acustica1985 marshall&meyer

Post on 26-Feb-2018

217 Views

Category:

Documents

0 Downloads

Preview:

Click to see full reader

TRANSCRIPT

  • 7/25/2019 Acustica1985 Marshall&Meyer

    1/11

    The

    Directivity

    and

    Auditory

    Impressions

    f

    Singers

    by A. H. Marshall * and J. MeYer

    *

    University

    of Auckland,

    New Zealand

    Information

    from

    the Physikalisch-Technische

    Bundesanstalt,

    Braunschweig

    Dedicated

    to Prof.

    Dr.-Ing.

    L. Cremer

    on the occasion

    of

    his 80th

    birthday

    S

    u m m a r y

    The

    directivity

    of the

    professional singers

    voice

    was measured

    in

    anechoic

    conditions

    for a

    male

    (Baritone) ind

    two iemales

    (Soprano

    and

    Alto). In

    each

    case he

    range

    of

    notes

    sung

    was

    t;;.

    and'comprised

    3

    vowels

    and two

    vocal styles.

    Resllts_are

    given

    at

    20o

    intervals

    in

    to.irontut

    and

    vertical

    planes

    down

    to

    40o depression

    below

    the

    singers,mouth.

    Particular

    atten-

    iio"

    ir

    giu"n to the

    'singer's

    formant'

    and con-clusions

    re drawn

    regarding

    the

    important

    direc-

    tions

    for

    reflecting

    surfaces.

    tn

    ttre

    second

    art

    of the

    paper

    experiments

    are

    described

    which explore

    the

    auditory

    impres-

    sion ofsingers in vocal ensemblesand as soloistsby exposingthe singers o qynthetic sound fields

    in

    tremi-aiectroic

    conditions.

    The

    singer's

    auditory

    impression

    is dominated

    by

    reverberation

    rather

    than

    the early

    reflections

    which

    are so

    important

    to

    instrumentalists.

    An adverse

    com-

    bination

    of discrete

    iarly

    reflections

    and

    reverberation

    occurs

    when

    the reflection

    delay

    approxi-

    mates

    o

    40

    ms.

    Ric

    htc harakteristik

    und Gehiirseindruc

    k beim Sdnger

    Z u s a m m e n f a s s u n g

    In

    einem

    reflexionsarmen

    Raum

    wurde die

    Richtcharakteristik

    von

    professionalen

    Siingem

    ge-

    messen,

    und

    zwar bei

    einer

    Miinnerstimme

    (Bariton)

    und

    zwei

    Frauenstimmen

    (Sopran

    *9

    {t):

    Der

    untersuchte

    Tonumfang

    reichte

    jeweiis

    iiber

    zwei

    Oktaven,

    alle

    TOne

    wurden

    mit

    drei

    Vokalen

    und

    zwei

    verschie&nen

    Stimmtechniken

    gesungen.Die Ergebnisse

    werden

    mit

    einer

    eunOsung

    von 20o

    in der

    horizontalen

    sowie

    zwei

    vertikalen

    Ebenen

    bis zu

    einem

    Neigungs-

    winkel

    v6n

    40o unter

    dem

    Mund des

    Siingers

    angegeben.

    Besondere

    Aufmerksamkeit

    ist

    dabei

    dem

    sog.

    ,,siingerformanten"

    gewidmet, und

    es

    werden Riickschliisse

    auf

    die fiir Reflexions-

    fl ichen wichtigen Abstrahlrichtungen gezogen.

    Im

    zweiten-Teil

    der Arbeit

    wird tibir einige

    Experimente

    berichtet,

    die der

    Untersuchung

    des

    Gehiirseindrucks

    von Chorsingern

    und

    Gesangssolisten

    gelten;

    dabei

    wurden

    die

    raumakusti-

    r.tr.n

    S"aingungen

    fiir

    die Siin-ger

    n einem

    Halbfreifeldraum

    durch

    ein synthetisches

    Schallfeld

    simuliert.

    Dir Gehtirseindruck-der

    Siinger

    wird

    mehr durch

    Nachhall

    geprbgt

    als

    durch

    die

    ersten

    Reflexionen,

    wlhrend diese

    bei

    Instrumentalspielern

    vorrangige

    Bedeutung

    besitzen.

