Transcript
Page 1: African American Children's Affective Attributions and Consequences Regarding Sociomoral Events

African American Children’s Affective Attributions andConsequences Regarding Sociomoral Events

Marisha L. Humphries

Department of Educational Psychology, University of Illinois at Chicago

Research Findings: This study examined 56 young (prekindergarten through 2nd grade)

urban-dwelling African American children’s understanding of the affective attributions and conse-

quences of 3 types of sociomoral rule systems: prosocial, active, and inhibitive morality. It also

tested the relationship of affective attributions and consequences to children’s behavior. As expected,

children’s affective responses differed by sociomoral rule system and character role type, supporting

the notion of a ‘‘happy victimizer’’ and a subtle attributional shift. Children provided affective attri-

butions that attempted to resolve the dilemmas presented in the different sociomoral vignettes regard-

less of the affect associated with the vignette. The relationship of children’s affective attributions and

consequences to their behavior in school was partially supported. Children’s affective attributions

were significantly associated with their prosocial behavior. However, contrary to predictions, no

other significant associations emerged between children’s affective attributions and negative beha-

vior or between children’s affective consequences and behavior. Practice or Policy: Those workingwith young African American children should consider the reasoning behind children’s emotional

and behavioral reactions and not just focus on the correct or appropriate response to understand

and promote children’s positive development. There are implications for supporting African

American children’s competence development at school through a behavior promotion approach.

Children’s social interactions typically occur within emotional contexts (Eisenberg, Fabes,

Carlo, & Karbon, 1992). A child who is able to successfully analyze and negotiate emotionally

charged interpersonal interactions and regulate these emotional experiences is said to be

emotionally competent (Saarni, 1990). Emotional competence includes one’s knowledge of

the affective consequences of interpersonal interactions that center on morally right or wrong

behavior (e.g., Saarni, 1997) or sociomoral events. These sociomoral events are often emotion-

ally charged interactions (Arsenio & Lover, 1995). It is necessary for children to develop such

affective knowledge and skills to help them successfully negotiate emotionally charged socio-

moral events like sharing, helping, and hitting, events that children experience almost daily with

siblings and=or with peers in day care and school settings. As a result, emotions play a critical

role in children’s social functioning (e.g., Eisenberg et al., 1995; Garner, 1996; Halberstadt,

Denham, & Dunsmore, 2001; Saarni, 1990). There has been great interest in understanding

Correspondence regarding this article should be addressed to Marisha L. Humphries, PhD, Educational Psychology

(MC 147), College of Education, University of Illinois at Chicago, 1040 West Harrison Street, Chicago, IL 60607-7133.

E-mail: [email protected]

Early Education and Development, 24: 212–232

Copyright # 2013 Taylor & Francis Group, LLC

ISSN: 1040-9289 print/1556-6935 online

DOI: 10.1080/10409289.2011.647610

Page 2: African American Children's Affective Attributions and Consequences Regarding Sociomoral Events

the relationship between affect and sociomoral reasoning and its subsequent impact on or

relationship with children’s behavior. For instance, social emotional competence and character

development are said to be critical to children’s prosocial behavior, academic success, cit-

izenship, and positive classroom environments (Elias, 2009). However, research has largely

focused on children’s aggressive behavior, with less attention given to exclusively examining

prosocial or normative development and minority populations.

Some research with African American children has indicated that these children are unfortu-

nately more likely to be exposed to conditions that can compromise their emotional and social

development (Burchinal, Roberts, Zeisel, Hennon, & Hooper, 2006; McLoyd, 1990; Mendez,

Fantuzzo, & Cicchetti, 2002). However, most African American children from urban and=orlow-income or impoverished backgrounds do not develop emotional and social problems

(Gabalda, Thompson, & Kaslow, 2010; Garner, 2006). There is a need to understand the norma-

tive development of African American children who experience challenging situations and con-

texts (American Psychological Association, 2008) from a within-group design. It is important to

understand the factors related to positive emotional and social development in order to maximize

African American children’s optimal development. The present study examined young

urban-dwelling African American children’s affective attributions and consequences of socio-

moral events and how this is related to their behavior with peers.

SOCIOMORAL AFFECT

Hoffman (1983) contended that children’s emotional states and their understanding of their emo-

tions are an important part in the internalization of moral norms; they are thought to be crucial in

the developmental of moral knowledge (Wiersma & Laupa, 2000). A moral sense and emotional

competence are integrated concepts, in that being emotionally competent entails making morally

correct behavioral choices (Saarni, 1997). Specifically, when emotional skills are separated from

a moral sensibility, an individual cannot be emotionally competent. Children’s knowledge of the

emotional or affective consequences of moral transgressions is a part of the situational determi-

nants of emotions (Nunner-Winkler & Sodian, 1988).

Sociomoral events or acts refers to social interactions that provide a context for defining what

is morally right or good (Gibbs, Widaman, & Colby, 1982). Intense emotional responses are

often evoked in those experiencing or witnessing such sociomoral events. These events focus

on the distribution of resources, social conventions, and victimization (Arsenio, 1988) and in

the current study are represented by prosocial, active, and inhibitive morality rule systems. Pro-

social morality is identified as the use of private resources to benefit the outcomes of others

(Eisenberg, 1982). This includes behaviors like sharing and cooperating. Active morality implies

intervention on behalf of someone being victimized or witnessing a transgression (Tisak & Ford,

1986). Behaviors like helping someone who has been treated unfairly and comforting someone

in distress represent active morality in the current study. Deliberately victimizing others and

depriving people of their rights is inhibitive morality (Tisak & Ford, 1986). Stealing and hitting

are examples of inhibitive morality (Tisak & Ford, 1986) that have been studied in other research

examining harm (e.g., Goldstein, Tisak, & Boxer, 2002; Smetana, Campione-Barr, & Yell,

2003; Tisak, Nucci, & Jankowski, 1996). These sociomoral behaviors are particularly germane

to young children because of the prevalence of such behaviors among young children who are

ATTRIBUTIONS AND CONSEQUENCES 213

Page 3: African American Children's Affective Attributions and Consequences Regarding Sociomoral Events

learning to navigate their ever-expanding social world. This becomes especially relevant as

young children begin to attend some form of schooling or out-of-home care.

Children develop general sociomoral principles by integrating their knowledge about affect-

ive attributions and consequences from various situations. It has been found that children use

affective information to infer sociomoral events (Arsenio, 1988). For example, a child who does

not like how it feels when someone hits her may conclude from her own experiences that her

friends and other peers will also not like how it feels to be hit by others. This contributes to

a fairly general moral principle about specific emotions associated with being hit and hitting

others.

Jagers, Bingham, and Hans (1996) found among a sample of inner-city African American

kindergartners that not sharing was viewed differently than other moral transgressions. However,

some research (Nucci & Herman, 1982) has not yielded race differences when comparing nor-

mal and behaviorally disordered African American and White children’s conceptions of moral,

social convention, and personal domains. Like Jagers and colleagues, other research with young

non–African American children (Malti, Gasser, & Buchmann, 2009; Smetana, Schlagman, &

Adams, 1993) has reported that children were less likely to view not sharing as a moral trans-

gression compared to hitting or stealing. This finding is surprising given the communal cultural

orientation associated with the African American community, whereby there is a sense of inde-

pendence and emotional connection to others. This conception of communalism, which is

defined and assessed from an Afrocultural worldview, implies an awareness of the fundamental

interdependence of people with a premium placed on social bonds and obligations (Boykin,

Jagers, Ellison, & Albury, 1997). Previous research has found a significant relationship between

communalism and morality from middle childhood through adolescence (Humphries & Jagers,

2009; Humphries, Parker, & Jagers, 2000; Woods & Jagers, 2003). Furthermore, a communal

orientation is thought to facilitate empathy (Kuther & Wallace, 2003). Examining the emotional

expectations associated with different sociomoral events may help to explain African American

children’s judgments regarding moral events or behaviors. Perhaps African American children

perceive and respond to moral transgressions differently than other non–African American

children given their cultural orientation, racial status, and position within the United States.

