reinterpreting the relation between external efficacy and political participation - the role of...

27
Joost de Moor University of Leuven, Center for Citizenship and Democracy, Parkstraat 45, B-3000 Leuven (Belgium), [email protected] Reinterpreting the relation between external efficacy and political participation - The role of output structures for state- and non-state oriented action forms Paper presented at the 5th ECPR General Conference Glasgow, 4-6 September 2014 Abstract. From an instrumental point of view, political efficacy is one of the most important predictors of political participation. The more effective people consider certain forms of action to be, the more likely they are to engage in them. Numerous studies have stressed the role of citizens’ perceptions of the state as a political context to explain (the perceived effectiveness of) political participation, yet in doing so they have mainly focused on perceptions of the state’s willingness to be responsive i.e., citizens’ external input efficacy. Perceptions of the state’s ability to be responsive i.e., external output efficacy are largely overlooked. The main goal of this paper is to address this gap in the literature by analyzing the link between external output efficacy and different forms of state- and non-state oriented forms of participation. To do so, data are used from the 2014 PARTIREP Belgian election survey (N = 2015). In line with the hypotheses, it is found that high levels of external output efficacy indirectly increase state-oriented political participation, while non-state oriented political participation is unaffected. Key words: Political participation, political efficacy, external efficacy.

Upload: su-se

Post on 06-Feb-2023

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Joost de Moor

University of Leuven, Center for Citizenship and Democracy, Parkstraat 45,

B-3000 Leuven (Belgium), [email protected]

Reinterpreting the relation between external efficacy and political

participation

-

The role of output structures for state- and non-state oriented action forms

Paper presented at the 5th ECPR General Conference

Glasgow, 4-6 September 2014

Abstract. From an instrumental point of view, political efficacy is one of the most important

predictors of political participation. The more effective people consider certain forms of

action to be, the more likely they are to engage in them. Numerous studies have stressed the

role of citizens’ perceptions of the state as a political context to explain (the perceived

effectiveness of) political participation, yet in doing so they have mainly focused on

perceptions of the state’s willingness to be responsive – i.e., citizens’ external input efficacy.

Perceptions of the state’s ability to be responsive – i.e., external output efficacy – are largely

overlooked. The main goal of this paper is to address this gap in the literature by analyzing

the link between external output efficacy and different forms of state- and non-state oriented

forms of participation. To do so, data are used from the 2014 PARTIREP Belgian election

survey (N = 2015). In line with the hypotheses, it is found that high levels of external output

efficacy indirectly increase state-oriented political participation, while non-state oriented

political participation is unaffected.

Key words: Political participation, political efficacy, external efficacy.

1

Introduction

Citizens’ political participation ‘beyond the vote’ is often considered to be an essential

condition for a good rule of, by, and for the people (most recently: della Porta, 2013). For

one, increased levels of political participation improve citizen-elite congruency, as well as

citizens’ trust in, and satisfaction with, democracy (e.g., Putnam, 1993; Verba & Nie, 1972).

Because of the importance of political participation to democracy, an important body of

literature has sought to explain why citizens become politically active, understanding political

participation as a means for citizens to change political outcomes and to foster social change

(e.g., van Stekelenburg & Klandermans, 2013; van Zomeren, Postmes, & Spears, 2008;

Verba, Schlozman, & Brady, 2002). From such an instrumental point of view, citizens are

considered to be most likely to engage in political participation if they consider doing so to be

an effective means to these goals (Marien, Hooghe, & Quintelier, 2010; van Zomeren et al.,

2008; Verba et al., 2002). In line with these studies, this paper aims to contribute to the

literature explaining political participation by further analyzing what determines citizens’

expectations of the effectiveness of participating, and by analyzing whether this affects their

actual political participation.

To explain such expectations of effectiveness, this study looks at citizens’ perceptions

of state responsiveness – i.e., at their external efficacy (Niemi, Craig, & Mattei, 1991).

Various studies have indicated that citizens’ perceptions of the political context play a major

role in determining their (perceived effectiveness of) political participation (Bandura, 1986;

Lee, 2010; McAdam, 1982). In particular, it is expected that if citizens consider state

authorities to be responsive to their demands, they will consider participation to be more

effective, and consequently, they will become more likely to participate (Corcoran,

Pettinicchio, & Young, 2011; Karp & Banducci, 2008; Kriesi, Koopmans, Duyvendak, &

Giugni, 1995). If they consider the authorities to be irresponsive instead, participation will

appear ineffective, and hence, unappealing. In short, in order to understand (the perceived

effectiveness of) political participation, it is important to take into account citizens’ external

efficacy.

However, it is often overlooked that state responsiveness consists of at least two

elements: 1) whether authorities are willing to take citizens’ demands into account, and 2)

whether or not they are able to effectively produce political output (Hutter, 2014; Kitschelt,

1986; Kriesi et al., 1995). Citizens’ perceptions of the willingness of authorities to take their

demands into account – i.e., external input efficacy – has often been studied, and it has been

2

found to relate positively to political participation (Karp & Banducci, 2008; Niemi et al.,

1991). In contrast, citizens' perceptions of the ability of the political system to produce

political outcomes – i.e., external output efficacy – has rarely been studied. As a result, it

remains largely unknown whether and how external output efficacy affects (the perceived

effectiveness of) political participation. Therefore, this study aims to address this gap in the

literature by answering the following research question: What is the indirect effect of external

output efficacy on certain forms of political participation, as mediated by the perceived

effectiveness of those forms of participation?

To address this question, this paper uses data from the 2014 PARTIREP election

survey. This survey is the first large-N survey to include a measure of external output

efficacy. These data are therefore uniquely appropriate to address the research question. As

the research question concerns an indirect effect, the data will be analyzed using mediation

analyses. The structure of the remainder of this paper is as follows: I will first discuss the

literature on the link between political participation, political efficacy, and the perception of

input and output structures. Next, I will introduce the data, measurements, and methods used,

after which I will present the results of the analyses. Finally, I will conclude with a discussion

of the theoretical implications of the findings.

Theoretical framework

State- and non-state oriented political participation

People are using an increasingly wide range of activities in order to pursue political goals or

to express their political views. In reaction to this ongoing trend, scholars have begun to re-

conceptualize political participation in order to include this expanding variety of activities

(e.g., Norris, 2002; Zukin, Keeter, Andolina, Jenkins, & Delli Carpini, 2006). Most recently,

Van Deth has conceptualized political participation as “citizens’ activities affecting politics”

(2014, p. 3). Building on this conceptualization, actions can be identified as political

participation if they are carried out on a voluntary and amateurish basis, and if they apply to

one of the following criteria: 1) they are carried out within the institutional arena of state

politics, 2) they are targeted at the state, its personnel or its institutions, 3) or they are targeted

at community problems by targeting non-state actors. Thus, while previous

conceptualizations of political participation focused mainly on the first two categories of

3

state-oriented activities (e.g., Verba, Nie, & Kim, 1978), more recent conceptualizations of

political participation have increasingly underlined the importance of both state- and non-

state oriented activities (Norris, 2002; van Deth, 2014).