    Ein

    ungiinstiger

    Bereich

    fiir die erste

    Reflexion

    (bei

    Schallfeldern

    mit Nachhall)

    liegt

    bei

    Verziige-

    rungszeiten

    um

    40 ms.

    Directiviti

    et impressions

    auditives

    des chanteurs

    S o m

    m a i

    e

    On

    mesure

    dans

    des conditions

    anchoiques

    a directivit6

    de la

    voix de chanteurs

    profession-

    nels:

    un

    homme

    (baryton), et deux femmes

    (soprano

    et

    alto). Chacun

    chante

    des

    notes

    reparties

    sur

    deux

    octaves,

    en

    pronongant

    3

    voyelles

    selon

    deux

    styles de

    voix

    diffrents.

    Les rsultats

    sont

    donn6s

    ous

    les

    20' dins

    les

    plans

    horizontaux

    et

    verticaux,

    en

    descendant

    usqu'A

    {O

    "

    en dessous

    de la bouche du chanteur. Le

  • 7/25/2019 Acustica1985 Marshall&Meyer

    2/11

    reflection

    level

    and delay,

    the

    frequency

    dependence

    of instrument

    directivity,

    and identify

    the

    frequency

    region

    of

    particular

    importance

    for on-stage

    com-

    munication

    between

    musicians.

    Amongst

    other

    results it is clear that reverberation does not con-

    tribute

    significantly

    to

    ease

    of ensemble

    or instru-

    mentalists

    but

    that

    appropriate

    early reflections

    are

    essential.

    The

    present

    work

    is

    intended

    to

    provide

    similar

    information

    for the

    singer's

    environment,

    or at

    least

    to

    address

    he

    questions

    of

    singing

    comfort

    and

    ease

    of

    ensemble.

    It is

    a

    common

    experience

    amongst

    singers that

    rooms

    differ

    markedly

    in the

    support they

    provide.

    In some

    here

    is a stimulating

    feedbackwhich

    links

    singer, listener

    and space

    and

    promotes

    artistic

    communication.

    In others

    control of the

    voice

    and

    the achievementof ensemble s much more difficult

    though

    of

    course,

    as the following

    experience

    llus-

    trates,

    the

    professional

    singer

    can still

    sing

    even in

    such

    extreme

    environments

    as anechoic

    conditions.

    For

    amateur

    choirs

    "difficult"

    rooms

    may

    affect

    performance

    acutely.

    The

    objective

    in

    designing

    stages

    and rooms

    is

    to

    provide

    an acoustical

    environment

    for the

    singers

    n

    which

    the

    complexity

    of tone

    and

    timbre,

    musical

    structure,

    expressive

    uances,

    nsemble

    and

    the like

    are achieved

    effortlessly in

    the

    act of artistic

    com-

    munication

    between

    performer

    and listener.

    Singers

    usually refer

    to

    such acoustical

    conditions

    as'room

    resonance'.

    'Room

    resonance'

    can be

    usefully considered

    as

    modal

    effects,

    'early'

    reflections

    from

    individual

    surfaces,

    and

    reverberation.

    It is assumed

    in

    this

    paper

    that the

    rooms

    concerned

    are too large

    for

    pronounced

    modal

    behaviour

    in the

    frequency

    range

    of the

    singers.

    Reverberation

    comprises

    the

    statistical

    decay of

    sound energy

    level in

    the

    space

    as

    a

    whole.

    The

    early individual

    reflections

    depend

    upon

    the location

    of

    surfaces relative

    to the

    singer

    and the

    directivity

    of the voice

    when

    singing.

    We

    start therefore with

    a set of measurements

    of

    the

    directivity

    of

    the human voice

    comprising

    hree

    vocal ranges, two styles of voice production and

    three

    different

    vowels.

    The

    second

    part

    of

    the

    paper

    addresses

    he

    questions

    of singing

    comfort

    and ease

    of ensemble

    for

    soloists,

    a

    quartette

    and a

    small

    choir in

    a

    variety

    of

    synthetic

    acoustical

    environ-

    ments.

