dimensions of affect modulated by perceived mood regulation ability

7
Dimensions of Affect Modulated by Perceived Mood Regulation Ability Christine A. Hovanitz Adrienne N. Hursh Adam D. Hudepohl Published online: 22 March 2011 Ó Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011 Abstract The ability to regulate mood is a facet of emotional intelligence that may contribute to an individ- ual’s physical and mental health. Precisely what is regu- lated when mood regulation occurs is dependent on what ‘‘makes up’’ mood. The purpose of this study was to evaluate whether perceived mood regulation ability can predict regulation of affect during task engagement and whether affect regulation is specific to valence or arousal. Measures of positive affect, negative affect, and frontal area sEMG (as a measure of arousal) were obtained from a sample of one hundred twenty-four participants catego- rized by their self report as possessing low or high mood regulation ability. Modulation of positive affect, but not negative affect, was predicted by perceived mood regula- tion ability. The results of sEMG were mixed. These data provide some support for the hypothesis that mood regu- lation ability can predict future efforts to regulate affect, at least in the context of task engagement. Keywords Mood regulation Á Positive affect Á Negative affect Á sEMG Meta-cognition has been defined as ‘‘thoughts about thoughts’’ (Metcalfe and Shimamura 1994). A special case of meta-cognition is mood (or affect) meta-cognition, which involves thoughts about affective experience; when so considered, the concept is subsumed under the label of emotional intelligence. The basic formulation is simple. When affect is a focus of thought, affect may become a target of regulation attempts. Because a number of psy- chological and biological disorders have accompanying issues of affective disturbance, the ability to regulate affect may be important to a variety of health-related concerns. Precisely what is regulated when affect regulation is attempted may be considered in part a function of what ‘‘makes up’’ affect. This question touches on definitional issues surrounding distinctions and equivalences between and among the terms emotion, mood, affect, and subjective state. A variety of theoretical models have been proposed that identify different features subsumed by these terms (Ekman and Davidson 1994; Gross 1998; Russell 2009). Despite variability in particular formulations, substantial consensus has been achieved about some of the essential elements of affective structure (Frijda 2006): Valence and arousal are considered the primary dimensions. In addition, emotion is differentiated from other affective states as the former possesses a specific motivational function, a spe- cific goal object, and tends to have a clearly demarcated onset and cessation (Gross 1998). There are exceptions: Brehm et al. (2009) have shown mood to possess certain motivational qualities, and the biologically-based moods (Thayer 1989) tend to accompany if not proceed choice to engage in energetic behavior. This paper will deal with what Russell defines as core affect, which is ‘‘that neurophysiological state consciously accessible as the simplest raw (non-reflective) feelings evident in moods and emotions’’ (Russell 2003, p. 148). Russell’s formulation is similar to what Watson and Tellegen (1985) have called affect, according to Russell (2003), although the two differ on some other substantive but testable elements. For the purposes of the present study, the terms mood and affect will be used interchangeably as shorthand terms for core affect. Watson et al. (1988) define negative affect as a general dimension of subjective C. A. Hovanitz (&) Á A. N. Hursh Á A. D. Hudepohl University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH, USA e-mail: [email protected] 123 Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback (2011) 36:113–119 DOI 10.1007/s10484-011-9154-1

Upload: christine-a-hovanitz

Post on 14-Jul-2016

213 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Dimensions of Affect Modulated by Perceived Mood RegulationAbility

Christine A. Hovanitz • Adrienne N. Hursh •

Adam D. Hudepohl

Published online: 22 March 2011

� Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011

Abstract The ability to regulate mood is a facet of

emotional intelligence that may contribute to an individ-

ual’s physical and mental health. Precisely what is regu-

lated when mood regulation occurs is dependent on what

‘‘makes up’’ mood. The purpose of this study was to

evaluate whether perceived mood regulation ability can

predict regulation of affect during task engagement and

whether affect regulation is specific to valence or arousal.

Measures of positive affect, negative affect, and frontal

area sEMG (as a measure of arousal) were obtained from a

sample of one hundred twenty-four participants catego-

rized by their self report as possessing low or high mood

regulation ability. Modulation of positive affect, but not

negative affect, was predicted by perceived mood regula-

tion ability. The results of sEMG were mixed. These data

provide some support for the hypothesis that mood regu-

lation ability can predict future efforts to regulate affect, at

least in the context of task engagement.