This study aims to increase our understanding of political participation by analyzing

people’s perceptions of state responsiveness. However, taking the above into account, it

becomes clear that perceptions of the state have different implications for different types of

participation, depending on the degree to which they are state-oriented. Therefore, this study

focuses on three different types of action that represent the varying degrees to which common

types of political participation can be orientated at the state (Norris, 2002; van Deth, 2014;

Zukin et al., 2006). Firstly, it looks at citizens contacting politicians through mail or email as

a form of political participation that is acted out within the institutional arena of state politics

and that is targeted at state authorities. Secondly, it looks at citizens joining a demonstration

as a form of participation that is located outside the institutional arena of state politics and

that may be targeted either at state authorities or at non-state actors. Thirdly, it looks at

citizens boycotting products as a form of participation that is located outside the institutional

arena of state politics, and that is mainly targeted at non-state actors. These variations will be

taken into account when analyzing the effect of people’s perceptions of state responsiveness.

In the remainder of this theoretical framework, I will first discuss how feelings of

political efficacy relate to political participation. I will then discuss the role of the perceived

political context in explaining feelings of efficacy regarding certain forms of participation.

Here, I wish to make the main argument of this paper, namely, that perceptions of the state’s

output structure, i.e., external output efficacy, are essential in this regard. Finally, I will

formulate a number of hypotheses concerning how external output efficacy may affect (the

perceived effectiveness of) different types of political participation, depending on the degree

to which these types of participation are targeted at the state.

Efficacy and political participation

The antecedents of political participation may vary, but often citizens become politically

active in order to alter social conditions they consider unjust or unfair (Klandermans, 1984;

van Zomeren et al., 2008; Verba et al., 2002). In other words, political participation is often

instrumentally motivated as it advances goals that are external to the action (Rucht, 1990;

Walgrave, Van Laer, Verhulst, & Wouters, 2010). For instance, people contact politicians to

encourage them to take into account environmental concerns, they protest to undo austerity

4

measures, or they boycott products because they want to reverse the unethical production of

certain products (e.g., Balsiger, n.d.; Graziano & Forno, 2012; Rüdig & Karyotis, 2013).

From an instrumental perspective, people are considered to be more likely to become

politically active if they consider doing so to be effective (Gamson, 1968; Klandermans,

1997; van Zomeren et al., 2008). Therefore, the literature explaining political participation

has paid great attention to the concept of political efficacy. Political efficacy refers to “the

feeling that individual political action does have, or can have, an impact upon the political

process, (…) the feeling that political and social change is possible, and that the individual

citizen can play a part in bringing about this change” (Campbell, Gurin, & Miller, 1954, p.

187). Unsurprisingly, political efficacy is often found to predict political participation: the

more effective people believe political participation to be, the more likely they are to engage

in it (Hooghe & Marien, 2010; Verba et al., 2002).

However, it is important to keep in mind that the link between political efficacy and

political participation may vary regarding different forms of participation, leading to

corresponding variations in people’s engagement in specific forms of participation (Hooghe

& Marien, 2013b; Morrell, 2005). For instance, someone may feel that joining a

demonstration is highly effective, but that signing a petition is useless. S/he consequently

becomes more likely to engage in the former activity, while abstaining from the latter. In

short, the relation between political efficacy and political participation depends on the form

of participation at hand, and existing research has shown that feelings of efficacy regarding

specific forms of participation are better predictors of those forms of political participation

than general feelings of political efficacy (Morrell, 2005; Wollman & Strouder, 1991).

Building on these arguments, this study aims to explain specific forms of political

participation by looking at how effective people consider those particular forms of

participation to be.

H1: The more effective respondents consider a certain form of political participation

to be, the more likely they are to engage in this form of political participation.

External efficacy and output structures

In order to explain people’s beliefs regarding the effectiveness of certain forms of political

participation, it is instructive to analyze their perceptions of opportunities (or constraints)

within the political context. This argument has been put forward primarily in the literature on

5

external efficacy (Karp & Banducci, 2008; Niemi et al., 1991). External efficacy refers “to

beliefs about the responsiveness of governmental authorities and institutions to citizen

demands” (Niemi et al., 1991, pp. 1407–8). People with higher external efficacy are thus

inclined to believe that their demands will be taken into account by state authorities, and they

will therefore consider political participation to be more effective (Niemi et al., 1991).

Consequently, they have a higher propensity for political participation (e.g., Copeland, 2014;

Karp & Banducci, 2008).

However, whether or not the state appears to be responsive depends on two aspects.

On the one hand, it is determined by whether state authorities are willing to take citizens

claims into account, or in other words, whether its input structure is open (as opposed to

closed). On the other hand, it depends on its ability to effectively produce political output, or

in other words, whether its output structure is strong, as opposed to weak (Hutter, 2014;

Kriesi et al., 1995; Micheletti, 2003). In order for the state to be responsive, it needs to be

willing, as well as able to translate citizens demands into political output. Hence, both

perceptions of input structures – i.e., external input efficacy – and output structures – i.e.,

external output efficacy – are expected to affect people’s (perceived effectiveness of) political

participation (Kriesi et al., 1995; Stolle, Hooghe, & Micheletti, 2005).

Nonetheless, existing studies have generally only measured external efficacy as

individuals’ perception of the state’s willingness to take citizens’ demands into account

(Balch, 1974; Niemi et al., 1991). Individuals’ perceptions of the state’s ability to produce

political output are rarely taken into account. Consequently, the literature on external efficacy

and political participation has painted a one-sided picture, that leaves unanswered the

question how external output efficacy affects (the perceived effectiveness of) political

participation. It is the main goal of this study to address this gap in the literature.

Hypotheses

To sum up, the current study builds on two common theoretical arguments to explain

variations in people’s political participation. Firstly, to understand why some citizens engage

in certain forms of political participation (while other do not), it is important to take into

account whether or not they believe such forms of participation can be effective (Marien et

al., 2010; Verba & Nie, 1972; Verba et al., 2002). Secondly, to understand why some citizens

believe certain forms of political participation can be effective (while others do not), we need

to take into account their perceptions of the political context (Karp & Banducci, 2008; Lee,

6

2010; McAdam, 1982). Combined these suggestions propose that there is an indirect effect of

perceptions of the political context – i.e., external efficacy – on certain forms of political

participation, as mediated by perceptions of the effectiveness of those participation forms.

While existing studies have mainly focused on external input efficacy, this study aims to

extend this argument to include external output efficacy.