    2.

    Review of the

    literature

    The

    directional

    characteristics

    of the

    human

    voice

    in

    the literature

    [5-8]

    refer

    almost

    exclusively

    to

    measurements

    on speech.

    Most

    frequently

    cited

    A.H MARSHALL and J MEYER:DIRECIIVtt AND AUDITORY IMPRESSIONS

    131

    are the

    results

    of

    Dunn

    and

    Farnsworth

    (1939)

    mea-

    sured

    with

    a microphone

    at

    a distance

    of

    only

    60

    cm.

    These

    show,

    n

    the

    horizontal plane

    a steady

    level

    reduction

    from

    front

    to

    back which

    is

    more

    marked at higher frequencies. In the vertical plane

    rather

    weaker

    secondary

    maxima

    occur

    which

    just

    exceed

    the

    level

    in

    the

    forward

    direction.

    Niese

    (1956)

    obtained

    similar

    results

    (deviations

    up to

    2

    dB) with

    running

    speech

    using

    a

    microphone

    distance

    of 2

    m. It

    appeared

    that with

    some

    test

    persons

    he

    level

    in the

    forward

    direction

    was

    about

    I dB

    below

    the maxima.

    Finally

    it

    is worth

    noting

    that

    Trendelenburg (1929)

    showed

    that

    the

    .reduc-

    tion

    to

    the

    back

    of the

    head

    s

    greater

    han

    is found

    in

    the shadowing

    of

    a

    point

    source

    by

    a spherical

    surface.

    He

    ascribed

    this

    to

    the

    projecting

    effect of

    the

    mouth

    and

    showed

    also

    differences

    with

    altered

    mouth shapes. It follows that one cannot assume

    that

    speech

    directivity

    data are

    applicable

    without

    further question

    to

    the sung

    radiation

    patterns.

    3. The

    directivity

    ofsingers

    The

    measurements

    on

    singers were

    carried out

    in

    an

    anechoic

    room in

    the

    horizontal

    and

    two vertical

    planes.

    The

    singer

    stood

    sufficiently

    high

    to

    permit

    the

    radiation pattern

    to

    be measured

    down to 40o

    below

    the horizontal plane

    at head

    height.

    Measure-

    ments generallywere made at 20o intervalsbut in

    part

    at l0o intervals

    in the

    horizontal

    plane.

    The

    range

    of sung

    notes was

    Baritone

    (G2

    to

    Ga), Alto

    (G3

    to G5), Soprano

    (Ca

    to

    C6). The

    programme

    included

    test

    syllables

    with

    three vowels

    sung n two

    vocal

    styles

    "full

    voice"

    and the

    so called

    "Rand-

    stimme"

    (or

    half voice).

    Evaluation

    was

    made after

    octave

    iltering.

    3.L

    Basic results

    Figs. I

    and 2

    show

    the

    polar

    diagrams for the

    7

    flequency

    bands

    and

    in two

    planes.

    These

    results

    were

    derived

    from the

    "full voice"

    measurement

    on

    the Baritone and values are averagedover the three

    vowels

    and

    all

    pitches

    within

    each

    octave

    band.

    Reference

    level,

    0 dB is

    the level

    in the

    horizontal

    plane

    in

    front

    of the

    singer.

    It should

    be noted

    that

    the

    singer inclined

    his head

    forward

    approximately

    l0o

    to

    achievea relaxedposture.

    In the horizontal plane

    it is immediately

    clear

    that

    the

    maximum

    level

    occurs

    at 0o

    only above

    4kHz.

    At lower

    frequencies

    the maxima

    shift

    to the

    sides.

    Up to 500

    Hz the

    effect is relatively

    small and

    is

    comparable

    with

    the

    profile

    indicated

    by Niese. t

    is worth

    noting

    that in

    the

    1000Hz

    band the

    level

  • 7/25/2019 Acustica1985 Marshall&Meyer

    3/11

    Fig. l.

    Directivity of a singer

    n

    the

    horizontal

    plane.

    remains

    above

    the

    reference

    evel at 0o back

    to

    I l5o. The

    greatest

    ncrement of about 3

    dB occurs

    at

    2000H2

    and at about

    40o. To the

    rear. as

    ex-

    pected,

    he shading effect of the

    head is clear and

    increasing

    with

    frequency.