Keywords Mood regulation � Positive affect � Negative

affect � sEMG

Meta-cognition has been defined as ‘‘thoughts about

thoughts’’ (Metcalfe and Shimamura 1994). A special case

of meta-cognition is mood (or affect) meta-cognition,

which involves thoughts about affective experience; when

so considered, the concept is subsumed under the label of

emotional intelligence. The basic formulation is simple.

When affect is a focus of thought, affect may become a

target of regulation attempts. Because a number of psy-

chological and biological disorders have accompanying

issues of affective disturbance, the ability to regulate affect

may be important to a variety of health-related concerns.

Precisely what is regulated when affect regulation is

attempted may be considered in part a function of what

‘‘makes up’’ affect. This question touches on definitional

issues surrounding distinctions and equivalences between

and among the terms emotion, mood, affect, and subjective

state. A variety of theoretical models have been proposed

that identify different features subsumed by these terms

(Ekman and Davidson 1994; Gross 1998; Russell 2009).

Despite variability in particular formulations, substantial

consensus has been achieved about some of the essential

elements of affective structure (Frijda 2006): Valence and

arousal are considered the primary dimensions. In addition,

emotion is differentiated from other affective states as the

former possesses a specific motivational function, a spe-

cific goal object, and tends to have a clearly demarcated

onset and cessation (Gross 1998). There are exceptions:

Brehm et al. (2009) have shown mood to possess certain

motivational qualities, and the biologically-based moods

(Thayer 1989) tend to accompany if not proceed choice to

engage in energetic behavior.

This paper will deal with what Russell defines as core

affect, which is ‘‘that neurophysiological state consciously

accessible as the simplest raw (non-reflective) feelings

evident in moods and emotions’’ (Russell 2003, p. 148).

Russell’s formulation is similar to what Watson and

Tellegen (1985) have called affect, according to Russell

(2003), although the two differ on some other substantive

but testable elements. For the purposes of the present study,

the terms mood and affect will be used interchangeably as

shorthand terms for core affect. Watson et al. (1988) define

negative affect as a general dimension of subjective

C. A. Hovanitz (&) � A. N. Hursh � A. D. Hudepohl

University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH, USA

e-mail: [email protected]

123

Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback (2011) 36:113–119

DOI 10.1007/s10484-011-9154-1

distress and unpleasurable engagement, while positive

affect refers to distinctive dimensions in which a person

feels enthusiastic, active and alert. Russell (2003, 2009),

like many, consider valence bipolar. Watson and Tellegen

(1999) believe positive valence to be independent of neg-

ative valence, although bipolarity may appear with some

measures and under some circumstances.

Because core affect is by definition nonreflective, self-

report of what ‘‘makes up’’ affect is not possible by the

experiencing individual. The phenomenon is not subject to

self conscious reflection as experienced. One means of

examining components of core affect, however, may be

through multiple assessments of the affective experience of

the individual. What may be considered the elements of

core affect, the fundamental elements of any affective

experience (valence and arousal), may be assessed simply

by evaluating the individual by multiple instruments that

evaluate potential components, such as positive affect,

negative affect, and arousal.

The focus of the current study is the regulation of core

affect. Because the two dimensions of valence and arousal

are considered essential to core affect, affect regulation

may take place with respect to either or both. The ques-

tion is both theoretical and practical in implication;

arousal reduction is targeted in stress-reduction interven-

tions as a goal of many physical health-related psycho-

logical interventions. Improvement in affective valence,

on the other hand, is a goal of therapies targeting mood

disorders. Thus, what is important about affect regulation

ability may depend on the disorder of concern. As little

direct evidence has been obtained to support the idea that

perception of affect regulation ability predicts objectively

measured changes in core affect in a prospective manner,

the intent of this study is to obtain evidence for this

claim. Further, this study will attempt to identify whether

positive or negative affect are similarly modulated. In

addition, an attempt will be made to determine if a

measure of central arousal variability is modulated by

perceived mood regulation.

