aronson 6e ch4_perception

73
Social Psychology Elliot Aronson University of California, Santa Cruz Timothy D. Wilson University of Virginia Robin M. Akert Wellesley College 6th edition

Upload: mrkramek

Post on 16-Jan-2015

141 views

Category:

Education


3 download

DESCRIPTION

Social Psychology Perception

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Social Psychology

Elliot AronsonUniversity of California, Santa Cruz

Timothy D. WilsonUniversity of Virginia

Robin M. AkertWellesley College

slides by Travis LangleyHenderson State University

6th edition

Page 2: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Chapter 4Social Perception:How We Come to

Understand Other People

“Things are seldom as they seem. Skim milk masquerades as cream.”

– W. S. Gilbert

Page 3: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Other people are not easy to figure out. Why are they the way they are? Why do they do what they do?

We all have a fundamental fascination with explaining other people’s behavior, but all we have to go on is observable behavior: – What people do– What they say– Facial expressions– Gestures– Tone of voice

We can’t know, truly and completely, who they are and what they mean.

Instead, we rely on our impressions and personal theories, putting them together as well as we can, hoping they will lead to reasonably accurate and useful conclusions.

Source of image: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 4: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Social PerceptionSocial PerceptionThe study of how

we form impressions of and make inferences about other people.

Source of image: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 5: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Nonverbal Behavior • What do we know about people

when we first meet them? • We know what we can see and

hear, and even though we know we should not judge a book by its cover, this kind of easily observable information is crucial to our first impression.

• With no words at all, we can communicate volumes.

Source of image: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 6: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Nonverbal Behavior Nonverbal CommunicationThe way in which people

communicate, intentionally or unintentionally, without words.

Nonverbal cues include:• facial expressions• tone of voice• gestures

• body position/movement• the use of touch• gaze

Page 7: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Nonverbal Behavior• We have a special kind of brain cell

called mirror neurons. • These neurons respond when we

perform an action and when we see someone else perform the same action.

• Mirror neurons appear to be the basis of our ability to feel empathy.

• For example, when we see someone crying, these mirror neurons fire automatically and involuntarily, just as if we were crying ourselves.

Source of image: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 8: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Nonverbal BehaviorNonverbal cues serve many functions

in communication. • You can express “I’m angry” by

narrowing your eyes, lowering your eyebrows, and setting your mouth in a thin, straight line.

• You can convey the attitude “I like you” with smiles and extended eye contact.

• And you communicate your personality traits, like being an extrovert, with broad gestures and frequent changes in voice pitch and inflection.

Page 9: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Nonverbal BehaviorSome nonverbal cues actually

contradict the spoken words. • Communicating sarcasm is the

classic example of verbal-nonverbal contradiction.

• Think about how you’d say “I’m so happy for you” sarcastically.

Page 10: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Facial Expressions of EmotionAre facial expressions of emotion universal?

The answer is yes, for the six major emotional expressions: anger, happiness, surprise, fear, disgust, and sadness.

All humans encode or express these emotions in the same way, and all humans can decode or interpret them with equal accuracy.

Source of images: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 11: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Facial Expressions of Emotion

• Paul Ekman and others have conducted numerous studies indicating that the ability to interpret at least the six major emotions is cross-cultural—part of being human and not a product of people’s cultural experience.

Source of images: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 12: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Facial Expressions of Emotion

• Other emotions such as guilt, shame, embarrassment, and pride occur later in human development and show less universality.

• These latter emotions are closely tied to social interaction.

Source of images: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 13: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Facial Expressions of Emotion

Decoding facial expressions accurately is more complicated than we have indicated, for three reasons.

1. Affect blends occur when one part of the face registers one emotion and another part, a different emotion.

2. At times people try to appear less emotional than they are so that no one will know how they really feel.

3. A third reason why decoding facial expressions can be inaccurate has to do with culture.

Page 14: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Culture and the Channels of Nonverbal Communication

Display rules are particular to each culture and dictate what kinds of emotional expressions people are supposed to show.

Source of images: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 15: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Culture and the Channels of Nonverbal Communication

Examples of display rule differences:• American cultural norms discourage

emotional displays in men, such as grief or crying, but allow the facial display of such emotions in women.