However, in doing so, it is important to recall that this argument applies to different

forms of participation in different ways. That is, depending on the degree to which specific

types of participation are state-oriented, they may be more or less affected by external output

efficacy. As argued above, political participation includes various activities that vary with

regard to their orientation towards the state (Norris, 2002; van Deth, 2014). The more state-

oriented an activity is, the more it will depend on state responsiveness in order to be effective.

State-oriented forms of participation advance social change by making claims towards state

authorities to demand political change. If the state remains irresponsive to such claims, they

remain ineffective. To state the obvious, non-state oriented forms of participation do not

depend on state responsiveness in such a way. Hence, depending on the degree to which a

form of participation is state-oriented, the hypothesized indirect effect of external output

efficacy will vary, resulting in three different effects. Firstly, it is expected that the effect will

be strongest in the case of types of political participation that mainly target state actors (e.g.,

contacting politicians through mail or email). Secondly, it is expected that the effect will be

present, yet weaker, in the case of types of participation that can be targeted either at state, or

non-state actors (e.g., joining a demonstration). Finally, it is expected that the effect will be

absent in the case of non-state oriented types of participation (e.g., boycotting a product).

H2: There is a positive indirect effect of external output efficacy on state-oriented

forms of political participation, as mediated by the perceived effectiveness of those

forms.

H3: The more state oriented the form of participation, the stronger the indirect

effect.

H4: There is no indirect effect of external output efficacy on non-state-oriented

forms of political participation.

7

Data and measures

In order to test the hypotheses proposed above, data is used from the 2014 PARTIREP pre-

election survey. This telephone survey contains data from representative sample of 2015

Belgian adults, including both French and Dutch speaking citizens. The survey was

conducted prior to the three-level (regional, national, and European) elections of May 25,

2014. The fact that the PARTIREP survey contains data from only the Belgian population

may have important implications for the generalizability of the findings, yet there is sufficient

evidence that the Belgian case is representative of most other developed European

democracies. As Hooghe and Marien (2012) have observed, “analyses of the European Social

Survey have shown repeatedly that it [Belgium] is not exceptional with regard to

participation patterns or political attitudes in the European context” (p. 7). Thus, although

further comparative analyses would merit the generalizability of this study, the Belgian case

is interesting beyond its own borders as well.

Dependent variables: state and non-state oriented political participation

This study aims to explain why people engage in, or abstain from, various common forms of

state and non-state oriented political participation. As a typical example of state-oriented

participation, contacting politicians through mail or email is used. As a typical example of

non-state oriented participation, boycotting products is used. Joining in a demonstration is

used as an example of a ‘mixed’ form, targeting both state- and non-state actors. For each of

these types of participation, respondents were asked whether they had made use of it during

the last 12 months. For each form of participation a binary measure was created where

respondents who had participated were coded as 1, while others were coded as 0.i

Independent variable: external output efficacy

Unlike external input efficacy, external output efficacy has not yet been measured in large-N

survey research. The PARTIREP survey is the first large-N survey that includes a measure of

citizens’ external output efficacy. Respondents were asked to what extent they agree or

disagree (1 = completely disagree, 5 = completely agree) with the following statement:

“Politicians in my country are capable of acting upon problems”. This item taps in on

respondents’ beliefs concerning the state’s ability to act, or in other words, their external

8

output efficacy. In the question wording, the national level is emphasized because this study

is concerned with respondents’ perceptions of the state’s ability to act. The survey question

was used and tested previously in a mixed-methods case-study on Belgian environmental

activists (de Moor, Marie, & Hooghe, 2013). This study confirmed the usefulness of the

question in terms of understandability and construct validity. Thus, respondents who score

higher on this question believe more strongly that the national political system is capable of

acting. In other words, they perceive the state’s output structure as strong.

Mediating variables: the perceived effectiveness of political participation

The PARTIREP survey contains detailed information on how effective respondents consider

several individual forms of political participation, including contacting politicians, joining in

a demonstration, and boycotting. Concerning each of these forms of participation,

respondents were asked the following question: “Citizens can do various things to affect

political decisions. Can you indicate how effective you think each of these actions are in

affecting political decisions?”. Answers were given on a Likert-scale, ranging from 1 (“very

ineffective”) to 7 (“very effective”).

Control variables

It is hypothesized that each form of participation is affected by the perceived effectiveness of

that specific form of participation, but not by the perceived effectiveness of other forms of

participation. Therefore, the perceived effectiveness of the those other forms of participation,

respectively, will be included in the analyses of each form of participation as control

variables.

Moreover, as argued above, the existing literature has often focused on external input

efficacy, and it has been found to affect (the perceived effectiveness of) political

participation. Therefore, this variable will be included in the analyses as well. It is measured

using a single item where responds were asked to what extent they agreed or disagreed (1 =

completely disagree, 5 = completely agree) with the following statement: “The average

citizen does affect political decisions and the actions government takes”.

Additionally, several personal characteristics and political attitudes have repeatedly

been linked to (the perceived effectiveness of) political participation and therefore need to be

controlled for in our analyses. As argued above, men and older people generally feel more

9

efficacious about politics, and they are more inclined to engage in institutional forms of

participation (Burns, Schlozman, & Verba, 2001; Marien et al., 2010; Stolle & Hooghe,

2011). Women and young people have a stronger tendency to engage in non-institutional

forms of participation (Marien et al., 2010; Stolle & Hooghe, 2011). People with higher

education generally feel more efficacious, and overall they participate in politics more often

(Blais & Rubenson, 2013; Niemi et al., 1991). As for political attitudes, in general, people

with higher political interest and internal efficacy – i.e., the feeling that one can understand

and participate in politics (Niemi et al., 1991) – are more inclined to participate in politics

(Burns et al., 2001; Verba et al., 2002). Political trust is found to have a positive relation with

institutional participation, whereas it relates negatively to non-institutional participation

(Hooghe & Marien, 2013a). Thus age, sex (0 = men, 1 = women), and a categorical variable

for level of education (recoded to 1 = low, 2 = middle, 3 = high)ii are included as background

variables in all analyses. Political interest is measured using a single item where 0 means very

low political interest and 10 very high political interest. Political trust is measured using a

scale of eight items with an Eigenvalue of 4.17 and a Cronbach’s α of .86. Internal efficacy is

also measured using a scale of four items with an Eigenvalue of 2.03 and a Cronbach’s α of

.67.

