conflict analysis and resolution - harvard university · perpetuation. this uunlysis of...

39
, / \ 10 Herbert C. Kelman arm Ronald 1. Fisner Conflict Analysis and Resolution This chapter presents a social-psychological approach ro the analysis and resolution of international and inrercornmunal conflicrs. Irs central focus is DO interactive conflict resolution (sec Fisher. J997), n family of models [or intervening in deep-roored, procracted conflicts between identity groups, which is anchored in psychological principle,.;, 1ntcmarional conflict resolution can be placed in the context of a larger, growing held of practice. applied at different levels and in diffcrentdomains, and anchored in differem disciplines, theoretical traditions, and fields of practice. Despite this diversity, certain common threads run tbrough most of the work in this ficld. Thus these approac!'cI to eonlllcr resolution \;CII- crally call for a nonadversarial framework for addressing the conflict, an analytic point of dl'p::lHllre, a or icnmrion, direct pnrrici- potion of the conflicting panics in joint efforts to shape a solution, and facilitation by ;;l third party trained ill the of conflict resolution. Crcsslevel exchanges arc very valuable for developing general principles, bur the application of these principles requires sensitivity (0 the "uuiqne features of the context in which they ale applird_ In this spilit, this chapter bcgir.s with prcsernarion of a social- psychological perspective on the nature of Inrernurional conflict and [he normative and perceptual processes that contribute ro its escalation and perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The chapter rhen rurns to a b,ic( dlscnssion of negotiation and mediation, the maS[ common diplomatic preaches to conflict, which have been subjects of extensive research in po- lnica! psychology. This review provides a useful reference point for our of interactive conflict resolution itself. To illustrate the family of approaches subsumed under this rubric, we proceed co a more detailed description of rhe assurnpnons and procedures of tnterac\ive problem solv- ing, 3S applied in particular (0 the Israeli-Palestinian ,onBiet [Kelman, 1997a, ]1)98b). The chapter concludes with an identification of some of (he challenges confronting scholar-praerlriollcl) in field of conflict anal- "nd resolutioll. ... The Nature of International Conflict A social-psycllOlogical perspective call expand on die view or inrcrnatioual conflict provided by the realist or neorealisr ofinrcrnarionel rciarious 315

Upload: others

Post on 28-May-2020

3 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

,

/

\

10 Herbert C. Kelman arm Ronald 1. Fisner

Conflict Analysis and Resolution

This chapter presents a social-psychological approach ro the analysis and resolution of international and inrercornmunal conflicrs. Irs central focus is DO interactive conflict resolution (sec Fisher. J997), n family of models [or intervening in deep-roored, procracted conflicts between identity groups, which is anchored in psychological principle,.;,

1ntcmarional conflict resolution can be placed in the context of a larger, growing held of practice. applied at different levels and in diffcrentdomains, and anchored in differem disciplines, theoretical traditions, and fields of practice. Despite this diversity, certain common threads run tbrough most of the work in this ficld. Thus these approac!'cI to eonlllcr resolution \;CII­

crally call for a nonadversarial framework for addressing the conflict, an analytic point of dl'p::lHllre, a prohleJJl-~olviJ1g or icnmrion, direct pnrrici­potion of the conflicting panics in joint efforts to shape a solution, and facilitation by ;;l third party trained ill the proce~ of conflict resolution. Crcsslevel exchanges arc very valuable for developing general principles, bur the application of these principles requires sensitivity (0 the "uuiqne features of the context in which they ale applird_

In this spilit, this chapter bcgir.s with prcsernarion of a social­psychological perspective on the nature of Inrernurional conflict and [he normative and perceptual processes that contribute ro its escalation and perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The chapter rhen rurns to a b,ic( dlscnssion of negotiation and mediation, the maS[ common diplomatic ~p­preaches to conflict, which have been subjects of extensive research in po­lnica! psychology. This review provides a useful reference point for our disclJ.~sioJl of interactive conflict resolution itself. To illustrate the family of approaches subsumed under this rubric, we proceed co a more detailed description of rhe assurnpnons and procedures of tnterac\ive problem solv­ing, 3S applied in particular (0 the Israeli-Palestinian ,onBiet [Kelman, 1997a, ]1)98b). The chapter concludes with an identification of some of (he challenges confronting scholar-praerlriollcl) in d,,~ field of conflict anal­YSI~ "nd resolutioll.

... The Nature of International Conflict

A social-psycllOlogical perspective call expand on die view or inrcrnatioual conflict provided by the realist or neorealisr .\Ch(){]L~ ofinrcrnarionel rciarious

315

Page 2: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

, /

316 INTERNATIONAL RELATIONS

or other, more rradhionnl approaches focusing on structural or strategic [actors (Kelman, 1997h). Withom denying the importance of objectively anchored national interests, the primacy of the state in the international system, the role of power in international relations, and the effcct of srruc­[ural factors in determining [he course of an inrernationa] conflict, it en­riches the analysis in a variety of ways: by exploring the subjective facrors that scr consrrainrs on raticnaliry: by opening the black box of [he stare as unitary actor and analyzing processes within and between the societies that underlie state action; by broadening the range of influence processes (and, indeed, of definitions of power) that playa role in international politics: and hy conceiving imcrnational conflict as a dynamic proc~s, shaped by changing realities, interests, and relationships between rhc conflicting' par­ties.

Social-psychological analysis suggcsts four propositions about interna­

\ tional conflict. These proposirions are particularly relevant to existential conflicts herween identity groups-conflicts in which the collective identi­ries of the parties are engaged and in which the continued existence of the group is seen to be at stake. Thus rhc propositions apply most directly to ethnic or ideological conflicts bur also co more mundane inrcrstare conflicts, insofar as issues of national identity and existence cornc into play-as they

often do. The first proposition says that international conflict is a process driven

by collective needs and fetUS rather than entirely a product of rational cal­culation of objective national inreresrs on the pan -of political decision­makers. Human needs arc often articulated and fulfilled through imporrnnr collectivities, such ;L,~ the ethnic group, the national group. and the srnrc. Conflict arises when a group is faced with nonfulfillment or threat to rhe fulfillment of basic needs: nor only such obvious material needs as food, shelter. physical safcry and physical well-being bur also, and very centrally, such psychological needs as identity, security, rccognition, autonomy. self­esteem, and a sense of justicc (Burton, 1990). Moreover, needs for identity and security aud similarly powerful collective needs, and the fears and con­cerns about survival associated with them, contribute heavily co the esca­larion and perpetuation of conflict once it has srnrrcd. Even when the con­flicring parries have come co rhc conclusion that it is in their best interest. to pur an cnd co thc conflict, rhcy rcsisr going co rhc negotiating table or making rhc accommodations uccessaty for rhc negotiations co move for­ward, for fcar that chcy will be propelled inro concessions thar in the end will leave rhcir vcry existcncc compromised. The fears that drive existential conflicts lie at the heart of the relationship between the conflicting parries, going beyond rlre cycle of fears re.~ulring from the dynamics of the security dilemma (jervis. (976).

Collective fears and needs, though more pronounced in ethnic couflicrs. play :J parr in all international conflicts. They combine with objective fac­tors-for example, a state's resources, rhc ethnic composition of its popu­

Page 3: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

, /

\

Conflict Analysis ~l1d Resolution .317

larion, or irs access or kick of access to the sea-in determining how dif­(trent scgmcIHs of a society perceive stare interests and what ultimately becomes [he national inrcresr as defined by (he dominant elites. SimihH!y, all conflicrs-Lintcrstace no less than erhnic-c-rcpresenr a combination of rariona] and irrational factors, and in each type of conflict rhc mix may vary from case to case. Some ethnic conflicts may be preponderantly rational, just as some imersmrc clmflicrs may be preponderantly irrational. Furchcr­more, in all imemariooal conflicts, (he needs and fears of populations arc mobilized and often manipul:l[cd by the leadership, wirh varying degrees of demagoguery and cynicism. Even when manipulated, collective needs and fears represent authentic reactions within the population and become the focus of sucicra] action. They may be linked to individual needs and fears. For example, in highly viclcm ethnic conflicts. die fear of annihilation of one's group is often (and for good reason) tied to a fear of personal anni­hilation.

The conception of conflict as a process driven by collective needs and fears implies, fim and foremost, that conflict rcsclution-c--if it is to lead to a stable peace that horh sides consider JUSt and to a new relationship that enhances the welfare and development of the two societies-most address the fundamental needs and deepest fcars of the populations. From a nor­mative point of view, such a solution can be viewed as the operarionaliaation of justice within a problem-solving approach to conflict resolution (Kelman, 1996b). Another implication of a human-needs oricnrnrion is that (he psy­chological needs on which it focuses-security. identity, recognition-are not inhcrernly r:ero sum (Burton. 1990), although they are usually seen as such in deep-tooted con fliers. Thus it may well be possible to shape nn inregrnrivc sol orion rhar satisfies both sets of needs, which may then make it easier to settle issues like territory or resources through distributive bar­gaining. Finally, (he view of conflict as a process driven by collective uecds and fears suggests that conflict resolution must, at some stage, provide for certain proee.\ses (hat take place at the level of individuals and interactions between individuals, such as taking the other's perspective or realistic em­pathy (\'Vhite, 1984). creative problem solving, insight. and learning.

Focusing on the needs and feats of the populations in conflict readily brings ro mimI a second social-psychological proposition: that international conflicr is 1111 imenocietalprousJ, not merely an intergovernmental or inter­stacc phenomenon. The conflict, particularly in the case of protracted ethnic struggles, becomes an inescapable part of daily life for each society and its component elements. Thus analysis of conflict requires aueruion nor only to its strategic. military, and diplomatic dimensions but also to its economic, psychological, cultural, and social-structural dimensions. Interactions on these dimensions. both within and between the confiicnng societies, shape the political environment in which governments function and define the political constraints under which they operatl:.

An imcrsociernl view of conflict alerts us to the role of internal divisions

Page 4: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

,

/

, !

\

318 lNTIORNATIONAL RELATIONS

ioithin each society, which often play a major pan: in exacerbariog or even creating ccniiicrs bcnoeen societies. Such divisions impose consrrainrs on political leaders pursuing a policy of accommodation, in the form of ac­cusations by opposition elements that [hey ace jeopardizing national exis­tence and of anxieties and doubts within the general population that the opposition elerncrus both [ester and exploit. The internal divisions, how­ever, lIl<ly also provide porenual levers for change in the direction of conflict resolution, by challenging the monolithic image of the enemy that panics ill conflict rend to hold and enabling them to deal with each other in a morc diffcrennarcd way. Internal divisions point to the presence on (he other side of potential partners for negotiation and thus provide the op­portunity for forming pro negotiation coalitions across the conflict lines (Kelman, 1993). To contribute to conflict resolution, any such coalition must of necessity remain an "uneasy coalition," lest its members lose their credibility and pcllncal effectiveness within their respective communities.

Another implication of an inrersocieral view of conflict is [hat negoti­ations and third-party efforts should ideally be directed ncr merely to a political settlement: of the conflict, in the form of a brokered political <Igree­mcnr, bur to its raoiuuan, A polincal agreemem may be adequate for rcr­ruinating relatively specific, comainnblc inrersrnrc disputes, bur conflicts that engage: the collective identities and existential concerns of the adversaries require a process that is conducive to Structural and altitude change, to

reconciliation, and to rhe transformation of the relationship between the tWO societies. Finally, an inrcrsocieral analysis of conflict suggests a view of diplomacy as a complex mix of official and unofficial efforts with compte. mentaty contributions. The peaceful rcrmiuarion or managemelu of conlficr requires binding agreemellts [hat can only be achieved ar [he offici,,1 level, but many different sectors of {he two societies have [0 be involved in cre­ating a f.worable environment for negotiating and implementing such agree­ments.

Our third proposition says rhar inurnruional conjlia is a mulriJuaed proUH 0/mumal influellCt' and not only a couresr in the exercise of coercive power. Much of illlnnational politics entails a process of murual influence, in which each party seeks [0 protect and promore its own interests by sbaping the behavior of the ocher. Conflict occurs when these: interests clash: wheu attainment of one parry's imcresrs (and fulfillment of the needs thal underlie them) threatens, or is perceived 10 threaten, the iurercsrs (and needs) of the other. In pursuing the conllicr, therefore, the parties engagc in mutual influence. designed to advance their own positions and to block the adversary. Similarly, in conflict resolurico-c-by ncgonation or other means-the panics exercise inllucnce to induce the adversary 10 come to

die table, to make conees~ions, to .accept an agreemellt that meets their intcresL~ and needs, and to live up to that ~greellleJl[. Third parties also exercise influence in eonllicr situations by backing one or {he other parry,

Page 5: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

,

/ /

\

Conllicr A,,~Jpis md RcmJulIOL\ }19

by media.ting between tnt:m, or by m;ll\cuvcring to protect {heir OWI\ in­[crests,

Influence in international conflict lypically relies on J mixture of threats and indocemems, with cl)( babl\Ct o(u:[\ em d)c side of force and Illl: dlfe;\(

of force. Thus, the u.S.-Soviet relationship in the Cold War was predom­inantly framed in terms of an elabornre theory of deterrence-a form of influence designed to keep rhe Otnt:[ side from doing wl1;1l you l{O ncr wall! it to do (George & Smoke. 1974; [ervis. Lebow. & Stein, 1985; Schdling. 1%3; Stein. 1991). In other conflicr relationships, (he emphasis rna)' be on ecmpeltence-c-a form of influence designed to lIuke {he other side do what you wanr it to do. Such coercive srraregfes email serious com and risks, and their effecrs may be severely lirnired. For C'xamplc, they are likely to be reciprocated by ,he other side and thus [cad to esc;\latioll of thl: conflict, and they are unlikely to change behavior [0 which rhe other is committed. Thus the effective exercise of influence in internariona] confiicr requires a hroadcniog of the repertoire of il\Rllcnce slr:m.gies, al k,nr t\> rne ex-relll of combining "carrots and s(ick~"-of supplementing the negative incentives that typically dominate inremational conRict relationships with positive in­centives (SCl.: Baldwin, 1971; Kricsbcrg. 1982) ~\Ich as economic bencfas. international approval, or a general reducrion in the level uf tension. An example of an approach based on the systematic usc of positive incentives is Osgood's (l(62) gr;u:!u:Hed end reciprocated initi.~ives III tension reduc­tion (GRIT) mategy. President Anwar Sadar of Egypt, in his 1977 trip to

Jerusalem, undertook ~ unilateral inirianvc, wirh the cxpccrarlon (partly prewcgcciaeed) of Israeli reciprocaciou, but-unlike GRIT-c-hc stancd with a large, fundamenral concession in the anticipation that negotiations would fill in the intervening steps (Kelman, 1985).

Effective use of positive ineentivc..s IC<juin:." more dian offeriltg che other whatever rewards, promises, or confidence-building measures seem mosr readily available. It requires actions rhar add tess the fimdnmerua! needs and fears of the other party. Thus the key to an di-cctive influence strategy based on rhc e:>:ch~nge of positive inccnuvcs is raponsiucness to the other's con­cerns: actively exploring ways that each call help meet the other's needs and ;\lhy the other's fears ;Lod way. co hdp each other overcome the constraints within (heir respective societies against taking the actions that each W<1J1{S

the other to take. The edvautage of a strategy of responsiveness i.~ dut II allows parties to exert influence 00 each other through pnsitivc steps (001

threats) that are within their own capacity [0 rake. The l'roc~ss i~ greatly Faciiimrcd by communication between the parties in order 10 identify actions dun arc polili...:J.1ly feasible for each party yet likely to have an impacr 00

the ocher. A key eiemcnr in an influence stratcgy hased on responsiveness is /1/11­

Irl(/{ reassurance, which is particularly critical il\ any effmt 10 resolve an c;\istemial conflict. The negotiation literature suggests that parries are often

Page 6: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

/

320 INTERNATIONAL REL .... TIONS

driven [0 [he rable by a mutually hurting stalemate, which makes negotia­tions more nrrractive than continuing the conflict (Touval & Zartman, (985, II. 16; Zartman & Berman, 19t1l). BUI parties in existential conflicts arc afraid of negotiations, even when the status guo has become increasingly painful and they recognize that a negotiated agreement is in their interest. To advance the negotiating prorcs_~ under .mc\t circumnaoo:s, it is at least as irnporranr to reduce the parties' fears us to increase their pain.

