coping self-efficacy, pre-competitive anxiety, and subjective performance

7
PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by: [European College of Sport Science] On: 22 April 2011 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 909201977] Publisher Taylor & Francis Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37- 41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK European Journal of Sport Science Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.informaworld.com/smpp/title~content=t714592354 Coping self-efficacy, pre-competitive anxiety, and subjective performance among athletes Adam R. Nicholls a ; Remco Polman b ; Andrew R. Levy c a Department of Psychology, University of Hull, Hull b Centre for Applied Sport and Exercise Sciences, University of Central Lancashire, Preston c Department of Sport Science, University of Leeds, Leeds, UK Online publication date: 08 February 2010 To cite this Article Nicholls, Adam R. , Polman, Remco and Levy, Andrew R.(2010) 'Coping self-efficacy, pre-competitive anxiety, and subjective performance among athletes', European Journal of Sport Science, 10: 2, 97 — 102 To link to this Article: DOI: 10.1080/17461390903271592 URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17461390903271592 Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.informaworld.com/terms-and-conditions-of-access.pdf This article may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug doses should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.

Upload: jp-merchant

Post on 18-Apr-2015

45 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Coping Self-efficacy, Pre-competitive Anxiety, And Subjective Performance

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

This article was downloaded by: [European College of Sport Science]On: 22 April 2011Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 909201977]Publisher Taylor & FrancisInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

European Journal of Sport SciencePublication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:http://www.informaworld.com/smpp/title~content=t714592354

Coping self-efficacy, pre-competitive anxiety, and subjective performanceamong athletesAdam R. Nichollsa; Remco Polmanb; Andrew R. Levyc

a Department of Psychology, University of Hull, Hull b Centre for Applied Sport and Exercise Sciences,University of Central Lancashire, Preston c Department of Sport Science, University of Leeds, Leeds,UK

Online publication date: 08 February 2010

To cite this Article Nicholls, Adam R. , Polman, Remco and Levy, Andrew R.(2010) 'Coping self-efficacy, pre-competitiveanxiety, and subjective performance among athletes', European Journal of Sport Science, 10: 2, 97 — 102To link to this Article: DOI: 10.1080/17461390903271592URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17461390903271592

Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.informaworld.com/terms-and-conditions-of-access.pdf

This article may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial orsystematic reproduction, re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply ordistribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden.

The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contentswill be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug dosesshould be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss,actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directlyor indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.

Page 2: Coping Self-efficacy, Pre-competitive Anxiety, And Subjective Performance

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Coping self-efficacy, pre-competitive anxiety, and subjectiveperformance among athletes

ADAM R. NICHOLLS1, REMCO POLMAN2, & ANDREW R. LEVY3

1Department of Psychology, University of Hull, Hull, 2Centre for Applied Sport and Exercise Sciences, University of Central

Lancashire, Preston, and 3Department of Sport Science, University of Leeds, Leeds, UK

AbstractThe aim of this study was to explore the relationships between (a) coping self-efficacy and subjective performance,(b) coping self-efficacy and pre-competitive anxiety, and (c) pre-competitive anxiety and subjective performance.Participants were 307 athletes (252 males, 55 females) aged 16�34 years (mean age 21.3 years, s�2.8) who competed atnational/international (n�18), county (n�54), club/university (n�139), and beginner (n�96) level. All participantscompleted a measure of coping self-efficacy and anxiety before a competitive event and a subjective performance measureafter competing. Our findings revealed that there was a significant and positive relationship between coping self-efficacy andsubjective performance. Negative relationships between coping self-efficacy and both somatic and cognitive anxiety werealso observed. However, somatic and cognitive anxiety did not predict subjective performance. The present findings supportprevious results regarding the influence of self-efficacy and provide applied practitioners with recommendations that mayenhance athletic performance, via improving the coping self-efficacy beliefs of their clients.