There is a great concern regarding children exhibiting or witnessing inhibitive morality beha-

viors, like hitting and stealing, and their reasoning about these behaviors (e.g., Arsenio &

Lemerise, 2004; Goldstein et al., 2002; Smetana et al., 2003; Tisak, 1986; Tisak et al., 1996;

Tisak & Turiel, 1988). Inhibitive morality has received a great deal of attention in terms of

the affective attributions that children ascribe to the victimizer (e.g., Arsenio, 1988; Smetana

et al., 2003). Previous research studies showed that a racially and ethnically diverse group of

young children who were behaviorally disruptive viewed the victimizer as feeling positive about

the transgressions (Arsenio & Fleiss, 1996; Arsenio & Lover, 1995; Barden, Zelko, Duncan, &

Masters, 1980; Lourenco, 1997; Nunner-Winkler & Sodian, 1988; Ramos-Marcuse & Arsenio,

2001; Wiersma & Laupa, 2000). Young children linked the victimizer’s positive emotions to

securing a desired object or reaching an obtained goal, not to the act of victimization. This con-

cept is known as the ‘‘happy victimizer’’ and is common among preschoolers (Arsenio & Lover,

1995; Nunner-Winkler & Sodian, 1988).

There is a developmental shift from viewing victimization strictly in terms of an obtained

object or goal to viewing victimization from victims’ perspectives and considering external

social standards, which leads to attributing more negative affect to transgressors. However, there

214 HUMPHRIES

Page 4: African American Children's Affective Attributions and Consequences Regarding Sociomoral Events

is disagreement in the literature as to when this developmental shift occurs. Nunner-Winkler and

Sodian (1988) found in their research that the developmental shift occurred around the age of 8,

in that 4- to 6-year-olds expected the victimizer to be happy and 8-year-olds expected negative

affect. However, Arsenio and Kramer (1992) and Lourenco (1997) found that the majority of

8-year-olds were ascribing mixed emotional consequences, both positive and negative, to the

victimizer as opposed to strictly negative emotions, suggesting a subtle moral attributional shift

among 8-year-olds. Given this developmental shift, the current study examined grade effects for

the happy victimizer effect among African American children.

AFFECTIVE CONSEQUENCES AND BEHAVIOR

Children’s knowledge about emotional consequences is thought to mediate their sociomoral

behavior, in that such knowledge aids them in predicting the outcomes of different actions

and planning their own behavior (Arsenio, 1988). African American preschool children from

low- to middle-income backgrounds who had greater emotional knowledge and provided less

violent responses to interpersonal conflict were identified as being more liked by their peers

(Smith, 2001). Ramos-Marcuse and Arsenio (2001) found similar findings among a sample of

normal and clinically referred young, largely African American and Latino, children. Specifi-

cally, children who reported more aggressive responses to moral narratives were rated by their

teachers or clinicians as having higher externalizing behavior scores. However, children who

discussed themes of reparations were less likely to be rated as having externalizing behaviors.

In a sample of ethnically diverse school-age children, these children’s aggressive attributions

were related to their negative peer interactions (Garner, 1996). Children’s affective knowledge

about sociomoral narratives or situations appears to be related to their behavior with others.

Although previous research has examined the link between affective attributions and behavior

for a racially and ethnically diverse sample, there has been less attention to the examination of

African American children’s affective attributions and consequences. It is important to further

explore this line of research with a sample of African American children to examine

within-group variability. Furthermore, the burgeoning use of social and emotional competence

programs targeted at African American children in urban schools calls for the need to understand

the typical development of these children in order to create and implement effective programs.

AGE AND GENDER

As children develop they have increased opportunities as they age to engage in social interac-

tions. Engagement in these interactions places more demands on their use and development

of social and emotional skills. There are marked changes in children’s emotional and social

development, especially during the preschool years and as children transition into the primary

grades. Thus, older children typically have more sophisticated sociomoral reasoning (Humphries

et al., 2000; Smetana et al., 2003) and competence skills (Denham, Mitchell-Copeland, Strand-

berg, Auerbach, & Blair, 1997) than younger children. However, Garner (1996) found no age

(grade) effects in a diverse sample of third and fourth graders’ affective moral attributions

and prosocial behavior. In addition to grade or age differences, gender differences have also been

noted. It has often been acknowledged that girls and boys can be socialized differently regarding

ATTRIBUTIONS AND CONSEQUENCES 215

Page 5: African American Children's Affective Attributions and Consequences Regarding Sociomoral Events

emotions and respond to and express emotions differently. Girls typically are identified as exhi-

biting greater affective skills (Bocknek, Brophy-Herb, & Banerjee, 2009; Garner & Waajid,

2008) and express emotions differently (Chaplin, Cole, & Zahn-Waxler, 2005) than boys. In

a study by Smith (2001), African American preschool girls were rated as exhibiting more empa-

thy behaviors than boys.

THE CURRENT STUDY

The purpose of this study was to examine young African American children’s understanding of

the affective attributions and consequences of sociomoral events and its association with their

behavior in the school context. I hypothesized the following: (a) Children will differentiate their

emotional responses across three types of sociomoral events (i.e., prosocial, active, and inhibi-

tive) and characters (i.e., actor and recipient), (b) children whose affective attributions and conse-

quences focus on beneficial outcomes for others will be rated by teachers as being prosocial with

their classmates, and (c) children whose affective attributions and affective consequences are con-

cerned with transgressions or personal gain will be rated as being more aggressive with and

excluded by their classmates. Given past findings and developmental differences, gender and grade

differences were also assessed. Finally, it was anticipated that (d) children in first and second grades

would vary in terms of affective valence, attributions, and consequences, including the happy

victimizer effect, in comparison to children in prekindergarten and kindergarten.

METHODS

Participants

A total of 56 African American children (29 boys and 27 girls) participated in this research study

with parental consent. The children attended a public elementary school in a large midwestern

city. This school was a public–private partnership initiative. According to school demographics,

100% of the student body was African American, and 79% of the children were from

low-income backgrounds (i.e., received free or reduced lunch). The children ranged in age from

3 to 8 years old. The original sample consisted of 66 children; however, during the course of this

study, six children dropped out of the study because of either attrition or exclusion. Four of these

six children transferred to new schools. The other two children were excluded because they

denied assent, despite parental consent, at the time of the interview. Four additional children

were not included in the analyses because their audiotaped interviews were inaudible. Therefore,

the final sample for this study consisted of 56 children: 13 prekindergartners (7 girls and 6 boys),

8 kindergartners (2 girls and 6 boys), 12 first graders (9 girls and 3 boys), and 23 second graders

(9 girls and 14 boys).

Procedures

The study was presented to the school staff at a teacher staff meeting and to parents at parent

meetings at the beginning of the school year. Parental consent forms were distributed in each

216 HUMPHRIES

Page 6: African American Children's Affective Attributions and Consequences Regarding Sociomoral Events

classroom and sent home with the children. The consent forms asked for the child to partici-

pate in a study about children’s social and emotional learning and development. Consent

forms were returned to the teachers and collected by the research assistants. All children

who returned their consent forms received a small treat, regardless of whether their parents

gave or denied consent. Each participating child received an educational workbook for being

in the study.