Mutual reassurance Ow take the form of acknowledgments, symbolic gestures. or confidence-building measures. To be maximally effective, such MCpS need LO address [he other', centra! needs and fears as directly as pos­sible. Wnel1 Presiden\ S:dal of Egypt spoke ro the Israeli Kncsset during his dramatic visit ro Jerusalern in November 1977, he clearly acknowledged Egypt's pasl hostility reward Israel and thus validared Israelis' own experi­\ ences. 1n so doing, he greatly enhanced the credibility of the change in course that he was announcing. At the opening of this visit, Sadac's symbolic

\ gesture of engaging in a round of cordial handshakes with the Israeli officials who had (0111\: to gren him broke a longmtnding taboo. By sigunling rhc beginning of a new relationship, it had an electrifYing effect on the Israeli public. In deep-rooted conflicts, admowiedgemem of whar was heretofore denied-in the form of recognition of the other's humarury, narioehood, rights, griev~nces, and interpretation of history-is an important source of reassurance that the other may indeed be ready to negotiate an agreement that addresses your fundamental cnnccrns. By signaling acceptance of the other's lcgidmacy, each parly reassures the other rhar negotiations and cnn­cessions no longer consrinnc mortal thrcars to it.~ security and national existence. By confirming the other's narrative. each reassures the other that a compromise docs not lepresem an abandonment of itsidentity.

An influence strategy based on responsiveness [Q each other's needs and fears and the resulting search for ways of reassuring and benefiting each other has important advantages from a long-term point of view. It docs not merely elicit specific desired behaviors fiom [he other party bur also can contribute to a creative redefinition of the conflict, joint discovery of mu­tually satisfactory solutions, and transformation of the relationship herwccn the parries.

rile influence strategies employed in a couilict relationship rake on special significance in light of rhe founh proposition: international col/fiier is 1/11 nurraaioe pro,",,!! with 1/1/ (II·I/fl/fory Uifpup(f11l/ling rip/Illuir, not merely ~ seguent:e of :lnion :lud r~ac[ion by sl<l.ble aCIOIS. III intense confiicr relationships. the natural course of imerncrion berweeu the parries tends to

eeiuforce and deepc« thc conflict rather rhan reduce and resolve ir. The inrcracricn is governed by a set of norms and guided hy a .'OCt of im:lges that create all escalatory, self-perpetuating dynamic. This dynamic can be reversed rhrougb skillful diplomacy, imaginative leadership, third-party in­tervention, nud institutionalized mechanisms for managing and resolving conflict, Rue in [he absence of such delib~'ntc eff"orB, the spoOlalleolls ill­

Page 7: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

, ./

Confi!cr Analysis end Resolution 3Jll

remcnon hcrwccn tbe parries is mere likely rhan nor to increase disrrusr, hostility, and the SCIlS~ of grievance.

The needs and fears of parties engaged in intense conflict impose per­cepruaJ and cognitive consrrainrs on rheir processing of new informacion, with a resulting tendency rc underestimate [he occurrence and the possi­bility of change. The ;lbili t}' to lake the role of the other is severely impaired. Dehumnnizarion of \Iu~ ellcmy makes it CVCI1 more difficult to acknowledge and acCC.IS [h(" perspective of rhe other The inaccessibility of rhe odlcr's perspective contributes significantly to some of the psychological barriers (0

confiicr resolution described by RQI.~ and Ward (I995). The dynamics of conflicr imcmcuon rend to entrench die parries in their own pcrspecuves on lusrorv and justice Conflicting panics display particularly strong ten­dencies to Find evidence dm confirms their negative images of each otbce and to resisr evidence that would seem to disconfirm these images (sec

\ chapter 9 lor a fuller discossion of the image concept). Thus interaction not only f:lils to contribute 10 a revision of the enemy image but actually helps to reinforce and pcrpcruarc it. Interaction guided by minor images of a demonic enemy and a Virtuous self (see Bronfcnbrenner, 1%1; White, 1965) crentcs self-fulfilling prophecies by inducing [he parries to engage in the hostile actions chcy CXPCCl from nne another.

Self-flilfilling prophecies arc also generated by the conflicr norms that typically gowrn the iOler:letion bcrwceu parries engaged in an Intense con­fliCi. Expressions of hmtility and distrust toward che enemy arc noe just spontancous manifestations of the conflict but arc ncrrnarively prescribed behaviors. Po!iticalll'a(\ers' aHumption ,hat the public's evaluation of them depends on their adherence to these norms influences their lactical and srrucegk decisions, their approach [0 negotiations, their public pronounce­mcnrs, and, ultimately, the way they educate chcir own publics. For rhe publics, in rum. adherence to rhcse norms is often taken as an indicator of group loyalty. Tbcs the discourse in deep-rooted conflicts is marked by mutual delegirirnizaricn :Illd delwmaniaenou. Imcr:tuion governed by this set of norms-at the micro nnd macro icvels-c-conrribures to escalation ancl perpetuation of rhc conflict. Panics char systematically treat each ocher with hmlili£)' and distrust are likely to become increasingly hostile and unrrusr­worthy.

The dYI1;](\1iQ of conflict interaction create a high probabdisy that op­porcunincs for confiicr rcxolunon will b,· missed. Parries whose inreracrlon is shaped by the norms and images rocred in the herory of the conflict are systematically consuaincd in their capaCI£)' to respond to the occurrence and possibility of o:.:hangc. They find it (\ifhCl11t to communicate Ihe dlallgeS that have occurred on their own sid(" or to notice Ihe changes 01\ the other side, :lOa to explore the pcsslbliitlcs for change that would serve both sides' interests. Conflict resolution cffons. therefore, require promotion of a dif­fctent kind of imeracrion, capable of reversing the escalatory and self­perpetuating dynamics of cllrdHu: an interaction lh:ll is conducive to shar­

Page 8: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

, /

322 INTEKNATJONAL KElATIONS

ing perspectives, differentiating the enemy image, and developing a language of mutual reassurance and a new discourse bused on the norms of respon­siveness and reciprocity.

... Normative and Perceptual Processes promoting Conflict

Social-psychological analysis can he particularly helpful in explaining why and how, once a conRier has started, normative and perceptual procc:sscs arc set into motion that proman: its escalation and perpetuation and create or intensify harriers to conflict resolution. By the same token. social­psychological analysis, in helping to identify and understand these barriers, can also suggest wa.ys of ovcrcoming them.

Nonnative Processes

\ A variety of interaction processes at the mass and elite levels within con­Hicring societies that influence the evolving course of the conflict are gov­erned by a set of powerful social norms that encourage actions and artirudes conducive to the generation, escalation, and perpetuation of conflict and that inhibit the perception and occurrence of change in the direction of tension reduction and conflict resolution (Kelman, 1997b, pp. 212-222).

One such process is the formation of CO/&cI;lJ~ moods. With periodic shifis in collective mood, public opinion can act as both a resource and a conscrninr for political leaders in the foreign policy process. In principle, it can provide suppor[ for either aggressive or conciliatory policies, hut under the prevailing norms in an intense, protracted conflict, leaders are more likely to expect-and to mobilize-public support for the fonner than for the latter. Apart from transitory moods, certain pervasive slates of coo­sciousness underlie public opinion in a society engulfed in a deep-rooted conflict, reflecting the existcntial concerns and the central national narratives widely shared within thc population. In many cases-such as Serbia, Northern Ireland, ami the Middle East-historical traumas serve as the points of reference For current events. Though these memories may be ma­nipulated by demagogic leaders, they-and the associated sense of injustice, abandonment, and vulnerability-are pan of the people's consciousness and available for manipulation. The effeet of such collective moods is to hring to the fore powerful social norms that suPPOrt escalatory actions and inhibit moves toward compromise and accommodation. \'ifhen fundamental con­cerns about survival and identity are rapped, national leaders, with fIlII expecration of public support, are far more ready to risk war than to rake risks for peace-in line with the proposition derived from prospect theory that people arc more reluctant to take risks to achieve gains than to avoid losses (sec Levy, 1992). An)' change in the established view of the enemy

Page 9: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

,"/

\

and of the imperatives of national defense comes to be seen as a rhrem to

(he nation's very existence. Public suppon is an csseminl resource for political leaders engaged In

a conjuct relationship, both in ;lSsuring the public's readiness to accept rhe costs (h~r their policies mar email uud in enhancing the credibility of their rhrcars and promises [0 the other side. The prim;lry racaos of gaining public suppon is she lIlo/Ji/iuuioll ofgrOllp !oyl1/ti,'/. Aro1l5al of nationalist and P'"

nioric sentiments, panicuiarly in a context of 1\;11100-;1\ secueiry and survival, is u powerful tool in mobilizing public support. Ir may evoke automatic endorsement of [he policies lilt leadership defines as necessary and a will­ingness [0 make sacrifices that cannot be entirely understood in terms of rarirmal rnlcularlone of costs and bcndlu. The narion generates ~uch pow­erful identifications and loyalties because it brings together tWO central psy­chologicd dispositions: the needs for self-protection ann sdf-rranseem\c:nce (Kelman 1%9, I 997c).

Group loyalties call potentially bc mobilized in suppon of conciliatory policies. Pcliricullenders may promote painful compromises and concessions to the adversary on the grou/llis that the security, well-being, imcgrity, and survival of the nation require such acrions. Indeed, leaders widl impeccable nationalise crcdcrnials-c-such as Charles de Gaulle, Yjrzhak Rabin, or r. w. de Klerk-c-are ofien Il1\JSf effecrive in leading their populations toward peaceful resolution of conflicts, once [hey have decided rhar this approach bcr serves the national interest. In general, however; group loyalties are more readily available to mobilize suppon for aggressive. policies than for conciliatory ones. Proposals for aggressive actions can more easily rely on the vocabulary of nationalism. which characteristically marks off the ingrcup [rom the ourgroup to the detriment of the latter. An appeal ro defend rhe nation against an imminent auack, in particular, is more compelling than an appeal to seize a promising opportunity-as prospect theory might pte­dicr (Farnham, 1992; Lev}', 1992). Such nn appeal also elicits almost unan­imous response among members of (he populalion, whereas an appeal to take advauragc of an opportunity far pt'ace holds no attraction to that Segmem of the population that equnres peace with surrender.

Processes of group loyalty create barriers to change in a conllict rcla­ticnship. Group loyalty requires adherence [Q the group's ncrms-c-which, in an intense conflict, call fur a militant, unyielding, and suspicious altitude toward rho enemy. Militancy and intransigence rhus become the measures of lcyalry, Hence, particularly in siruarions of perceived national Lfi~is, the militants n.crcise disproportionate power and often a Veto over officiaJ ac­tions and policies. They impose severe constraints on the ability of leaders \0 explore peaceful options. Dissent from the dominant conflict norms be­comes defined as an act of disloyalty and is suppressed, thus Further un­dermining dlC exploradon nf peaceful alternatives.

Derisioll-mnking pr()cesJfJ ill a conflict siruarion [end to inhibit the search for alternntives and the exploration of new possibiiiues, parricula.Iy

Page 10: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

,

,

\

/"

324 INTERNATIONAL RELATIONS

when decision-makers arc operating in an atmosphere of crisis. These ten­dcncics are by no means inevitable, and chert: arc historical insrances-c-such as rhe Cuban missile crisis-of creative: decision-making in dangerous crisis situations (Allison, 1971; Lebow. 1981). Conflict norms do, however, im­pose serious burdens on the decision-making process.

A major source of reluctance [Q explore new options arc the domestic constraints under which decision-makers labor. In an intense conflict situ­ation, adherence to rhe conflict norms tends to be seen as rhe safeS[ course of action. Cautious decision-makers assume that they are less vulnerable domestically if they smy wirh the confiicr's Slaws quo, adhere to a discourse of hostility and distrust vis-a-vis the odier side. or threaten escalatory aceions than if they take steps wward accommodaricn and compromise. The search for nhcrnarives in response to changing realities is also inhibited by insti­tutionalized rigidities in the decision-making apparatus. Decision-makers and their bureaucracies operate within a framework of assumptions about available choices, effective strategies, and constituency expectations, shaped by the prevailing conflict norms, which may make them unaware of rhe occurrence and possibility of change. Punucrmorc, they often rely on es­tablished procedures and technologies, which arc more likely to be geared toward pursuing the conflict-by mllhary and other means-than toward resolving it.

The microprocesses of acrion and inreracrion in crisis decision-making further inhibit the exploration of ncw opuorw. At the level of individual decision-makers. the stress rhey experience in sirunrions of crisis-c-when consequential decisions have to be made under severe time pressures-limits the number of alternatives they consider and impels them ro settle quickly on the dominant response, which, in intense conflicts, is likely to be ag­gressive and escalatory (Holsri, {972: Lebow, 1987). At the level ofdecision­making groups, crisis decision-making ofreu leads [0 "groupthink" (Janis, 1982), a concurrence-seeking tendency (hat is designed to maintain the cohesiveness of the group. Decision-making under these circnmsrances is much more likely to produce policies and acrions that perpetuate and es­calate the conflict than innovative ideas for conhicr resolution.

The norms governing l1£'gorittt;ol1 end bargaining procasaes between par­ties involved in longstauding cnnllicr strongly encourage zero-sum thinking, which l:quatcs the enemy's loss with one\ own gain. Negcnarico-c-eveu disrrihurivc bargaining in its narrowest form-is possible only when both parties define [he situation, at least at some level, as a win-win, mixed­mcuve game in whieh (hey have both comperirive and cooperative gU;lls. While pursuing its own uueresrs. each ]'<lrtr musr actively seek out ways the adversary an also win and ~ppear to he winning. Bur [his is precisely the kind of erron that is discouraged by the conhicc nonns.

At the micro level, negorintors ill <In intense conflict tend W evaluate their performance by the forcefulness with which they presellt rheir own case and by their effectiveness in resisting compromise. Til listen to what

Page 11: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

;

//

\

the other side needs and to help rhc other side: achieve: irs goals would violate the conflict norms and might subject the negoriarors to criticism from their own constituencies and particularly from their hard-line domestic opposition. At rhc macro level, the panics-even when rhey rccoguo.e their common interest in ncgoriaring certain specific issues-c-rcnd [Q pursue an overall outcome that strengthens [heir own strategic position and weakens the adversary's. Such a Slr.llCg}' reduces the other's incentive fi)f concluding an agreement and ahiliry [Q mobilize public support for wharever agreement is negotiated. Zero-sum thinking at both levels undermines the negotiating process, causing delays, setbacks, and repeated failures.