Keywords: Coping self-efficacy, anxiety, performance

Introduction

Self-efficacy refers to the belief an individual has in

his or her ability to execute a task and thus obtain the

desired outcome (Bandura, 1997) and is considered

to be an important attribute within sport. In

their review, Moritz and colleagues (Moritz, Feltz,

Fahrbach, & Mack, 2000) found a ‘‘positive’’ and

‘‘moderate’’ relationship between sport-specific skill-

based self-efficacy and performance in a variety of

sports (r�0.38). It has been suggested that indivi-

duals have a number of efficacy-related beliefs, but

that these beliefs can vary greatly in each person

(Feltz, Short, & Sullivan, 2008; Forsyth & Carey,

1998). It is therefore essential that researchers

explore different components of self-efficacy. One

such distinct efficacy belief relates to an athlete’s

ability to cope, which is referred to as ‘‘coping self-

efficacy’’. Coping self-efficacy (Bandura, 1997;

Chesney, Neilands, Chambers, Taylor, & Folkman,

2006) refers to a person’s belief in his or her ability to

deploy strategies that will assist in coping with

diverse threats or stressors and would therefore

play an important role in reactions to stress

(Perraud, Fogg, Kopytko, & Gross, 2006). Bandura

(1997) suggested that coping self-efficacy beliefs

determine, to a large extent, the subjective threat

of environmental events, which is the core relational

theme of anxiety (Lazarus & Averill, 1972).

Park and Folkman (1997) suggested that variables

such as self-efficacy ‘‘clearly’’ exert strong influences

on situational appraisals and the way in which an

individual responds to these appraisals such as

anxiety. Anxiety consists of cognitive (e.g. cognitive

anxiety) and behavioural (e.g. somatic anxiety)

components that form a multi-dimensional con-

struct (Martens, Burton, Vealey, Bump, & Smith,

1990). Cognitive anxiety refers to negative expecta-

tions and the concerns a person may have, whereas

somatic anxiety refers to the person’s physiological

arousal (Martens et al., 1990).

The relationship between anxiety and athletic

performance is somewhat equivocal. Many theories

Correspondence: A. R. Nicholls, Department of Psychology, University of Hull, Cottingham Road, Hull HU6 7RX, UK. E-mail:

[email protected]

European Journal of Sport Science, March 2010; 10(2): 97�102

ISSN 1746-1391 print/ISSN 1536-7290 online # 2010 European College of Sport Science

DOI: 10.1080/17461390903271592

Downloaded By: [European College of Sport Science] At: 18:11 22 April 2011

Page 3: Coping Self-efficacy, Pre-competitive Anxiety, And Subjective Performance

and models have tried to clarify the relationship

between anxiety and sport performance. It has been

suggested that cognitive anxiety might influence all

forms of athletic performance in a negative linear

fashion, whereas somatic anxiety tends to disrupt

fine motor skill in a quadratic way (Lavallee,

Kremer, Moran, & Williams, 2004). More recently,

it has been suggested that the interpretation of

anxiety symptoms is also of importance in the

experience of anxiety. That is, the way an athlete

perceives his or her arousal may result in the

situation being judged as either (a) positive and

challenging or (b) negative and overwhelming

(Mellalieu, Hanton, & Fletcher, 2006). The asser-

tion that the interpretation of anxiety symptoms can

be either facilitative or debilitative has received some

support in the sport literature (Jones & Swain,

1992). However, two recent meta-analyses argued

that the relationships between anxiety and sporting

performance is relatively weak, but in the negative

direction (e.g. Craft, Magyar, Becker, & Feltz, 2003;

Woodman & Hardy, 2003).

In summary, searches on SPORTdiscus, Psych-

LIT, and PsychINFO in June 2009 did not yield any

studies that have examined the relationship between

coping self-efficacy and state anxiety among athletes.

There is, however, some evidence that other forms of

self-efficacy are inversely related to anxiety (e.g.

