically motivated and more intensely focused on grades and

the need to prove themselves.

Page 13: Dr. Richard Sisley - ANZAM

Why there should be this gender difference remains open to speculation, but one possibility is personality

differences. For example, recent research has shown women scored more highly on agreeableness and

openness to experience than did men, with openness directly predicting academic grades (Farsides &

Woodfield, 2003; Woodfield, Jessup and McMillan, 2006; O’Connor & Paunonen, 2007), and

agreeableness predicting attendance rates at classes, which in turn was a powerful predictor of academic

grades (Farsides & Woodfield; Woodfield, et al.). However the role of personality in the superior

academic performance of women is far from clear in the literature. Another rather complex explanation

revolves around gender differences, assumed to be due to different socialisation experiences, in men’s and

women’s responses to the informational and controlling aspects of positive feedback, with such feedback

being said to activate intrinsic motivation in males and extrinsic motivation in females (Deci & Moller,

2005). Elucidation of these possibilities awaits further research.

CONCLUSIONS

A fundamental question that emerges from the study reported here is the perennial one of “What are our

educational goals for our students and how do we measure their level of attainment of these goals?” If, as

one would hope, we aim to produce graduates with a passion for their chosen discipline or professional

speciality, coupled with a life-long addiction to learning, then research strongly supports the promotion of

student autonomy and intrinsic motivation as a means to this end (Reeve, 2002; Sisley, 2004). However it

appears that grades achieved in our current assessment programmes may measure, along with ability,

something different and arguably equally important, namely a strong determination to succeed. This is

likely to remain the case whatever form our assessments take, and we are left with two alternative

strategies. Either we attempt to design assessments that better reflect autonomy and intrinsic passion,

while attempting to avoid the well-documented risk that linking these to the extrinsic rewards of grades

may actually undermine intrinsic motivation (Lepper & Henderlong, 2000), or we accept as inevitable the

separation of the two functions of instilling student intrinsic interest in a subject on the one hand, and

Page 14: Dr. Richard Sisley - ANZAM

attempting to objectively assess students’ understanding and application of subject content on the other.

Whether the second strategy is a counsel of despair or the path of wisdom I leave to your judgement.

Page 15: Dr. Richard Sisley - ANZAM

REFERENCES

Amabile TM, Hennessey BA & Tighe EM (1994) The Work Preference Inventory: Assessing intrinsic and

extrinsic motivation, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66 (5): 950-967.

Baker SR. (2003) A prospective longitudinal investigation of social problem-solving appraisals on

adjustment to university, stress, health, and academic motivation and performance, Personality and

Individual Differences, 35: 569-591.

Baker SR (2004) Intrinsic, extrinsic, and amotivational orientations: Their role in university adjustment,

stress, well-being, and subsequent academic performance, Current Psychology: Developmental, Learning,

Personality, Social, 23 (3): 189-202.

Barron KE & Harackiewicz JM (2000) Achievement goals and optimal motivation: a multiple goals

approach, in Sansone C and Harackiewicz JM (Eds) Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation: The Search for

Optimal Performance, Academic Press, New York.

Barron KE Schwab C & Harackiewicz JM (1999) Achievement goals and classroom context: A

comparison of different learning environments. Paper presented at the meeting of the Midwestern

Psychological Association, Chicago.

Benware CA & Deci EL (1984) Quality of learning with an active versus passive motivational set,

American Educational Research Journal, 21 (4): 755-765.

Brandstatter H & Farthofer A (2002) Prediction of university students’ performance: Configurative or

linear additive? Zeitschrift fur Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 23 (4): 381-391.

Brownlow S, Gilbert NM & Reasinger RD (1997) Motivation, personality preferences, and interests in

college students. Journal of Psychological Practice, 3(4): 128-140.

Burton KD, Lydon JE, D’Asselandro DU & Koestner R (2006) The differential effects of intrinsic and

identified motivation on well-being and performance: Prospective, experimental and implicit approaches

to Self-Determination Theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(4): 750-762.

Conti R (2000) College goals: Do self-determined and carefully considered goals predict intrinsic

motivation, academic performance, and adjustment during the first semester? Social Psychology of

Education, 4: 189-211.

Costello AB & Osborne J (2005) Best practices in exploratory factor analysis: four recommendations for

getting the most from your analysis. Practical Assessment Research & Evaluation, 10 (7). Available

online: http://pareonline.net/getvn.asp?v=10&n=7

Covington MV & Dray E (2002) The developmental course of achievement motivation: A needs-based

approach, in Wigfield, A and Eccles, JS (Eds), Development of Achievement Motivation, 33-56, Academic

Press, San Francisco.