Methods

The hypotheses formulated above suggest that the effect of external output efficacy on

political participation is mediated by perceptions of effectiveness. Mediation effects are

usually modeled using a method of decomposition, where the total effect of the independent

variable X on the dependent variable Y is decomposed into a direct effect and an indirect

effect. This is done by comparing regression parameters between the reduced model (i.e.,

without controlling for the mediator Z) and the full model (i.e., including the mediator Z)

(Alwin & Hauser, 1975; Baron & Kenny, 1986; Karlson, Holm, & Breen, 2010). Three steps

are used to decompose an effect. Firstly, separate regressions are used to predict the effect of

an independent variable X on a mediator Z (effect a), of a mediator Z on a dependent variable

Y (effect b), and of an independent X on dependent variable Y (c, or the total effect of X on

Y). An indirect effect (ab) occurs when a and b are both significant (Baron & Kenny, 1986;

Kohler, Karlson, & Holm, 2011). In a second step, the full model is predicted, where the

10

effect of X on Y is controlled for Z, hence predicting the direct effect (c`). Finally, by

subtracting c` from c, the size of the indirect effect (ab) is obtained.

The decomposition method builds on the assumption that the mediation effect is built

up of linear effects between the independent, mediating and dependent variables (Baron &

Kenny, 1986; Karlson et al., 2010). However, this assumption cannot be met in the current

study, because unlike the mediating variables, the dependent variables are dichotomous and

predicted using logistic regression analysis. As a result, the decomposition method cannot be

applied in the straightforward fashion described above. The problem is that the inclusion of

an additional (mediating) variable in a logistic regression not only affects the effect sizes, but

also in the scaling of the parameters (for more information see: Karlson et al., 2010; Kohler et

al., 2011). Therefore, comparing c and c’ using logistic regressions would conflate mediation

and rescaling. In this scenario, we cannot assess whether or not a mediating effect occurs, nor

how large it is.

To address this problem, Karlson, Holm and Breen (2010) propose the KHB-method

that corrects this limitation of the decomposition method for nonlinear probability models. It

includes the standardized residuals of the regression of X on Z in the reduced model, thereby

ensuring that the coefficients in the different models are measured on the same scale.

Consequently, the KHB-method warrants against the conflation of mediation and rescaling,

and coefficients can be standardized and compared across different linear and non-linear

regression models, thereby enabling decomposition. This method will be applied using the

‘khb’ program in Stata 12.

Throughout all regression analyses, robust standard errors will be used.

Results

Before turning to the analyses, it is useful to take into account some descriptive statistics that

give us a broad picture of (the perceived effectiveness of) political participation within our

sample (see Appendix for further descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations). On

average, people feel more efficacious about participating in a public demonstration (x̄ = 3.68,

SD = 1.57) than about boycotting products (x̄ = 3.53, SD = 1.63) or contacting politicians

(x̄ = 3.12, SD = 1.87). Boycotting products was the most prevalent form of participation (36

percent of the respondents indicated to have done so), followed by contacting politicians (18

11

percent), and joining a demonstration (14 percent). T-tests indicate that all the differences

between these means are statistically significant.

The hypotheses formulated above concern the indirect effect of external output efficacy on

different forms of political participation, as mediated by the perceived effectiveness of each

of these forms. To test this mediation effect, the direct effects between the independent,

mediating and dependent variables will be analyzed, i.e., effects a, b, and c in Figure 1

(Alwin & Hauser, 1975; Baron & Kenny, 1986). If both effects a and b are significant, a

mediation effect occurs. The KHB method will then be applied to decompose the total effect

of external output efficacy into a direct (c’) and an indirect (ab) effect on political

participation (Karlson et al., 2010; Kohler et al., 2011). These analyses will be repeated

separately for each of the three individual forms of political participation, starting with

contacting politicians, followed by participating in a demonstration, and finally, boycotting

products.

Perceived effectiveness

of:

1: contacting politicians

2: demonstrating

3: boycott product

c Political participation:

1: contacting politicians

2: demonstrating

3: boycotting products

External output efficacy

b

c' Political participation:

1: contacting politicians

2: demonstrating

3: boycotting products

External output efficacy

a

Figure 1: the hypothesized mediation effects

12

Contacting politicians

Table 1 contains the results from the logistic regressions on contacting politicians. In Model I

we see that people with higher education, higher political interest and higher internal efficacy

are significantly more likely to contact politicians. This is in line with previous findings in the

literature. External output efficacy is negatively associated with contacting politicians (path

c), but this effect is not significant.

Table 1: Logistic regression of contacting a politician

Predictor Model I Model II Model III Model IV

Control Variables

Intercept .024*** (.011) .006*** (.003) .006*** (.003) .010*** (.005)

Age 1.002 (.004) 1.001 (.004) 1.000 (.004) 1.002 (.004)

Sex (1 = female) 1.075 (.136) .998 (.133) 1.003 (.135) .991 (.133)

Education (ref. = low)

Middle 1.760** (.309) 1.755** (.314) 1.714** (.312) 1.7221** (.309)

Higher 2.459*** (.424) 2.234*** (.388) 2.185*** (.384) 2.158*** (.375)

Political Interest 1.064** (.024) 1.077** (.026) 1.076** (.027) 1.073** (.026)

Political Trust .987 (.043) .948 (.051) .943 (.050) .976 (.049)

Internal Efficacy 1.628***(.136) 1.595*** (.137) 1.584*** (.138) 1.589*** (.135)

External input efficacy 1.114 (.063) 1.002 (.061) 1.000 (.062) 1.042 (.063)

Variables of Interest

External output efficacy

.882 (.059) .772** (.056)

PE Contacting politiciansa

1.564*** (.070) 1.561*** (.074) 1.605*** (.073)

PE Demonstratinga

.987 (.046)

PE Boycotting producta

1.050 (.039)

McKelvey & Zavoina’s pseudo R2

N

.12

1910

.22

1901

.22

1878

.23

1896

Note: *p < .10, **p < . 05, ***p < .01. Coefficients are odds ratios. Robust standard errors between brackets. a:

PE = Perceived effectiveness.

Model XIII in Table 4 (p. 17) presents the results from an OLS regression on the perceived

effectiveness of contacting politicians. We see that people with high education perceive

contacting politicians as more effective than people with low education. Moreover, a positive

effect exists for people with higher internal efficacy and for people with higher external input

efficacy. Again, these findings are in line with the literature discussed above. Finally, in

support of hypothesis 2, we see that there is a significant positive effect of external output

efficacy on the perceived effectiveness of contacting politicians (effect a). An increase of one

13

unit on external output efficacy is predicted to increase the mediating variable by .25. It is

interesting to note that this effect is stronger than that of external input efficacy (.17). This

supports the argument made earlier that while the literature has mainly focused on the latter,

the former is at least as important in understanding why people feel more or less efficacious

about state-oriented political participation.