Method

Participants

Approximately 300 undergraduates completed initial test-

ing in groups ranging in size up to eight. One hundred

twenty-four (66 male, 58 female) agreed to participate in

the second phase and successfully completed the hour-long

experimental protocol conducted approximately one week

later. The second phase included 102 Caucasians, 13

African Americans, and 9 of other ethnicities or who

declined to report ethnicity.

Measures

Self report. The Trait Meta-Mood Scale (TMMS; Salovey

et al. 1995) and the Manifest Anxiety Scale (Taylor 1953)

were administered during an initial screening. The Positive

and Negative Affect Scales (Watson et al. 1988) were

completed during the experimental phase.

The Trait Meta-Mood Scale consists of three subscales,

derived by factor analysis. One subscale, Repair, was

employed in the present study. The Repair score is a sum of

ratings on six items; participants respond to each item on a

5-point scale (1 = strongly agree; 5 = strongly disagree).

Repair was shown to possess good internal consistency and

both convergent and discriminative validity. The items

which make up this scale refer to general beliefs about

habitual thoughts and feelings related to maintaining

optimal mood. The six items that make up the scale are the

following: ‘‘I try to think good thoughts no matter how

badly I feel’’; ‘‘Although I am sometimes sad, I have a

mostly optimistic outlook’’; ‘‘When I am upset I realize

that the ‘good things in life’ are illusions’’ (reverse scored);

‘‘When I become upset I remind myself of all the pleasures

in life.’’; ‘‘Although I am sometimes happy, I have a mostly

pessimistic outlook’’ (reverse scored); and ‘‘No matter how

badly I feel, I try to think about pleasant things)’’. Although

multiple translations and versions of the entire TMMS are

available, and multiple studies have examined the relation

between the subscales of the TMMS and indices of mental

and physical health (Gross and John 2003; Salovey et al.

1995; Thompson et al. 2007), norms or cut-off scores on

the 30-item instrument are not available at this time.

The Manifest Anxiety Scale (MAS) is a 50-item mea-

sure of trait anxiety derived from items present in the

original MMPI. Very widely used, the measure consists of

items that reflect diverse symptoms of anxiety. Included are

cognitive (‘‘I am more sensitive than most other people’’),

behavioral (‘‘I have periods of such great restlessness that I

cannot sit long in a chair’’ and physiological (‘‘I sweat very

easily even on cool days’’) items. Participants respond by

indicating whether an item is true or false as it applies to

them; scores range from 0 to 50. Standard scoring of the

instruments was employed, with the sum of the keyed items

analyzed as a continuous variable. Among university stu-

dents, a median score of 13 was cited in the manuscript

covering the initial construction of the test; this can be

compared to the median of 34 obtained from psychiatric

inpatients. Reasonable psychometric properties are also

reported, given the heterogeneity of the item content. Test

retest reliability over a 4 week period was found to be .88

among nonpsychiatric patients, while Kuder-Richardson

internal consistency estimates have ranged from .78 to .84.

The MAS was included in this study to evaluate the

independence of negative affect from anxiety (Tellegen

114 Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback (2011) 36:113–119

123

1985), and the independence of arousal from anxiety

(Hazlett et al. 1994; Hoehn-Saric and McLeod 2000).

The Positive and Negative Affect Scales (PANAS) is a

brief questionnaire consisting of 20 words that assess the

constructs of positive affect (POS) and negative affect

(NEG). Each adjective is rated on a 5-point scale (1 = very

slightly or not at all to 5 = extremely) in response to the

instructions ‘‘indicate to what extent you feel this way right

now, that is, at the present moment’’. Examples of the

10 items which assess positive affect (valence) include

‘‘interested’’ and ‘‘excited’’. Some of the 10 items which

assess negative affect include ‘‘distressed’’ and ‘‘nervous’’.

Cronbach’s coefficient alpha for positive affect as a state,

i.e., assessed under the instructions ‘‘at the present moment’’

is .89; for negative affect, this is .85. Test–retest reliability

over an 8-week interval is .54 for positive affect and .45 for

negative affect when mood is rated as a state; higher test–

retest reliability is reported when the participant is asked to

rate ‘‘typical mood’’, or mood as a trait. These scales have

been used extensively as measures of the two dimensional

models of self report affect. The positive and negative scales

are not significantly correlated according to studies reported

in Watson et al. (1988); others have found these scales to

correlate under some conditions.