Deci EL & Moller AC (2005) The concept of competence: A starting place for understanding intrinsic

motivation and self-determined extrinsic motivation, in Elliott AJ & Dweck CS (Eds) Handbook of

competence and motivation, 579-597, The Guilford Press, New York.

Page 16: Dr. Richard Sisley - ANZAM

Deci EL & Ryan RM (1985) The general causality orientations scale: Self-determination in personality,

Journal of Research in Personality, 19: 109-134.

Farsides T & Woodfield R (2003) Individual differences and undergraduate academic success: The roles

of personality, intelligence and application, Personality and Individual Differences, 34: 1225-1243.

Furnham A, Chamorro-Premuzic T, & McDougall F (2003) Personality, cognitive ability, and beliefs

about intelligence as predictors of academic performance, Learning and Individual Differences, 14: 49-66.

Garcia T & Pintrich PR (1996) The effect of autonomy on motivation and performance in the college

classroom, Contemporary Educational Psychology, 21: 477-486.

Ginsberg GS & Bronstein P (1993) Family factors related to children’s intrinsic/extrinsic motivational

orientation and academic performance, Child Development, 64: 1461-1474.

Hair JF, Black WC, Babin BJ, Anderson RE & Tatham RL (2006). Multivariate data analysis (6th ed.)

Pearson Education Inc., Upper Saddle River, NJ.

Harackiewicz JM, Barron KE, Carter SM, Lehto AT & Elliot AJ (1997) Determinants and consequences

of achievement goals in the college classroom: Maintaining interest and making the grade, Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 73: 1284-1295.

Harackiewicz JM, Barron KE, Tauer JM, Carter SM & Elliot AJ (2000) Short-term and long-term

consequences of achievement goals in college: Predicting continued interest and performance over time,

Journal of Educational Psychology, 92 (2): 316-330.

Lepper MR & Henderlong J (2000) Turning “play” into “work” and “work” into “play”: 25 years of

research on intrinsic versus extrinsic motivation. In Sansone C & Harackiewicz J (Eds) Intrinsic and

extrinsic motivation: The search for optimal motivation and performance pp257-307, Academic Press,

San Diego.

Li W, Lee AM & Solmon,MA (2005) Effects of ability conceptions and intrinsic motivation on

persistence and performance: An interaction approach, Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 76 (1):

A81.

Lloyd J & Barenblatt L (1984) Intrinsic intellectuality: Its relations to social class, intelligence, and

achievement, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46 (3): 646-654.

Mason L & Arcaomo S (2001) Motivation for school engagement, causal attributions, and performance in

middle and high-school students, Psicologia

Clinica dello Sviluppo, 5 (3): 423-450.

Metofe PA (2002) Prediction of academic performance in historically Black colleges and universities

(HBCU’s), Dissertation Abstracts International Section A: Humanities and Social Sciences, 62 (12-A):

4075.

Miao CF, Evans KR & Zou S (2006) The role of salesperson motivation in sales control systems –

Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation revisited, Journal of Business Research, 60: 417-425.

Page 17: Dr. Richard Sisley - ANZAM

Mitchell JV (1992) Interrelationships and predictive efficacy for indices of intrinsic, extrinsic, and self-

assessed motivation for learning, Journal of Research and Development in Education, 25: 149-155.

Moneta GB & Siu CMY (2002) Trait intrinsic and extrinsic motivations, academic performance, and

creativity in Hong Kong college students, Journal of College Student Development, 43: 664-683.

O’Connor MC & Paunonen SV (2007) Big five personality predictors of post-secondary academic

performance, Personality and Individual Differences, 43: 971-990.

Pelletier LG, Fortier MS, Vallerand RJ, Tuson KM, Briere N & Blais MR (1995) Toward a new measure

of intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, and amotivation in sports: The Sport Motivation Scale,

Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 17: 35-53.

Pintrich PR & Garcia T (1991) Student goal orientation and self-regulation in the college classroom, in

Maehr ML & Pintrich PR (Eds), Advances in Motivation and Achievement (vol.7, 371-402), JAI Press,

Greenwich, CT.

Reeve J (2002) Self-Determination Theory applied to educational settings, in Deci EL & Ryan RM (Eds),

Handbook of self-determination research, pp183-203, University of Rochester press, Rochester, NY.

Schmidt FL (2002) The role of general cognitive ability and job performance: Why there cannot be a

debate, Human Performance, 15 (1/2): 187-210.