Cartoni, Minganti, & Zelli, 2005; Haney & Long,

1995). In response to Bandura’s (1997) recommen-

dation that sport psychology scholars focus on

arousal and coping self-efficacy, in this study

we explored the relationships between (a) coping

self-efficacy and subjective performance, (b) coping

self-efficacy and pre-competitive anxiety, and

(c) pre-competitive anxiety and subjective perfor-

mance. Although the literature suggests that there is

a relationship between task self-efficacy regarding

sport-specific activities and performance (e.g.

Moritz et al., 2000), the relationship between coping

self-efficacy and athletic performance has not been

explored and is worthy of exploration given that

coping self-efficacy is quite different to athletes’

efficacy to perform a specific task and the suggestion

that self-efficacy beliefs differ across different skills

(Feltz et al., 2008; Forsyth & Carey, 1998). We

hypothesized that there would be a positive relation-

ship between coping self-efficacy and subjective

sport performance. We also hypothesized that there

would be a negative relationship between coping

self-efficacy and anxiety. That is, athletes with

greater coping self-efficacy would experience less

pre-competitive anxiety. Finally, we predicted that

there would be a negative relationship between

anxiety and subjective athletic performance.

Methods

Participants

Participants were 307 athletes (252 males, 55

females) aged 16�34 years (mean age 21.3 years,

s�2.8) with an average competitive sport experience

of 9.1 years (s�5.2). The sample consisted of team

(n�198) and individual (n�109) sports performers

competing at national/international (n�18), county

(n�54), club/university (n�139), and beginner

(n�96) level. The sample consisted of 297 Cauca-

sian and 10 Black/Asian participants. Participants

completed an informed consent form before the

study began. The study was approved by a

university’s research ethics committee.

Questionnaires

The Coping Self-Efficacy scale (CSES; Chesney

et al., 2006), which was developed together with

Dr Albert Bandura of Stanford University, was used

to assess coping self-efficacy. The CSES is a 26-item

measure, with three higher-order dimensions: use

problem-focused coping, stop unpleasant emotions

and thoughts, and get support from family and

friends. The scale uses an 11-point scale, rating the

extent to which the participants feel that they can

perform a behaviour important to effective coping.

As such, the CSES assessed the athletes’ confidence

with regards to carrying out coping strategies.

Therefore, a higher CSES score would suggest that

a person is more confident in his or her ability to

cope (Chesney et al., 2006). The participants

responded to the stem ‘‘when things aren’t going

well for you, or when you’re having problems, how

confident or certain are you that you can do the

following?’’ Examples of ‘‘use problem-focused cop-

ing self-efficacy’’ included questions relating to the

athletes’ ability to ‘‘sort out what can be changed

and what cannot be changed’’, ‘‘find solutions to

your most difficult problems’’, and ‘‘talk positively to

yourself ’’. Examples of ‘‘stop unpleasant emotions

and thoughts’’ included ‘‘make unpleasant thoughts

go away’’, ‘‘take your mind off unpleasant thoughts’’,

and ‘‘stop yourself being upset by unpleasant

thoughts’’. Finally, ‘‘get support from family and

friends’’ comprised questions relating to the athletes’

perceptions of being able to ‘‘get emotional support

from friends or family’’, ‘‘get a friend to help you

with the things you need’’, and ‘‘do something

positive for yourself when you are feeling discour-

aged’’. The scale is anchored at 0�‘‘cannot do at

all’’, 5�‘‘moderately can do’’, and 10�‘‘certain can

do’’. The scale has adequate reliability, with a

Cronbach alpha of 0.95 (Chesney et al., 2006). In

the present study, the scale had a Cronbach alpha of

0.88.