Physiological Measure

An Autogenics Biolab module M130 was used to assess

frontal area EMG. The EMG signal was obtained with a

band pass filter at 100–250 HZ. The RMS method was used

to process the signal. Electrodes with a diameter of 12.5

were attached horizontally approximately � inch above

each eyebrow on a vertical line above each pupil. The

ground was attached horizontally between the two active

electrodes.

Signals were sampled at the rate of 10 per second.

Means were collected over 2� min intervals before and

after administration of the Free Choice Protocol, and dur-

ing completion of each of the 7 math problem trials. This

instrument and these filter settings were employed by

Hoehn-Saric et al. (1997), who found evidence that fron-

talis area sEMG reflected central nervous system arousal.

However, means in the present study were obtained over a

2� min interval as opposed to the 5 min interval employed

in that study. Further, as the present study focused on

reactivity as opposed to tonic measures, analysis involving

sEMG controlled for baseline measure by entering the

baseline sEMG as a covariate in relevant analyses.

Experimental Procedure

After the purpose of the study was presented and consent

to participate received, some demographic data (sex,

ethnicity, age) and information on consumption of caffeine

within the past 4 h were obtained.

The ‘‘Free Choice’’ protocol consists of series of trials

during which math problems are presented (Hovanitz et al.

2002). Ten levels of math problem difficulty were created.

The levels of difficulty are differentiated by number of

digits, simplicity of mathematical computation by number

(e.g., by difficulty of operation and by complexity of

numbers operated on, for instance, ‘‘5’’ or ‘‘0’’ vs. ‘‘137’’

and ‘‘263’’).

For this study, 7 trials of math problems were presented

manually to the participant. Each trial involved 15 indi-

vidually-presented problems. Participants chose the diffi-

culty of math problems they wished to work on prior to the

administration of each trial. Their choice was informed:

Before the participants’ initial selection of a trial difficulty,

participants were shown an example problem from each of

the 10 difficulty levels. Participants were told that their

goal was to achieve as high a score as possible, that points

were given for correct answers and that achievement at

higher levels of math difficulty was awarded with more

points. Each problem in a trial was displayed for 5 s, and

then removed from sight. The participant was to provide an

answer within 10 s after the problem was removed. Access

to paper or a calculator was not provided. Any failure to

produce a correct response in this time frame, regardless of

cause (i.e., sneezing, inattention, incorrect answer or no

answer) was considered an incorrect response.

After completion of the first math trial, the participant

was administered the PANAS. The participant was asked

for the math problem difficulty level s/he wished to work

on next, and, following completion of the trial, was again

administered the PANAS. The process was repeated for a

total of 7 trials.

Analysis

Groups reporting high and low mood regulation were

constructed by identifying participants through a mean split

(mean Repair = 22.80). Participants obtaining a score of

22 or 23 were not placed in a low or high mood regulation

group, given the ambiguity of such an assignment. The low

Repair group consisted of 25 males and 17 females; the

high Repair group, 36 males and 35 females.

The evaluation of moment by moment change in

direction of affect valence was made by identifying the

number of times a drop in the quality of affective valence

(report of lower positive affect, higher negative affect, or

higher EMG) was not corrected after choice of the next

level of task difficulty. This technique of assessing a failure

of homeostasis was introduced in the research on baro-

ceptor control of fluctuations in blood pressure (Reyes del

Paso 1994). For example, consider a trial (T ? 1) where a

Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback (2011) 36:113–119 115

123

participant’s self reported mood declined from the trial

previous (T). If the subsequent trial (T ? 2) also resulted in

a decline in mood quality, then that participant would be

considered to have produced a ‘‘ramp’’, serving here as a

measure of failure to regulate. Simply put, a ramp was

produced by two consecutive drops in positive affect, two

consecutive rises in negative affect, or two consecutive

increases in EMG; the lower the number of ramps, the

better the regulation. The seven trials of math problems

created the opportunity for 5 ramps (trials assessing mood

were allowed to overlap; e.g., the second trial for the first

‘‘ramp’’ was the first trial for the second ‘‘ramp,’’ and the

third trial was the first trial for the third ‘‘ramp,’’ etc.). The

number of ‘‘ramps’’, then, served as a dependent variable

assessing objective mood regulation.