As for path b, we see in Model II (Table 1) that perceiving contacting politicians as

effective positively affects one’s likelihood of contacting a politician. More precisely, a one

unit increase on the mediating variable increases one’s odds of having contacted a politician

by 56 percent. In order to test whether this link is specific for the perceived effectiveness of

this form of political participation, and not for the perceived effectiveness of political

participation in general, the perceived effectiveness of joining a demonstration and of

boycotting a product are included in Model III. We see that there is only a significant effect

for the perceived effectiveness of contacting politicians, and not for the perceived

effectiveness of the other forms of participation. These results support the hypothesis (H1)

that there is not one general feeling of efficacy leading to political participation, but that the

effect of efficacy beliefs are specific to the type of participation.

In Model IV the full model is presented, thus showing the effect of the independent

variable on the dependent variable under the control of the mediating variable. We see that

the direct negative effect of external output efficacy has become stronger and is now

significant (OR = .772**). This suggests that the mediating variable performs the role of

suppressor (Baron & Kenny, 1986; MacKinnon, Fairchild, & Fritz, 2007). That is, when the

positive association between the independent variable and the mediator are controlled for, the

negative effect of the independent variable becomes isolated and thereby significant.

Although no assumptions were made about the direct effect of external output efficacy in this

study, this negative effect is somewhat surprising, and it will therefore be touched upon

further in the discussion below. However, it does not contradict the hypothesized indirect

effect that is found, as we are in fact dealing with an ‘inconsistent mediation’ (i.e., the direct

effect of a predictor has the opposite sign from its indirect effect) (Alwin & Hauser, 1975;

Baron & Kenny, 1986; MacKinnon et al., 2007). This inconsistent mediation suggests that,

although people who perceive the state’s output structure as strong are generally less inclined

to contact politicians, they are more likely to perceive contacting politicians as effective. If

they do so, this increases the likelihood that they will contact politicians, thereby balancing

out the negative direct effect.

14

As hypothesized (H2), there is thus a significant indirect effect of external output

efficacy on contacting politicians. In order to assess how strong this effect is, and how much

of the total effect is mediated, the KHB-method is applied. The KHB method provides

average marginal effects (AME) of the total (c), direct (c’) and indirect effect (ab), where the

indirect effect is the difference between the total and the direct effect (ab = c-c’). The AME

of external output efficacy in the reduced model (c) is -.033, which implies that on average,

the probability of someone contacting a politician decreases by 1.3 percent points for one

standard deviation change of the independent variable. Under the control of the

mediator/suppressor, this effect increases (c’ = -.036). The indirect of external output efficacy

is thus .013, suggesting that an increase of one standard deviation of the independent variable

increases the probability someone contacts a politician by 1.3 percent points. In order to

calculate the share of the total effect that is mediated in an inconsistent mediation, ab should

be expressed as a percentage of the sum of the absolute values of ab and c’ (Alwin & Hauser,

1975; MacKinnon et al., 2007): |.013|/(|.013|+|.036|)=.265. Put differently, 27 percent of the

total effect of external output efficacy on contacting politicians is mediated by the perceived

effectiveness of doing so.

Joining a demonstration

Demonstrations can be targeted at both state and non-state actors. Therefore it was

hypothesized (H3) that although there is still a mediated positive effect of external output

efficacy on joining a demonstration, this effect is weaker than in the case of the essentially

more state-oriented act of contacting politicians. Again, we use the method of decomposition,

after which the indirect effect will be calculated using the KHB method.

In Model V (Table 2) we observe that higher education, internal efficacy and external

input efficacy are positively associated with joining in a demonstration. There is a negative,

but non-significant direct effect of external output efficacy (c). However, in Model XIV

(Table 4) we see that there is a significant positive effect of external output efficacy on the

perceived effectiveness of joining a demonstration (a), while in Model VI (Table 2) we see

that people who perceive protesting as more effective are more likely to join a demonstration

(b). Again, Model VII suggests that joining a demonstration is only affected by the perceived

effectiveness of this form of action, and not of the others, thus offering further support for

hypothesis 1.

15

There is thus a significant positive indirect effect (ab). This finding is further

supported when we look at the full model, where the effect of external output efficacy is

controlled for the perceived effectiveness of joining a demonstration. Here, we see that

although effect becomes stronger and significant. This suggests that again we are again

dealing with a mediator that functions as a suppressor, constituting an inconsistent mediation.

Table 2: Logistic regression of joining a demonstration

Predictor Model V Model VI Model VII Model VIII

Control Variables

Intercept .067*** (.032) .014*** (.007) .013*** (.007) .019*** (.009)

Age .991* (.004) .993 (.004) .993 (.004) .994 (.004)

Sex (1 = female) .781 (.111) .754 (.109) .768 (.112) .747* (.108)

Education (ref. = low)

Middle 1.469* (.270) 1.434 (.268) 1.421 (.269) 1.418 (.265)

Higher 1.517* (.279) 1.453* (.273) 1.451 (.277) 1.424 (.267)

Political Interest 1.023 (.023) 1.034 (.025) 1.034 (.025) 1.031 (.025)

Political Trust .987 (.049) .979 (.057) .992 (.058) .996 (.056)

Internal Efficacy 1.498*** (.135) 1.450*** (.131) 1.475*** (.135) 1.441*** (.129)

External input efficacy 1.115 (.068) 1.036 (.067) 1.051 (.068) 1.063 (.068)

Variables of Interest

External output efficacy

.895 (.063) .857* (.063)

PE Contacting politicians

.930 (.046)

PE Demonstrating 1.430*** (.065) 1.466*** (.073) 1.440*** (.065)

PE Boycotting product .996 (.040)

McKelvey & Zavoina’s pseudo R2

N

.08

1911

.15

1913

.16

1879

.16

1908

Note: *p < .10, **p < . 05, ***p < .01. Coefficients are odds ratios. Robust standard errors between brackets. PE

= Perceived effectiveness.

The average marginal effects of the total and the direct effect are again obtained using the

KHB-method to calculate how much of the total effect is mediated. The difference between

the total effect (c = -.013) and the direct effect (c’ = -.016) is .003. In other words, an increase

of one standard deviation of external output efficacy significantly increases the probability

that someone participated in a demonstration with 0.3 percent. This indirect effect ab

constitutes 16 percent of the total effect, and is very small. However, this is in line with the

hypothesis that although there should be an indirect effect, this effect should be smaller than

in the case of the more state oriented action form of contacting politicians. Comparing effect

a and b between contacting politicians and joining a demonstration further indicates that the

16

indirect effect for the latter is smaller because external output efficacy less strongly predicts

the perceived effectiveness of joining a demonstration, whereas perceptions of effectiveness

of each form of participation are equally predictive of whether an individual engages in them.

Boycotting products

Whereas a positive mediation effect was hypothesized of external output efficacy on

contacting politicians, and to a lesser extent, on joining a demonstration, no such effect was

hypothesized (H4) to exist for non-state oriented forms of participation like boycotting

products. That is, although it is expected that people who consider boycotts to be effective

will be more likely to engage in them, their perceptions of effectiveness are most likely not to

be affected by their perceptions of the state. Hence, it is expected that path b is significant

and positive, but that path a is not, in which case, no significant indirect effect (ab) can be

established.