98 A. R. Nicholls et al.

Downloaded By: [European College of Sport Science] At: 18:11 22 April 2011

Page 4: Coping Self-efficacy, Pre-competitive Anxiety, And Subjective Performance

The participants completed the revised Competi-

tive State Anxiety Inventory-2 (CSAI-2R; Cox,

Martens, & Russell, 2003). The CSAI-2R is a

multidimensional domain-specific instrument to as-

sess state anxiety in competitive sport contexts. It

consists of 17 questions rated on a 4-point Likert

scale (‘‘not at all’’ to ‘‘very much’’). The response set

‘‘how are you feeling right now?’’ was used. The

CSAI-2R contains three constructs, somatic anxiety

(n�7 questions), cognitive anxiety (n�5 ques-

tions), and self-confidence (n�5 questions). Scores

for each subscale were obtained by adding up all

items for each scale and dividing this by the number

of items and multiplying by 10. Scores ranged

between 10 and 40. Good psychometric properties

(reliability and fit indicators) were reported for the

CSAI-2R in the confirmatory factor analysis study

by Cox et al. (2003). The Cronbach alphas for the

present study were 0.80 for somatic anxiety, 0.73 for

cognitive anxiety, and 0.84 for self-confidence.

Finally, participants subjectively rated their perfor-

mance satisfaction following the competition they

just competed in on a scale from 1�‘‘totally dissa-

tisfied’’ to 10�‘‘totally satisfied’’, as recommend-

ed by Biddle and colleagues (Biddle, Hanrahan, &

Sellars, 2001).

Procedure

Athletes and coaches of sports teams within the

United Kingdom received letters detailing the nature

of the study and participant requirements. If the

coaches granted permission for the data collection,

an information letter and consent form was distrib-

uted. Research assistants, who had received training

in quantitative techniques, administered the ques-

tionnaires in the same order. Participants completed

the CSES (Chesney et al., 2006) and the CSAI-2R

(Cox et al., 2003) within 3 h of a competitive event

starting. The CSES was completed in relation to the

participants’ ability to cope generally and not

specifically to the competition they were due to

compete in. Conversely, the CSAI-2R was com-

pleted in relation to how the participants were feeling

at the time of completing the scale. The subjective

performance scale (Biddle et al., 2001) was com-

pleted within 30 min of the competitive event

finishing.

Data analyses

Data were initially screened for outliers and normal-

ity. Cronbach alphas and descriptive statistics were

calculated on all study variables. Then, correlations

between the variables were calculated. Differences in

both self-efficacy beliefs (Lirgg, 1991) and state

anxiety (Woodman & Hardy, 2003) have been found

between the sexes and differences in anxiety between

athletes competing in either individual or team

sports (Martens et al., 1990) or athletes competing

at different standards of competition (Campbell &

Jones, 1995). We therefore conducted separate

multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVA) to

determine whether gender, sport type (individual

vs. team), or level of achievement influenced ratings

of coping self-efficacy, anxiety (cognitive, somatic,

and self-confidence), or subjective performance.

Univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) was con-

ducted in the instance of a significant MANOVA

main effect. Tukey post-hoc comparisons were con-

ducted in the instance of significant effects for level

of achievement.

To idetermine whether coping self-efficacy pre-

dicted subjective performance or anxiety, we con-

ducted hierarchical regression analysis. The

dependent variables were subjective performance,

somatic anxiety, cognitive anxiety, or self-confi-

dence. At Step 1 we entered gender, sport type,

and highest level of achievement. At Step 2 coping

self-efficacy was entered. Hierarchical regression

analysis was also conducted to ascertain whether

anxiety predicted subjective performance. At Step 1

we controlled for the possible effects of gender, sport

type, and level of achievement. At Step 2 somatic

anxiety, cognitive anxiety, and self-confidence were

entered. Since the main aim of the present study was

to determine whether coping self-efficacy or anxiety

predicted subjective performance, we were inter-

ested in the additional variance (DR2) coping self-

efficacy or anxiety added above and beyond the

possible variance explained by gender, sport type,

and level of achievement.