Finally, conflict creates certain strucncra] and plJchologicnl cannnitmrnts, which then take on a life of their own (sec Pruitt & Gahagan, 1974: Rubin, Pruitt, & Kim, 1994). Mosr obviously, in a conflict of long standing, various individuals, groups, and organizctions-c-rnilitary, political, industrial, schol­arly-devdop a vested interest in maintaining the conflict as a source of profit, power, status, or raison d'erre. Others, though nor benefiting from the conflict as such, may have a strong inreresr in forestalling a compromise solution because it would nor address their particular grievances or fulfill their particular aspirations. Vested interests do not necessarily manifest themselves in deliberate attempts to undermine efforts at conflict resolution. They may take indirect and subtle forms, such as interpreting ambiguous realities and choosing between uncertain policy alreruativcs in W;lyS that favor continuation of the conflicr.

Vesrcd interests and similar srrucrural couunirmcnrs to the conflict arc bolstered by psychological commitments. People involved in a longstanding and deep-rooted conflict rend to develop a worldview that is built around the conflict and would be chrcarcncd by an end to the conflict. Resistance to change is likely ro be more pronounced the more elaborate the cognitive structure or ideology in which the view of the conflict is embedded, since changing this view would have wider ramifications. In an intense conflict, the image of the enemy is often a particularly important part of people's wcrldvicw, with implications for their national identity, view of their own society, and interpretation of history. This is one reason why images of rbe enemy, (Q which we turn IIe:Xt, arc highly resisrnnr to change: and conrribure to the escalatory and sclf-perpccuaciog dynamic of conflict.

Perceptual Processes

Perceptual and cognitive processes-the: ways we interpret and organize conflict-related information-playa major role in the cscalaricu and pe:r­pcruarion of conAict and create barriers In redefining and resolving the confiicr despite changing realities and interests. Two pcrceprual proeessc~

that characterize mutual images of panics in conflicr can acconnr for this e:ffCCl: the formation nf mirror imagcs and the resistance: of images to COIl­tradicrory information (Kelman. 1997b. pp. 222-231; see: alsn chapter 9).

Page 12: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

, /

,

326 INTERNATIUI"AL RELATIONS

Social psychologists writing abom U.S.-Sovlc:r relations (Bronfcnbrcn­lin, 1961; Whire, 1965) firs[ noted rhc phenomenon of mirror image /or­mauon as a characteristic of many conflier relationships. Both parties tend ro develop parallel images of self and other, except with the: value reversed The core conrenr of mirror images is captured by the good-bad dimension: each side sets Itself as virtuous and peaceful, arming only for defensive reasons and prepared to compromise. The enemy. by contrast, is seen :L~

evil and hostile, arming for aggressive reasons and responsive only to Lht language of force.

A typical corollary of the good-bad images in prorracrcd conflicts is the view that the other's aggressiveness is inherent in iL~ nature (its ideology, religion, national character, or pcliricnl system), whereas any signs of ag­gressivcness on one's own pan are entirely reactive and defensive. ln rhe

l language of attribution theory (see hereafter), the enemy's aggression is ex­

\ plained in dispositional terms and one's own aggre-\Sion in situational terms. John Foster Dullc.<;'s "inherent bad faith" model of the Soviet Union (Hoi­sri, 1962). with its counterpart in Soviet views of rhe west, illustrates this feature of mirror images. Another common corollary of the good-bad im­age-one that derives from the virtuous self-image-is the assumption on each side that the enemy knows very well that we are not threatening them. Our own basic decency and peacefulness, and the provocation to which we have been subjected, are so obvious to us that they must also be obvious to the other side (sec [he discussion of naive realism in Ross & Ward, \ 995). Apart from such generic features of mirror images, which arise from the dynamics ofioeergroup coaRict across Ihe board, mirror images ill any given case may reflect the dynamics of the specific conflict. Thus ethnic (()[lAiet~

may he characterized by mutual denial of the other's nauoun] identity, ac­companied by efforts to delegitimize the other's national movement and claim to nationhood (see Kelman 1978, 1987); mutual fear of national and personal annihilation; a mutual sense of victlmizarion by the other side; or :t mutual view of rhe other as a source of one's own humiliation and vul­ncrabiliry,

The mirror image concept implies rhnr certain symmetries ill the par­rk-s' reactions arise from the VCf)' nature of conAict imemcrion and that they play an important role in escalating the conflict. There is 110 ;nmmp­rion Ihat nil images of self and enemy arc mirror images; that images Oil

the two sides arc equally inaccurate: or that there is empirical symmetry in the two sides' historical experiences and current situation or mnra] equiva­lence in their posiriuns. The dynamics uf the conAict relationship, however, produce a degree of parallelism in some of the images developed by borh participants in that relationship, arising out of the motivational and CO~­nlrivc contexts in which they operatc. Motivationally, each side is concerned with "looking good" when blame for the conilicr events is being appor­tioned: pnliticalleaders, therefore, fed a strong need to persuade themselves, their own people, rhe rest of the world, and future historians thai [he blame

Page 13: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

,

//

\

Conflict Analysis ~"J Rerolurion 3Z7

rests with the enemy. Cognitively, tach side views rhe conljiee from irs own perspective and-c-painfully aware of its OWIl needs, fears, historical traumas, gricv:lJlccS, suspicions, and political constraints-is convinced rlun j\ is act­ing defensively and with the best intentions and that this is so sell-evident rhar it must be equally clear [Q the: enemy.

Mirror images produce a spiraling dTtCt (exemplified by the classical paucrn of an arms race) 1)(,~<':;H1sc each side interprets any hostile anion by the orbcr as an indication of aggressive inrenr against which it must defend itself, ytt its own rcacrions-c-whosc defensive nature, it assumes, should be obvious to the enemy-arc taken by the ocher as signs of aggressive intent, The eflecr of mirror images is accentuated insofar as the enemy's ideology or national character is perceived to be inherently aggressive and expan­sionist. In addition to their escalatory cflccr. mirror images tend to rnnkc conflicts more intractable because: the sharp corurasr between the: innocent self and the aggressive other makes it difficult to break out of a zero-sum conception of the conflict. However, the COllCe:pt of mirror images may be a useful tool in conflict resolution. In problem-solving workshops, for ex­ample, rhc parries' discovery that their own actions arc perceived differently by the other side than by themselves may open them up to the possibilicy that the reverse may be true. Thus they lIlay gain access to each other's perspective, insight into the escalatory eflecrs of such rwo-dirccticnal rlif­fcrcnces in perception, and awareness of the need for mutual reassurance in order to set a decscalarory process in motion.

The second feature of conflict images, their high degree of resistance to

contmdiaory illfim/(uion, inhibilS rhe perception of change and [he cxpec­ration of furure change. A glear deal of social-psychological theorizing and research has addressed the general phenomenon of the persistence of atti­tudes and beliefs in the face: of new information that, from an outside point of view, challenges their validicy but is somehow ncurralixed or ignored. Research has focused on several rypes of mechanisms [hat account for re­sistance ro contradictory information: selectivity, eonsisrcucy, attribution, and the self-fulfilling prophecy. The eonecprs nF selective expo.mre, selective perception. and selcerive recall all point to the fact that our attitudes help determine the kind of information that is available to us. We are more likely to seek OUt and be exposed to infonuaunn that confirms our existing ani­tildes and to perceive and remember new information ill ways [hat fit into our preexisting cognilive Framework. The various models of cognitive con­sistcucy-c-such as Heider's (L 958) theory of cognitive balance and Festin­gcr's (1957) theory of cognitive disson:lf)ce--sugge.~t that, in the interest of maintaining consistency, people tend to screen OUt information thal is in­congruent wab their existing beliefs and attitudes. Though inconsisrenr information may also instigate attitude change, it is more likely to be resisted whcn the existing anitudes are strongly held and have wide ramifications­as is the case with enemy imago. Attribution mechanisms (jones & Nisbett, J97 I) promotc confirmation of the original enemy image, because hostile

Page 14: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

,

/

328 INTERNATIONAL RELATIONS

actions by the enemy tend to be anribuied dispo~ilio[J;I!ly, Ihus providing [urrhcr evidence of the enemy's inherently aggressive, implacable character, while conciliatory actions are explained away as reactions to situational fOIC('.I. thus requiring no revision of the Oligi1l31 image (for research ill support of this proposition, see Herndsrveir, 1981; Rosenberg & WolfsfCld, 1977; ROll hana, 1997). Finally, interactions between conflicting parties tend to create self-fulfilling prophecies uy GllJ.~ine our :!dver5Jrj~.!: [0 bch:H'C in line with our cxpecrarions-c-m take ou rhc roles in which we have cast them (Weinstein & Deurschbergcr, 1963)-thus confirming our original nui­tudes

The mechanisms that account for resistance lU disconfirming Infor­mation are particularly powerful in a coullict relationship, for several rea­sons. First, im::ages of the enemy and conhicr-relatcd self-images arc centra] aspects of the national consensus, and resistance [Q disconfirming infor­mation is therefore reinforced by strong normative pressures. Second, in a

\ conflict relationship, the opportunities and capacity for taking the per~pec­tive of the orhcr side arc limited, which reduces the impact of porcmially new inlorrnaticn about the varieties, changes, and signs of flexibility ill the other side's views. Third, [he resistance of enemy images (0 discouhrmarion is magnified by strong beliefs about the unchangcability of rhe enemy, re­inforced hy the view thar it is dangerous or even treasonous (0 propme rhar the enemy has changed or will change.

Despite all rhe reasons why conflict images are particularly resistant llJ

conrrndictory informacion, they arc nor immutable. Social-psychological ev­idence suggests that rhey can change, and historical evidence shows rhur they elf> change. The challenge for scholars and practitioners ofiuteruar icnal conflict resolution is to devise (he means to overcome (heir resistance co chang". Interactive conflict resolution is specifically designed 10 address these kinds of resistances, along with the other social-psychological processes that <,.lllHlibute [() the escalation and perpetuation ofconflict. Before turning to interactive conflict resolution, however, we prcsem a brief review of ne­gotiation and mediation-the more traditional approaches to dealing with inrernarional coullictc--and scmc of the: social-psychological literature that nddrcsses them.

lilt.. Negotiation

The l1lUH common approach to addn:~~ing international conflict within (he domain of diplomacy is that of negotiation, an interactive process thar ap­pelrs 10 have a semblance of uJli"cr~alit)' at a generic level, even dlOugh cultural diffcrcuces in approaches and «yles are :1 current focus of study (e.g.. Cohen. 1997). Negotiation is typically defined as a discussion among parries aimed at resolving incompatible goals (Pruitt & Carnevale, 1993), 'lldlOugh:l broader dcfiuitlon sees negotiarion .1S a process by which parries

Page 15: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

, I

Conllicr An~lysjs ~nd Resolution 329

develop agreements to guide and regulate their future behavior (Sawyer & Cucrakow, 1965). The broader Jdlniliotl alerts us 10 [he fact rhur all man­ner of issues ;H die international level arc approached through negotiation. from trade disputes to financial arrangemcm.~ to cuvironrnental problems, while the more focused definition places negotiation nr the center of conflict resolution over terri LOry, gOVCrJI<tJll.:C, and idennry with other methnds ;llId

inrerveurions playing J supplementary and supportive role. It is dierclore essential to understand rhe processes. outcomes, and context ofinccrncnonal negoriarion. so thar a range of efforts can be directed reward achieving mutually acceptable scujernems Ill;).! contribute to susraiuabic and l'Hgdy cooperative relationships.

There are [WO important disciccnous in coosidNing expression> of in­ternational negotiation: hilarerai versus rnulriiateral and ccmpctirive versus integrative. The furmer distinction has gained importance since the end of the Cold War, with the shift away from" bipolar power snuggle to a Frcld

\ uf multiple actors nrrcmpriug to forge u new world order. A concise treat­menr of multilateral negotiation by Touval (1989) covered the phases, im­pediments. facrlitaring facr(}[~, and rhe challenge of building consensus, 'Ill in comparison to bilateral ncgorimion. Effofls to understand the complexity of multiparty, mulriissue negotiations seeking unanimity of agrecmellt must go beyond rhe common concepts applied ro bilateral ncgutialivl\s (bnga(t\­ing, informarion processing] to include additional concepts (coalition for­mation, role diflcrcnriarion) in the context of a system perspective. Treat­ments of muleilarcra! ncgoriarion, ir is hoped. will enable us to understand more deeply this increil~ingl}' common way of dealing with inrernarional issues (e.g., Hampson, 1995).

I

The sccoml distinction ha~ bent ccuual rc rhe ncgoriaeton literature for some time, stemming front the differences between domination, compro­mise, and integration. identified by Mary Parker Follen (1924), with the latter approach secking tu find expression of all parties' interests without s:\erincing ',lny essential ones. The distinction was crystallized in die orga­nizntinnnl literature through Walton and McKersic's (J 965) differellli:ltion of disn-ihut.ivc Yer.S\I~ ilHcgl,lLive bnrgaining, the former involving competing interests over rcsnurccs in short supply and the laucr eng:lging cooperative moves to increase the resource domain so that all primary interests can be satisfied. This duality has been rcprescnten In numerous trcaunctus of ne­i gotiation to the point where we can speak of competing theories of nego­riarion [Murray, 1986), one concern co witn hanl bsrgaioiog and resistance 10 concession making in order to maximize one's gains, the other geared to joint analysis ami prohicm sotviog yielding «iutually high outcomes. This distinction is applied to the imernarinna] level by Hoprnann (l ~95), who contends cmpuicalty thai b;trgaining is more frequent in irucrnariona] ne­gotiations, even though problem solving produces greater flexibility and superior agreements. Pan of the reason for this discrepancy between pnelice and effectiveness is that the more traditional, competitive bargaining style

Page 16: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

,

/

330 INTERNATIONAl. RELATION.~

find~ SUPPOTt in the J\.l1llill;\f"lt p;\f:J.digm of realism in imcf!\;\tio[\:u relations, whereas problem solving is more corupnuhle with rhe assumptions and ori­cnl;ltions of liberalism, and as such is only more recently gaining consid­erarion by international diplomars and other negotiators. The primary thrust of theory and rcscnrch on negotiation ill social and political psy­chology has been to arpport tbe ~hjfl from a dimibmivc, zero-sum men­taliry to an integrative. non-rem-sum pcrspcctivc, the lance being expressed through a firm and cooperative oricnraricn.

\

A number of approaches «r the study of international neSQ[juion have been taken. Fisher (1990) idcnrillesgeneral descriprions basedon diplomatic experience, studies that draw on marhcmnricnl models and g~me rheory, and comparative C35e analyses of a systcmaric nature. Carnevale and Pruin (1992) ncre books of advice to negcriarors rhnr are largely prescriptive (e.g., Fisher & Dry, 1980, mathematical treatments of carion.l negoriarion that

, are mainly descriptive (e.g.. Raiffa, 1982), and behavioral studies in both the field and laboratory [hat are descriptive yet yield prescriptions (hat can be useful to negotiators (e.g., Pruitt. 1981). Druckman (1997) provides tile broadest sweep of peespecrivcs dIal have been taken to understanding ne­gotiation, seeing it as puv.le solving dirc-cted toward ffi.king optimal choices, as ~ hargaining game in which concessions are exchanged, as or­ganizational management requiring consensus building both within and be­tween panics, or as diplomatic politic_~ in which negotiation is OIlC strand of multifaceted international relations. Social and political psychologists have made contrjburions to both r1u dcscnprive and prescriptive trc;\(mcnts of negotiation.