    In the vertical plane the polar diagramsshow a

    sharp maximum at high frequencies,

    and directed

    downwards at 20

    o

    as their

    most conspicous eature.

    The maximum

    value

    is about 4 dB

    in the 2000Hz

    octave.

    This maximum though

    weaker remains ap-

    parent

    at the low frequencies

    (in

    comparison

    with

    the

    results n the horizontal

    plane where

    as

    we

    saw

    the high frequency maxima are directed

    outwards

    at about 45" and virtually disappear below

    l000Hz). Other note-worthy features are the sec-

    ondary maximum

    above and

    to the rear at 1000Hz

    and 2000Hz and the sharp reduction

    n level

    below

    the

    horizontal

    plane

    at the back.

    The intermediate

    vertical plane (not shown in this figure) gives a

    smooth transition

    in level between he

    maxima

    just

    described in the horizontal and

    vertical

    planes

    without

    any other

    noticeable

    secondary

    maxima.

    The

    principal

    directional

    properties

    of the sung

    voice are summarized in Table I which shows the

    direction of maximum radiation, the range from

    this maximum to the

    minimum

    value measuredand

    the angular extent of the 3 dB-down

    region in the

    horizontal and vertical

    planes.

    Fig. 3

    gives

    the 3 dB- and l0 dB-down

    regions

    graphically.

    Note in

    the

    horizontal the doubling

    of

    the 3 dB region

    below

    1000Hz and the

    gap

    at

    2000Hz due to the

    downward

    direction

    of the

    maximum

    at

    this

    frequencyand

    above.The narrow-

    nessof the 4000Hz

    3 dB

    radiation

    pattern

    (+

    35")

    is

    particularly

    significant since

    this is the region of

    the

    so-called

    "singer's

    formant",

    which

    gives

    the

    trained voice its

    carrying

    power

    and brilliance

    [9,

    l0]. At

    8000

    Hz

    the

    pattern

    again

    broadens but

    one

    should note that the radiation in

    this

    band

    com-

    prises

    he

    high

    frequency

    partials

    of the sung

    vowels

    rather

    than the consonantswhich dominate mea-

    surements

    on speech n this octave. Niese's mea-

    surements, or

    example,'onspeech onsonants

    ive

    a

    much narrowerpattern (+ 30').

    The

    pronounced

    downward inclined maximum

    in

    the vertical

    plane

    indicates he

    great

    importance of

    floor reflections to

    singers.

    There is also a markedly

    smaller

    vertical

    dispersion than horizontal

    which

    suggests hat reflectors

    to the

    sides of a singer are

    likely to be rd6re useful

    han

    thoseoverhead.

    Fig.

    4

    gives

    a-colnpariion of the

    intensity in

    the

    three most important

    d{rections,

    referred to the

    radiation

    to the front 6f the singer

    (0").

    Overhead,

    the level'drops about

    I

    dB/Octave

    with increasing

    frequency.

    To

    the

    side the

    level is lower at low fre-

    ig. 2. Directivity

    of a singer n the

    vertical

    plane.

  • 7/25/2019 Acustica1985 Marshall&Meyer

    4/11

    Table .

    Characteristical

    alues

    f the singer's

    irectivity.

    133

    Centre Direction of the

    frequency maximum

    of

    the

    octave

    band

    [Hz]

    AZ*"

    3 dB-down region

    dB

    horizontal

    vertical

    125

    250

    500

    1000

    2000

    4000

    8000

    6 . 0

    7 . 5

    9 . 5

    11

    20.5

    23.5

    3 1 . 5

    +45

    (front)

    +40

    (frOnt)

    } 20

    (side)

    -40

    cFront)

    -20

    (frOnt)

    -20

    (frOnt)

    -20

    (frOnt)