• Japanese women will often hide a wide smile behind their hands, whereas Western women are allowed—indeed, encouraged—to smile broadly and often.

• Japanese norms lead people to cover up negative facial expressions with smiles and laughter and to display fewer facial expressions in general than is true in the West.

Source of image: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 16: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Culture and the Channels of Nonverbal Communication

• Members of American culture become suspicious when a person doesn’t “look them in the eye” while speaking, and they find talking to someone who is wearing dark sunglasses quite disconcerting.

• Cultures vary greatly in what is considered normative use of personal space. Most Americans like to have a bubble of open space, a few feet in radius, surrounding them; in comparison, in some other cultures, strangers think nothing of standing right next to each other, to the point of touching.

Page 17: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Culture and the Channels of Nonverbal Communication

The important point about emblems is that they are not universal.Each culture has devised its own emblems, and these need not be

understandable to people from other cultures.

EmblemsNonverbal gestures that have well-understood definitions within a given culture; they usually have direct verbal translations, like the “OK” sign.

President George H. W. Bush once used the “V for victory” sign, but he did it backward—the palm of his hand was facing him instead of the audience. Unfortunately, he flashed this gesture to a large crowd in Australia—and in Australia, this emblem is the equivalent of “flipping the bird”!

Source of image: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 18: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Multichannel Nonverbal Communication

• Because nonverbal information is diffused across these many channels, we can often rely on one channel to understand what is going on.

• This increases our ability to make accurate judgments about others.

Page 19: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Multichannel Nonverbal Communication

• Except for certain specific situations, such as talking on the telephone, everyday life is made up of multichannel nonverbal social interaction.

• Typically, many nonverbal cues are available to us when we talk to or observe other people.

• How do we use this information? • And how accurately do we use it?

Page 20: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Gender and Nonverbal Communication

In general, women are better at encoding and decoding nonverbal cues.

Source of images: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 21: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Gender and Nonverbal Communication

One exception is that women are less accurate at detecting deception.

Source of images: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 22: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Gender and Nonverbal Communication

Social role theory of sex differences suggests that this is because women have learned different skills, and one is to be polite and overlook lying.

Source of images: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 23: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Gender and Nonverbal Communication

According to Alice Eagly’s social role theory, most societies have a division of labor based on gender:

• Men work in jobs outside the home.

Source of images: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 24: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Gender and Nonverbal Communication

According to Alice Eagly’s social role theory, most societies have a division of labor based on gender:

• Men work in jobs outside the home.• Women work within the home.

Source of images: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 25: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Gender and Nonverbal Communication

This division of labor has important consequences.

• First, gender-role expectations arise: Members of the society expect men and women to have certain attributes that are consistent with their role. Thus women are expected to be more nurturing, friendly, expressive, and sensitive than men because of their primary role as caregivers to children and elderly family members.

Page 26: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Gender and Nonverbal Communication

This division of labor has important consequences.

• Second, men and women develop different sets of skills and attitudes, based on their experiences in their gender roles.

Source of images: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 27: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Gender and Nonverbal Communication

This division of labor has important consequences.

• Second, men and women develop different sets of skills and attitudes, based on their experiences in their gender roles.

• Finally, because women are less powerful in many societies and less likely to occupy roles of higher status, it becomes more important for women to learn to be accommodating and polite than it is for men.

Page 28: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Implicit Personality Theories: Filling in the Blanks.

• To understand other people, we observe their behavior but we also infer their feelings, traits, and motives.

• To do so, we use general notions or schemas about which personality traits go together in one person.

Page 29: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Implicit Personality Theories: Filling in the Blanks.

Implicit Personality TheoryA type of schema people use to

group various kinds of personality traits together; for example, many people believe that someone who is kind is generous as well.

• If someone is kind, our implicit personality theory tells us he or she is probably generous as well.

• Similarly, we assume that a stingy person is also irritable.

Page 30: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Implicit Personality Theories: Filling in the Blanks.

But relying on schemas can also lead us astray.

• We might make the wrong assumptions about an individual.

• We might even resort to stereotypical thinking, where our schema, or stereotype, leads us to believe that the individual is like all the other members of his or her group.