A research team of three African American women interviewed the children. To increase the

children’s familiarity and comfort with the interviewers, the research team interacted with the

participants during class, lunch, and recess prior to beginning data collection. Individual inter-

views were conducted in a separate room in the school during regular school hours. The children

were audiotaped during these sessions. A professional transcription company transcribed the

audiotaped interviews verbatim.

The teachers were asked to complete the Child Behavior Scale (Ladd & Profilet, 1996) for

each of the children in the study. This measure was distributed at the close of the school year

in order to obtain a full assessment of each child’s behavior. Teachers were paid $2 for each

rating form they completed.

Measures

Situational sociomoral affective vignettes. Children were presented with a series of six

short vignettes reflective of three types of sociomoral events. Two vignettes were developed

for each type of morality: prosocial, active, and inhibitive morality. Prosocial morality vignettes

reflected the use of private resources to provide beneficial outcomes for others (e.g., sharing and

cooperating; Eisenberg, 1982). Vignettes for active morality featured interventions on behalf of

someone who was being victimized (e.g., helping and comforting; Tisak, 1986; Tisak & Ford,

1986). Inhibitive morality vignettes focused on victimizing others and unfairly depriving the

rights of others (e.g., stealing and hitting; Tisak & Ford, 1986; Turiel, 1983). Each vignette

was two to three sentences in length and was represented by two to three 8� 11’’ colored picture

cards featuring two African American child characters, an actor and a recipient. Sample vignettes

for the three moralities are as follows:

Malik brought his favorite transformer to school. He wanted to play with it during free play. When it

came time for free play, Dave asked to play with the transformer. (Prosocial)

Jasmine was walking home from school. She sees another child picking on Sharon. Jasmine

stops the child from picking on Sharon. (Active)

Lisa and Mya are running for the last swing. Mya gets to the swing first. Lisa pushes her down

and gets in the swing. (Inhibitive)

The characters did not have facial features so as not to lead the children in how they responded

to the vignettes. The gender of the characters matched the gender of the responding child.

After the children were read and shown the vignette picture cards, they were asked to identify

(a) how they would feel if they were the actor and the recipient in the vignettes (i.e., affective

valence), (b) why they would feel that emotion (i.e., affective attribution), (c) the intensity of the

emotion, and (d) what should happen next in the story (i.e., affective consequence). Consistent

with Arsenio (1988), scoring for the labeling or identification of emotions was as follows:

ATTRIBUTIONS AND CONSEQUENCES 217

Page 7: African American Children's Affective Attributions and Consequences Regarding Sociomoral Events

negative valence emotions (i.e., anger, fear, and sadness) were scored as�1, neutral emotions

(i.e., okay and fine) as 0, and positive emotions (i.e., happiness) as þ1. After labeling the emo-

tion, the children were asked to identify the intensity of that emotion (e.g., How [insert emotion

given by child] would you be?) while being presented with an emotional intensity card.

Emotional intensity was represented by five faces of varying emotional intensity on two separate

cards (one positive and one negative emotion) and scored on a 5-point scale, with 1 being low

emotional intensity and 5 being high emotional intensity.

Children’s qualitative responses regarding affective attributions (i.e., why the character felt a

particular emotion) and affective consequence (i.e., story completion) responses were coded

based on the social cognitive coding categories of Tisak (1986), Smetana et al. (2003), and

Arsenio and Fleiss (1996). Children’s affective attribution and consequence responses to each

morality vignette were read in their entirety before being coding for the following categories:

(a) Other’s Welfare—act involves creating negative outcomes for others, including violation

of one’s rights; (b) Rule Orientation=Authority—acts regulated by rules or authority;

(c) Order=Pragmatic Concerns—acts or justifications focused on social concerns, expectations,

and order; (d) Beneficial Outcomes—act involves creating positive outcomes for another; (e)

Hedonistic=Personal Gain—act involves gains or concern for one person over another; (f) Per-

sonal Business—involves an act or issue that is seen as the actor’s own personal business; (g)

Unelaborated—acts justified for no specific reason; and (h) Uncodable (see Table 1). The attri-

butions and consequences were coded categorically with 1 indicating endorsement of the cate-

gory and 0 indicating that a category was not endorsed. The average proportion of endorsements

of each attribution and consequence was obtained for each participant.

The scoring of the story completion task (e.g., affective attributions) was not only based on

these social cognitive categories but was also based on a 3-point story completion scoring sys-

tem by Dunn, Brown, and Maguire (1995). Story completions were scored according to who

attempted to resolve the conflict or dilemma in the sociomoral vignette: the actor and recipient

TABLE 1

Coding Categories for Affective Attributions and Consequences

Coding Category Response Types

Other’s Welfare Physical and emotional aggression

Transgressions

Rule Orientation=Authority Violation of norms, family, and classroom

Order=Pragmatic Concerns Courteous behavior

Tit-for-tat behavior

Beneficial Outcomes Intervention

Prosocial behavior

Hedonistic=Personal Gain Possession

Competition

Personal Business Missed opportunity

Play

Unelaborated Behavior continues or ceases

Characteristics of target object or character

Uncodable Missing data

Inaudible

218 HUMPHRIES

Page 8: African American Children's Affective Attributions and Consequences Regarding Sociomoral Events

resolved the conflict together or separately (2 points), the conflict was resolved by an external

influence (1 points), or there was no resolution or no story completion provided (0 point).

Piloting of these vignettes with children ages 3 to 11 years old supported the use of these

vignettes and interview procedure. Along with a second coder, an advanced female graduate stu-

dent, I read and coded all of the children’s interviews. The second coder participated in all

phases of coding in order to create stability and reliability during the coding process. Interrater

reliability for the coding of the affective attributions and affective consequences (i.e., the story

completion task) was 92.13% utilizing Miles and Huberman’s (1994) method.

Child behavior scale. This is a 35-item teacher rating scale that asks how often a child

exhibits a broad range of behaviors with his or her peers in school (Ladd & Profilet, 1996).

In the present study, three subscales, each composed of seven items, were utilized: aggressive

with peers (e.g., ‘‘This child fights’’), prosocial with peers (e.g., ‘‘This child helps’’), and

excluded by peers (e.g., ‘‘This child is excluded from peer activities’’). Each of the items

was responded to on a 3-point scale (1¼ rarely applies, 2¼ applies somewhat, and 3¼ certainly

applies). Subscale scores were obtained by averaging items in each subscale. For this study, the

aggressive and excluded subscales were combined to create one negative behavior subscale.

Cronbach’s alphas using the data from the current study ranged from .87 to .92.

RESULTS

The data analysis strategies presented here were used to test the following hypothesized relation-

ships among children’s affective attributions of emotions, their affective consequences, and their

behavior at school: (a) Children will differentiate their emotional responses across three types of

sociomoral events (i.e., prosocial, active, and inhibitive) and characters (i.e., actor and recipient),

(b) children whose causal attributions and affective consequences focus on beneficial outcomes

will be rated by teachers as being prosocial with their classmates, (c) children whose affective

attributions and affective consequences are concerned with transgressions or personal gain will

be rated as displaying more aggressive and negative behaviors with their classmates, and (d)

children in first and second grades will vary in terms of affective valence, attributions, and con-

sequences, including the happy victimizer effect, in comparison to children in prekindergarten

and kindergarten.