    - 45

    Ec

    + 45

    - 80

    E

    c

    + 80

    - 9 0

    Ec

    + 9 0

    \

    H 5

    Ec

    + H 5

    } 20

    EEE

    } 60

    - 35

    Ec

    +359

    and }

    70

    Ec

    }

    80

    - 50

    E

    c

    + 50

    (-40 )c

    E

    +120

    (-40

    )c

    E

    110

    (-40 )c

    E

    75

    (-40 )c

    E

    60

    - 3 2 c - 8

    - 35 c

    +35

    - 35 c

    +25

    0 ]3dB n

    0 ]10 dB

    Fig.3. Principal

    radiation directio ns of a singe r in the

    different octave

    bands.

    quencies

    but reaches + 2

    dB at 1000

    Hz

    -

    about

    5 dB higher than the level overhead.Apart from the

    pronounced peak

    at 1000Hz,

    the sideways

    level

    remains

    constant at about 3 dB below the frontal

    level

    until the high frequency

    drop-off above

    about

    2000 Hz. By

    comparison the level to the rear falls

    steadily at about

    2.5 dB/Octave. The

    peak

    at

    1000

    Hz may be ascribed to the

    diffraction

    around

    the singer'shead.

    3.2.

    nfluence

    ofvoice

    production

    technique

    Fig.4

    also

    indicates

    the effect of dynamics

    on

    directivity. Results for

    forte

    (full

    voice)

    and

    piano

    (half voice) singing are plotted. Overhead and

    behind the singer he

    curvesare close

    ogether.Pro-

    jection

    to

    the side is markedly weaker with

    piano

    singing

    in the

    frequencies above 1000Hz, the differ-

    ence being about 3 dB in the important region

    for

    vocal

    timbre. That means that

    to the side of

    the

    singer an

    exaggerated dynamic

    can

    be expected

    with

    significant ossoftone colour in

    piano passages.

    Apart from this,

    size and location of the

    principal

    radiation regions

    change

    insignificantly during

    transition

    between "full

    voice"

    and "half

    voice".

    -

    the variation being only in the order of

    5

    o.

    3 . 2 .. C o m p a r i s o n o f m a l e v o i c e s

    a n d f e m a l e v o i c e s

    Analysis

    of results for the

    female

    voices

    showed

    substantive

    agreement n the radiation

    patterns

    with

    those obtained

    for the Baritone

    differences being

    smallest n the

    8000Hz region. The

    only significant

    deviation

    was

    in the 2000 Hz

    octave

    where

    for

    the

    female voices

    the sharp maximum

    downwards

    to

    the front

    (Fig.

    2) almost disappears

    and

    the 3 dB

    region is

    spread o approximately

    30o upwards.

    An

    additional secondary

    maximum occurs for

    female

    voices at 60o upwards

    n both the 2kHz and

    4kHz

    @

    @

    @

    @

    @

    E

    P

    @

    @

    @

    @

    P

    @

    @@

    @

    @

    E

    Q

    P

    E

    B

    C

    B

    R

    B

    v

    B

    O

    v

    v

    O

    O

    ~

    R

    WS

    _

    B_

    \oerrino

    R

    _

    125 ?50 500 1000 2000

    1000Hz 8000

    FreQuency

    Fig.4.

    Sound

    pressure

    evel

    in

    three directions

    referred

    to

    the

    front

    as a function of vocal technique:

    a< forte,

    O-t

    piano.

  • 7/25/2019 Acustica1985 Marshall&Meyer

    5/11

    octaves.

    An

    interesting

    deviation

    occurred

    in the

    1000Hz

    octave

    to

    the side

    where the female

    voices

    showed

    similar

    tendency

    o the

    "half

    voice" of the

    Baritone.

    It is

    quite possible

    however that

    these

    differences

    are

    due as

    much to

    the

    quality

    of

    voice

    production as

    to voice type, since the female test

    persons were far

    less experienced

    than the Baritone

    and

    found

    singing

    in

    anechoic

    conditions

    much

    more difficult.

    3.3.

    Effect

    of dffirent

    vowels

    A similar

    comparison

    was made for the

    three

    vowels o, a,

    e. Differences

    were

    most marked

    to the

    side of

    the singer

    as

    indicated in Fig. 5. Up to

    500

    Hz the radiation

    of all three

    vowels is identical,

    from 500

    to 2000

    Hz

    there is a sharp deviation

    with

    a subsequent

    eduction

    in difference above

    4000 Hz.

    Fig. 6 presents he polar diagrammes for thesethree

    vowels in the

    2000

    Hz octave

    band.