Page 31: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Culture and Implicit Personality Theories

These general notions, or schemas, are shared by people in a culture, and are passed from one generation to another.

Source of images: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 32: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Culture and Implicit Personality Theories

A strong implicit personality theory in this culture involves physical attractiveness. We presume that “what is beautiful is good”—that people with physical beauty will also have a whole host of other wonderful qualities.

Source of image: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 33: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Culture and Implicit Personality Theories

In China, an implicit personality theory describes a person who embodies traditional Chinese values: creating and maintaining interpersonal harmony, inner harmony, and ren qin (a focus on relationships).

Source of image: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 34: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Culture and Implicit Personality Theories

In Western cultures, saying someone has an “artistic personality” implies that the person is creative, intense, and temperamental and has an unconventional lifestyle.

The Chinese, however, do not have a schema or implicit personality theory for an artistic type.

Source of image: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 35: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Culture and Implicit Personality Theories

Conversely, in China, there are categories of personality that do not exist in Western cultures.

For example, a shi gú person is someone who is worldly, devoted to his or her family, socially skillful, and somewhat reserved.

Source of image: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 36: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Causal Attribution: Answering the “Why” Question

If an acquaintance says, “It’s great to see you!” does she really mean it?

The point is that even though nonverbal communication is sometimes easy to decode and our implicit personality theories can streamline the way we form impressions, there is still substantial ambiguity as to what a person’s behavior really means.

• Perhaps she is acting more thrilled than she really feels, out of politeness.

• Perhaps she is outright lying and really can’t stand you.

Source of image: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 37: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Causal Attribution: Answering the “Why” Question

According to attribution theory, we try to determine why people do what they do in order to uncover the feelings and traits that are behind their actions.

This helps us understand and predict our social world.

Page 38: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

The Nature of the Attribution ProcessFritz Heider (1958) is frequently referred to

as the father of attribution theory. Heider discussed what he called “naive” or

“commonsense” psychology. In his view, people were like amateur

scientists, trying to understand other people’s behavior by piecing together information until they arrived at a reasonable explanation or cause.

Heider was intrigued by what seemed reasonable to people and by how they arrived at their conclusions.

Page 39: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

The Nature of the Attribution Process

When trying to decide what causes people’s behavior, we can make one of two attributions:

• An internal, dispositional attribution or

• An external, situational attribution.

Page 40: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Internal AttributionThe inference that a person is

behaving in a certain way because of something about the person, such as attitude, character, or personality.

External AttributionThe inference that a person is

behaving a certain way because of something about the situation he or she is in.

The assumption is that most people would respond the same way in that situation.

Page 41: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

The Nature of the Attribution ProcessSatisfied spouses tend to show one pattern:• Internal attributions for their partners’ positive

behaviors (e.g., “She helped me because she’s such a generous person”).

• External attributions for their partners’ negative behaviors (e.g., “He said something mean because he’s so stressed at work this week”).

In contrast, spouses in distressed marriages tend to display the opposite pattern:

• Their partners’ positive behaviors are chalked up to external causes (e.g., “She helped me because she wanted to impress our friends”).

• Negative behaviors are attributed to internal causes (e.g., “He said something mean because he’s a totally self-centered jerk”).

Page 42: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

The Nature of the Attribution Process

Although either type of attribution is always possible, Heider (1958) noted that we tend to see the causes of a person’s behavior as residing in that person (internal explanation).

• We are perceptually focused on people—they are who we notice.

• The situation (the external explanation), which is often hard to see and hard to describe, may be overlooked.

Page 43: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

The Covariation Model: Internal versus External Attributions

Harold Kelley’s major contribution to attribution theory was the idea that we notice and think about more than one piece of information when we form an impression of another person.Covariation Model

A theory that states that to form an attribution about what caused a person’s behavior, we systematically note the pattern between the presence or absence of possible causal factors and whether or not the behavior occurs.

Page 44: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

The Covariation Model: Internal versus External Attributions

The covariation model focuses on observations of behavior across time, place, actors, and targets.

It examines how the perceiver chooses either an internal or an external attribution.

We make such choices by using information on:

• Consensus,• Distinctiveness,• Consistency.