Results

Descriptive Data

A significant difference was not found on ethnicity

between low and high scorers on Repair, however, caffeine

consumption did significantly differ [F(1,122) = 9.06,

p \ .005]. Drinking caffeine prior to the experimental

phase was associated with having reported a higher ability

to regulate mood on Repair (23.55 vs. 21.11). The possible

confounding effect of this variable was addressed by

including this variable as a covariate.

Gender differences were also calculated among these

variables. Significant differences in mood regulation ability

were not found [F(1,122) = .61, ns]; however, as typically

found, trait anxiety showed a significant gender difference

[F(1,120) = 7.94, p \ .01; mean for males = 16.70 (8.10);

females, 20.86 (8.15)]. Significant gender differences were

not found on the summed mean scores across the 7 trials

for positive affect, negative affect, or sEMG.

Data presented in Tables 1 and 2 were calculated after

participants at the mean on Repair were removed, to

facilitate comparisons to analyses addressing hypotheses.

Table 1 presents correlations among the predictors and the

mean of variables administered or obtained during the

second phase. Trait anxiety negatively correlated at a

moderate but significant degree with Repair. Repair also

correlated negatively with mean sEMG (the high mood

regulation ability reported, the lower the average EMG

over trials). Negative affect positively correlated, but to a

modest degree, with trait anxiety.

Table 2 provides the means and standard deviations for

the three criteria (positive affect, negative affect, and

sEMG). A decline in positive valence is seen over the

seven trials; this is statistically significant [F(6,

630) = 2.76, p \ .05]. An increase in negative valence is

also seen over the seven trials and this is also statistically

significant [F(6,636) = 2.81, p \ .05]. In contrast, arousal

as assessed by frontal area sEMG did not show a direc-

tional change over the 7 trials of the task [F(6,612) =

1.08, ns].

Analysis Evaluating Hypotheses

The statistical significance of the number of ramps was

evaluated by use of MANCOVAs. In the first ANCOVA,

Repair (low vs. high) was entered as a predictor of the

number of positive affect ramps. In the second ANCOVA,

Repair (low vs. high) predicted negative affect ramps. In

the third, Repair predicted EMG ramps. In all three equa-

tions, caffeine consumption and anxiety (MAS) were

included as covariates.

The second form of regulation addresses the linear tra-

jectory of change in positive or negative affect, or arousal,

throughout the 7 trials. This was accomplished with two

repeated measures MANCOVAs; Repair (low vs. high)

predicted positive and negative affect. In these two equa-

tions, caffeine consumption and anxiety (MAS) were

included as covariates. In a third MANCOVA, Repair (low

vs. high) predicted EMG over the 7 trials. Caffeine con-

sumption, anxiety, and baseline EMG were covariates. All

Table 1 Correlations among predictors and criteria averaged across

trials

MAS REPAIR POS NEG EMG LEVEL

MAS – -.41** -.14 .24** .06 -.05

REPAIR – – .14 .01 -.22** -.09

POS – – – .20** -.08 .19*

NEG – – – – .08 -.25**

EMG – – – – – -.06

* p \ .10

** p \ .05

Table 2 Mean positive and negative affect and EMG

POS NEG EMG

Low High Low High Low High

Baseline 2.81 2.42

TRIAL 1 21.90 21.04 15.45 15.04 3.05 2.93

TRIAL 2 20.33 21.53 14.90 14.51 2.85 2.48

TRIAL 3 18.92 21.59 16.20 15.60 3.07 2.55

TRIAL 4 18.46 20.51 16.13 15.71 3.10 2.50

TRIAL 5 17.26 20.44 16.60 16.46 3.09 2.46

TRIAL 6 17.46 19.78 15.95 16.84 3.10 2.45

TRIAL 7 17.87 18.94 15.80 17.04 2.93 2.43

Baseline 2.68 2.61

116 Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback (2011) 36:113–119

123

repeated measures MANCOVAs controlled sphericity by

the use of the Huynh–Feldt Epsilon.