Table 3: Logistic regression of boycotting products

Predictor Model IX Model X Model XI Model XII

Control Variables

Intercept .280*** (.094) .086*** (.029) .094*** (.033) .111*** (.039)

Age .998 (.003) .994 (.003) .994* (.003) .994 (.003)

Sex (1 = female) 1.063 (.108) 1.051 (.112) 1.054 (.112) 1.045 (.111)

Education (ref. = low)

Middle 1.362* (.177) 1.329* (.182) 1.323* (.181) 1.306 (.179)

Higher 2.099*** (.270) 1.888*** (.252) 1.870*** (.252) 1.858*** (.249)

Political Interest .991 (.018) .984 (.019) .983 (.019) .982 (.019)

Political Trust .942 (.033) .950 (.037) .952 (.037) .962 (.037)

Internal Efficacy 1.390*** (.090) 1.340*** (.093) 1.352*** (.094) 1.344*** (.093)

External input efficacy 1.050* (.047) .978 (.045) .980 (.046) 1.000 (.047)

Variables of Interest

External output efficacy

.900* (.047) .884* (.048)

PE Contacting politicians

.989 (.036)

PE Demonstrating .980 (.036)

1.448*** (.043) PE Boycotting product 1.448*** (.043) 1.456*** (.046)

McKelvey & Zavoina’s

pseudo R2

N

.06

1906

.17

1887

.17

1876

.18

1883

Note: *p < .10, **p < . 05, ***p < .01. Coefficients are odds ratios. Robust standard errors between brackets. a:

PE = Perceived effectiveness.

17

In Model IX (Table 3) we see that people with higher education are again more likely to

participate. People with higher internal efficacy and higher external input efficacy are also

more likely to participate. Again, these findings are in line with previous studies. External

output efficacy has a significant negative direct effect, which will be discussed further below.

In Model XV (Table 4) we see that there is no significant effect (a) of external output

efficacy on the perceived effectiveness of boycotting product. In Model X (Table 3) we see

that there is a significant positive effect of perceiving boycotting products as effective on

actually participating in this form of action. In Model XI we see that this effect is exclusively

significant for the perceived effectiveness of boycotting products, and not for the perceived

effectiveness of other forms of participation (in support of H1). Finally, in the full model

(Model XII, Table 3) we see that the direct effect of external output efficacy is not affected

by controlling for the perceived effectiveness of boycotting products. In short, as

hypothesized, there is no indirect effect of external output efficacy on the probability that

someone will engage in a boycott.

Table 4: OLS regression of the perceived effectiveness (PE) of individual forms of political

participation

Model XIII Model XIV Model XV

Predictor PE contacting

politicians

PE joining a

demonstration

PE boycotting a

product

Control Variables

Intercept 1.449*** (.238) 3.273*** (.250) 2.352*** (.292)

Age -.001 (.002) -.011*** (.002) .008** (.003)

Sex (1 = female) .159* (.073) .058 (.077) .066 (.088)

Education (ref. = low)

Middle .055 (.089) .080 (.096) .150 (.109)

Higher .394*** (.091) .284*** (.094) .483*** (.110)

Political Interest -.018 (.013) -.039** (.015) .012 (.016)

Political Trust .021 (.027) -.032 (.029) -.079* (.034)

Internal Efficacy .101* (.048) .113 (.051) .122* (.059)

External input efficacy .166*** (.033) .149*** (.034) .041** (.038)

Variables of Interest

External output efficacy

.248*** (.038) .143** (.041) .044 (.048)

R2

N

.07

1897

.05

1908

.03

1886

Note: *p < .10, **p < . 05, ***p < .01. Robust standard errors between brackets.

18

In sum, we have seen that each specific form of participation is affected by the perceived

effectiveness of that form of participation, and not by the perceived effectiveness of the other

forms of participation. These findings support hypothesis 1, and are in line with previous

studies that have suggested that political participation becomes more likely when people

believe that specific action forms are effective (Marien et al., 2010; Morrell, 2005; van

Stekelenburg & Klandermans, 2013; Verba et al., 2002).

This study has primarily aimed to increase our understanding of why people do, or do

not, believe that certain forms of participation can be effective, thereby indirectly explaining

actual participation. More precisely, it has sought to explain political participation by looking

at people’s perception of the state’s ability to act – i.e., their external output efficacy. It was

hypothesized that this mediation effect should exist mainly for state-oriented forms of

participation, and to a lesser extent for forms of participation that can be targeted at both state

and non-state actors. No such effect was expected to exist for non-state oriented forms of

participation. In support of Hypothesis 2, 3 and 4, the results indicate that there is a

significant indirect effect of external output efficacy on contacting politicians and joining in a

demonstration. The effect is indeed stronger for the former. In further support of the

hypotheses, no such effect is found for the non-state oriented act of boycotting products.

Discussion

In line with most political participation literature, this study indicates that citizens are more

likely to engage in political participation if they consider doing so to be effective (Marien et

al., 2010; van Stekelenburg & Klandermans, 2013; Verba et al., 2002). This is the case for all

three forms of participation analyzed in this study: contacting politicians, joining a

demonstration, and boycotting products. Moreover, it suggests that perceptions of the

political context are significant predictors of the perceived effectiveness of political

participation, as well as actual political participation. In particular, it proposes that if citizens

consider the state to have a strong output structure, they are more likely to perceive state-

oriented political participation as an effective means to social change. Consequently, there is

a positive indirect effect of external output efficacy on state-oriented forms of political

participation. No such effect occurs in the case of non-state oriented forms of participation

like boycotting products.

Whereas there is thus a positive indirect effect of external output efficacy on state-

oriented political participation, the direct effect of external output efficacy on political

19

participation is in all cases negative. If citizens perceive the state to have a strong output

structure, it is found that they become less likely to act. This surprising finding does not

contradict our hypothesis, yet it still begs further reflection. In the case of state-oriented

forms of action, it may be that citizens who feel that the state is capable of addressing

problems in society consider political participation as less urgent or less necessary. In their

eyes, the state is effectively dealing with society’s challenges, and therefore, it does not need

correction through political action. In contrast, if citizens feel the state is not capable of

solving society’s main problems, citizens act to correct. As such these findings resonate an

argument that William Gamson made in 1968: “high trust in authorities implies some lack of

necessity for influencing them” (p. 7). In a similar vein, Almond and Verba (1963) described

the satisfied citizen, who may well feel efficacious, but who considers corrective political

participation to be unnecessary. In line with these descriptions, the results in this paper

suggest that if citizens trust in the authorities’ ability to act, they will feel less urged to

perform corrective pressure through state-oriented political participation. As for non-state

oriented action, it may be that if citizens perceive the output structure as weak, state-oriented

action may appear ineffective, and non-state oriented action becomes a reasonable

alternative.