    Note that the

    40o sideways

    maxima

    evident

    with

    "o" and "a"

    are

    missing

    with "e"

    in the horizontal

    plane.

    In the

    vertical

    plane,

    o and

    a radiate strongly forward and

    down

    with "e"

    again

    much weaker.

    In this context

    it is noted that Slavik and Tichy

    [8]

    found

    differences

    in the directivity,

    particularly

    to

    the side, for

    different

    vowels. A

    possible

    explana-

    tion for this

    phenomenon

    could be

    phase

    cancella-

    tion across

    he

    openings similar to that known

    in

    the

    radiation

    from

    airflow

    across

    ipe

    openings l].

    An influence

    of

    pitch

    on

    directivity

    is

    discernible

    only at

    the highest notes

    n each

    of the

    individual

    vocal ranges.

    Above the singer

    these

    tones

    were

    2

    "'

    4 dB

    less attenuated

    than

    the other tones

    and

    this further

    strengthened

    the

    upwards

    secondary

    maxima. The effect was strongest n the top Sopra-

    no c

    in which the upward

    secondary

    maxima

    were

    8 dB stronger

    than the

    middle-

    and low-frequency

    tones.

    To the

    rear

    of

    the singer some

    sound

    components

    are less attenuated

    -

    by

    as much

    as

    4

    "'

    5 dB be-

    tween 500

    to

    1000

    Hz

    with

    "o"

    and "a"

    and from

    500

    to 2000 Hz

    with

    "e".

    At high

    frequencies

    there

    is no difference between

    them.

    Sideways

    however

    the components

    at 4000

    Hz in "a"

    and "e" suffer

    3

    "'

    5

    dB

    greater

    attenuation

    han

    the

    other sounds.

    The consequence

    s that the

    "singer's

    formant"

    is

    less

    prominent

    to the side

    of a singer

    for these

    vowels.

    3.4. Summary

    of conclusions

    We

    can summarize

    the

    results of this

    section as

    follows:

    l) Noticeable differences

    in

    vocal timbre

    will follow

    if the singer

    turns more than

    40" from the

    normal

    position.

    Beyond 80o the

    problem

    will become

    acute. Reflectors

    should be

    designed

    to minimize

    this effect.

    2) The floor reflection

    is a

    particularly

    important

    component of the

    radiation

    pattern

    to the lis-

    teners.The area 2 "'5 m in front of the singer s

    the most

    significant region.

    Carpet

    on this region

    of floor

    is to be avoided.

    3)

    Stage design should exploit

    the side

    reflections

    rather than overhead

    reflections,

    particularly

    with

    surfaces

    within

    an

    angle

    of 60o to the

    view

    direction.

    4) To avoid occlusion

    of one

    row of choristers by

    another

    a steep step between

    rows

    is necessary.

    The rake should be I

    : I

    (45o)

    minimum.

    5) Microphone

    placement

    right and

    left of the

    singer

    gives

    richer high

    harmonics

    than overhead

    but also

    eads to weaker low

    frequencies.

    \

    O

    C

    O

    R

    O

    v

    O

    O

    x

    v

    O

    O

    /

    _

    M

    _

    _

    15

    1 2 5 2 5 0 5 0 0 1 0 0 0 2 0 0 0 4 0 0 0 H 2 0 0 0 0

    Frequency \

    Fig.5,Sound pressure level to the side referred to the front

    For three vowels.

    4. The singer's

    acoustical

    environment

    We describenow the experiments

    imed at

    termining

    preferred

    acoustical

    environments

    singers.

    4.1.

    Procedure

    The singers

    performed

    n hemi-anechoic

    ondi-

    tions it being recognized

    hat

    virtually all stages

    Fig. 6. Directivity

    in the 2000

    Hz octave band

    for

    3

    vowels.