I

An early and infiucnnal model by Sawyer and "Guerzkow (I %5) cast the negotiation process as <1 temporal Row aflecred by antecedent, concur­rem, and consequent conditions. Druckman (1973, 1983) has utilized their model W crganue research In the field and <15 a base for elaborating d\l~

proc~ss of negotiation into a series of stages, turning points, and crises in which morncnrum can be built reward the final agreement. He also makes a strong case for expanding ncgoriauon research (0 consider a rangc of conrexrual factors, a direction that latCI studies ar the imernauonal level are taking seriously {e.g., Hoprnann. l')9cJ). Carncv:tle and Pmiu (1992) review behavioral studies of negotiation in reems of a mcuvarional orienrarion, which predicts outcomes based on strategic choices rooted in negotiator motives. and a cognitive oriematioll. whidl predicts ourccrncs based o n negotiator perceptions and information praces.• ing. The descriptive srnnce of (he behavioral onemnion s11ifrs in the prescriptive direction in tbe theory and research on problem solving by Prujn (l9H6), which identifies methods for nnaining integralive agreements. in addition ill the time-honored tech­oiquc of !ngrolling to transform dimihurive sitmrions iuvol ... ing multiple issues into inrcgradve Outcomes, Pmin identifies expanding the pie, non­spt:l:iflc eornpcnsauon, cosr cutting, alld bridgillg, wherein a new option isI

I created to s:;llis!y underlying interests. Such curccmes are achievable if it is

!

Page 17: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

,

//

\

\

possihle to inj(C[ suFficiclll flexibility imo the m:goriari<Jn procc~~ along with rhe essenrinl amount of firmness (Druckman & Mitchell, 1995).

A common question in negotiation research is how dements of the

negotiating situation (e.g., prcllcgot;.uion experience, constirucnr pressure] affect process and outcomes. Druckman (2001) adds the more challenging question of how the processes and outcomes nffect the lcng-rerm, postsc:r­rlemeoc relations among the pJrtics, wl\ich h;\s ,ignificance for conRiet res­olution. At the same time, we must also ask how orher methods in the domain of conflicr resolution call he directed toward achieving and imple­mcming imegrmivc agreements that work to improve rclarious among: for­mer adversaries and thus help to build a lasting peace.,

lrrrrr.. Mediation

When ncgonarion is nonexistent or unsuccessful in situations of destructive and protracted conflict, a Common response is for a neutral third party to

enter the arena. either by invitation from the parties or on its own initiative. There are a wide variety of activities that inrermediaries can undertake, or more generally, a number of differenr roles they call enact in conflicts. Kricsberg (19%, 1998) identifies activiues that range from providing a ,pace for communication to saving face, helping invent new options, and adding resources and generating prc.5surcs ro reuch an agreement. Fisher and Keashly (1990) provide a taxonomy of third-parry intervention, which de­scribes roles approximately in line with traditional terminology found in the literature at horh the domestic and international levels. Six roles are idemificd in terms of their primary functions and along a continuum of

the control that the third p;ury pos~esscs over both the process and the outcome of the interaction between or among the parries. Conciliation and consultation are at the low power end of the continuum and are essentially defined as providing an informal communication link and mcilitating cre­ative problem solving respectively At the high end of the concrol conrin­uum, pracrkuping is seen as maintaining a cease nrc supplemented by bu­rnanimrian and political acrivirics, while arbitration provides a binding third-parry serrlcmcnr on the subsranrivc issues in dispute.

AI the intermediate level of control is rhc rhird-pany role of mediation, which. like conciliation and consultation, is a noncocrcivc and nonbinding approach [Q managing conflict with the consent of rhe panics. Specifically, mediation is defined as the intervention of an impartial third parry designed (Q create a mutually acceptable Ilegotiated settlement on the substantive issues of (he conflict [11 addition. Fidler and Keash[y (1990) follow [he lead of other theorists in the field by distinguishing pure mediation from pouer mediation. The former works toward agreemem through rhe usc of

reasoning. persuasion, the control of information, and the su~estion of alrcrn~tjvcs. The lauer goe~ beyond these facilitative functions to include

Page 18: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

, /

332 INTEIINATION,\L RELATIONS

i

\

the U5t: of leverage in (he form of rewards and punishments and etten involves [he third parry as a powerful guarantor of the sctrlerncnt. This disrincnon can he connected to the primary functions of mediators iden­rificd by Touval and Zartman (1985) in that pure mediation involves the functions of cammimiouion and .!ol7l1u((ltion, whereas power mediation goes beyond [hat to include manipulation. While this function can be seen as comp.uible with the world of power politics in which it operates. it does raise both ethical concerns over [he use of coercion by powerful third pnnies and strategic concerns about the garnering of agreements that involve SCt­

rlemenr !lUI do not result in la.~tillg resolution. til the past 20 years, the method of mediation has witnessed a signifi­

cant growth in theory, research, and practice at both [he domestic and international levels (e.g., Krcssel, Pruitt, & Associates, 1989). Moore (1996) provides a comprehensive coverage of rhe hisrory and expression of medi­arion, which is found in almost all cultures ill the world, practiced by a variety of individuals and instinuions in both informal and formal roles. In Western societies, the last three decades have seen a profusion of mediator roles to address various rype.~ of conflict, often as an nlrcmacive ro formal, legal processes of litigation or adjudication. AI the international level, me­diarion has a history ns long as that of diplomacy itself and has also received increased scholarly arrention in rccenr limes (e.g.. Bercovirch & Rubin, 1992). Bercovirch (1997) provides a concise overview of this domain of study, indicating [he unique characteristics of mediation as the continuation of negotiations by other means. He garners empirical evidence that arrests to the frequent use of mediation in inremarionnl relations by a variety of individuals, states, and iusnnmons. and he identifies a number of variables that have been related to mediation effectiveness (e,g., Bcrccvitch & Hous­toll, 1996). It is clear that inrcrrncdiary activities need to be a central com­ponent of conflict managemnu and resolution, as the world searches for alrernarivc mechanisms to deterrence, compcllcnce, and warfare.

\'V'ith the growth of mediation, anention is being given to the lllany difliculr issues that arise rbroogh third-parry interventions in the conflicts of ochers. Fisher (2001) idemihcs a number of these in relation to the identity, motives, qualities, and competencies of [he intervenor and (he timing, ethics, and effectiveness of iurcrvenrion. The issue of cultural ge­»eralivabiliry is especially salient when the third pany enters from a dilfcrenr and dominant culture in relation to rhose of rhc parties in conAicr. Pnwer asymmeTry between the parries and the enrry of a powerful rhird parI}' both raise questions about the limits of applicability of inrervcmion methods. The rraditioual view of an impartial third parry is being challenged by [he prnclnimcd cflecuvencss of biased mediators wh~lse interests ma}' help de­liver a sculcmenr. The question of timing asks whether conflicts must reach u level of destruction ;lnt! a point of impasse before parties arc willing to abandon [heir unilateral, coercive measures to seck a mediated compromise. TIle effectiveness of mediation is an issue of considerable import, with snnl­

Page 19: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

, /

Conflict AmJr"il and Rt'solurion JJJ

ies of domestic Interventions generally showing higher success rates than those at the intcmarional Ievcl, especially in protracted crhnopoluic»l con­fliclS oyer identity and governance. Finally, the ethics of intervention is a continuing concern, which can be addressed through the development of mediation :IS a form of professional practice, regardless of the forum in which It is practiced. All of these iSSlK'S must be addressed for mediation and other [)Ipes of third-party intcrvcurion (Q achieve [heir potential for reducing human destructiveness and f:lcilit:lring social transformation to­

ward gn::lfcr harmony, cqoiry, and juslice.

.. Interactive Conflict Resolution

\ FruSU;ItIOns in achieving negoriared settlements and Failures ar mcdiacion, particularly in irnr acrable echoopolirical conflicts, were pan of the irnperus for exploring ahernarive methods of conflict resolution that did not arise from a base in realist assumptions about inrernarionnl relations. John Burton is credited 110[ only with challenging the dominant paradigm of realism hut aim wid! rhc creation of a problem-solving approach to inrcmarional con­flict analysis and resolution, which he initially termed controlled eommnni­anion (Burton. L%9). Following Burton's method, high-level reprcscnta­rives of parties in destructive conliicr are brought together in unofficial discussions with a third-parry panel of social scientists, who work to build an open ami supportive cliruarc in which die amagcnisrs can analyze their siruarurn, examine their perceptions and evaluations, and create mutually acccprahle options for conflict rcsoluriou. Herbert Kelman was a panel member in one of Burton's early workshops on the Cyprus con flier and W~lH on ro develop his own method of interaaiue prob/mJ wl/ling, which is described hereafter with reference to rhc [sracli-Palcsriuian conflict. Lennard Dooh experimented with the application ofhuman-rdarions rraining merh­ods to destructive conflicts in the Horn of Africa and Northern Ireland (Dooh, 1970; Doob & Polrz, 1973). A vnricry of interventions and studies applying these types of methods to intergroup nnd intcmnrlonal conflict arc reviewed by fisher (1972, 1983), who also developed a gcncric model of third parI]' consultation to rcp rcscnr the essential compon~lHS of the ap­proach,

fisher (1997) has recently captured the work of Burron, Kelman, ami O[I\((s under the rubric of im,'raailJl' [On/lier resolution, which is defined as "small-group, problem-solving discussions between unofficial represenrarivcs of idcnri t}' grotlJl.'i nr states etlgaged in destructive conflict that arc Facilirarcd by all imp;Hli;ll third parry of social sciennsr-pracritioncrs" (p. 8). Givcn rhe prolifer.nion ofinrerxctivc methods over rhc past decade, Fisher (1997) also provides a broader view of Interactive conflict resolution as involving Facil­itated, Incc-ro-facc acuvuics in com municario n, training, education, or con­sultation rhar pmmllte colbhorativc cunfiicr analysis and problem solving

Page 20: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

/ 334 INTERNATIONAL RELATIONS

among amagonisis. In cirher (:;1,51:, the method is based III social­psychological assumptions about intergroup and intcrnnrionai conflict that see the Impommce of subjective [actors {aulu_dcs, perceptions, emotions) alongside objective clements und that propose that mcaningfnl inreracrion ;\mollg conAiclillg panics is as necessary to dccscalare t!:x: CfmAict as it was co escalate it. However, the mcdrcd abu Lakes a system perspective, knowing

that any changes in individuals that take place in problem-solving work­shops or other interactive for<l muxr be transferred successfully co the level of political discourse and policy-making for ally positive effects ro occur. Interactive conflict resolution is therefore a form of [lilofficial or [mrk two diplomacy (Moruvil!e. 1987), which inItially gained its currency through complementary coruribucions 1I1<1.t jt (an rnakc ro official peacemaking cf

\ \ fans. At the same time, inrerncrive tlle[hod.~ arc becoming increasingly im­

ponam in poseconflice peace-building, to help implement secclcrncnrs and rebuild war-torn relationships so that recscalnnog cycles of violence are pre­ventc.d.

There arc a variety of different forms of interactive conflict resolution, ill ac\c\irion m the classic problem solviug workshop model nniculared by Bnrron (1987), Mitchell (l9fil), Kelman (I9BG), Azar (1990), f-isher (l986), and others. Vamik Volkxn and his colleagues have developed :I

psychodynamic approach ro borh oodcrsrandiog and ameliorating crhno­political conflict among comesling communal groups. Volkan (1990 (011­

rends th:lt deeper psychological pJU(;(;S,leS, such as projectiou and victim]­znrion, need (0 be addressed along with poli[ical and economic issues, and he has developed a workshop methodology for bringing [Oeerlwf inRuemial members of confiicring gronp' to establi.~h workable relationships and de­velop mumally acceptable opuous. The approach- ha.~ been successfully ap­plied the Arub-Istacli ronthct (julius, 1~9l) aud ro conliicu in the pOSt­[Q

Sevier Baltic republics bcrwccu majority populations and Russian minorities (Volknn & Harris 1993). Alrhougb che psychodynamic ur..dcrpinniugs of Volkan's method urc different (rum those or rhc social-psychological model, the design or the workshops and role of [he third-parry faciluarors arc rc­molLlbly similar.

Another form of interactive conflict resolution has been developed by Harold Sannders, :I former US. diplornar and policy-makc r, whu has worked as a member of the third-parry ream in workshops organized by borh Volkan and Kelman. For mall)' years, Saunders was involved in the Darrruourh Conference, bringing togerher Soviet (now Russian) and Amcr­ican inhucntials to engage in citiaen-to-citracu dlallJg\\e. He served as lhe Aml:'riClO cochair of rhe rcgiunal CUJlllill task force rbar examined super· power interaction in Cold War hot SpOlS as a means of \)[lder5t;ltl~lill~ the relationship between [he two countries, Based on rhis expr-rie-ncc. Chufrin and Saunders (1993) articulated a public pt'ace I)HKes.~ 'In\'o!villg five stages of unofficial dialogue between conflicting ~rOU[lS. Following the end of [he C(JIt! War, Saunders and Randa Slun worked with American and Russian

Page 21: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

, /

r

Cannier An~lpi., and Resoluricn 3.15

CO\tCilgUC5 coapply rhe dialogue model with considerable SUt:l:C:SS to the civil war in rhc former Soviet republic of Tajikistan (Saunders, 1995). Based on this and other experiences, including a dialogue on race rclarions in the United States, Saunders (1998) has arrjculared :I broadly applicable model of facilitating sustained dialogue bcrwccu members of conflicting groups.

A number of scholar-pracririoncrs have contriburcd [0 the development of methods for imercommuual dialogue: that have \l~cr\d application (0

cthncpolitical conflicts at the international level. Tbc.\c forms of inreracrive conflict resolution tend [Q involve ordinary members of conflicting groups or [heir diasporas, who are ccnccr ncd if not influential in policy-making but who represent [he modal senrimcms or the confiiccing panics. Such dialogue also rends [0 FOClLS more on developing murunl understanding through conflict analysis rather than crcariog ajtcmntivc solutions to the conflict. However, it may result in policy options that call For useful de­e.~[alatory moves by involved or interested parties. Louis Krie.~berg and his

\ colleagues initialed the Syracuse: Are:a Middle East Dialogue in the: early 1980s to bring Jewish-Americ;m and Arab-American citizens together to increase mutual undemanding and develop policy ideas for the u.s. gtW­erumcnr to improve Israeli-Palestinian relations. Richard Schwanz (J989) provides a useful description or the: ve:ry challenging dialogue: process alld a valuable exposition or the rationale: and procedures of the: methodology that was developed. Another example: of the: erection or structured dialogue proeesses comes From the work or Richard Chasin and his colleagues, For­merly at the Center For Psychology and Social Change <lnd now at the Public Conversations Project. Based in ramily sysre:ms therapy, rhis approach fellows a systcmatie process for increasing understanding hcrween hostile parries and creating cooperation across lines of rhe conflict. The approach was initially applied to Sovici and American relations during the Cold \'\lar and has been further developed through application to a venery or other conflicts, including the abortion issue ill rhe United States (Chasin & Her­zig, 1993; Chasin cr al., 1996). These and other examples of dialogue pro]­ccrs provide a rieh source [or developing a gcneric methodology of dialogue that is highly contributive [0 the field of imeracnve conflict resolution (Fisher, 1997).

lrrrrrr... Problem-Solving Workshops

To illustrate the microprocess of interacuvc conflict resolution. we .~halJ

describe the problem-solving workshops earned out by Herbert Kelman and 1,'ls colleagues with politically inllueruialIsrnclis and Palesuuians, starring in the early [970s (Kelman, 1992, \ 998b; Kelman & Cohen, 1976; Rouhana & Kelman, (994). Kelman's approach. mteracriue prof,/<'IJI wIlling (Kelman,

1986, 1998a), derives From rhc work of [ohn Burton (1969, 1979. 1984, 1987; see also Kelman, 1972). It is an academically based, unoflicur], third­

Page 22: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

,

/

336 INTERNATIONAL KELATIONS

p:my approach (0 conflict resolution, anchored in social-psycbotogicd prin­ciples. It brings together politically involved and often politically inlluenrinl members of conflicting parties for direct cornmnnicarion, facilitated by a panel of social scientists with expertise ill group process, intcmmional con­flict, and the particular region in which the conflict rakes place.