Page 45: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Consensus InformationInformation about the extent to which

other people behave the same way toward the same stimulus as the actor does.

Distinctiveness InformationInformation about the extent to which

one particular actor behaves in the same way to different stimuli.

Consistency InformationInformation about the extent to which

the behavior between one actor and one stimulus is the same across time and circumstances.

Page 46: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

The Correspondence Bias: People as Personality PsychologistsSpecific errors or biases plague the

attribution process. One common shortcut is the

correspondence bias:the tendency to believe that people’s behavior matches (corresponds to) their dispositions.

Page 47: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

The Correspondence Bias: People as Personality Psychologists

• The pervasive, fundamental theory or schema most of us have about human behavior is that people do what they do because of the kind of people they are, not because of the situation they are in.

• When thinking this way, we are more like personality psychologists, who see behavior as stemming from internal dispositions and traits, than like social psychologists, who focus on the impact of social situations on behavior.

Page 48: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

The Correspondence Bias: People as Personality PsychologistsThe correspondence bias is so

pervasive that many social psychologists call it the fundamental attribution error.

Source of image: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 49: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

The Correspondence Bias: People as Personality Psychologists One reason is that when we try to explain someone’s

behavior, our focus of attention is usually on the person, not on the surrounding situation.

If we don’t know someone made a F earlier in the day, we can’t use that situational information to help us understand her current behavior.

And even when we know her situation, we still don’t know how she interprets it.

The F may not have upset her if she’s planning to drop the course anyway.

Source of image: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 50: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

The Correspondence Bias: People as Personality Psychologists We can’t see the situation, so we ignore its

importance.

People, not the situation, have perceptual salience for us.

We pay attention to them, and we tend to think that they alone cause their behavior.

Perceptual SalienceThe seeming importance of information that is

the focus of people’s attention.

Source of image: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 51: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

The Correspondence Bias: People as Personality Psychologists The culprit is one of the mental shortcuts we

discussed in Chapter 3: the anchoring and adjustment heuristic.

The correspondence bias is another byproduct of this shortcut.

When making attributions, people use the focus of their attention as a starting point.

Source of image: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 52: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

The Two-Step Process

In sum, we go through a two-step process when we make attributions.

1. First, we make an internal attribution; we assume that a person’s behavior was due to something about that person.

2. Then we attempt to adjust this attribution by considering the situation the person was in. But we often don’t make enough of an adjustment in this second step.

Page 53: Aronson 6e ch4_perception
Page 54: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

The Two-Step Process

In sum, we go through a two-step process when we make attributions.

1. First, we make an internal attribution; we assume that a person’s behavior was due to something about that person.

2. Then we attempt to adjust this attribution by considering the situation the person was in. But we often don’t make enough of an adjustment in this second step.

Why? Because the first step occurs quickly and spontaneously whereas the second step requires more effort and conscious attention.

Page 55: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

The Two-Step Process

We will engage in the second step of attributional processing if we

• Consciously slow down and think carefully before reaching a judgment,

• Are motivated to reach as accurate a judgment as possible, or

• Are suspicious about the behavior of the target person (e.g., we suspect lying).

Page 56: Aronson 6e ch4_perception
Page 57: Aronson 6e ch4_perception
Page 58: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Culture and the Correspondence Bias

For decades, it was taken for granted that the correspondence bias was universal:

People everywhere, we thought, applied this cognitive shortcut when forming attributions.

Source of image: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 59: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Culture and the Correspondence Bias

People from individualistic and collectivistic cultures both demonstrate the correspondence bias.

Members of collectivist cultures are more sensitive to situational causes of behavior and more likely to rely on situational explanations, as long as situational variables are salient.

Page 60: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Culture and the Correspondence Bias

North American and some other Western cultures stress individual autonomy. A person is perceived as independent and self-contained; his or her behavior reflects internal traits, motives, and values.

In contrast, East Asian cultures such as those in China, Japan, and Korea stress group autonomy. The individual derives his or her sense of self from the social group to which he or she belongs.

Page 61: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Culture and the Correspondence Bias

It would be a mistake to think that members of collectivist cultures don’t make dispositional attributions.

They do—it’s just a matter of degree.