When testing for grade effects, I combined prekindergartners and kindergartners into one

group (n¼ 21) and combined first and second graders to create a second group (n¼ 35) as

opposed to examining each grade individually, given the small sample size. The means and stan-

dard deviations for all study variables are presented in Table 2. Affective valence, affective

intensity, and story completion represent the mean response ratings provided by participants.

Affective attributions and consequences represent the mean ratings of coding categories ident-

ified by participants across characters and sociomoral events.

Affective Valence—Character Role by Sociomoral Rule Systems

It was hypothesized that children would differentiate their emotional responses across the three

types of sociomoral events (i.e., prosocial, active, and inhibitive). Grade effects were tested by

ATTRIBUTIONS AND CONSEQUENCES 219

Page 9: African American Children's Affective Attributions and Consequences Regarding Sociomoral Events

comparing prekindergartners and kindergartners to first and second graders. To test this hypoth-

esis, I conducted a 2 (grade)� 2 (character role)� 3 (sociomoral event) repeated measures

analysis of variance on affective valence scores (positive, negative, or neutral emotion). Signifi-

cant main effects emerged for both character role, F(1, 52)¼ 56.41, p< .001; and sociomoral

rule system, F(2, 51)¼ 44.33, p< .001. Children’s affective valence scores differed in terms

of the characters (e.g., actors and recipients) and the sociomoral vignettes. However, there

was no significant main effect for grade.

Post hoc comparisons revealed that children’s affective valence of the prosocial sociomoral

rule system differed significantly from that of the inhibitive rule system, F(1, 52)¼ 56.41,

p< .001. The prosocial events were characterized by positive affect, whereas the inhibitive

events were characterized by negative affect. There was no significant difference between the

affective valence of the active and inhibitive sociomoral events. Results also revealed that actors’

and recipients’ affective valence differed between the prosocial and inhibitive sociomoral events,

F(1, 52)¼ 21.78, p< .001; and between the active and inhibitive sociomoral events, F(1,52)¼ 11.69, p< .01. The actors in the active sociomoral events were characterized with more

negative affect than the actors in the inhibitive sociomoral event.

It was hypothesized that children would vary their affective valence by character role for the

sociomoral events. There was a significant interaction of character role (actor and recipient) and

sociomoral event on affective valence, F(2, 51)¼ 10.68, p< .001. This indicated that children’s

affective valence for actors and recipients differed by the sociomoral event (i.e., prosocial,

active, and inhibitive morality). Figure 1 presents the mean affective valence scores by character

role and sociomoral event. This figure shows the affective valence that children assigned to each

TABLE 2

Mean Scores

Variable M SD Range

Affective valence �0.16 0.34 �1.00 to 0.50

Affective intensity 3.35 0.83 1.75–5.00

Story completion: Solver 1.25 0.45 0–2.00

Affective attributions

Other’s Welfare 0.30 0.16 0–0.58

Rule Orientation 0.04 0.07 0–0.33

Order=Pragmatic Concerns 0.08 0.09 0–0.33

Beneficial Outcomes 0.13 0.12 0–0.50

Hedonistic=Personal Gain 0.16 0.11 0–0.42

Personal Business 0.11 0.09 0–0.33

Affective consequences

Other’s Welfare 0.05 0.10 0–0.50

Order=Pragmatic Concerns 0.17 0.15 0–0.67

Beneficial Outcomes 0.36 0.23 0–1.00

Hedonistic=Personal Gain 0.03 0.07 0–0.33

Personal Business 0.08 0.11 0–0.33

Child Behavior Scale scores

Prosocial behavior 2.54 0.46 1.29–3.00

Negative behavior 1.37 0.41 1.00–2.46

220 HUMPHRIES

Page 10: African American Children's Affective Attributions and Consequences Regarding Sociomoral Events

character across the three sociomoral events. Children assigned more positive affect to both char-

acters in the prosocial morality vignettes and negative affect to both characters in the active mor-

ality vignettes. Unlike the affect assigned to the characters in the prosocial and active morality

vignettes, there was a difference in the affect assigned to the characters in the inhibitive morality

vignette. Actors were assigned affect that was more neutral to positive, whereas recipients were

assigned more negative affect.

Based on these findings, t tests were performed to determine whether the characters differed

in terms of the affective valence assigned to them. The recipient (M¼�0.90) in the inhibitive

morality vignettes was characterized with a more negative affective valence than the actor

(M¼ 0.15), t(55)¼�18.02, p< 001. The mean valence rating alone does not completely illus-

trate children’s responses to the inhibitive morality vignette, especially in relation to the happy

victimizer effect. Specifically, half (53.6%) of the children characterized the actor with positive

affect, whereas the other half (41.1%) reported negative affect. Upon further examination of

these findings, it was found that the majority (61.9%) of children who characterized the actor

with positive affect were young children (prekindergartners and kindergartners). The older chil-

dren (first and second graders) equally assigned positive and negative affect to the actor. Figure 2

presents affective valence by grade, morality type, and character role.

Affective Attributions

To better understand the reasons why children assigned emotions to each of the characters in the

vignettes, I analyzed children’s qualitative responses to their affective attributions. Two cate-

gories, Other’s Welfare and Hedonistic=Personal Gain, supported more than two thirds of the

children’s explanations of the cause of emotions for both actors and recipients in the inhibitive

events. Those children who characterized the actor with negative affect largely focused on the

welfare of the character. An example of a response that represents the category Other’s Welfare

was ‘‘Because somebody pushed me out of the way and just took the swing that I [she] was

going to get’’ (first-grade girl). Those children who characterized the actor with positive affect

FIGURE 1 Mean ratings for affective valence by character role and morality type. (Color figure available online.)

ATTRIBUTIONS AND CONSEQUENCES 221

Page 11: African American Children's Affective Attributions and Consequences Regarding Sociomoral Events

often provided responses that focused on acts involving gains or concerns for the actor over the

recipient. This supported the Hedonistic=Personal Gain category.

Both actors and recipients in the prosocial vignettes were overwhelmingly characterized with

positive affect. Most responses focused on the Beneficial Outcomes and Order=Pragmatic Con-

cerns categories. Children who reasoned that the character’s feeling state was due to another

character or someone else engaging in a positive behavior (e.g., ‘‘[She’s happy] because she

let her see my baby doll,’’ prekindergarten girl) supported the Beneficial Outcomes category.

A representative response by a prekindergarten girl for the Order=Pragmatic Concerns category

was ‘‘ ‘Cause she didn’t let Kelly [recipient] see it.’’

Themes most frequently provided by children for active morality supported the Other’s

Welfare, Beneficial Outcomes (only for actors), and Hedonistic=Personal Gain categories.

Responses that supported the Beneficial Outcomes category were ‘‘Because, my friend was cry-

ing. And I’m not happy if my friend’s not happy’’ (second-grade girl) and ‘‘Because he gave

him a hug’’ (kindergarten boy).

Affective Consequences

By examining the affective consequences of each vignette, I was able to understand how chil-

dren may or may not attempt to resolve the conflicts in the sociomoral events. The percentages

of children’s responses for affective consequences for both characters across the three morality

types by coding category are presented in Table 3. Children provided affective consequences

that typically represented the Beneficial Outcomes category in the prosocial and inhibitive mor-

ality vignettes. They provided affective consequences that created positive outcomes for the

characters. A second-grade boy indicated in his affective consequence to a prosocial morality

vignette that ‘‘He let him see it [transformer] and they played together.’’ In response to an inhi-

bitive morality vignette, a prekindergarten girl stated, ‘‘She says sorry and lets her get in the

swing. To share.’’ This was a similar response provided by other children in regard to the

FIGURE 2 Mean ratings for affective valence by grade for character role and morality type. A¼ actor; R¼ recipient;

PreK¼ prekindergarten; K¼ kindergarten. (Color figure available online.)