Results

Table I provides the coping self-efficacy, CSAI-2R,

and subjective performance satisfaction means and

standard deviations for the whole sample, male

athletes, female athletes, team sport athletes, indivi-

dual athletes, and athletes of different levels of

achievement separately. Table II provides an over-

view of the Pearson product�moment correlations.

The MANOVA for gender was significant (Wilks’

l�0.81; PB0.001; h2�0.19). The follow-up AN-

OVAs revealed significant differences for somatic

anxiety (F1,305�27.25; PB0.001; h2�0.08), self-

confidence (F1,305�41.65; PB0.001; h2�0.12),

and performance scores (F1,305�7.14; P�0.01;

h2�0.02), but not for coping self-efficacy (P�0.15) or cognitive anxiety (P�0.12). The females

scored lower in self-confidence but higher on

somatic anxiety and performance than the males.

The MANOVA for sport type was also

significant (Wilks’ l�0.93; P�0.001; h2�0.07).

Coping self-efficacy 99

Downloaded By: [European College of Sport Science] At: 18:11 22 April 2011

Page 5: Coping Self-efficacy, Pre-competitive Anxiety, And Subjective Performance

The follow-up ANOVAs revealed significant differ-

ences for somatic anxiety (F1,305�7.92; P�0.01;

h2�0.03) and self-confidence (F1,305�17.69; PB

0.001; h2�0.06), but not for coping self-efficacy

(P�0.11), cognitive anxiety (P�0.28), or subjective

performance (P�0.12). The individual athletes

reported lower self-confidence but higher somatic

anxiety.

Finally, the MANOVA for achievement was also

significant (Wilks’ l�0.83; PB0.001; h2�0.06).

Follow-up ANOVAs revealed significant differences

for somatic anxiety (F1,305�3.39; P�0.02; h2�0.03), cognitive anxiety (F1,305�5.05; P�0.002;

h2�0.05), coping self-efficacy (F1,305�8.39;

PB0.001; h2�0.08), and performance scores

(F1,305�2.72; P�0.05; h2�0.03), but not for self-

confidence (P�0.06). Post-hoc comparisons showed

that the international/national athletes scored sig-

nificantly higher for coping self-efficacy and cogni-

tive anxiety than all other groups. Also, the

international/national athletes scored higher than

the beginner athletes on subjective performance

and higher on somatic anxiety than the county

athletes (all PB0.05). Because differences were

found for gender, sport type, and level of achieve-

ment, all three variable were entered first in the

regression models.

The regression analysis for coping self-efficacy and

subjective performance, after controlling for gender,

sport type, and lkevel of achievement, was significant

with coping self-efficacy explaining 4% of the

variance (DR2�0.04; b�0.21, PB0.001). Coping

self-efficacy was negatively associated with somatic

anxiety (DR2�0.03; b��0.16, P�0.003) and

cognitive anxiety (DR2�0.03; b��0.18, P�0.002), but positively associated with self-confidence

(DR2�0.17; b�0.43, PB0.001). Finally, the re-

gression analysis for subjective performance and

anxiety was also significant (DR2�0.03; P�0.02),

but only self-confidence (b�0.16) contributed

significantly to the model.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to explore the relation-

ships between (a) coping self-efficacy and subjective

performance, (b) coping self-efficacy and pre-com-

petitive anxiety, and (c) pre-competitive anxiety and

subjective performance. The results of the study

indicate that there was a significant and positive

relationship between coping self-efficacy and sub-

jective performance. Negative relationships between

coping self-efficacy and both somatic anxiety and

cognitive anxiety were observewd. However, there

was no significant relationship between subjective

performance and either cognitive or somatic anxiety.