Missing data points resulted in the elimination of that

measure from the relevant analysis.

Positive Affect

Repair [F(1,107) = 9.42, p \ .005] predicted the number

of ramps (reflecting decline in positive valence). Partici-

pants reporting low mood regulation ability on Repair

produced a mean of 3.48 (STD = 1.15) ramps whereas

those reporting high Repair obtained a mean of 2.80 ramps

(STD = 1.21). In other words, participants who reported

low levels of mood regulation ability were less able to

correct declining positive affect valence than those who

reported high ability.

Repeated Measures

The mean differences between Repair groups over time are

not statistically different when family wise error rates are

controlled using the simulate option for general mixed and

repeated measures models from the Statistical Analysis

Software (SAS). Table 2 provides the mean data for all

trials.

Negative Affect

Repair did not significantly predict the number of negative

valence ramps [F(1, 109) = .01, ns] nor the linear pro-

gression of negative affect over the 7 trials [F(6,612) =

.91, ns].

sEMG

Repair predicted sEMG ramps at a trend level of signifi-

cance [F(1,107) = 3.79, p = .054]. However, the mean

number of ramps are not in the predicted direction (low

mood regulation produced a mean of 2.19 and std of 1.23,

whereas high mood regulation obtained a mean of 2.55 and

a std of 1.00).

Repair did not significantly predict linear change in

sEMG over the 7 trials [F(6,582) = .40, ns]. It should be

noted that Repair had previously been found to negatively

correlate with the mean sEMG across trials.

Trait Anxiety

When employed as a covariate in the equations evaluating

repeated measures hypotheses, trait anxiety (MAS) exerted

a significant main effect on negative valence [F(1,102) =

6.62, p \ .05] and a trend level main effect on sEMG

[F(1,97) = 2.97, p = .08].

Caffeine

When employed as a covariate in the equation evaluating

the repeated measures hypothesis caffeine exerted a posi-

tive main effect with positive affect [F(1,101) = 4.33,

p \ .05).

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to obtain evidence that

perception of affect regulation ability predicted objectively

measured changes in core affect. Findings demonstrated

that low scores on the TMMS—Repair scale predicted less

ability to alter declining positive affect when participants

engaged in a task that permitted substantial moment by

moment control over task difficulty. The mood regulation

ability was apparent when analyzed in a nonlinear fashion;

more successive trials of decreasing positive affect were

found among those reporting low mood regulation ability.

Interestingly, Repair did not predict ability to avoid

increasing negative affect. Perhaps the most remarkable

aspect of this failure to find a significant effect is that most

items on the TMMS—Repair scale refer to the regulation

of a negative mood state, as opposed to the maintenance of

a positive one. A possible contributor to the failure to find

an effect may be observed in a close examination of the

mean data in Table 2. Whereas the mean positive affect

scores declines, for both low and high mood regulation

ability groups, in a clear linear fashion, a simple linear

increase is not observed for both groups with respect to

negative affect. A post hoc analysis was performed to

determine if the covariate MAS was responsible for the

absence of a regulation effect. The covariate was not found

to account for the absence of the hypothesized findings.

Two other aspects of descriptive information shed some

additional light on interpretation of results, however. First,

whereas math problems are perhaps the most common

laboratory stressor employed, the math problem protocol

used here may be the only one employed where the par-

ticipants choose the difficulty of the problems they work

on. The intent, of course, is to formally structure a means

of behavioral regulation for the participants. However, that

freedom fundamentally alters the task—it may no longer be

a laboratory stress task. As seen in Table 1, negative affect

is correlated with the level of math problems chosen—but

in a negative direction. In other words, the higher the

average level of difficulty chosen, the lower the average

Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback (2011) 36:113–119 117

123

negative affect. It would seem that average level chosen is

more like a measure of positive engagement than a measure

of stress. Some data support this interpretation. In Table 1,

a positive correlation is found between chosen level and

positive affect (at a trend level of significance). If this is the

proper interpretation, then at least in part, these participants

were not first and foremost regulating stress, but instead

regulating their interest or engagement with the task.