Taking the above into account, the state’s perceived output structure thus performs an

important role in establishing a democratic linkage between citizens and the state in at least

two ways. On the one hand, when citizens perceive the output structure as strong, it appears

they feel it is less necessary to ‘correct’ authorities through political action. On the other

hand, a strong output structure presents an important perquisite for citizens to believe that

political participation can be effective, thus inciting them to participate in case they believe

doing so is necessary. Given this dual role of the state’s output structure, it is remarkable that

it has received such limited attention in the literature on political participation. Thus,

although the current study does support the common understanding that an open input

structure facilitates political participation, it stresses that perceptions of the output structure

clearly affect political participation as well.

Finally, it is important to note certain limitations of this study, and possible venues for

future research as well. Firstly, it needs to be recognized that the structure of the used data is

cross-sectional, and that this limits our ability to make strong causal claims regarding the link

between political attitudes (like efficacy) and political behavior. Most studies on this subject

indicate that political attitudes predict political behavior – e.g., higher political efficacy leads

to higher political participation (van Stekelenburg & Klandermans, 2013; van Zomeren et al.,

20

2008; Verba et al., 2002). However, other studies stress that the effect may actually go in the

opposite direction as well, arguing that political attitudes are shaped by the experience of

participation (Klandermans, van der Toorn, & van Stekelenburg, 2008; Quintelier & van

Deth, 2014). Notwithstanding the importance of this argument, however, most political

participation literature suggests that the direction of the effect goes mainly from attitudes to

political behavior. For one, people who have never engaged in political activities have

political attitudes nonetheless. In other words, political attitudes precede political

participation. Moreover, as Inglehart (2008) has suggested, political attitudes remain

relatively stable throughout people’s lives, rather than being changed after each individual

experience of political participation. Still, the findings in this study would merit from a

longitudinal or experimental study that could more strongly assess questions of causality.

Secondly, the findings in this study are limited to one case. Although the Belgian case

is often considered to be representative of other developed European democracies, assessing

whether this study’s findings will hold in different national contexts would advance the

generalizability of the findings. Moreover, a comparative approach would allow to assess

whether differences between countries in terms of output strength are reflected in citizens’

political attitudes and behavior. As for now, this study strongly suggests that the effect of the

perceived output structure on political participation will be supported by such a comparative

study.

21

References

Almond, G., & Verba, S. (1963). The Civic Culture. Political Attitudes and Democracy in

Five Nations. Newbury Park: Sage Publications.

Alwin, D. F., & Hauser, R. M. (1975). The Decomposition of Effects in Path Analysis.

American Sociological Review, 40(1), 37–47.

Balch, G. (1974). Multiple indicators in survey research: the concept “sense of political

efficacy.” Political Methodology, 1, 1–43.

Balsiger, P. (n.d.). Between shaming corporations and promoting alternatives. An in-depth

analysis of the tactical repertoire of a campaign for ethical fashion in Switzerland.

Journal of Consumer Culture.

Bandura, A. (1986). Social Foundations of Thought and Action: Social Cognitive Theory.

Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.

Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social

psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173–82.

Blais, A., & Rubenson, D. (2013). The source of turnout decline: New values or new

contexts? Comparative Political Studies, 46(1), 95–117.

doi:10.1177/0010414012453032

Burns, N., Schlozman, K. L., & Verba, S. (2001). The Private Roots of Public Action.

Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Campbell, A., Gurin, G., & Miller, W. E. (1954). The Voter Decides. Evanston: Row &

Peterson.

Copeland, L. (2014). Conceptualizing political consumerism: How citizenship norms

differentiate boycotting from buycotting. Political Studies, 62(1), 172–186.

doi:10.1111/1467-9248.12067

Corcoran, K. E., Pettinicchio, D., & Young, J. T. N. (2011). The Context of Control: a Cross-

National Investigation of the Link Between Political Institutions, Efficacy, and

Collective Action. The British Journal of Social Psychology, 50(4), 575–605.

doi:10.1111/j.2044-8309.2011.02076.x

De Moor, J., Marie, S., & Hooghe, M. (2013). Linking Lifestyle Politics and State Oriented

Action. A mixed Methods Inquiry into forms of engagement among lifestyle activitst in a

a Belgian Environmental Movement. Leuven.

Della Porta, D. (2013). Can Democracy Be Saved? Participation, Deliberation, and Social

Movements. Cambridge: Polity.

22

Gamson, W. A. (1968). Power and Discontent. Homewood: The Dorsey Press.

Graziano, P. R., & Forno, F. (2012). Political Consumerism and New Forms of Political

Participation: The Gruppi di Acquisto Solidale in Italy. The ANNALS of the American

Academy of Political and Social Science, 644(1), 121–133.

doi:10.1177/0002716212454839

Hooghe, M., & Marien, S. (2010). Political Trust, Political Efficacy and Forms of

Participation. Oxford: Nuffield College.

Hooghe, M., & Marien, S. (2012). How to reach Members of Parliament ? Citizens and

Members of Parliament on the Effectiveness of Political Participation Repertoires, 1–25.

Hooghe, M., & Marien, S. (2013a). A Comparative Analysis of the Relation Between

Political Trust and Forms of Political Particiaption in Europe. European Societies, 15(1),

131–52.

Hooghe, M., & Marien, S. (2013b). How to reach Members of Parliament? Citizens and

Members of Parliament on the Effectiveness of Political Participation Repertoires.

Parliamentary Affairs, 66, 1–25. doi:10.1093/pa/gss057

Hutter, S. (2014). New Cleavages and Protest Politics in Western Europe. Minneapolis:

University of Minnesota Press.

Inglehart, R. F. (2008). Changing values among Western publics from 1970 to 2006. West

European Politics, 31(1-2), 130–146. doi:10.1080/01402380701834747

Karlson, K. B., Holm, A., & Breen, R. (2010). Comparing Regression Coefficients Between

Models using Logit and Probit : A New Method. Retrieved July 07, 2014, from

http://www.yale.edu/ciqle/Breen_Scaling effects.pdf

Karp, J. A., & Banducci, S. A. (2008). Political Efficacy and Participation in Twenty-Seven

Democracies: How Electoral Systems Shape Political Behaviour. British Journal of

Political Science, 38(2), 311–334. doi:10.1017/S0007123408000161

Kitschelt, H. (1986). Political Opportunity Structures an Political Protest: Anti-Nuclear

Movements in Four Democracies. British Journal of Political Science, 16, 57–85.

Klandermans, B. (1984). Mobilization and participation: social-psychological expansions of

resource mobilization theory. American Sociological Review, 49(5), 583–600.

Klandermans, B. (1997). The Social Psychology of Protest. Oxford: Blackwell.