The ultimate goal of interactive problem solving is to promote chang!' in indivlduals-e-througb face-to-face interaction in small groups (Kelman. 1997a)-a5 a vehicle for change in national policies and in the larger COI1­

flier sj'srem. The core of the work is a particular microprocess, best exem­plified by problem-solving workshops, thai is inren dcd 10 coruriburc 10 rh,· macroproccss of conflict resolution.

Relationship to Negotiations ,

\ Problem-solving workshops and related activities are nor negotlatlng ses­sions. Negotiations can be carried OUt only by officials authorized (0 COJl­

clude binding agreements, and workshops-by definition-are completely nonbinding. Their nonbinding character, in L"..r , reprc~clllS their special strength and is the source of their unique conrriburion to the larger proceSS. They provide an oppcrruniry for sharing perspectives. exploring options, and joint thinking. Such exploratory inreraccion is essential to ncgodadon at all of its stages, but it is usually difficult to arrange in an official comexr, especially around the negotiating table.

Ellen though workshop. must be dearly dislinguisncp From officinl ne­goti:ltions, they can be viewed as an integral pan of the larger negnti:ltillg­proceS" relevant at all stages of that process. At the prcnegcriariou stagc, rbey can help rhe parries move toward the negoriruing-rabic by wnrribliling (0 the crcariou of a political environment that is conducive to negotiation. At the ncgoriarion stage itself chey can perform useful paruncgoeiarion [nne­rions: they can contribute to overcoming obsracles to the negotiations. to creating momentum and reviving the sense of possibility, and 1O identifying options and rcframing issues so that they can be negotiated more effccrivelv once they get to the table. Finally, at the posmegoriarion st:lge, workshops cnn contribute to resolving problems in the implemcmarion of negotiated agreements, 3.S well as to the process of peaccbuilding and rcconcilinrion in the aftermath of an agreemerH and to the rransformnrion of rhc relationship between the former enemies.

Israeli-Palestinian Experiences

Kelman's and his colleagues' lsraeli-Palesrinluu work has sought to cnnrrib­ute ro all three of these stages of the negotiating process over the course of the years. All of [he workshops in the 1970s and 1980s rook place. of course, ill the prencgoriatioll stagl' and Were designed to explore the pos­sibilities for movement toward [he negotiating table. A variery of workshops

Page 23: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

,

../

Conflict Analysis and Rc~{)lminn 337

were carried am during that period-in different conrcxrs and with diflcrcnr types of participants. All of rhc participants, however, were members (or were soon [Q be members) of the political elite. They included political aCWfS, such as parliamentarians and leaders or activists of political parties

or political movements: political inlluenrials, such as journalists, editors, directors of think ranks, politically involved academicians, and former dip­lomats or military officers; and preinflucnrinls. such as advanced graduate students who seemed headed for politically irnporranr careers (some of whom did indeed become political influenrials as their careers progressed}, Mmeovcr, all of the workshops during this period were "one-rime" events: me particular group of Israelis and Palestinians who [Oak part in J given workshop convened only for this one occasion-usually over an extended weekend. Some of the individuals participated in more than one such work­

\ , shop, and the one-time workshops held over the years had a cumulative

effect within the [\VO SOGielies and helped to inject new ideas into the two political cultures. But until 1990 no al[empt was made LO reconvene the same group of participants for another occasion.

ln 1990, for the first time in this program, Kelman and Nadim Rou­hana organized a continuing workshop: a group of highly iulluenrial lsraelis and Palesrininns-c-six on each side-who agreed to parricipare in a series of three meetings over the course of a year, and in [he end continued to

meet (with some 'changes in personnel) until August 1993 (Rouhana & Kelman, 1994). As it happened, with the onset of official negotiations in 1991, fim in Madrid and then in Wa~hingwn, this continuing workshop also provided the organizers' firsl experience with interactive problem solv­ing as a paranegoeiaeion process. The political relevance of this work was enhanced by the appoinrrncnr, in 1991, of four of the six initial Palestinian participants in the group to key positions in the official negorialing teams and, in 1992, of several Israeli participants ro ambassadorial and cahiuet positions in the new Rabin government. Some participants left rhe group at this point because they saw a conflict of interests between [heir roles in the official and unofficial process (Kelman, 1998b, pp. 19-20).

These workshops from the 1970s ro the early 1990s, along with other unofficial activities, helped ro lay the groundwork for the Oslo agreement of September 1993 (Kelman, 1995, 1997d). Such efforts contributed by developing cadres prepared to carry out productive negotiations; by sharing information and formulating new ideas rhat provided substantive inputs into the negotiations: and by fostering a political atmosphere rhar made clic

parties open to a new relationship. After rhc Oslo agreemenr, Kelman and Rouhana initiated a new project:

rhe [oim Working Group all Israeli-Palestinian Relations, which mer reg­ularly hcrwecn 1994 and 1999. Per the first time in this prugtam, the group set itself the goal of producing written documents: joint concept papers on the issues in the final-status ncgociarions. viewed in the conrcxr of what would be required to establish a long-term peaceful and mutually enhancing

Page 24: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

,

/ 338 INTEKNATIONAI. KELATrONS

relarinnship bcrween the two socrcues. The group thus intended [0 con­niburc both (Q the negotiations themselves and to the posmegctiarion proce~s of I)C;lct-building :lad reconciliation. Three papers, on general prin­ciples for the final-slatus negotiations (joint Working Group 011 Israeli­Palestinian Relations. 1998), the problem of Palestinian refugees and the tight of return (Alphcr & Shikaki, 1998), and the future Israeli-Palesllnian relationship (Joint Working Group, 1999), have been published. A fourth. 011 Israeli settlements, was dose to completion bur remains unpublished.

DI,Jal Purpose

Problem-solving workshops can best be vjcwcd as "workshops" in [he [ircral SCI)SC of [h~ rcrm: as providing a specially constructed space in which the parties can engage in the process of exploration, observation, and analysis and f.lshion new products (hat can be exported into the political arena, \'I;'orkshoJls thus have a duni purpose. They arc designed. firsr, (0 produce changc-c--new learning~ in the form of new understandings. new insights,

\ and new ideas for resolving the conflict-in the particular individuals who participate in the workshop and, second, to transfer these changes into the political debate and the decision-making process in the two societies. De­pending on their particular positions in the society, individual participalHs carl rumniunicare their new Illsigbts and ideas through their writing, lec­turing, and political xcrivirics, or the advice (hey give to political decision­makers. The participants in the Joint Working Group rook a further step by shaping these insights and ideas into concept papers, which were made available to decision-makers. political elites. and (he wider public as the two sides moved into the final-status negotiations. - .

An important th~(]":liL'11 and practical consequence of the dual pnrpose of workshops is that the two purposes may create contradictor), require­rnencs. The best example of these dialectics of interactive problem soking is provided by the sdcction of jMlliLipanl.S. Tt-amftr into the political proce~~

would be maximized by uAi.eial.~ who are close to [he decision-making ap­par:llus and tnUS in a position to apply immediately what cbey have learned. r.IJflJ1g~, however, would be maximized by par ricipanrs who are removed from rhc decision-making process and therefore less constrained in their inrerncrious and freer to play with ideas and explore bypornelic;t( scenarios. To halance these conft:ldiclOry requirements, sekCfioll has focused un par­ncipams who are not official~ hur arc politically mfiuential. They <Ire elms relatively free to engage in rbe process, bur, at the same time, an}' new ideas rhey develop in the course of a workshop ean have an impaCt 011 the lhiuk­illg of decision-makers and the society at large.

Ground Rules tortn teraction

Problem-solving workshops follow a set of ground rules that arc presented La the pamcipanrs in great delil.il. The central ground rule is the principle

Page 25: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

,

/

Conllicr Analysis and R<:.Iolmion 339

of privacy and confidcntialiry. ln the early lsracli-I'alestiniun workshops, coufidcnrialtry was particularly important for the protection of the partici­pams, because rhc mere fact of mecrln!! with the l.'ocmy was controversial and exposed them ro political. legal. and even physical risks. Confidcmialiry is equally important, however, for protection of rhe process that workshops seck to promote. The: ground fults ;JfC designed 10 encourage rbe partici­pants ro talk :Iud listen ro each other, rmhr-r than foclIs Oil their constitu­encies, an audience, third parries, or the record. They art: asked to think our loud. ro experiment with ideas, and explore different opliollS, without having to wony about how others would react if their words were quoted outside. This is why [Il~re is no audience. no publiciry, no record, and no artriburion. focusing Oll t';lch orhcr eD:lhJcs and elleouragcs the p:wicipams [Q enter into a eype of interaction that is generally nor feasible among pnrties engaged in a bitter conjlicr-c-a rype of interaction, indeed, that deviates from rhe conflict norms ,h:H mmJJy t;0vcrn rheir behavior: an imcraecion rhar is ilrlfl/ytic rather than polcmicn], that is, in which the parties seck to

\ explore each other's perspective and gain insight into the causes and dy­namics of rhe conflj(l; and ;In inrcracrioll dlat is proUt'm Jolving radler chan ;\JversariaL char is, in which rhe panics sidestep the usual attempt (0 allocate blame and insrcad take the conflict as a shared problem thnr requires joint efrarr to find a mutually s;llis(acwq< s<llution,

Another ground rule is that in a workshcp-c-unlike a negociating ses­sion~thf'rf' is no expectation thar the parries will reach an agreement, A5 ill any conflict resolution effort, there is an inrercsr in finding common ground, bur the amount of ngrcemcnr achieved in the discussions is nor necessarily a measure of the success of the enterprise. If panicipanrc come ;lway with a better undersr:lmling of rhe <liller's perspective, their own pri­orities, and the dynamics of rhe conflict, the workshop will have fulfilled its purpose, even if it has UOl produced an outline of a peace rreary, The ]oim Working Group was all exception in lhis respcec, ill th:H ir.s purpose wax to produce joint concept papers, ahhollgh even these papers-while they explore diffcrenl options and seek to rcframe issues-do ncr necessarily come up with a singl~ agreed-all soJurion. The Joinr Working Group also differs from the earlier work in tlrar rhe paerkipaucs eveurually went public with the issuance of [he completed concepr papers; up (0 the point of publication, however, the prillciple of mnfidenri:l:liry was sHiedy observed.

Yet another grolilld rule calls for the equality of rhe tWO panics within the workshop setting, Asymmetries in power, moral position, or rcpmrnion ekITly pl.ly an illlporc:tl!{ role in tile conflict and muse be taken into account in the workshop discussions. Bur the rwo parties are equals in rhe workshop setting in [he sense that each parry has the same righr to serious consider­<Ilion of irs needs. fe;us, and concerns. Wichin che coles (If rhc workshop, Israeli panicipnnts canner dismiss Palestinian concerns on the grounds [hat the Palcsriniuns are the weaker parry and, therefore, in a poor bargaining po,~irion, nor can P;llescini.ln parcieipancs dismiss lsraef concerns on rhc

Page 26: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

, /

.'\<i0 INTERNATION,\l RE1.ATIONS

grounds that the Israelis arc the oppressors and, therefore, not enudcd to

sympathy. Each side has the right [Q be heard in the workshop and each side's needs and tears must be given equal arrenricn in the search for a mutually satisfactory solution.

\

A Ii:n~J ground lull' concerns the facilicati~'c role of the third pJrry. The third party in this model docs nor take part in the substantive discussion; it does not give advice or uncI its own proposals, nor does it rake sides, evaluate rhc ideas presented, or arbitrate between different interpretations of historical [acts or inremnrionnl law. Its task is (0 create the conditions dl;/.[ allo\\ idc>L~ for resolving Ihe conflic r co emerge our of the inremrrion between die panics themselves. The facilitation of the third party, however, i,~ an important part of the process, The third party SC[S the ground rules and monitors adherence to them; it helps 1O keep the discussion moving in constructive directions, tries ro stimulate movement, and intervenes as rel­evant Wilh questions, observations, and even challenges, relating both to

the content and the proccss of the interaction, It also serves as a repository of trust for panics who, by dchnirion, do not trust cncb other. They feci safe to participate because they cruse the third parr)' and irs abiliry to mail!" rain confidentiality and protect their interests.

Workshop Agenda

III rhc C)-picat ouc time, free-standing workshop, rhe agenda is relatively open and unstructured wirb respect [0 the substantive -issucs under discus­sion. Tht way these issucs arc approached, however, and rhc order of dis­cussion 'Ire structured so as to facilitate the kind ofdiscomse that the ground rules are designed to encourage. A similar structure, with some necessary modifications, characterizes the agenda within and across rhe meetings of a continuing workshop.

The nrst discussion session of any workshop is usually devoted [0 an exchange of information between the two sides, which serves ro break the icc and scr the tone for the kind of exchange the workshop hopes [0 gen­crnre. E:Jch party is asked [0 ralk about til(: .,iruatio/l on the gmt/nd and the current mood in its own cornruuniry, about the issues in the conflict as seen in that community, about rhc spectrum of views on the confiicr and its SOIUliul1, and abour participants' own positions within that spectrum.

This exchange provides a shared base of informarion and sets a precedent for {he two sides to deal with each other as mumal resources ra.ther dlall wlcly as cornbaranrs.

Following the opcning discussion, the core agenda of the workshop consists of four pans. lr begins with a needs analysis, in which members Oil each side are asked to discuss their central concerns in the conflicr-c-rhe /i.llldame{llal needs that would have to be addressed and rhe cxistential fcan that would have to be allayed if a soluriou is to be satisfactory to them. The panics arc asked not to debate the issues raised, although they may ask

Page 27: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

/ Conflicr Analysis and Rc~{)llltion 341

for clarification of what the: orher says. The: purpose: is for tach side (0 gain an adequate understanding of rhc orhcr's needs, fears, and concerns, from rhe perspective of the other..Once they have dcmomrrarcd that they un­derstand each other's needs to a substantial degree, the workshop moves to

rhc second phase of the agenda: joint thinking nbour possible solutions. The difficult assigumcnr that participants arc given in Ihis phase is to develop, through an interactive process, ideas about [he overall shape of a solution

for the conflict as a whole Or, perhaps, a particular isme in the conflict. that would address rhe needs and [tars of both sides.

\

As participants develop some: common ground ill this Prl)ChS of joint chinking, rhey turn to the nexr phase of rhe workshop: discussion of the political and psychological constraints within rhe two societies that would create barriers ro carrying Out the ideas for solution that have been devel­oped in the group. This is a very important part of the discussion, because parties in conflict usually find it extremely difficult to understand the con­srrainrs on [he other sidt:-or even to recognize that the other, Jikt: them­selves. has constraints. But it is best to leave the discussion of constraints to [his later phase. so that it does not hamper the creative process of jointly generating new ideas. Finally, depending on how much progres5 has heel! made and how much time is left, the parties are asked to engage in another round of joint thinking-this time about ways of overcoming the COIl­

srrainrs that have been presented. The parricipams are asked to come up with ideas about- what their governmellts, their societies, and they them­selves might do-c-separarely or joindy-c-chac would help to overcome the barriers to negotiating mutually satisfactory solutions ro the conflict.

~ Challenges Facing the Field

Confiicr analysis and resolution from a social-scientific base with a profes­sional practice orientation is a relatively new field of endeavor, which, in addition co [he fundamental complexity and iruractahiliry of the phenom­enon that it addresses. must also confront and overcome many difficult issues This brief section will only be able to identify a numher of die most important of these.