As such, two of the three hypotheses were supported.Table

I.C

opin

gse

lf-e

ffica

cy,

an

xie

ty,

an

dsu

bje

ctiv

eper

form

an

cesa

tisf

act

ion

(mea

ns

an

dst

an

dard

dev

iati

on

s)

Gen

der

Sp

ort

typ

eA

chie

vem

ent

Lev

el

Ove

rall

(N�

307)

Male

s

(n�

252)

Fem

ale

s

(n�

55)

Tea

m

(n�

198)

Ind

ivid

ual

(n�

109)

Nati

on

al/

inte

rnati

on

al

(n�

18)

Cou

nty

(n�

54)

Un

iver

sity

/clu

b

(n�

139)

Beg

inn

ers

(n�

96)

Copin

gse

lf-e

ffic

acy

167

(24.3

)168

(23.8

)161

(25.8

)168

(24.2

)163

(24.1

)193

(26.5

)**

168

(24.4

)165

(25.2

)164

(19.2

)

Som

ati

can

xie

ty18.0

(5.8

)17.2

(5.0

)**

21.5

(7.6

)17.3

(5.3

)**

19.2

(6.4

)20.9

(7.2

)*16.6

(4.8

)18.5

(6.6

)17.4

(4.3

)

Cogn

itiv

ean

xie

ty22.3

(6.5

)22.0

(6.4

)23.5

(6.6

)22.0

(6.5

)22.8

(6.5

)27.1

(7.2

)**

21.2

(6.8

)22.7

(6.9

)21.3

(5.0

)

Sel

f-co

nfi

den

ce27.5

(6.8

)28.6

(6.5

)**

22.5

(6.2

)28.7

(6.2

)**

25.4

(7.4

)29.9

(9.5

)28.8

(6.6

)26.5

(6.2

)27.9

(7.1

)

Su

bje

ctiv

ep

erfo

rman

ce

sati

sfact

ion

6.3

(1.8

)6.2

(1.8

)**

6.9

(1.8

)6.4

(2.6

)6.1

(2.1

)7.2

(2.0

)**

6.4

(1.8

)6.3

(1.8

)6.0

(1.7

)

*PB

0.0

5;

**

PB

0.0

1.

100 A. R. Nicholls et al.

Downloaded By: [European College of Sport Science] At: 18:11 22 April 2011

Page 6: Coping Self-efficacy, Pre-competitive Anxiety, And Subjective Performance

Even though self-efficacy scholars such as Feltz

et al. (2008) and Forsyth and Carey (1998) have

suggested that individuals have a number of self-

efficacy beliefs that can vary greatly, the positive

relationship between coping self-efficacy and sub-

jective performance is in line with Moritz and

colleagues’ (2000) findings. Moritz and colleagues

reported a ‘‘positive’’ and ‘‘moderate’’ relationship

between sport-specific skill-based self-efficacy and

performance. The present findings add to the

literature, because they suggest that in addition to

specific skill-based self-efficacy among athletes, a

person’s belief relating to how effectively they will be

able to deploy strategies that will assist in coping

with threats or stressors (i.e. coping self-efficacy) is

also associated with performance.

The positive association between coping self-

efficacy and athletic performance may have impor-

tant applied connotations, as enhancing an athlete’s

coping self-efficacy may have a positive impact on

performance, but research is required to test this

assertion. Feltz et al. (2008) provided some excellent

ideas on how to increase an athlete’s coping self-

efficacy, including modelling and demonstrating

coping strategies. For instance, athletes could ob-

serve performances of athletes who exhibit confident

behaviours during times of stress. Another technique

to enhance coping self-efficacy, suggested by Feltz et

al., is to encourage athletes low in coping self-

efficacy to talk to other athletes who have improved

their coping self-efficacy and the specific strategies

they adopted to enhance it.