The mood regulation scale Repair significantly predicted

changes in sEMG, as hypothesized, but not in the predicted

direction when analyzed nonlinearly. The low regulation

group produced fewer ramps (made more adjustments) than

the high regulation group. Repair did not predict linear

change over the course of the 7 trials. However, Repair

significantly although modestly predicted average sEMG

(as shown in Table 1) in the expected direction; examina-

tion of Table 2 reveals an ‘‘eyeball’’ view. Clearly these

findings are complex; interpretation of these data can be

only tentative. The sEMG as a measure is very difficult; the

very high noise to signal ratio creates difficulties in the

simple collection of valid data. Even if one assumes that

the presence of statistically significant data is an indicator

of valid data, other problems exist. Another ‘‘problem’’

may be that the sEMG may assess a general factor and be

relevant to many constructs. The frontal area sEMG cor-

relates with anxiety, headache, and depression (Hovanitz

et al. 2002), to name but a few. In this particular study,

sEMG could even have tapped into the arousal necessary

for successful task engagement. These variables may be in

active competition. Having made note of the possibilities,

there are no analyses here that suggest task performance

was improved by heightened sEMG. One point that is

perhaps important to note is that the temporal window of

reactivity for sEMG, as it relates to the kind of arousal that

was intended to be assessed here, may be rather long.

Repair correlated with sEMG in the expected direction

when assessed over about a 30 min window.

These data addressing the hypotheses needs to be placed

in the larger context of the descriptive data. In terms of the

analyses of linear patterns, positive affect declined, negative

affect rose, and arousal did not show significant change for

both low and high mood regulation ability groups. Thus,

while there was evidence of ability to regulate positive affect

among those claiming mood regulation ability while the task

was underway, and there was a difference favoring the high

regulation group overall, the successful efforts were not

sufficient to sustain the degree of positive affect present at the

start of the protocol. Statistically speaking, negative affect

declined overall as well. In addition, those claiming good

mood regulation ability did not, at the completion of the

protocol, report a significantly more optimal mood state

(more positive or less negative) than those reporting low

mood regulation ability. Thus, although evidence was found

that those reporting mood regulation ability were able to

engage in mood valence trajectory corrections, the magni-

tude or persistence of these corrections was not large.

A look at recent reflections on the nature of the PANAS

positive and negative affect scales and what they measure,

beyond perhaps the intended valence, may provide more

insights. Positive affect as assessed by the PANAS is now

described as related to approach motivation (Watson 2000)

and the PANAS scales have been renamed by the author to

reflect interest in approach/avoidance motivation models.

More recent descriptions for what the positive affect PANAS

scale measures, by the author of the scale, reflect a change

from the original formulation that may explain why this scale

was more susceptible to mood regulation efforts: ‘‘Positive

affect is composed of positively valenced mood states,

including enthusiasm, energy, interest, pleasure, confi-

dence’’ (Gray and Watson 2007, p. 173), and some have

suggested that the scale measures approach motivation that

may even be negative in nature (Harmon-Jones et al. 2009).

Indeed, some kind of an explanation like this might account

for why the positive and negative PANAS scales not only

failed to demonstrate a bipolar relation, as would have been

evidenced by a negative correlation, but were in fact posi-

tively (albeit weakly) correlated in the present study.

This study provided some preliminary data suggesting

that perceived mood regulation ability can predict objec-

tively measured affect regulation in a laboratory setting. It

appears that the type of affect modulated concerned positive

task engagement, in a protocol designed to study the self

regulation of affect and behavior (Carver 2004). However,

the methodology employed here limited what kind of regu-

lation could be found; the protocol optimized the partici-

pant’s ability to regulate mood through positive task

engagement. Future studies should consider use of other

measures of affect, and might benefit from considering a

multisession or longitudinal design employing protocols that

include passive coping tasks of a long duration. Assessment

of the consumption of other substances, such as nicotine or

prescription medications, would also eliminate other sources

of unintended variability in mood. Alternative methods of

assessing arousal may be attempted as well. While some

success has been achieved, as of yet, mood regulation ability

has not been demonstrated to be a generalized skill that

allows modulation of affect by means other than positive task

engagement, by populations other than college students, and

by other measures of affect.