Culture and Gender

Scholars and practitioners of conflict resolution need 10 lake the questions of cuhnral and gender influences seriously (Avruch, 1998; Taylor & Miller, 1994). It is 110{ appropriate ro assume the universality of concepts and methods, regardless of che sncictal environment to which they ate applied. Each society has its "culture of conllict," which incorporates the beliefs, practices, and institutions relevant to managing differences and which affects what is defined as conflict and how it is addressed (Ross, 1993a). CuI lure

Page 28: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

, /

342 INTERNATIONAL RELATIONS

IS Imporlant in how it affects negotiating styles and third-party roles, and representatives and intermediaries who work across cultural boundaries re­quire sensitivity [0 their own culture and crossculrural understanding ill order to interact appropriatel}' and effecrively. A lirsI step is to carr), OUt a cultural analysis of the situation, 50 that rhc effects of cultural differences on the etiology and expression of the confiicr are clearly understood (Avruch & Black, 1993). Simih r points cnn be made about gender differences as they are expressed in conflict, cspecially given rhe patriarchal and hierar­chical nature of most societies, which incorporates significant differences in Slams and power. An analysis based on gender differences created b)' tra­ditional socialization contrasts the dominant male, competitive, udversanal, righrs-bnsed approach with ;l relanonship-onenred, cooperative, and caring female style. However, research in Nonh America rends not to support these differences clearly in studies of either negotiation or mediation (Kccshlj-,

i \

1994: Stamato, 1992), possibly because the variable of biological sex is often confused wirh that of gender, which is socially constructed. Nonetheless, there arc indications rhar the mnnuer in which women versus men enter into conflict analysis and resolution may be different, with important im­plications for the focus and outcomes of ,he aceiviry For example, based on an analysis of interactive problem-solving workshops, d'Esrree and Bab­bin (1998) conclude that women tend to engage in deeper self-disclosure, leading to empathy for the enemy, and a reciprocal acknowledgment of concerns, coupled with an orientation (Q huild relationships and a c.apaciry to surface emoricnal .n well as strategic issues. This implies that women ma)' be heuer equipped (Q build relationships in the pre-negotiation phase and ro craft more integrative agrccments that have increased susminabiliry following settlement. Continuing attention W both gender and cultural is­sues is rims warranted.

Professionalization, Training, and Ethics

Most people who come to the work of ccntlict analysis and resolution are professionals from a related field, such as international relations, law, psy­chology. human relations, diplomacy, or psy(hialry, which enahles them to analyze social problems and prnvide some form of service. Only receudy have a small number of interdisciplinary graduate programs been established to train scholar-prnctirioners in the many intricacies of conflict and irs res­olution,:t daunting task rh.rr involves the application of a variety of concepts and models from social science and the acquisition of a range of strategies and skills from various domains of social practice. Man)' practitioners begin their practice with only a modicum of the analytical tools and social skills they need and must learn through experience from more seasoned profes­sicnals. Thus there is a challenge to develop training programs, both at the gradllate and mideareer levels, thrn will provide practitioners with the knowledge and capacities they require to engage successfully as negotiators,

Page 29: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

/

\

mediators, third-parry consultants, dialogue facilicators, or trainers or con­fhct resolution. Then: is also a need to provide continuing professional development opportunities for scholar-pracccioners [Q broaden their COI1­

ccprunl knowledge and to enhance their strategic and metical repertoire. Such offerings nQW exist, but mere is no understanding of their quality or depth. or how some collecrion of them might coalesce toward an :ldl:IIU:llC

level of professional competence. Thus it would be valuable [0 iniriare ac­riviriex that would assist in the profcssionaliaarion of the field at rhc inter­national level, so that knowledge bases and best practices could be shared coward the improvement of human welfare. Currently, many scholar­practitioners connect through existing associations, such as the International Society of Political Psychology, and have engaged in some useful networking accivirics in these fora. Such interactions need to be enhanced in order to

provide an ongoing arena for the discussion of developmcnrai issues, such as training and the ethics of praClicc. that affect the character and effect­iveness of the field.

Evaluation

Onc of the key challenges confronting the field of interactive conflict res­olurion is evaluation of [he effectiveness of its efforts in achieving the goals It sets 0111 to achieve. As a field that proposes to introduce inuovanve. academically based forms of iutervcnncn in confiicc into the larger diplo­mane process, Interactive conflict resolution has a special obligation ro dem­onstrate its utility and snCCess by way of sj'stetuaric. empirical evidence that is consistent with scholarly standards. Writers in the field have increasingly moved 1O respond ro this challenge (e.g., Ross & Rothman, 1999; Rouhana, 2000; Saunders, 2000; Chnrnway in press: d'Esrree, Fast, Weiss, & Jacob­sen, 200 I). The ultimate goal of interactive conflict resolution is 10 con­trilnnc to the achievement of a negotiatcd ,'greemem that is mutually S2t­isfacrory and la.~ting and rhar transforms the relationship beeveen the conflicting parries. Since inrcracrive problem solving-which is not in the business of negotiating ngreemenn-c-cannot prodlta such an ourcome but only contribute ro it, [he most relevant criteria for evaluating it refer to irs success in ;u:hieving its intermediate goals rather than its ulrimnre goal. The imermcdiare goals constitute changes in the political cultures of dlC con­!lining panics rhar would make them more receptive to negotiation with each other (Kelman, 1996a). Standard models of evaluarion-c-such :l~ Ihe c:xperimcnral field test-are not applicable to this problem. Furthermore. the ll~e of obtrusive observations and experimental manipulations is often ethically or «ierhodologicdly uuacccprable in research on ongoing inter­ventions. The challenge, therefore, is to develop evaluation models and research methods rhar are appropriate to the nature and purpose of the enterprise. Appropriate models have to he based on the gradual accumu!a­tion of pieces of evidence ill support of the underlying assumptions of the

Page 30: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

,/

341 INTEIlN,\T!ONAL RELATIONS

approach. These may involve identifying and resting the individual steps in the process ofinreracrive conflict resolution that arc hyporhesized to account for its effecnvcness, or testing some of the theoretical assumpcions of the approach in other scnings, including experimental analogs and laboratory simulations.

Complementarity of Interventions

There is a challenge ro understand how variations in third-parry roles COJl­

rribure differentially and uniquely to ocgoeiaeion SUCCC5S and sustainable resolution. The early proponents ofinteractive conflict resolution were clear on its potential as a useful prcnegotiation acriviry (c.g .. Burton, [969, Kel­man & Cohen, 1976), in line with a rationale more fully articulated by

,

\ Fisher (L989) but it is now evident that it can make conrribucions at all stages of the negotiation and resolution process (Kelman, 1992, L998b). Given that conflict, especially of an ethncpolirical nature between identity groups, is a pOlem mix of objective and subjective factors, irnerveruions are required to address rhe latter, in terms of the rnispcrceptions, misattribu­rions, hostile attitudes, mistrust, hatred, and vengeance that fuel escalation and intractability, In fact, it is difficuh to see how identity-based confiicrx can be addressed without methods that focus on the human and psycho­logical side of the equation (Ross, 1993b; Rothman, 1997). The question is how these methods can be related to and sequenced with the more rrn­dirional forms of conflict management. Fisher and Keashly (1991) devel­oped a contingency approach to third-party intervention, proposing that different methods be matched to the stage of conflict escalation for maxi­mum utility. They also propose that methods need to be sequenced in a complementary fashion, 50 that a lead intervention givc.s w ay to ochers designed to decscalate and resolve the conflict. There arc tWO points of complementarity between interactive conflict resolution (represented by third-party consultation) and mediation, in both ics pure and power forms. The first occurs where consultation serves as a prernedinrion activity that improves understanding and builds trust in the relationship so that pmc mediation can deal more effectively with objective issues, The second sees consultation as following power mediation, which has achieved a cease-fire or initial serdemenr on substantive issues, in order [Q rebuild the lorn n-.

lationship toward a comprehensive agrc:elllem and a sustainable peace. While a limited amount of experimental and empirical research supports the contingency approach (Keashly & Fisher. 19%), it remains a skeletal rcpresentatiou of a complex set of relationships that may not play (Jilt as diagramed in the complexity uf real-world dynamics. Nonetheless, the con­tingency model and similar attempts (e.g., Kciesberg, 19%) challenge the­orists and practitioners to think more seriously about the coordination and complcmeuramy of interventions that may well be required to adequately address intractable erhnopolirical conflicts.

Page 31: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

/

Conflict Analy,is :Jlld Resolution 345

A Comprehensive Approach to Diplomacy

An inrersocietal view of conflict, :1.5 we have proposed, calls for :J. complex mix of official and unofficial processes, complementing each other ill the achievement or the overall diplomatic goal. While hinding agreements can be signed only through official negotiations, Other [racks-public diplo­macy, people-to-people projects, media programs, curricular changes, non­violent action campaigns, along wirh interactive confliCt resoluriou-c-can each make their own unique contributions to the larger enterprise. Inter­active conflict resolution is particularly useful in providing opportunities for the parties to engage in the processes of exploring ideas, sharing perspectives, analytic thinking, and joint problem solving rhat arc essential to the search for a mutually satisfactory solution 10 the confiicr bur that are oueo inhib­

i ired by rhe constraints rhar characccrizc interactions around the negOli;lling

\ table. The microprocess of inreracrivc conflict resolution thus helps to pro­mote four components of conflict resolution that must take place sonic. where in an effective macroprocess of confllcr resolution: idennficancn and analysis of the problem in the relationship rhnt the conflict represents; joint shaping of ideas for a mutually acceptable solution; mutual influence

-rhrough reassurance and other positive incentives: and creation of a SllP­

portive political cnvirouiuenr (Kelman, 2000). The challenge is to make effective use of rhc potential contributions of interactive conflict resolution and orhcr unofficial tracks in the official diplomatic process. Ideally, the products of problem-solving workshops and related ncciviries can he used for exploring possibilities, formulating options, and framing issues ill ways that can advance negotiations at irs various stages. This has indeed happened on occasion, but it needs to be done systematically, while making sure that track tWO effons maintain their imegriry and independence and do not become-or come to be seen as-merely another component of the rrack one process. Official negotiations can also benefit from adopting some of the exploratory, analytical, and problem-solving methods of interactive con­Hicr resolution in their own proceedings, insofar as they can be accommo­dared within the constraints of the official process. Practitioners of inter­active conflict resolution, on their parr, need to be well informed of rhc issues, problems, and progress of the official ptoccss so that rhey can provide input that will be most directly relevant to rhc status of ongoing ucgorin­nons.

Ins!itutiona/iza uon

At the level of a particular conflict, it might he useful to institutionalize interactive conflict resolution as part of the peace-building proccss that niusr accompany and follow the negotiation of a peace agreemenl. An ongoing mechanism for conflict resolution is generally an essential COlllJlonent of the civil socicry institutions across the national lines chat must be huilt to

Page 32: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

//

"

\

3-iG INTERNATIONAL RELATIONS

ensure a stable peace and cooperative relationship between former enemies who must coexist ill close proxuniry to one another, AT the global level, the persistence and proliferation of deadly conflicts between ethnic groups around the world suggest the urgent need for a large. well-endowed, mostly nongovernmental organi".,atioll devoted to monitoring such conflicts as they evolve and ready to intervene with efFons to help prevent and resolve them (Burton, 1983). The purpose of such an insnrurion would be co supplement rhc work of existing govcrnmcm:tl, inrergovernmcnral, and nongovernmen­tal organi7..arions dcvorcd to peacemaking, peacekeeping, and posrconflicr humanitarian aid hy h-inging together politically influential representatives of the opposing sides in an active or impending conflict for joint explora­tion, within a problem-solving framework, of steps toward prevenuog, de­escalating, or resolving the conflict. The insriunion might include a per­manent staff to monitor confiicc regions and provide the infrastructure for workshops as the need arises; a cadre (If regional and conflict resolution specialists avnilahlc to organi7.e and lead workshops; and a cadre of local representatives to recommend appropriate anions or evaluate proposals from the staff and to assist by organir:ing and participating in workshops as needed. There is no direct evidence of how much a global institution or­ganized along these lines and dedicated ro the systematic application of interactive conflict resolution techniques to ethnic conflicts around the world could concrihurc [0 preventing such conflicts, defusing them once they have turned violent, and rebuilding the societies torn apan hyviolence. But research and observation suggest that die assumptions behind inrerac­rive conflict resolution arc sound, and experience suggests that it has the potential for transforming conflict relationships. If the resources needed for a large-scale effort of this kind can be generated. there is at least [he hope that it can begin to tackle the problem of ethnic violence that has been plagning the inremarioonl cornrnuniry,

lirrl... Note

This chapter rcprc~~ms a joint eflon to which the ('.'>'0 authors madeequalcontributions. Herbert Kelman graldully adillowledge~ the \Villiam and Flora Hewlett Foundation's support for the Program on lmernacicnal Conflict Analysis and Reschnion (PICAR) dUI he directs at rhe \Veatherh('ad Cenrer for InrernarinnaJ Affairs, Harvard University.

lirrl... References

Allison, G. T (J9i I). EllCilU "/decision: E~plailli"g rhr Cuban mi5rilerrisis. Bml<>n: Little, Brown.

Alpher; j., & Shikaki. K., with the parriciparinn of the additional members of the Joint Working Group on Isr.leli-Pabtinian Relations {1998), fl,e l'a!e5fi"ial/

Page 33: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

I ,

347

rrjilgr-'" ,mbum OJld :/Jr ri,gh' ofrrlum. \VI.·:llherh...ad Center for lrnematiorml

Affairs Working Pafl<:l No. 98-7. Cambridge, MA: Weatherhead Center (or in­ternational Aff:l,rs, Harvard University (Reprinted in Midd/r l:-im "obey. hbru­

;IrY 1999. 6l3J, J67-189). Avruch, K. (l998). Cll/om IIl1d amjlia ma/Illioll. W:L\hingmn, DC: United Scares 10­

"ilute or Pesce.

A"rodl, K., & flJ.Kk, P. /l993). eoullin rt50Jution in imcrcuhunJ sctlinp' l'mbJcm' and prOlpcCl.l. In D.1- D. Saudolc & H, V::In der Merwe (&k), Crmjlicr uso/u­tion r!Jcory fwd prtltlia: /IIlq;mrioJl lind nppJicati01r [pp. 131-145). M:mcheSlcr, I:'.Il!;bnd: M:lllchc.<LCI Uni"!.'>;l)" PIC",

At:J.r, E. E. (J9~O). The nJflllagr",c>l/ o!promw('d social conf/icr. Hampshire. England:

Dacunouth. BClIdwin. D. (l971). TJH~ power of pQf;ch'c ~:ln<:lians. lVorkt Po/itia, 24. /9-38. Beecoviech, J. (1997). Medi:l(ion in internarional conflict. In I. W. ZamnJn &]. I..

\ Ha5mU5len (Eds.), 1'rtlCnnaki7lg ill illl~mariollal conjlia: Milhom fwd ttdmi"""

(p p. J25-153}. W:L<hingron, DC: V"ired Sr;z{(:5 InJrin,ll; of Pesce. Rercnvirch. j., & Houston, A. (1996). The srudy of iruerrmnooal mediation: Tbeor«.

ic.:d ISSU~ and empirical evidence. In J. Bercovirch (Ed.), ReIoloing hlfcnwiolinl

cMjliClJ: 'l7'e r/uory a"d prtteria ojmt'diulian (pp. I J-35). Boulder, CO: Lynl11: Rienncr.