Klandermans, B., van der Toorn, J., & van Stekelenburg, J. (2008). Embeddedness and

identity: How immigrants turn grievances into action. American Sociological Review,

73(6), 992–1012. doi:10.1177/000312240807300606

Kohler, U., Karlson, K. B., & Holm, A. (2011). The Stata Journal. The Stata Journal, 11(3),

420–38.

23

Kriesi, H., Koopmans, R., Duyvendak, J. W., & Giugni, M. (1995). New Social Movements in

Western Europe: a Comparative Analysis. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

Lee, F. (2010). The perceptual bases of collective efficacy and protest participation: The case

of pro-democracy protests in Hong Kong. International Journal of Public Opinion

Research, 22(3), 392–411. doi:10.1093/ijpor/edq023

MacKinnon, D. P., Fairchild, A. J., & Fritz, M. S. (2007). NIH Public Access. Annual Review

of Psychology, 58(Hebb 1966), 1–22.

doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085542.Mediation

Marien, S., Hooghe, M., & Quintelier, E. (2010). Inequalities in Non-institutionalised Forms

of Political Participation: A Multi-level Analysis of 25 countries. Political Studies,

58(1), 187–213. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9248.2009.00801.x

McAdam, D. (1982). Political Process and the Development of Black Insurgency, 1930-1970.

Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Micheletti, M. (2003). Political Virtue and Shopping: Individuals, Consumerism, and

Collective Action. New York: Palgrave.

Morrell, M. E. (2005). Deliberation, democratic decision-making and inernal political

efficacy. Political Behavior, 27(1), 49–69. doi:10.1007/s

Niemi, R. G., Craig, S. C., & Mattei, F. (1991). Measuring internal political efficacy in the

1988 National Election Study. American Political Science Review, 85(4), 1407–1413.

Norris, P. (2002). Democratic Phoenix. Reinventing Political Activism. Cambridge:

Cambridge University Press.

Putnam, R. D. (1993). Making Democracy Work: Civic Traditions in Modern Italy.

Princeton: Princeton University Press.

Quintelier, E., & van Deth, J. W. (2014). Supporting democracy: Political participation and

political attitudes. Exploring causality using panel data. Political Studies, 62(1934),

153–171. doi:10.1111/1467-9248.12097

Rucht, D. (1990). The strategies and action repertoires of new movements. In R. J. Dalton &

M. Kuechler (Eds.), Challenging the Political Order: New Social and Political

Movements in Western Democracies (pp. 156–75). Cambridge: Polity.

Rüdig, W., & Karyotis, G. (2013). Who Protests in Greece? Mass Opposition to Austerity.

British Journal of Political Science, 44(03), 487–513. doi:10.1017/S0007123413000112

Stolle, D., & Hooghe, M. (2011). Shifting inequalities. Patterns of exclusion and inclusion in

emerging forms of political participation. European Societies, 13(1), 119–142.

Stolle, D., Hooghe, M., & Micheletti, M. (2005). Politics in the Supermarket: Political

Consumerism as a Form of Political Participation. International Political Science

Review, 26(3), 245–269. doi:10.1177/0192512105053784

24

Van Deth, J. W. (2014). A conceptual map of political participation. Acta Politica, 1–19.

doi:10.1057/ap.2014.6

Van Stekelenburg, J., & Klandermans, B. (2013). The social psychology of protest. Current

Sociology, 0(0), 1–20. doi:10.1177/0011392113479314

Van Zomeren, M., Postmes, T., & Spears, R. (2008). Toward an integrative social identity

model of collective action: a quantitative research synthesis of three socio-psychological

perspectives. Psychological Bulletin, 134(4), 504–35. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.4.504

Verba, S., & Nie, N. H. (1972). Participation in America. Political Democracy and Social

Equality. Chicago, London: The University of Chicago Press.

Verba, S., Nie, N. H., & Kim, J. (1978). Participation and Political Equality: A Seven-Nation

Comparison. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Verba, S., Schlozman, K. L., & Brady, H. E. (2002). Voice and Equality. Civic Voluntarism

in American Politics. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Walgrave, S., Van Laer, J., Verhulst, J., & Wouters, R. (2010). Why People Protest.

Comparing Demonstrators’ Motivations Across Issues and Nations.

Wollman, N., & Strouder, R. (1991). Believed efficacy and political activity: a test of the

specificity hypothesis. The Journal of Social Psychology, 131, 557–566.

Zukin, C., Keeter, S., Andolina, M., Jenkins, K., & Delli Carpini, M. X. (2006). A New

Engagement? Political Participation, Civic Life, and the Changing American Citizen.

Oxford: Oxford University Press.

25

APPENDIX: Descriptives of survey items

Item

N

Min.

max.

Mean

Std. dev.

Pearson’s correlations with variables of interest

1.

2.

3.

4.

5.

6.

1. PEIPPa

1981 1-7 3.79 1.29 -

2. PENIPPb

1966 1-7 3.35 1.18 .503*** -

3. IPP 2011 0-1 .21 .41 .232*** .110*** -

4. NIPP 2007 0-1 .63 .48 .144*** .243*** .282*** -

5. External efficacy 1998 1-4.67 2.43 .87 .396*** .168*** .120*** .105*** -

6. Perceived output structure 1998 1-5 3.12 .98 .246*** .102*** -.024 -.077*** .267*** -

Control variables

5. Age 2015 18-84 47.96 17.32 -.052* -.050* .008 -.146*** -.118*** .074**

6. Sex (1 = female) 2015 0-1 .50 .50 .023 .021 -.048* -.014 -.053* -.038

7. Education 2015 1-3 - - - - - - - -

8. Political interest 2013 0-10 6.09 2.80 -.030 -.053* .099*** -.031 -.003 -.034

9. Political trust 1945 0-10 4.42 1.35 .083*** -.025 .038 -.021 .177*** .128***

10. Internal efficacy 1989 1-5 2.72 .80 .101*** .089*** .216*** .177*** .103*** .013

Note: *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. a: perceived effectiveness of institutional political participation. b: perceived effectiveness of non-institutional political

participation.

26

Notes

i The original questionnaire contained 4 possible answers as to whether someone had engaged in a form of

political participation: 1) often, 2) sometimes, 3) rarely, 4) never. For reasons of distribution (all items are

heavily skewed toward the ‘never’ category, with only few respondents indicating the ‘often’ and ‘sometimes’

categories), the items were recoded into two categories. The first three categories were recoded into

‘participated’, while the fourth category became ‘did not participate’. ii The original questionnaire contained 10 categories of educational level that cover the complexities of the

Belgian educational system. For reasons of comparability and clarity, these categories were recoded. People

with academic or non-academic higher education were coded as ‘high’. People who finished secondary

education were coded as ‘middle’. Otherwise people were coded as ‘low’.