Sheldon KM, Ryan RM, Deci EL & Kasser T (2004) The independent effects of goal contents and

motives on well-being: It’s both what you pursue and why you pursue it, Personality and Social

Psychology Bulletin, 30: 475-486.

Sisley R (2004) Fostering a love of learning, in Yourn B & Little S (Eds) Walking to Different Beats:

Good Practice and Innovation in Higher Education, Dunmore Press, Palmerston North, NZ.

Thill EE, Mailhot L & Mouanda J (1998) On how task-contingent rewards, individual differences in

causality orientations, and imagery abilities are related to intrinsic motivation and performance, European

Journal of Social Psychology, 28 (2): 141-158.

Valas, H & Sovik, N (1993) Variables affecting students’ intrinsic motivation for school mathematics:

Two empirical studies based on Deci and Ryan’s theory on motivation, Learning and Instruction, 3: 281-

298.

Vallerand RJ & Bissonette R (1992) Intrinsic, extrinsic and amotivational styles as predictors of behavior:

A prospective study. Journal of Personality, 60: 599-620.

Vallerand R.J, Pelletier LG, Blais MR, Briere NM, Senecal C and Vallieres EF (1992) The academic

motivation scale: a measure of intrinsic, extrinsic, and amotivation in education, Educational and

Psychological Measurement, 52: 1003-1017.

Vallerand RJ, Pelletier LG, Blais MR, Brier NM, Senecal C and Vallieres EF (1993) On the assessment of

intrinsic, extrinsic, and amotivation in education: Evidence on the concurrent and construct validity of the

academic motivation scale, Educational and Psychological Measurement, 53: 159-172.

Vallerand RJ & Ratelle CF (2002) Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: A hierarchical model, in Deci EL &

Ryan RM (Eds), Handbook of self-determination research, University of Rochester press, Rochester, NY.

Page 18: Dr. Richard Sisley - ANZAM

Vansteenkiste M, Simons J, Lens W, Sheldon KM & Deci EL (2004) Motivating learning, performance,

and persistence: The synergistic effects of intrinsic goal contents and autonomy-supportive contexts,

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87 (2): 246-260.

Walker CO, Greene BA & Mansell RA (2006) Identification with academics, intrinsic/extrinsic

motivation, and self-efficacy as predictors of cognitive engagement, Learning and Individual Differences,

16 (1): 1-12.

Whitehead J (1984) Motives for higher education: A study of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation in relation

to academic attainment, Cambridge Journal of Education, 14 (2): 26-34.

Wong MM & Csikszentmihalyi M (1991) Motivation and academic achievement: The effects of

personality traits and the quality of experience, Journal of Personality, 59 (3): 539-574.

Wong EH, Wiest DJ & Cusick LB (2002) Perceptions of autonomy support, parent attachment,

competence and self-worth as predictors of motivational orientation and academic achievement: An

examination of sixth- and ninth-grade regular education students, Adolescence, 37 (146): 255-266.

Woodfield R, Jessop D & McMillan L (2006) Gender differences in undergraduate attendance rates,

Studies in Higher Education, 31 (1): 1-22.

Page 19: Dr. Richard Sisley - ANZAM

TABLES

Table 1: Confirmatory Factor Analysis: Fit Indices for the Modified WPI

(Using a Random Half-Sample)

χ2 df χ

2/df GFI CFI IFI RMSEA P ratio

78.24

p = .004

48

1.63

.94

.94

.94

.06

(.03-.08)a

.73

Note: χ2 = chi-square (the difference between estimated and observed covariance matrices), df =

degrees of freedom, GFI = goodness of fit index, CFI = comparative fit index, IFI = incremental fit index,

RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation, P = parsimony, p = probability. a confidence intervals for RMSEA.

Table 2: Items Comprising the Modified Version of the Work Preference Inventory (WPI)

WPI

Item

No.

Items comprising the four sub-scales

EFA

Factor

loadings

CHALLENGE (Intrinsic Cognitive) ( α = .81)

26 I enjoy trying to solve complex problems .84

3 The more difficult the problem, the more I enjoy trying to solve it .81

13 I enjoy tackling problems that are completely new to me .66

GRADE GOAL MOTIVATION (Extrinsic Cognitive) (α = .78)

10 I am keenly aware of the GPA (grade point average) goals I have for myself .83

4 I am keenly aware of the goals I have for getting good grades .78

19 I am strongly motivated by the grades I can earn .55

ENJOYMENT (Intrinsic Affective) (α = .64)

30 What matters most to me is enjoying what I do .69

20 It is important for me to be able to do what I most enjoy .67

22 As long as I can do what I enjoy, I’m not that concerned about exactly what grades or

awards I can earn

.47

OUTWARD ORIENTATION (ExtrinsicAffective) (α = .60)

24 I am strongly motivated by the recognition I can earn from other people .70

29 I want other people to find out how good I can really be at my work .67

25 I have to feel that I’m earning something for what I do .45

Note. α = factor internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha), EFA = Exploratory Factor Analysis.