Furthermore, the finding that coping self-efficacy

was negatively associated with somatic state anxiety

and cognitive state anxiety supports previous re-

search from the sport psychology literature, which

has explored the relationship between anxiety and

self-efficacy. Cartoni et al. (2005) and Haney and

Long (1995) both found a negative relationship

between self-efficacy and anxiety. Bandura (1988)

previously suggested that there is a negative relation-

ship between perceived threat (e.g. anxiety) and

coping self-efficacy, because if people do not feel

they can cope with potential threats they will

experience disruptive arousal (i.e. anxiety). As

such, enhancing an athlete’s coping self-efficacy

beliefs through teaching them a variety of coping

strategies has the potential to reduce pre-competitive

state anxiety. Although the threat of playing sport

competitively will remain, athletes that are taught

coping self-efficacy strategies will feel that they are

more equipped to deal with such threats and may

experience less anxiety, due to the assumed relation-

ship between coping self-efficacy and anxiety (e.g.

Bandura, 1988). However, research is required to

test the effects of coping self-efficacy interventions in

relation to pre-competitive state anxiety.

The third hypothesis, that there would be a

negative relationship between state anxiety and

performance satisfaction, was not supported. This

finding partially supports Woodman and Hardy’s

(2003) meta-analysis, which suggested that there is

no relationship between somatic anxiety and perfor-

mance. However, the lack of association between

cognitive anxiety and performance in the present

study was unexpected. It should be noted that we

used a subjective performance rating and future

studies could include both subjective and objective

ratings of performance. This, however, was not

possible in the present study because the sample

consisted of athletes from a diverse range of sports

and it would have been impossible to have a

consistent measure of performance across the

sample.

Overall, the relationships observed in this study

between coping self-efficacy and subjective perfor-

mance and between coping self-efficacy and anxiety

are in line with previous research that has explored

sport-specific skill-based self-efficacy in relation to

performance (e.g. Moritz et al., 2000) and anxiety

(e.g. Haney & Long, 1995). Even though coping

self-efficacy is considered to be a separate construct

from self-efficacy (Chesney et al., 2006), based on

the current findings it could be argued that both

coping self-efficacy and self-efficacy share some of

the same predictive capabilities in relation to state

anxiety and athletic performance.

One limitation of the present research is that we

cannot infer causality from our findings between the

observed variables. Experimental research designs

are required to identify whether coping self-efficacy

influences performance or anxiety, or whether it is

Table II. Pearson product�moment correlations between coping self-efficacy, CSAI-2-R subscales, and subjective performance satisfaction

Global coping self-efficacy Somatic anxiety Cognitive anxiety Self-confidence

Global coping self-efficacy

Somatic anxiety (SA) �0.17**

Cognitive anxiety (CA) �0.15** 0.49**

Self-confidence (SC) 0.45** �0.38** �0.20**

Subjective performance satisfaction 0.22** �0.03 �0.08 0.13*

*PB0.05; **PB0.01.

Coping self-efficacy 101

Downloaded By: [European College of Sport Science] At: 18:11 22 April 2011

Page 7: Coping Self-efficacy, Pre-competitive Anxiety, And Subjective Performance

anxiety and performance that determines an athlete’s

coping self-efficacy. Furthermore, although there

were a number of positive and negative correlations

in the present study, the amount of shared variance

between some of the constructs was only low to

moderate.

In summary, this study found a significant and

positive relationship between coping self-efficacy

and subjective performance. Additionally, negative

relationships between coping self-efficacy and both

somatic and cognitive state anxiety were observed.

These findings suggest that coping self-efficacy

shares some of the predictive capabilities of self-

efficacy. Furthermore, applied practitioners might

be able to enhance the performance of their clients

and reduce anxiety by improving the coping self-

efficacy beliefs of their clients, but research is

required to test this assertion.

References

Bandura, A. (1988). Self-efficacy conceptualization of anxiety.

Anxiety Research, 1, 77�98.

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York:

W. H. Freeman.

Biddle, S. J. H., Hanrahan, S. J., & Sellars, C. N. (2001).

Attributions: Past, present, and future. In R. N. Singer, H. A.

Hausenblas, & C. M. Janelle (Eds.), Handbook of sport

psychology (pp. 444�471). Chichester: Wiley.