222 HUMPHRIES

Page 12: African American Children's Affective Attributions and Consequences Regarding Sociomoral Events

TABLE

3

PercentagesofAffectiveConsequencesandSolvers

ofAffectiveConsequencesAcrossSociomoralRule

Systems

Morality

Affective

Consequences(%

)So

lversof

Dilem

ma(%

)

Other’s

Welfare

Order=

Pragm

atic

Concerns

Beneficial

Outcomes

Hedon

istic=

Persona

lGain

Persona

lBusiness

Unelabo

rated

Uncodab

leActor=Recipient

Together

orSepa

rately

Outside

Intervener

No

Resolution

Prosocial

5.35

31.25

40.2

8.05

3.6

4.45

5.35

69.65

2.7

25.2

Active

1.8

1.8

26.75

1.8

17.85

33.95

14.3

45.55

23.9

27.7

Inhibitive

8.9

17

41.95

02.7

14.25

12.25

39.3

25.9

29.5

223

Page 13: African American Children's Affective Attributions and Consequences Regarding Sociomoral Events

inhibitive morality vignette. Children attempted to correct an inappropriate or negative behavior.

However, some children’s responses to the inhibitive morality vignette introduced another indi-

vidual into the vignette to assist in creating positive outcomes. For instance, a kindergarten boy

responded, ‘‘He told the teacher, and the teacher said give him that truck and, he gave him the

truck and then the teacher give it back.’’

Children provided affective consequences to the active morality vignettes that typically repre-

sented the Unelaborated and Beneficial Outcomes categories. The Unelaborated category repre-

sented responses that did not provide an explanation for why the behavior ceased. For instance,

in the helping vignette for active morality, children would have the victimizer walk away from

the recipient after the actor told him or her to stop picking on the recipient. In addition to the

Beneficial Outcomes category, the Order=Pragmatic Concerns category was also representative

of attribution consequences provided for prosocial morality vignettes. Children’s responses

focused on maintaining social order.

Also included in Table 3 is who the child identified as the solver of the dilemma in each

sociomoral type (i.e., the actor or recipient together or separately, an outside intervener, or no

resolution or story completion). Children were more likely to indicate that the actor or the recipi-

ent either would solve the dilemma individually or would work together to solve the dilemma

across all three morality types.

Teacher-Rated Children’s Behavior

It was hypothesized that children’s affective attributions and consequences would be associated

with children’s behavior. Specifically, children whose affective attributions and affective conse-

quences focused on positive outcomes for others or social concerns and expectations would be

rated by teachers as being prosocial with their peers. In contrast, children whose affective attribu-

tions and affective consequences were concerned with transgressions or personal gain would be

rated as being more aggressive and excluded.

A median split was conducted on children’s prosocial and negative behavior scores because

of the lack of variability in children’s scores in the mid-range (e.g., children were rated at the

extreme ends of the Child Behavior Scale). The median split scores for prosocial and negative

behavior were utilized in subsequent analyses.

A series of 2 (grade)� 2 (gender) analyses of variance were conducted to examine possible

differences in teacher ratings of children’s prosocial behavior and negative behavior variables.

These analyses yielded nonsignificant findings for both grade and gender. No grade or gender

differences emerged for children’s behavior. Thus, grade and gender were not included in sub-

sequent analyses.

Correlation analyses. Correlation coefficients were computed among the affective and

behavior variables (see Table 4). Children’s Beneficial Outcomes theme of affective attribution

was positively related to teacher-rated prosocial behavior (r¼ .39, p< .01). However, there was

a significant negative relationship between children’s Personal Business affective attribution and

prosocial behavior (r¼�.31, p< .05; see Table 2). Children who were rated as prosocial were

less likely to provide affective attributions that focused on the actors own personal domain. No

other significant relationships emerged regarding children’s behavior.

224 HUMPHRIES

Page 14: African American Children's Affective Attributions and Consequences Regarding Sociomoral Events

TABLE

4

CorrelationsofEmotio

nalVariablesandChildren’s

Behavior

Variable

12

34

56

78

910

1112

1314

1.Affectivevalence

Affectiveattributions

2.Other’s

Welfare

�.27�

3.Rule

Orientation

�.20

�.21

4.Order=Pragmatic

Concerns

�.08

.02

.12

5.BeneficialOutcomes

.36�

�.16

.02

�.08

6.Hedonistic=Personal

Gain

.23

�.21

.00

�.01

�.12

7.Personal

Business

�.05

�.02

.22

�.20

�.22

.07

Affectiveconsequences

8.Other’s

Welfare

�.03

.11

�.09

�.22

.04

�.23

�.04

9.Order=Pragmatic

Concerns

�.39��

.20

.24

.12

�.20

�.15

.08

�.21

10.BeneficialOutcomes

.28�

.04

.04

.21

.17

.24

�.03

�.09

–.31�

11.Hedonistic=Personal

Gain

.17

�.06

�.13

�.08

.02

.20

.15

�.24

�.14

�.11

12.Personal

Business

�.06

.22

�.09

�.11

�.07

.12

.11

�.13

�.07

�.14

.11

Behavior

13.Prosocial

behavior

.04

.19

.17

�.10

.39��

�.12

�.31�

�.03

.12

.12

�.24

04

14.Negativebehavior

.26

�.12

�.09

.23

�.11

.08

.15

.23

�.18

�.01

.21

.00

�.44��

� p<.05;��p<.01.

225

Page 15: African American Children's Affective Attributions and Consequences Regarding Sociomoral Events

Regression analyses. Standard multiple regressions were conducted to determine whether

children’s affective attributions (Other’s Welfare, Rule Orientation, Order=Pragmatic Concerns,

Beneficial Outcomes, Hedonistic=Personal Gain, and Personal Business) and emotional conse-

quences (Other’s Welfare, Order=Pragmatic Concerns, Beneficial Outcomes, Hedonis-

tic=Personal Gain, and Personal Business) were associated with teacher reports of their

behavior. There was no theoretical orientation for the ordering of the affective attributions

and consequences in the regression equation, and as a result they were entered together in

one step. Separate regression equations were computed for children’s prosocial and negative

behaviors, affective attributions, and affective consequences, respectively.

The first regression equation examined whether affective attributions were predictive of chil-

dren’s prosocial behavior. The overall regression model for children’s prosocial behavior was

significant, F(5, 40)¼ 3.29, p< .01, and explained 29% of the variance (R2¼ .29). The effect

size was large (Cohen’s f2¼ 0.41). As predicted, the Beneficial Outcomes (b¼ 1.47, p< .01)

along with the Personal Business (b¼�1.60, p< .05) attributions were significant predictors

of children’s prosocial behavior. Children who provided affective attributions that focused on

beneficial outcomes for others were rated by their teachers as exhibiting prosocial behavior,

whereas children who provided affective attributions that centered on one’s personal business

were less likely to be rated as prosocial by their teachers.