Bcrcovllch, J., & Rubin, J. Z. (Ed •.). (J991). M,diatioll in illtr'mnrionnl rrlarioM.' Multipb approaclm ro confJi£{ maflagmf('//(. New York: 51. MUlin'f Press.

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1961). The mirror image ill Soviet-American relations: A social psychologist's report. jortmal ofSociallmw, 17(3), 45-56.

Uurtun. J. VI. (I 'lG9). Coafiicf and {Q/fIIII""i~,rri'lfI" TIJ.- ,u.- ofcO"{TOII";com""m;COf/Otf

;11 il1unlmionall1!/tJ'i01U. London: Macmillan. Burton, J. W. (1979). D{l'U,l1U, terromm "nd war: TI,e prows <ljJo/vinK IIMoloed Jocial

I1mlpoliricI11 probln,u. New York: 51. Marrin's Press.

Burtun.]. \Y/. (1983, April). A coII/i1llliIlK urni""r and a" ,,,'mllllionalJitdlita,;,'gscr­lIice. A proposal by membcrs of the U:ntrc far the Analnis uf ConAict (Uni­\'cr,iq' of Kent, unrcrlJury, England) p,cscnced ~, che ~nnual mce:'ing of ,he International Smdies A.s.sociation, Mexico Ciry

Burton, J.W. (1984). Globll! c07if/ir" Thr domestic sourca ojimrmatiol/t:ll crisiJ. BriJ;h­ton. Er.J;/and: \Vhcauheaf.

BllrlUn,]. \Y/. (1987). Rmlriil'g dup-rooud lOlljlia: A bandbook. unhanl, MD: Uni­vc.,il)' I'r~. of AIneri<:~.

Burton, I. W: (Ed.l. (I 99GJ. CoJJjlkr: HI/TII4" ucrds r!mJry, New York: 51. MJnin's

Press. CatJle\'~le, [~J., & Pruitt. D. G. (1')92). Negociodon ~nd medi~tiun. A,ulllal Reuirw

of {'tj'dJo{nr:Y. 4.1. 531- 582. Chasin. R., & l-Icr-~ig, M_ (1993). Cfl-":ltlng ~ysrclllic imcr...entions for the sociopoliri­

c,1 ~rena. In D. B~f];<:r Gould & D. llillcboe DeMuth [Eds.}, The globalfim,.

il] rhu,Jpisl: IlIuKuri,'K rlu perrm1l1!. pro[cJI'-olftJ!. m,d polirical(pp. 149-192). Bo.<tun: Allyn and B~CUlI.

Ch:lSiJl, R., Hcrl.ig, M., Roth,S., Ch,,-,io, L. Becker, C" & St~jn." R., jr, (19%).

From di:Jrribc to di"roguc all divisivc public issue'; Approaches dr~wn (rolll Cunily rh..r~py. M,di"rioll Q"nncrb\ 13, 323-344.

Ch~law~y. C. (ill press). lUIC5Sing the 5Oeial'psychologlC<l1 ,uppal! (or Kelman's imer­active [>r"hfem-.",r,;ng work'(,ops. In A. £lgl)', R. M Baron. & V. L Hamilton

Page 34: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

,

,//

," INTF.RNATIONAL RELAl'lOtH

i

\

(EJs.), The wdn{ pryrholof:l ofgrOlip idmTiry and Jocin! fo>rjlicl: ThroT): tlPftim­uon, tIIlt! prtlrriu. W~hing(Qn, DC: Amer;cn Psychological Association.

Chufrin. G_ L. & Saunders, H. H. (1993). A public peace praCtsl. Negotil/tiolJ jour­1I1l/.9. 155-177.

C"hm, R. (19')7). Ncgotitllillgllrrol! culturn (2nd cd.). W:lShingmn, DC Unilt.J SIJlC~ Institute of Peace.

d'bll':C, T. P., & Babbin, E. E (1998). Womm and [he art of pl"J.ccmaking: D:lW (rum I~radi-P:l[tslinian interactive prohlem-solving workshops. Poliliml P,yclml­ofJ', 19, 185-209.

d'Esrree T P, FJJI, L. A., Wciu, J. N., & Jacob~cn. M. S. (1001), Changing rhe de­b.,te ;Ihnat "success" in CDnJIiCt resolution dI6m. Negolinr;(Jllj""n/Ilf, 17. lUl­I 13.

boob. [.. \'(I, (Ed.). (1970). Rno/ving confliccin Africff: TlJt FmlltdL/ WlJrklJMp. New Haven: Yale Unjver~ity Press.

Doob. L. W., & Folra, W,], (1973). The Delr",-lt workshop: An appliC:Hion of grollp techniques TO :I dc..ltrunive cOllfliu. jOUnJlI! of Conflict RtJol"ri()1], !7, 489-5 [2.

Druckman. 0. (1973). [fumllnfqClon ill i,runlllti()1/11! ntgorillriol/s; Socill!-pI)'chlJlogi<"d' {UptCll IIfillltn/ffr;()1In! (onflici. Sage Profeuiollal Paper in lntcrnancual Smdies 02-020.

Druckman, D. (1983}, Social psychology ill imernational negotiations. III R. F. Kidd

& M. J. S~ks (Eill.), AdllOIlW in opplitd $Ocill! pIJcho/0K! (Vol. 2, pp. 51-Rl). Hillsditle. N]: Erlbaum.

Druckman, D, (1997). Negotiaring in the intern~(ional ccutext. In 1. W. 7...inman & ]. L Rasmussen [Eds.], !'tl/ulliokillg in iJllemoriollo!conflicr: Mnhoih 11m! [(rh­ni'flltJ (pp. 81-123). \'if:Llhingmn, DC: Unitcd States Institute of Peace.

Druckman, D. (2001, Juuc). Nt/lll1dllff"rn ill IIcgo/ill/ioll rheory Old ft/fllrch. Paper presented ,1t the annual conference of me International Association for Conflict l\.hnagemcnt, Paris.

Druckman, D., & Mitchell, C. {Eds.). (1995, Nnvcmber). Fl.:xibiliry in inlerna'ional ncgoiiauon and medjat;on. Amrllb ofIht AmericlIlI AClldemJ ofPolirim! fllld Sn­cill! Srirl{f"t, 542,

r','rnham, B. (Ed.I. (1992). Special j~ilIe: Prospect theory and p<llitical psychology. Pa· Ii/irll! Psyc!JlJlaK!' 13. 167-329,

Fc.li"ger. L (1957). A tluory (Jfc(Jgl1it;V~ disso/lf/l1u, St~nrord, CA: Stanford Univct­si.)" Pre.".

Fisher. R.. & Ury, \Y/. (I '.>81). Gmillg to )(1 ''. NtgtJ/ill/ing "grummr wit!JoM gilling in. nn~tnll: Houghton Mimin.

Fisher. R.]. (J972). Third pHt)' consulrerior» A method for the .<Cudy and resolution or ccnllicr. jOllr1l1/J 0fConjlio R,(!O!urilJ1I, i6. 67-'.>'1.

fi,hcr, It J. (I ~83), Third party consulrarion as a method or intergroup conflict r"';O­lunon: A review or studies. journlll ofc..(JIlf/jtt R~slJlflfilJl1, 27. 301-.D4.

fisher, R.J. (1986), 1'h,,-d party consultation; A problem-solving approach ror dl'­cscalanng intcrnatiollal conflict, In J. f! Maas & R. A. C. Stewart (1'.(15.). 'Iv· ward ff world ofptOCt: Ptop!e cr(au '1lttnurrivrs (pp, 18-32). Suva. Fiji: Univrr­suy or the South Pacific Pre.'."

Fi.,hn. It {. (I 'J8'))_ Prcncgouaricn prohlem-solving dilcu~inns; Enh~ncjng (he pOlen­tiJ.l for successful negotiation. In J- G. Stein (Ed.). G~ffillg to tbr t,/M(: 'OJ" pro­ml ofiltftn10Iiff1/1I1 prllltgotillf;on (pp. 206-238). Bahlmore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Page 35: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

,,/

ConfliCl Analj-sis and Resolution 34'

Fisher, R. J. (1990). Tilt SOcill!PIJ,!Ju/0K'l of i",agrollp lI/1ff (Il/(nltuimwl wI/flier mul,,· non. New York: Springer.Verhg.

Fi~her. R.J. (1997). Interactive cOIif/iel ran!lItlo/l. Syr~cu.c. NY: Syracuse University

rr~.

Fisher, RJ. (2001). Methods of third party inrervenrion. In N. Ropers, M. Fischer. & E. Manton (Eds.), 1'lJt Baghojllllflfibool' ofamjliu IrllmfomWlioll (pp. 1­

27). Berlin: The Belghof Centre for Conflict Management. Fisher, R] .. & Ke::uhly. L. (1991J). Third parI}' consultation :1.< ~ m...rhod of uuer­

group and imcrnalion:>1 contlicr resolution. In R. J. Eisbcr. 7!Jr Jorilll pJ)'rhololJ' oji"urgroup find i"UnJllliO'1"1 conflict rtJol,lIio" [pp. 211-238) New York:

Springer-Verlag Fisher, R].. & Kc.uhl)'. L (1!!91). The pmenrial complc menranry <lr ruediaricn and

ronsuharion within ... coruingcncv model of third pJrt}' inlervmliull. -'oumlll of

Pmrr RtJmrr/', 28. 29---'12.

Follett. M. 1'. (1924). C1?lIril/t txptrimC(. BO'lon: Lcngm ... os Green_ Cecrgc, A. L, & Smoke, R. (1974). Drurrmcr in A",airl111ford!!! poliC)': Throry (lnd

prptlia. New York: Columbia Univernry Press.

\ Hampson, F. O. (1995), Mlllli-Mumi ntgorit/tiom. Ihl\;more: Juhm Hopkins Univer­

sity Press.

Heider. E (I958). Thc pI)'rholoKY of imrrpmolllll ulmillm. New York: Wiley. Heradsrveir, D. (1981). TIlt Amb-Ismai cOJ/flirt: Ptychlllogimf olmac/o m p<tIICC (2nd

ed.). 0510: Universirets[QrI ... gCL

Holsri, O. R. (1962). The belief ~stcm and uaricnal im:l.geo: A L~-'C 'Iudy. -'oumlll of Confl;cl Rmfutioll, 6, 244-252.

Holsd, O. R. (972). CciliJ, litaMlioll, toar. Momrc... l: Me(;ill.Queen·s University Press.

Hoprnann, P. T. (1995, November). Two p... rad;gms of negotiation: Batsaining and prohlern solving. T!J( Amrlllr of '/'e ,lme,inl'/ Acpdml)' ofPoli/iwl alld Socilll Sd­

Iller. 542, 24----47. Hopmann, 1'. T. (996). The /I'2,lIIil1lilll1 procm {/nd r/,t ulolmioll IIf ill/tnlaliolltll Hm­

fiim. Columbia: Universiry of SOlllh C.,rolina Press. Jan;s, I. L. (1982). GrOllplhillk (2nd cd.), Hoseon: Houghton MifHin.

Jerv;s, R. (1976), Percrptiom alld miJpmtpliom i" iurernmioual politics, Princeton:

Princeton Univer~iry Press.

Jervis, R., l.cbow, R. N" & :'ilc;n, J. G. (1985). PI)'dlOlogy l111d drrc"mcr. BaJtimore:

Johns Hopkins Universiry Press. Joint Working Group 011 lsraeli-Palesrinlan Relations (1998). Gn/<,ral prillcipftos for rh<'

jillnI157ncli.Pnltlli/Jin1l ngru/1Imf. PICAR Wnrking l'J.pet. Cambridge, /I.-1A:

Progr.rm On lurernauonal Conflict AnJ.ly.,i., and Resolution, Weatherhead Cen­

ler rnt International AJ'EI-irs, Harvard Un;,-e~i/y. (Reprinted in Middlc Em/

foun/nl, February 199'1,53[1].120-175)_

Joint Working Group on Israeli-Palevrinian Relations (1'1'19). ThtjimlTc brodi­

I'nlmillinll rtMriomlJip. Weatherhead Center fot lnreruarionnl Affairs ''''''rking

paper No. 99-12. Cambridge•.Il.IA: ''''eatherhc.1d Center for Inte'nal;onal Af­fair•. Harvard University. (Reprinted in Mid,lIt East Policy, I'ebruary 20110.

712]. 90-112).

Jane.,. E. E.• & Nuben, R. E. (1971), The :let"r and rIll" observer: Divergenr percep­

tions of the causes of behavior. ln 1'.. E, Jone." D, E. Kanouse, H. II. Kellcy,

R. E. Nisbett, S. Valius, & B. Weiner (Iids.}, Am-ilm/iflll: 1'((((ivillK rill Cil/IltJ

ofbtlJl1vio, (pp. 79-91). Morristown, Nj: General L"arnil1g I're55.

Page 36: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

, I

350 INTERN",TION"'L RELATIONS

Julim. D. A. (t 991). The pIJC(;'" (If track rwo dipl"mo.cy in rhe Arab-Israeli con(,r­(I1C'~. In V. D. Volbn,). V. Monroillc, & D. A. Juliu~ (Eds.), Tb, pryrlJudy­IIllm;n 0/ il/un/miol/al ,rI",iom"ipl. Vol. 2: Vlloffici,,/ diplol1lllC)' ar I<JUrI. [pp. 193-205). l"xing«Jl1, /1.·1/\: L:xllIgron UOllb.

K,,,,,bly, L. (J994). Gender and conflict: WhJ[ does p_'ydlOJOgic:l1 rcscJ"h rellns? In A. Tavlor &]. B. Miller (Eds.). Coujlia tII,,1 Kmdrr (pr. 217-235). Crcllkill,

NJ: Hampton Press. Kc,,,h1y, L. & Fisher, R. J. (1996). A contingency p"fSP'C!;'-' on (ooAin llU'n'cn­

rlon.<: Theoretical and pr~crjcJ.1 <:onsidcraLin"s. In J. Bcrcovhch (Ed.), Result)ing ;/lur,/4fioWII COl/fliers: TI" tlltory 4"d pmaic( 4 "'rdimioll (pr. 235-2(1). I:bul­

det, CO: Lynne Rimnel. Kelman, 1-1. C. {1969}. Pamrn~ ofpCf-mn~1 iuvolvemcut in rhc ll,tinn;u system: A

soci.,I-pJyc!l(Jlogicalnnnlysis of pnliticallegirinllcy_ In [. N. Ro~enau (Ed.]. !,,­ruwlIioJlr.l plllilirs awi ftrrigll /'Illicy: A uadrr ill ,rms".}, awl r!"ory (Rev. ed.,

pp. 276-288), New York: Free I'rc~s.

Kdtnnn, H. C. {1972J. The l'",Glcm ..solving wurkshop in conflict re.<olmion. In R. L. , Merrill (Ed.). Commlfn;mrioll ill jllunlfllirmlll!'olirirs [pp. IG3-Z0<\}. Ulb~Il:l:

University of Hlinois I'rns_\ Kelman, H. C. (978). Israelis and PalcJlini~nJ: 1',)"ehoJoginl prerequisites for rnutuul

"cc~pt"nc•. f"r~nrMiQI"" ."U"';I)". J, IGZ-lfl6. Kelman. H. C. (1985). Overcoming the ps)'chologicnl harrier: An ~n;uy.<is of lhe

Egypli:ln-15raeli pesce process, Nrgdriacifmjolmlfl/, 1,213-234.