Page 20: Dr. Richard Sisley - ANZAM

Table 3: Means and Standard Deviations of Cognitive Ability Scores, Motivation Scale Scores and

GPA for the Whole Sample and by Gender.

Cognitive

Ability

IM EM Enjoyment Challenge Outward

Orientation

Grade Goal

Motivation

GPA

Whole

sample:

mean

22.98

17.07

17.35

9.05

8.02

8.23

9.13

67.43

SD 5.84 2.73 3.00 1.77 1.99 1.97 2.07 8.47

n 158 280 280 280 280 280 280 316

Males:

mean

23.12

17.17

16.88

9.06

8.11

8.28

8.60

65.13

SD 5.21 2.86 2.91 1.76 2.06 1.83 2.12 7.54

n 68 120 120 120 120 120 120 132

Females:

mean

22.88

17.00

17.69

9.04

7.95

8.19

9.53

69.07

SD 6.3 2.64 3.02 1.78 1.94 2.08 1.94 8.74

n 90 160 160 160 160 160 160 184

Note: IM = overall intrinsic motivation, EM = overall extrinsic motivation.

Table 4: ANOVA of Gender Differences in Motivation Measures, Cognitive Ability and GPA

Measure

df

F

Effect size

Partial ή2

Cognitive ability 1, 156 0.04 0.00

Enjoyment 1, 278 0.01 0.00

Challenge 1, 278 0.40 0.00

Outward Orientation 1, 278 0.18 0.00

Grade Goal Motivation 1, 278 14.04*** 0.05

Grade Point Average (GPA) 1, 314 16.74*** 0.05

Note: ***= p < 0.001.

Page 21: Dr. Richard Sisley - ANZAM

Table 5: Correlations Between Cognitive Ability Scores, Motivation Scales and GPA

Enjoyment Challenge Outward

Orientation

Grade

Goal

Motivation

Cognitive

Ability

Challenge .05 ns

Outward

Orientation

.07 ns .01 ns

Grade Goal

Motivation

-.13* .18** .11 ns

Cognitive

Ability

.01 ns .09 ns -.26** -.06 ns

GPA -.12* .16** -.05 ns .29*** .32***

Note: ns = not significant, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001

Table 6: Regression Analysis Assessing Predictors of GPA

Measure B SE

B

β t Model F Model

R2

Adjusted

R2

∆ R2

Model 1 16.55*** .12 .11 .12***

Cognitive Ability .50 .12 .35 4.07***

Model 2 15.50*** .21 .19 .09***

Cognitive Ability .52 .12 .36 4.43***

Grade Motivation 1.18 .33 .29 3.58***

Model 3 10.79*** .22 .20 .01

Cognitive Ability .50 .12 .35 4.31***

Grade Motivation 1.11 .34 .28 3.32**

Challenge .40 .35 .10 1.14

Model 4 8.50*** .23 .20 .01

Cognitive Ability .50 .12 .35 4.31***

Grade Motivation 1.05 .34 .26 3.13**

Challenge .43 .35 .10 1.23

Enjoyment .47 .39 .10 1.22

** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.

Page 22: Dr. Richard Sisley - ANZAM

Table 7: Summary of Hypothesis Results

Hypothesis Result

1

High WPI intrinsic motivation scale scores (Challenge

and Enjoyment) will predict high GPA scores, after

controlling for cognitive ability.

Rejected

2 High cognitive ability (WPT) scores will predict high

GPA scores.

Supported

3 Female student participants will average higher GPA

scores than male student participants, after controlling

for cognitive ability.

Supported

4 Female student participants will exhibit higher levels of

trait-intrinsic motivation, as measured by WPI primary

and secondary scales, than will male student

participants.

Rejected: intrinsic motivation

scores were the same, but females

scored higher on one extrinsic

scale (grade goal motivation)

5 WPI grade goal motivation subscale scores will predict GPA

scores better than will scores on the other extrinsic motivation

subscale, Outward Orientation.

Supported : in fact this was the

only motivation scale that

predicted GPA