A second regression equation examined the predictive ability of affective attributions on

negative behavior. Findings did not support affective attributions as predictors of children’s

negative behavior, F(5, 40)¼ 1.12, ns.To determine whether children’s emotional consequence themes (Other’s Welfare,

Order=Pragmatic Concerns, Beneficial Outcomes, Hedonistic=Personal Gain, and Personal

Business) were significant predictors of behavior, I conducted two standard multiple regressions

for prosocial and negative behavior. Contrary to my prediction, children’s emotional conse-

quences were not significantly associated with either prosocial or negative behavior.

DISCUSSION

This study examined young (prekindergarten through second grade) African American chil-

dren’s affective attributions and consequences for characters in different sociomoral events

and their relationship to their behavior at school. Consistent with the proposed hypothesis, chil-

dren’s affective responses were informed by sociomoral rule systems. Specifically, children

attributed positive affective valence to characters in the prosocial morality events while associ-

ating more negative affect to characters in both the active and inhibitive morality events. Chil-

dren’s affective attributions and consequences also varied by sociomoral event and character

role. Regardless of the type of sociomoral event, children attempted to resolve the dilemmas pre-

sented in the different sociomoral vignettes. Their solutions often had either both characters’

needs being met or the recipient being vindicated, indicating awareness of the moral dilemma.

Although the actors and recipients in the prosocial morality rule system were characterized

with positive affect, some children reported negative affect being associated with acts of sharing

and cooperating. The affective attributions children provided to explain the actors’ negative

affect focused on not wanting to share or cooperate (Beneficial Outcomes) or not having the

chance to use the object that the recipient had requested (e.g., Personal Business category).

226 HUMPHRIES

Page 16: African American Children's Affective Attributions and Consequences Regarding Sociomoral Events

The negative affect assigned to the recipient centered on the recipient’s request being denied or

refused by the actor (Order=Pragmatic Concerns). It may be inappropriate to expect all young

children to be happy for the opportunity to share. Despite the negative affect associated with

prosocial morality, the majority of the children provided affective consequences that had the

characters engage in prosocial morality behaviors (e.g., sharing and cooperating behaviors).

Children often had the actor and the recipient work together to resolve the dilemma presented

in the sociomoral event. This finding differs from prior research in which 6- to 11-year-old

middle-class, majority Caucasian children relied on an authority figure when dealing with active

and inhibitory events (Tisak & Ford, 1986). It is unclear whether the difference in the present

findings and the aforementioned study’s finding is one of race, age, socioeconomic status,

or an intersection of these variables. This illustrates that children are cognizant of and largely

conform to social norms around sharing and cooperating, despite their negative affect about

the situation. Future research might focus on how African American children of different socio-

economic statuses (e.g., low income, working class, middle class) and older children reconcile

their emotions and behaviors regarding sociomoral events.

The examination of the affective attributions and consequences provides a better understand-

ing of how African American children think about sociomoral situations and how they use this

information. If there had been a focus on only the affective labels and not on the reasoning

behind children’s responses, the children in this study may have appeared to be lacking socio-

moral or social-emotional competence skills. Instead, it appears that these African American

children are quite aware and knowledgeable of sociomoral rules, as evidenced by attempts to

rectify the sociomoral dilemmas despite having conflicting or uncomfortable feelings about

the events. This provides support for understanding of the contributors to African American

children’s social competence (American Psychological Association, 2008).

There has been significant discussion regarding children’s tendency to attribute positive affect

to actors in inhibitive events. This happy victimizer phenomenon focuses on positive affect due

to gaining material resources. Similar to previous research with racially diverse samples of

young children (e.g., Arsenio & Kramer, 1992; Nunner-Winkler & Sodian, 1988; Smetana

et al., 2003; Wiersma & Laupa, 2000), the younger African American children (e.g., prekinder-

gartners and kindergartners) tended to conform to this pattern. About half of the older children

characterized the actor with negative affect, whereas the remaining children characterized the

actor with positive affect (e.g., the happy victimizer). A more subtle developmental shift

occurred for the older African American children in the current study. Specifically, 8-year olds

presented a more mixed emotional characterization of the victimizer, equally identifying both

positive and negative emotions. It appears that first and second graders may be experiencing

a developmental shift whereby victimization is no longer strictly viewed in terms of the material

gain. Rather, they are beginning to acknowledge the negative emotional experiences of the vic-

tims (i.e., the recipient). These findings support a more subtle moral attributional shift proposed

by other researchers (e.g., Arsenio & Kramer, 1992; Arsenio & Lover, 1995; Lourenco, 1997),

but among an African American sample.

Some children, however, suggested retaliatory actions in their affective consequences (i.e.,

story completion task) to inhibitive sociomoral rule systems. These children appeared to be

attempting to right a wrong that had occurred within the vignette. For instance, the recipient

would engage in a retaliatory behavior of hitting in order to re-obtain an object that was unfairly

taken from them. This finding is consistent with the notion that although children understand the

ATTRIBUTIONS AND CONSEQUENCES 227

Page 17: African American Children's Affective Attributions and Consequences Regarding Sociomoral Events

immorality of inhibitive behaviors, some children view retaliation in nonfamily relationships as a

necessary form of reciprocal justice (Astor, 1994: Herzberger & Hall, 1993; Jagers, 2001; Ward,

1988). This finding has practical implications for social and emotional competence. Helping

children understand culturally and contextually acceptable ways to right a wrong without retali-

ation is very promising, especially for positive behavior promotion and negative behavior

reduction among young African American children. However, there may be some contexts

for African American children, especially those characterized with limited resources and

environmental risks, in which certain acts of retaliation may be deemed an appropriate response.

There needs to be an understanding of African American children’s cultural and socioeconomic

context and how this influences children’s behavior and conceptions of moral events.

The hypothesized association between children’s knowledge of affective attributions and con-

sequences to children’s behavior was partially supported. Specifically, the Beneficial Outcomes

and Personal Business affective attributions were associated with children’s prosocial behavior.

Children who reasoned that the cause of emotions was due to the creation of positive outcomes

for others and issues that were seen as private to the person were more likely to be rated as exhi-

biting prosocial behaviors. However, no affective attribution themes were associated with nega-

tive behavior with peers. In addition, children’s reasoning about affective consequences was not

associated with either prosocial or negative behavior. These inconsistent findings are surprising

given that several studies have found a significant relationship between other emotional and

social competence abilities and behavior (Arsenio, Cooperman, & Lover, 2000; Garner,

Dunsmore, & Southam-Gerrow, 2008; Garner, Jones, & Miner, 1994; Schultz, Izard, Ackerman,

& Youngstrom, 2001). Similar to other research (Garner, 1996), there were no grade and gender

differences among this sample of African American children.

Perhaps the relationship between affective consequences and behavior did not emerge

because I only examined a direct relationship between affective consequences and behavior.

The relationship between affect and behavior may be an indirect one that is mediated by chil-

dren’s actual emotional experiences or the social context. The absence of a relationship between

affective consequences and behavior may also be partially due to an exclusive reliance on tea-

cher reports. It might be more informative to include multiple raters (e.g., peers, family mem-

bers, and observers) to gain a more diverse and possibly more accurate assessment of

children’s behavior (Humphries, Keenan, & Wakschlag, 2010). The use of multiple informants

may allow for a more comprehensive view of African American children’s behavior and have

greater predictive power for emotional competence abilities.

Limitations

This study utilized a small sample with a cross-sectional design to examine African American

children’s affective attributions and consequences to sociomoral events. However, this type of

design does not provide information on how children’s affective knowledge develops over time,

and thus only inferences can be made regarding developmental changes. Future research should

also utilize a longitudinal design to study African American children from diverse backgrounds

to see how their affective knowledge changes over time as a function of age and social experi-

ences. Information obtained from this and future research also could and should be utilized to

benefit and inform clinical and education practice and prevention or intervention programs.