Source of image: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 62: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

The Actor/Observer Difference

The actor-observer difference is an amplification of the correspondence bias:

We tend to see other people’s behavior as dispositionally caused, while we are more likely to see our own behavior as situationally caused.

The effect occurs because perceptual salience and information availability differ for the actor and the observer.

Source of image: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 63: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

The Actor/Observer DifferenceActors have more information about

themselves than observers do. Actors know how they’ve behaved over

the years; they know what happened to them that morning.

They are far more aware than observers are of both the similarities and the differences in their behavior over time and across situations terms, actors have far more consistency and distinctiveness information about themselves than observers do.

Page 64: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Self-Serving AttributionsSelf-Serving AttributionsExplanations for one’s successes that

credit internal, dispositional factors and explanations for one’s failures that blame external, situational factors.

Defensive AttributionsExplanations for behavior that avoid

feelings of vulnerability and mortality.

Page 65: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Self-Serving AttributionsWhy do we make self-serving attributions? 1. Most people try to maintain their self-

esteem whenever possible, even if that means distorting reality by changing a thought or belief.

• We are particularly likely to engage in self-serving attributions when we fail at something and we feel we can’t improve at it.

• The external attribution truly protects our self-esteem, as there is little hope we can do better in the future.

• But if we believe we can improve, we’re more likely to attribute our current failure to internal causes and then work on improving.

Page 66: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Self-Serving AttributionsWhy do we make self-serving attributions? 1. Most people try to maintain their self-

esteem whenever possible, even if that means distorting reality by changing a thought or belief.

2. We want people to think well of us and to admire us. Telling others that our poor performance was due to some external cause puts a “good face” on failure; many people call this strategy “making excuses.”

Page 67: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Self-Serving AttributionsWhy do we make self-serving attributions?

1. Most people try to maintain their self-esteem whenever possible, even if that means distorting reality by changing a thought or belief.

2. We want people to think well of us and to admire us. Telling others that our poor performance was due to some external cause puts a “good face” on failure; many people call this strategy “making excuses.”

3. We know more about our own efforts than we do about other people’s.

Page 68: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Self-Serving Attributions• One form of defensive attribution is to

believe that bad things happen only to bad people or at least, only to people who make stupid mistakes or poor choices.

• Therefore, bad things won’t happen to us because we won’t be that stupid or careless.

• Melvin Lerner called this the belief in a just world—the assumption that people get what they deserve and deserve what they get.

Source of image: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 69: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Self-Serving AttributionsThe just world belief has unfortunate

consequences:• Victims of crimes or accidents are often

seen as causing their own fate.• People tend to believe that rape victims

are to blame for the rape.• Battered wives are often seen as

responsible for their abusive husbands’ actions.

Source of image: Microsoft Office Online.

Page 70: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Culture and Other Attributional Biases

There is some evidence for cross-cultural differences in the Actor-Observer Effect and in Self-Serving and Defensive Attributions.

Typically, the difference occurs between Western, individualistic cultures and Eastern, collectivistic cultures.

Page 71: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

How Accurate Are Our Attributions and Impressions?

Our impressions are sometimes wrong because of the mental shortcuts we use when forming social judgments.

To improve the accuracy of your attributions, remember that the mental shortcuts we use, such as the correspondence bias, can lead us to the wrong conclusions sometimes.

Even with such biases operating, we are quite accurate perceivers of other people.

We do very well most of the time.In fact, most of us are more accurate than we

realize.

Page 72: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

How Accurate Are Our Attributions and Impressions?

Our impressions are sometimes wrong because of the mental shortcuts we use when forming social judgments.

To improve the accuracy of your attributions, remember that the mental shortcuts we use, such as the correspondence bias, can lead us to the wrong conclusions sometimes.

Even with such biases operating, we are quite accurate perceivers of other people.

We do very well most of the time.In fact, most of us are more accurate than we

realize.

In short, we are capable of making both stunningly accurate assessments of people and horrific attributional mistakes.

Page 73: Aronson 6e ch4_perception

Social Psychology

Elliot AronsonUniversity of California, Santa Cruz

Timothy D. WilsonUniversity of Virginia

Robin M. AkertWellesley College

slides by Travis LangleyHenderson State University

6th edition