References

Brehm, J. W., Miron, A. M., & Miller, K. (2009). Affect as a

motivational state. Cognition & Emotion, 23, 1069–1089.

Carver, C. S. (2004). Self-regulation of action and affect.

In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of

118 Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback (2011) 36:113–119

123

self-regulation: Research, theory and applications. New York,

NY: Guilford Press.

Ekman, P., & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.). (1994). The nature of emotion:Fundamental questions. New York: Oxford University Press.

Frijda, N. H. (2006). The laws of emotion. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Gray, E. K., & Watson, D. (2007). Assessing positive and negative

affect via self-report. In J. A. Coan & J. J. B. Allen (Eds.),

Handbook of emotional elicitation and assessment (pp. 171–183).

New York: Oxford Press.

Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An

integrative review. Review of General Psychology, 2, 271–299.

Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two

emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relation-

ships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 85, 348–362.

Harmon-Jones, E., Hormon-Jone, C., Abramson, L., & Peterson, C. K.

(2009). PANAS positive activation is associated with anger.

Emotion, 9, 183–196.

Hazlett, R. L., HcLeod, D. R., & Hoehn-Saric, R. (1994). Muscle

tension in generalized anxiety disorder: Elevated muscle tonus or

agitated movement? Psychophysiology, 31, 189–195.

Hoehn-Saric, R., Hazlett, R. L., Pourmotabbed, R., & McLeod, D. R.

(1997). Does muscle tension reflect arousal? Relationship

between electromyographic and electroencephalographic record-

ings. Psychiatry Research, 71, 49–55.

Hoehn-Saric, R., & McLeod, D. R. (2000). Anxiety and arousal:

Physiological changes and their perception. Journal of AffectiveDisorders, 61, 217–224.

Hovanitz, C. A., Christianson, A. L., Stokes-Crowe, L., & Scheff, J.

(2002a). Effects of headache-state and headache-proneness on

objective and perceived performance. Headache, 42, 603–611.

Hovanitz, C. A., Filippides, M., Lindsay, D., & Scheff, J. (2002b).

Muscle tension and physiologic hyperarousal, performance and

state affectivity: Assessing the independence of effects in

frequent headache and depression. Applied Psychophysiologyand Biofeedback, 27, 29–44.

Metcalfe, J., & Shimamura, A. P. (1994). Metacognition: Knowingabout knowing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Reyes del Paso, G. A. (1994). A program to assess baroreceptor

cardiac reflex function. Research Methods, Instruments, andComputers, 26, 62–64.

Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction

of emotion. Psychological Review, 110, 145–175.

Russell, J. A. (2009). Emotion, core affect, and psychological

construction. Cognition & Emotion, 23, 1259–1283.

Salovey, P., Mayer, J. D., Goldman, S. L., Turvey, C., & Palfai, T. P.

(1995). Emotional attention, clarity, and repair: Exploring

emotional intelligence using the trait meta-mood scale. In

J. W. Pennebaker (Ed.), Emotion, disclosure, and health(pp. 125–154). Washington, D.C.: APA.

Taylor, J. A. (1953). A personality scale of manifest anxiety. TheJournal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 48, 285–290.

Tellegen, A. (1985). Structures of mood and personality and their

relevance to assessing anxiety, with an emphasis on self-report.

In A. H. Tuma & J. D. Maser (Eds.), Anxiety and the anxietydisorders (pp. 681–706). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Thayer, R. E. (1989). The biopsychology of mood and arousal. New

York: Oxford University Press.

Thayer, R. E. (1996). The origin of everyday moods: Managingenergy, tension, and stress. New York: Oxford University Press.

Thompson, B. L., Waltz, J., Croyle, K., & Pepper, A. C. (2007). Trait

meta-mood and affect as predictors of somatic symptoms and

life satisfaction. Personality and Individual Differences, 43,

1786–1795.

Watson, D. (2000). Mood and temperament. New York: Guilford

Press.

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and

validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The

PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,54, 1063–1070.

Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual structure of

mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 219–235.

Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1999). Issues in the dimensional

structure of affect—effects of descriptors, measurement error,

and response formats: Comment on Russell and Carroll (1999).

Psychological Bulletin, 125, 601–610.

Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback (2011) 36:113–119 119

123