Campbell, E., & Jones, G. (1995). Pre-competition anxiety and

self-confidence in elite and non-elite wheelchair sport partici-

pants. Journal of Sports Sciences, 13, 416�417.

Cartoni, A. C., Minganti, C., & Zelli, A. (2005). Gender, age, and

professional-level differences in the psychological correlates of

fear of injury in Italian gymnasts. Journal of Sport Behavior, 28,

3�17.

Chesney, M. A., Neilands, T. B., Chambers, D. B., Taylor, J. M.,

& Folkman, S. (2006). A validity and reliability study of the

coping self-efficacy scale. British Journal of Health Psychology,

11, 421�437.

Cox, R. H., Martens, M. P., & Russell, W. D. (2003). Measuring

anxiety in athletics: The revised Competitive State Anxiety

Inventory-2. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 25,

519�533.

Craft, L. L., Magyar, M., Becker, B. J., & Feltz, D. L. (2003). The

relationship between the Competitive State Anxiety Inventory-

2 and sport performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Sport and

Exercise Psychology, 25, 44�65.

Feltz, D. L., Short, S. E., & Sullivan, P. J. (2008). Self-efficacy in

sport: Research and strategies for working with athletes, teams, and

coaches. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Forsyth, A. D., & Carey, M. P. (1998). Measuring self-efficacy in

the context of HIV risk reduction: Research challenges and

recommendations. Health Psychology, 17, 559�568.

Haney, C. J., & Long, B. C. (1995). Coping effectiveness: A path

analysis of self-efficacy, control, coping and performance in

sport competitions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 25,

1726�1746.

Jones, G., & Swain, A. B. J. (1992). Intensity and direction

dimensions of competitive anxiety and relationships with

competitiveness. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 74, 467�472.

Lavallee, D., Kremer, J., Moran, A. P., & Williams, M. (2004).

Sport psychology: Contemporary themes. Basingstoke: Palgrave

Macmillan.

Lazarus, R. S., & Averill, J. R. (1972). Emotion and cognition:

With special reference to anxiety. In C. D. Spielberger (Ed.),

Anxiety and behavior (2nd edn., pp. 242�283). New York:

Academic Press.

Lirgg, C. D. (1991). Gender differences in self-confidence in

physical activity: A meta-analysis of recent studies. Journal of

Sport and Exercise Psychology, 8, 294�310.

Martens, R., Burton, D., Vealey, R. S., Bump, L. A., & Smith,

D. E. (1990). Development and validation of the Competitive

State Anxiety Inventory-2 (CSAI-2). In R. Martens, R. S.

Vealey, & D. Burton (Eds.), Competitive anxiety in sport

(pp. 193�208). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Mellalieu, S. D., Hanton, S., & Fletcher, D. (2006). A competitive

anxiety review: Recent directions in sport psychology research.

In S. Hanton, & S. D. Mellalieu (Eds.), Literature reviews in

sport psychology (pp. 1�45). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science.

Moritz, S. E., Feltz, D. L., Fahrbach, K. R., & Mack, D. E.

(2000). The relation of self-efficacy measures to sport perfor-

mance: A meta-analytic review. Research Quarterly for Exercise

and Sport, 71, 280�294.

Park, C. L., & Folkman, S. (1997). Meaning in the context of

stress and coping. Review of General Psychology, 1, 115�144.

Perraud, S., Fogg, L., Kopytko, E., & Gross, D. (2006). Predictive

validity of the Depression Coping Self-efficacy Scale (DCSES).

Research in Nursing and Health, 29, 147�160.

Woodman, T., & Hardy, L. (2003). The relative impact of

cognitive anxiety and self-confidence upon sport performance:

A meta-analysis. Journal of Sports Sciences, 21, 443�457.

102 A. R. Nicholls et al.

Downloaded By: [European College of Sport Science] At: 18:11 22 April 2011