228 HUMPHRIES

Page 18: African American Children's Affective Attributions and Consequences Regarding Sociomoral Events

Specifically, this information could inform the cognitions and behaviors that prevention

and intervention programs target to support and possibly maximize African American children’s

prosocial behavior.

Before researchers begin to create social and emotional competence curricula and programs

targeted at young African American children, children from other minority backgrounds, and=orchildren from economically stressed environments, there needs to be an understanding of not

only the classroom environment but also the individual variability of social and emotional com-

petence among these populations in order to understand which behaviors and=or cognitions

should be targeted for promotion or intervention. There is a call for more strengths-based assess-

ments of low-income children’s competence abilities in order for competence curricula and=orintervention programs to target the appropriate emotional and social competence skills for this

population (McWayne, Hampton, Fantuzzo, Cohen, & Sekino, 2004). Programs with the inten-

tion of promoting the positive and optimal development of African American children from

economically stressed backgrounds should be based on the normal development of these chil-

dren as opposed to their deviant or compromised development, as prosocial behavior is not

merely the absence of aggressive or negative behavior (Malti et al., 2009).

Despite initial empirical support for the association among affective attributions, conse-

quences, and behavior, there is still much to be explored regarding African American children.

Given that children’s development is influenced by their race, socioeconomic status, and gender,

it is important to understand African American children’s development from their own perspec-

tives. It is also important to acknowledge that children can often identify appropriate ways to

behave but have a difficult time enacting the desired behavioral repertoire in school and peer

environments. Despite this, the link between children’s affective attributions and behavior

may be a promising one for prevention scientists. This is evident in the increase in the number

of prevention and intervention programs that are developed to target emotional and social com-

petence (e.g., PATHS, Smart Kids, and Second Step) and character development (e.g., Character

Counts, Voices Reading, Lions Quest) among children and adolescents. These programs focus

on promoting competence and character development to increase children’s prosocial behaviors

with their peers and decrease negative, aggressive, and=or disruptive behaviors, as well as

increase academic engagement and success. The findings from the current research point to

the need to truly understand the reasoning that African American children use when they experi-

ence a sociomoral event in order to best support the development of the emotional and social

competence of these children. This may provide both teachers and clinicians who work with

young children an understanding of how to support and effectively promote African American

children’s prosocial development (Malti et al., 2009), in particular around sociomoral behaviors

that are often present in classroom peer interactions.

Future research can be used to inform the creation and implementation of more effective

social and emotional competence programs and curricula for African American children. Thus,

there is a need to promote the development of problem-solving skills within the context of emo-

tions or affect among African American children during social interactions in which moral beha-

viors like sharing, comforting, and hitting are experienced and witnessed. It may not be

sufficient to exclusively focus on the emotion children ascribe in various sociomoral interactions,

but there needs to be an understanding of how children rectify their emotional knowledge and

reasoning when dealing with provocative events. Perhaps competence promotion programs can

help children understand that engaging in contextually appropriate behavior may generate mixed

ATTRIBUTIONS AND CONSEQUENCES 229

Page 19: African American Children's Affective Attributions and Consequences Regarding Sociomoral Events

emotions (i.e., sharing can produce sadness and happiness in the sharer) and that this mixed

emotional experience is appropriate. More research is needed to continue to examine the link

between affective attributions and consequences and African American children’s behavior.

Experts need to understand the developmental processes that allow for children’s social suc-

cess (Raver & Zigler, 1997). This is especially relevant if they are interested in developing pre-

vention and intervention programs that are effective. There is a need to understand the multiple

components of competence, including its cultural, cognitive, and behavioral components, and

their positive impact on African American children’s lives. The field must make certain that

the developmental process of competence for children from diverse racial=ethnic and socioeco-

nomic environments is represented.

REFERENCES

American Psychological Association, Task Force on Resilience and Strength in Black Children & Adolescents. (2008).

Resilience in African American children and adolescents: A vision for optimal development. Washington, DC:

Author.

Arsenio, W. F. (1988). Children’s conceptions of the situational affective consequences of sociomoral events. Child

Development, 59, 1611–1622.Arsenio, W. F., Cooperman, S., & Lover, A. (2000). Affective predictors of preschoolers’ aggression and peer accept-

ance: Direct and indirect effects. Developmental Psychology, 36, 438–448.

Arsenio, W. F., & Fleiss, K. (1996). Typical and behaviorally disruptive children’s understanding of the emotional

consequences of socio-moral events. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 173–186.Arsenio, W. F., & Kramer, R. (1992). Victimizers and their victims: Children’s conceptions of the mixed emotional

consequences of moral transgressions. Child Development, 63, 915–927.

Arsenio, W. F., & Lemerise, E. A. (2004). Aggression and moral development: Integrating social information processing

and moral domain models. Child Development, 75, 987–1002.

Arsenio, W., & Lover, A. (1995). Children’s conceptions of sociomoral affect: Happy victimizers, mixed emotions, and

other experiences. In M. Killen, & D. Hart (Eds.),Morality in everyday life: Developmental perspectives, (pp. 87–128).

New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Astor, R. A. (1994). Children’s moral reasoning about family and peer violence: The role of provocation and retribution.

Child Development, 65, 1054–1067.

Barden, R. C., Zelko, F. A., Duncan, S. W., & Masters, J. C. (1980). Children’s consensual knowledge about the experi-

ential determinants of emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 968–976.Bocknek, E. L., Brophy-Herb, H. E., & Banerjee, M. (2009). Effects of parental supportiveness on toddlers’ emotion

regulation over the first three years of life in a low-income African American sample. Infant Mental Health Journal,

30, 452–476.

Boykin, A. W., Jagers, R. J., Ellison, C., & Albury, A. (1997). Communalism: Conceptualization and measurement of an

Afrocultural social ethos. Journal of Black Studies, 27, 409–418.

Burchinal, M., Roberts, J. E., Zeisel, S. A., Hennon, E. A., & Hooper, S. (2006). Social risk and protective child, parent-

ing, and child care factors in early elementary school years. Parenting: Science and Practice, 6(1), 79–113.Chaplin, T. M., Cole, P. M., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2005). Parent socialization of emotion expression: Gender differences

and relations to child adjustment. Emotion, 5, 80–88.

Denham, S. A., Mitchell-Copeland, J., Strandberg, K., Auerbach, S., & Blair, K. (1997). Parental contributions to

preschoolers’ emotional competence: Direct and indirect effects. Motivation and Emotion, 21, 65–86.Dunn, J., Brown, J. R., & Maguire, M. (1995). The development of children’s moral sensibility: Individual differences

and emotion understanding. Developmental Psychology, 31, 649–659.

Eisenberg, N. (1982). The development of prosocial behavior. New York, NY: Academic Press.

Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Carlo, G., & Karbon, M. (1992). Emotional responsivity to others: Behavioral correlates and

socialization antecedents. In N. Eisenberg, & R. A. Fabes (Eds.), New directions for child development: No. 55.

Emotion and its regulation in early development (pp. 57–73). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

230 HUMPHRIES

Page 20: African American Children's Affective Attributions and Consequences Regarding Sociomoral Events

Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Murphy, B., Maszk, P., Smith, M., & Karbon, M. (1995). The role of emotionality and regu-

lation in children’s social functioning: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 66, 1360–1384.