Kdmno, H. C. (l986). lnlcr;lceive problem mlvinl',: A \'1Cial-pJycho!or,icalnpproad, to ccnfiicr mrolmion. In \V'. KIa:;""n (Ed.). Di"log~c: TQWIl,d ilJlu[r'i/h Imdrnffll1d­

jllg (pp. 293-31<\). lanlur, Jerusalem: EcumenicalJnsrinne for Theological Re­

search,

Kelman, H. C. 0987}. The political p~rch"lnl',J' or Ihe hrJdi.~ale5ein;nn conllicr. How "30 we ov~rCome d,e h3rrier< '0 a ne60limccl 50Ju~iun: Polifi",! PlJdlfl!Of!:Y,

8.3<\7-3G3. Kehnan, H. C. (1992). luforrnal mediarion by Ihe .«hobrlrl~ctieiunn, In 1- Bnco\'­

itch & 1- Z. Rubin (Eds.), Mrditltill" in intrrnariormi rdluioll1: ,Hu/tipll! "/,­

pmr.dJrJ to eanj/iu mnnngnnml (pr· G4-96). Ne\\' YU<k 51. Mntlin's Pc".". Kelman, H. C. (1993). Coalitions across (onlliu !illl'<; The inlefphr of mnfli("l~

within and belween rhe Israeli and l'a!c..liniln communllie.,. In S. \'Vorchel &

]. Silllp,o~ (Elk), COI{lirr braum) prop/rmltl grO"pl (pp. 236-258). Chic.;!g":

Nehon-H.l1. Kelman, H. C. (1995). Coembmious nf au un"fficial conRiet resolution efforr 10 rhc

hr<ldi-P;UeJlini<ln breakrhrough. Nrsoliatio" jOlinJal, II, 19-27.

Kdttlln, H. C. {19961, November}. A" Iff·ptO<ldf rll (l.,r.{"anllIl of N(,"O fOlHr;!mriom /0 rhr rrJolmiD" ,,[((Imonorial/ol cOlljlia(. f'c,l'l"l prrs,.",,,,1 .1r ,he (:, rn~6'~ (:"fpO­

r~rion Conference on rI,e Role uf lnreru.nional NGO.< in Ethnic and Narioonl

CunflicCJ, New York. Kdman, H. C. (199Gb) Neguli1lion 3S imeracuvr problem <ol"ing.l"rrrl/tllilJ//II/ Ne­

gotiarion: A !mm",1 af 'J1)!!o,] aml Prarricr. I( I), 99_l7T

Kelman. H. c. (19971). Gruup precesses in the r"<almiu,, of imcmancnal conAi,[s:

Experiences from th" lsraeli-l'alcvrini.m Ca<". Aml!nom Pl)·,.IJIII".~i!l. 52, 212:­

220. Kelman, H. C. (1997b). SUclll-psydU1lagiGL! dimrnsiOl:., of inll'rn:"ion~1 mnAin. In

r. \Ii. Zarlm"n & J- L. RJsl11us,rn (Ed".), Pmu11.'aJ:i"g ill imrrl/"Iiol1a/ rVl/flier:

Page 37: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

.I/

ConAin An~ly"i., and Resolution 351

Me/hom Dud udm;qlln (pp. 191-2-'7). W~~),jnI\IOn, DC: lJni,~d Srares lnni­

rure of Peec:.

Kelmeo, H. C. (l997c). Nationalism, p~lfiOli'lIl, and nal;ollJ.l identity: Social·

rsychologic~1 dirncnsions, In D. Ihl-T:!I & E. 51.,u!> (Eds.). P,ur;'>lIim ill ,hr fj""$ ofi",lividll<1U tflld ",u/om (pp. 1(,'5-1R9). Chicago: Nelson- Hall.

Kelman, H. C. (1997d). Some dClc:rrnillams of I{,e Oslo hrc:Lkrl"Qugh. l"unllf/jaUilt

Ncgorilllioll.2. 183-1')4. Kelman, H. C. (1998a). Imcracri\'c problem 101.·ing: An .1ppm:!,,], [0 wollin resolu­

tion :Jnd iu appliCldon in rhe Middle: 1.:,.<1. 1'.£- I'olrricnl .'l,imrr 6' Polirics, 31(2). 190-198.

Kelman, H. C. (199Bb), $ociJJ-psycholllgjeal co rnriburions co p,.,..,cmaking :mo pC,JCC­

building In the: Middk EaSL Ifpplinl {'1)'cI",loD' tl" In/mUlI/omd R~vi~w, 17. 5­

28. Kdman. H. C (2000). The role of the scholar.praruriouer in imcrn;uiona! conllkr

\ resohxion. Inr~matiolllll Smdi~< I'~TJp~rth'n. I. 27.1-2R7.

Kelman. H. C. & Cohen. S. n (1976). The probkm-so!ving workshop: A soelal­

psychological ronrriburioo Ul dIe resolution of imerna!ional conAie!. jDlm"d of ('MC( R~mm;/'. 13, 79-90.

Kres~d, K., Prui«, D., & Associates. (Eds.). (198'). fH~di,,(ion rtUllfC},; TIJ~ procm and 4fiailltllm of r!,j,d.paTly imuvcn,io1l. San l:ranci"o: Josscy-Ih~,.

Kricsberg, L (1982). Non-coercive inJllccmCllr5 in inlnn~lion,1 connin. 10 C. M.

Stephen50n (Ed.), AlumalilJ~ mnhodl }o' i"'rnl/uio",d (leurif)' (pI'. 105-120). W:Llhingwn, DC: Uoiversicy I'rc~. of America.

Kriesberg. L. (1996), Varieties (If mediating ~[/;,'i!ics and medi"lOrS in irnernarional

relations. [n J, Bercovircb (Ed.), Rool"i"&. ill/antlli,wal cDlzj{im: Thr IlJlOry lind

practice ofnud/a(ion (PI" 219- 233). Boulder, C(): L}'nne Ricoiler. Kricsberg. L (1998). Commlclillr cOllflirll. Lanham, MlJ: Rowrnan uud Liulcfield.

Lebow, R, N. (1981). /J''''''lfI pmu 4wl ""If. Ihltimorc: Johm Hopkins Unive"i[}' Pre.il.

L:bow, R. N. (1987). Nucka' "isis 11II1II/'.i'/"UlJI: A d",,~m1l(J ilfmion_ Irlraca. NY: Cornell Unlversiry Press.

Levy, J. S. (1992). Prospect theory and inrenlJI;ol1.,1 r,blions: ThmrClicaJ application~

and analyrical problems. ('0Iili<4ll'J)'c!Jolofj', 11,283-Jlll.

Mitchell, C. R. (1981), hau/lJakillg ,IUd ,hc conmlra",i ro]c. \'\Intn\cad, England: Gower.

Montville, J. V. (1987), The alo;:>w aml the olive brauch: The nse lor tr"ck ovo di­

plomJC)'. In J, \YI. McDan..ld & D. B. Bcndahmanc (1".<1,.), Conflict raohnion: '!i-ack two diplomacy (1'1'.5-20). \'\Ia5h ingl" n , DC: 1'01<:;1;11 Service lnsrinne, Dcpanrnenr of Stale.

Moore, C. \'<f. (1996). TJJl mrdimiw! p'''<"m: {'me/i.-,ll '/rillc};ir! }o' m"l"illg COl/flier

(2nd ed.). S~n Francisco, jessey-Bas•.

MIlI,ar. J. S. (1986). Understanding compeling theories of m·(;'jtiation. Nrgolitui"" journal. I, 23--29.

Osgood, C. E. (19(,2). A'l a/rcnulIilJr W 11.'Ilr ", lJItTrmk,. Urb~,,~: Uni"e";t}' of IIli­noi~ Press.

Pruirr, D. G. (1981). Ncgllli,(//oll b~/1tflli",. New YOlk: Academic I'le'5.

Pruiu, 0, G. (1986). AclJieving integrali ......'greeme,m in I1C~oli~tiOIl. 1n R. K. \'(fJI'te (Ed.), PsycbololJIIlId /IJt pra/cnsion of ,m,.{rtlr w,lr (pp. <1(13--1.78). New Yurk:

New York Univer5icy Press.

Page 38: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

, ,/

i

\

352 INTERJ'JATlONAl RELATlnN.~

Pruitt, D. G., & Carnevale. r. J. (I ')').~). Nrgotitfli'''' il/ loriol conflicr. Pacific Creve:

Broob/Colc. ['ruin, D. G., & G;UUg:l,II, J. n (l')7<1), Campus crisis: The search for p<Jwcr. In

J.T. Tedeschi (Ed.), PaJparil'rr <m sorialpOUia (PI" 349-3?Z). Chbgo: Al­dine.

RaiAil, H. (1982). n,r lin lind Jrim,.(" II!,ugo/;mim,- Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni­

ve"il)' PlIO'S,

Rosenberg, S. W.. & Wol[,fcld, G. (1977). International conflict and the problem of anribution. jounlll! ofConflict Roo/urio", 21. 75-103.

Ross, L.. & \Vard, A. (1')'))). rsychulogiL:.l barriers to dispute resolution. In M.I~

z.:.'ma (Ed.), Aduancrs in t.cpaimmfill mc;"tpJJrhology (Vol. 27, pp.255-304). New York: Academic PIts>.

Rou, M. H. (l993a), The wll/lrt ,1ronJficr- fUlaprrMfiom tl1/d inUWfJ i" CDmpllTtlrili~

prrJl'u:rilJ~. New Haven: Yalc Uni'·CJ.,il)' Press. Ross, M. H. (J'J'J3\J), Till "l1l11l1gm:mr ofw"/firl. New Haven: Yale l.Iniversiry Press. Ross, M. H., & Ro-hman, J. ((999). nJrory om/l'rttflirr in trl}/jicconflier /l/Il11l1gtmmr:

TJJtorizirig l,temJ <J/1d [ilil"". Loudoo: Macmillan. ROIIJnlln, J. (1997). RtsollJilig idmli:j'./llIud "mftier. S~11 Francisco: [osscy-Bass. Rouhana, N. N. (1997).· Pl1lmim,'" rllierm ill 'w ulmic jrwilJ, rll//(." Jdmtiriu ill CO/l­

/fief. New Haven: Yale Univw;il)' Press.

Rouhaaa. N. N. (2000). Imerani,'c conll.i<:l rcsolutinn; Issues in theory, melh<JJology, and evaluation. In 11. C. Slc'rn & D. Druckman (Eds.), Intcrnmianai CVllj/':1;f ra­

nlllli,,/, t1fur rlu Cold Wwr (pp. 294-3J7). \'(/l~h;"gwn, DC: National AGld"my

l'rcs~.

Rouhana, N. N., & Kdm~n, H. C. (1994)_ Promoting jouu thinking in jnterrrationa] conflicts: An Isr.,di-l'"k'lini~n continuing workshop. jmf'nlill "rSorilll hnes, 50

(1), 157-178. Rubin, J, Z .. Pruitt, D. G .. & Kim. S, 1l. ([')94). SlIrinl COliflier: Emlln/io/I. IfnlmlllU,

,wd sur/muff( (211d ed.). Ncw Yurk: I\kG.~w-HHI.

Saunders, H.I'-J. (1995). SU5wined Ji'J!ugul' llll Tajikistan. "Mind t:1rd Human lntcmc­

lilm.6(3), 123-135_ Saunders. H. H. (l998). A public pmrr promL Ncw Y<;lrk: St. Marrin's Prc'~,

Saunders, H. H. (2000). luccracuvc conRin resolutiun: 1\ vecw for policy m:J.kcr5 on making and building peace. In I'. C. Stern & I). Druckman [Eds.}, Inr~mn­riOJlll1 cOtrJlia molruivu I/jiu Ilu Cold 11l,'" (pp. 251-293). W~shington, DC: National AGldtmy Prr5S.

Sawyer, J., & Guerzkow. 1-1. (1'J65). Ihl~~ining and negotiation in intcrnarion:u tela­rions. In H. C Kelman (Ed.), 111107/1((00"111 1I(IItl~io,.' A socitd-prF""logim{lIIl1tl­ysis {pp. 4(;6-510), New 'I'ork: Holt, Itincbrr and Winston.

Schelling, T. C. (l9<J3). Hu srr,lU.f'J' ofro"f/lrs. C:lm"rid~c, ""lA: Han'ard University Press.

Schwartz, R. D. (1989). Al~b-Jcwi~h di.lloguc ill thc United SI:lrrl: ·!uward track two t("crabilir)". In L. KsiC5bcrg, T A. NOllhrul" & S, J- Tborseu (Ed.,.), I'llme/llbl"

cOIJj/im lIlId rluir Irllmfimmuinn (Pl'. 180 -1(J9). Syr:lcu>e, NY: SYl":lc\l5e Univer­

IiI)' Press. Srarooto. L (992). voice, pIaLe and proccs>: l':oc-:lrLh un gender, ncgoriation :rnd

conflict resolution. "'hdinlioll Q"(1rrfrly 9.37i-3.':16. Stein, J. G. (1991). Dct"rrcncc and reassuraucc. I" P E. Terlock, J. L. Husbands, R.

Page 39: Conflict Analysis and Resolution - Harvard University · perpetuation. This uunlysis of international conflict has clear implications for our approach to conflirr resolution. The

,/

Conflict Analysis and Resolution 353

Jervis. P. eStern, & c. Tilly [Eds.). lIr1mvior, ,uricry, ,,,,d ,,,,rlmr ,..lIr (VoL 2,

pp. 8-72). New York: Oxford Univerviry Pre'." Taylor. A.• & Miller. J. B. [Eds.) (l9\}~). Conflia a"d "mdtr. Cresskill. NJ; Hampton

Pre51'. Touval, S. (1989). Mulril:ncr:tl negotiation: An Jnalylil approach. Nrglllinr;o/l jVlmm{.

5.159-173. Touval, S., & Zartman, J. W. (Eds.). (l98S). IJllrmllliPllllllllrdi",i'N/ hi ,liro? nnd

prllc/ice. Boulder, CO: \,\Icsrvirw Pren. Volkan, V. D. (1991). Psychological procc51c, in unofficial diplomlg: rneerings. In

V. D. Volkan, J. V. MOllrvillc, & D. A. [uhus (Eds.), Tlit pfJrhodJ'Iamin of ;1/­IlnUlfiollol ff'ltfionr!Jip,. Vol. 2: Unofficinl diplollli/ry ill work (pr. 207-222). Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.

Vclkan, V. D., & Harris. M. (199.~). VaccinJrins the poliriral plOcen: A second P1Y­chopolilic:tl analysis of relationships berween Russia and the Bakic Scares. ,\find

a"d HI/TIl/III "nuaCliOll, 4(tJ}, 169-190. Walton, R. E., & Mclcersie. R_ B. (l96S). II brlli/vior'll tl)(~r)" off.,lhor Ilrgotillliom.

\ New York: McGraw-Hill. Weinstein, E. A.. & Dcurschherger, P. (1%3). Some dimensions of Jltcrcasling. Sori­

ometry. 26, 4SS---466. Whire. R. K. (196S). Images in rhe COnlc'XI of inrernalional (onfliCt: Sovice percep­

tions of (he U.S. and rhe U.S.S.H.. I" H. C Kelman (Ed.), IntrnlluionallulullJ­

ior: A social-psJ<"hologiwll/lwlyJiJ (pp. 238-276). New York: Hair, Rinehart and

WinslOn. Whire, R. K. (19?4). FCilr}id wilrTiMI: A pIJchologi"nl profile of U.S.-Souia relastans.

New York: Free Press, Z:mnlan. I. \VI., & B~rn1an, M, R. (1982). ]1Jr pmerical nrgoriillor. New Haven: Yale

Universiry Press.