Elias, M. J. (2009). Social-emotional and character development and academics as a dual focus of educational policy.

Education Policy, 23, 831–846.

Gabalda, M. K., Thompson, M. P., & Kaslow, N. J. (2010). Risk and protective factors for psychological adjustment

among low-income African American children. Journal of Family Issues, 31, 423–444.

Garner, P. W. (1996). The relations of emotional role-taking, affective=moral attributions, and emotional display rule

knowledge to low-income school-age children’s social competence. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology,17, 19–36.

Garner, P. W. (2006). Prediction of prosocial and emotional competence from maternal behavior in African American

preschoolers. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 12, 179–198.

Garner, P. W., Dunsmore, J. C., & Southam-Gerrow, M. (2008). Mother–child conversations about emotions: Linkages

to child aggression and prosocial behavior. Social Development, 17, 259–277.

Garner, P. W., Jones, D., & Miner, J. L. (1994). Social competence among low-income preschoolers: Emotion socializa-

tion practices and social cognitive correlates. Child Development, 65, 622–637.

Garner, P. W., & Waajid, B. (2008). The associations of emotion knowledge and teacher-child relationships to preschool

children’s school-related developmental competence. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 29, 89–100.

Gibbs, J. C., Widaman, K. F., & Colby, A. (1982). Construction and validation of a simplified group administrable

equivalent to the moral judgment interview. Child Development, 53, 895–910.Goldstein, S. E., Tisak, M. S., & Boxer, P. (2002). Preschoolers’ normative and prescriptive judgments about relational

and overt aggression. Early Education & Development, 13, 23–40.

Halberstadt, A. G., Denham, S. A., & Dunsmore, J. C. (2001). Affective social competence. Social Development, 10,

79–119.

Herzberger, S. D., & Hall, J. A. (1993). Consequences of retaliatory aggression against siblings and peers: Urban

minority children’s expectations. Child Development, 64, 1773–1785.

Hoffman, M. L. (1983). Affective and cognitive processes in moral internalization. In E. T. Higgins, D. N. Ruble, &

W. W. Hartup (Eds.), Social cognition and social development: A sociocultural perspective (pp. 236–274).

Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

Humphries, M. L., & Jagers, R. J. (2009). Culture: A possible predictor of morality for African American adolescents.

Journal of Research on Adolescence, 19, 205–215.Humphries, M. L., Keenan, K., & Wakschlag, L. (2010). Teacher-observer ratings of young African American children’s

social & emotional competence. Unpublished manuscript.

Humphries, M. L., Parker, B. L., & Jagers, R. J. (2000). Predictors of moral reasoning among African American children:

A preliminary study. Journal of Black Psychology, 26, 51–64.Jagers, R. J. (2001). Cultural integrity and social and emotional competence promotion: Work notes on moral com-

petence. Journal of Negro Education, 70, 59–71.

Jagers, R. J., Bingham, K., & Hans, S. L. (1996). Socialization and social judgments among inner-city African-American

kindergartners. Child Development, 67, 140–150.Kuther, T. L., & Wallace, S. A. (2003). Community violence and sociomoral development: An African American cultural

perspective. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 73, 177–189.

Ladd, G. W., & Profilet, S. M. (1996). The Child Behavior Scale: A teacher-report measure of young children’s aggress-

ive, withdrawn, and prosocial behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 32, 1008–1024.Lourenco, O. (1997). Children’s attributions of moral emotions to victimizers: Some data, doubts and suggestions. British

Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 425–438.

Malti, T., Gasser, L., & Buchmann, M. (2009). Aggressive and prosocial children’s emotion attributions and moral

reasoning. Aggressive Behavior, 35, 90–102.

McLoyd, V. C. (1990). The impact of economic hardship on Black families and children: Psychological distress, parent-

ing, and socioemotional development. Child Development, 61, 311–346.

McWayne, C., Hampton, V., Fantuzzo, J., Cohen, H., & Sekino, Y. (2004). A multivariate examination of parent

involvement and the social and academic competencies of urban kindergarten children. Psychology in the Schools,

41, 1–15.

Mendez, J. L., Fantuzzo, J., & Cicchetti, D. (2002). Profiles of social competence with low-income African American

preschool children. Child Development, 73, 1085–1100.

ATTRIBUTIONS AND CONSEQUENCES 231

Page 21: African American Children's Affective Attributions and Consequences Regarding Sociomoral Events

Miles, M. B., & Huberman, A. M. (1994). Qualitative data analysis: An expanded sourcebook. Thousand Oaks,

CA: Sage.

Nucci, L. P., & Herman, S. (1982). Behavioral disordered children’s conceptions of moral, conventional, and personal

issues. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 10, 411–425.

Nunner-Winkler, G., & Sodian, B. (1988). Children’s understanding of moral emotions. Child Development, 59,1323–1338.

Ramos-Marcuse, F., & Arsenio, W. (2001). Young children’s emotionally-charged moral narratives: Relations with

attachment and behavior problems and competencies. Early Education & Development, 12, 165–184.Raver, C. C., & Zigler, E. F. (1997). Social competence: An untapped dimension in evaluating Head Start’s success.

Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 12, 363–385.

Saarni, C. (1990). Emotional competence. In R. Thompson (Ed.), Nebraska symposium: Socioemotional development

(pp. 115–161). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

Saarni, C. (1997). Emotional competence and self-regulation in childhood. In P. Salovey, & D. Sluyter (Eds.), Emotion

development and emotional intelligence: Educational implications (pp. 35–69). New York, NY: Basic Books.

Schultz, D., Izard, C. E., Ackerman, B. P, & Youngstrom, E. A. (2001). Emotion knowledge in economically disadvan-

taged children: Self-regulatory antecedents and relations to social difficulties and withdrawal. Development &Psychopathology, 13, 53–67.

Smetana, J. G., Campione-Barr, N., & Yell, N. (2003). Children’s moral and affective judgments regarding provocation

and retaliation. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 49, 209–236.Smetana, J. G., Schlagman, N., & Adams, P. W. (1993). Preschool children’s judgments about hypothetical and actual

transgressions. Child Development, 64, 202–214.

Smith, M. (2001). Social and emotional competencies: Contributions to young African-American children’s peer accept-

ance. Early Education & Development, 12, 49–72.Tisak, M. S. (1986). Children’s conceptions of parental authority. Child Development, 57, 166–176.

Tisak, M. S., & Ford, M. E. (1986). Children’s conceptions of interpersonal events. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 32,

291–306.

Tisak, M. S., Nucci, L. P., & Jankowski, A. M. (1996). Preschool children’s social interactions involving moral and

prudential transgressions: An observational study. Early Education & Development, 7, 137–148.

Tisak, M. S., & Turiel, E. (1988). Variation in seriousness of transgressions and children’s moral and conventional

concepts. Developmental Psychology, 24, 352–357.Turiel, E. (1983). The development of social knowledge: Morality and convention. Cambridge, England: Cambridge

University Press.

Ward, J. V. (1988). Urban adolescents’ conceptions of violence. In C. Gilligan, J. Ward, & J. Taylor (Eds.), Mapping the

moral domain (pp. 175–200). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Wiersma, N., & Laupa, M. (2000). Young children’s conception of the emotional consequences of varied social events.

Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 46, 325–341.

Woods, L. N., & Jagers, R. J. (2003). Are cultural values predictors of moral reasoning in African American adolescents?

Journal of Black Psychology, 29, 102–118.

232 HUMPHRIES


Top Related