  • 7/25/2019 Acustica1985 Marshall&Meyer

    6/11

    Fig.7. Plan of the hemi-anechoic

    oom showing

    the

    arrangement f the

    vocal

    quartette,

    he microphones nd

    the oudspeakers:

    S

    =

    side

    eflections,

    R

    =

    rear

    eflections,

    C

    =

    ceiling eflections,

    Rev,

    Rev'

    reverberation.

    have.a

    reflective floor. Side, overhead

    and

    rear re-

    flections

    corresponding to a

    variety of

    stage

    sizes

    were simulated with a digital delayline, feeding

    back directly

    to the singers

    via cardioid micro-

    phones

    at

    0.5

    m from each singer's

    mouth, and

    loudspeakers at a

    distance of

    about

    3 m,

    see

    Fig. 7.

    Levels

    were

    calculated

    according

    to spherical

    diver-

    gence

    with some correction

    for vocal directivity.

    Reverberation

    was

    generated

    on the two inco-

    herent

    feeds from an

    EMT Goldfoil

    reverberation

    plate

    and radiated

    to the ensemble

    rom three loud-

    speakers arranged

    to simulate

    the reverberance of

    the auditorium,

    see Fig. 8 a for

    schematic

    synthetic

    sound fields and

    Fig. 8 b for simulated

    stage sizes.

    Table II

    gives

    the delay applied

    through

    the

    delay-

    line plus the delaycausedby the 3 m dimension be-

    tween the singing

    group

    and

    the respective

    loud-

    speakers.

    The music

    chosen consisted

    of unaccompanied

    chorales

    and choruses

    from the J.

    S. Bach "St Mat-

    thew Passion"

    and

    "Elijah"

    by Mendelssohn.

    Ex-

    cerpts approximately

    30

    s long

    were

    sung

    for each

    trial and then the

    musicians

    were

    asked

    to rate the

    conditions

    on

    7-point scales for

    "ease

    of

    singing"

    and

    "ease

    of

    ensemble",

    before

    the

    next

    presenta-

    tion. Fourteen

    presentations

    wer made in

    random

    order in each series.

    A.H.MARSHALL and J.MEYER:DIREC

    YW AND AUDITORY IMPRESS10NS

    ~ m g

    | c A A c

    m u

    135

    @

    Fig.

    8.

    a) Temporal

    structure of

    the test sound

    fields.

    b) Schematic

    of the simulated stage

    plans.Simulation code:

    lst letter:

    ceiling and rear

    reflections,

    2nd letter:

    side reflections.

    Table II.

    Delay of

    reflections and reverberation.

    Code

    AKG delay] Total dday Appro

    mate

    line surfa

    distance

    E

    B

    E

    E

    E

    N

    X

    no reflection

    A 6.25

    ms

    B

    12.5ms

    C

    18.75 s

    D 2 5 m s

    E 31.25

    s

    F 37.5

    ms

    G 5 0

    Rever-

    75

    beration

    16.25 ms

    22.5 ms

    28.75 ms

    35 ms

    41.25 ins

    47.5 ms

    60

    85

    2.75m

    3.82m

    4.89m

    5.95m

    7 . 0 1 m

    8.07m

    10.2 m

    (13 .6o

  • 7/25/2019 Acustica1985 Marshall&Meyer

    7/11

    l, 1.5 and

    3s, The

    onset time

    of the

    reverberated

    signal

    was

    varied

    through

    60, 85 and

    ll0

    ms and for

    asymmetrical stage

    simulations

    (i.e.

    side "walls"

    at

    a different distance rom

    simulated

    rear and over-

    head

    "surfaces")

    preference

    for

    wide or deep

    stages

    wasexplored,seeFig. 8b.

    Fig.9. Photograph

    f

    the vocal

    quartette

    n the anechoic

    room.

    The

    greatest

    number

    of experiments

    were

    per-

    formed by the

    quartette,with the choir of 14

    voices

    and the

    soloists

    each

    available on only one

    occasion.

    To

    maximize

    dependence

    on the "reflected"

    sound

    singers

    faced

    away

    from

    each other on the

    hemi-

    anechoic

    pace.

    See

    Fig.

    9.

    5. General

    observations

    It had b'een

    observed

    in the field that singers,

    both

    solo and

    choral,

    are sensitive

    o

    the

    reverberant

    conditions.

    The

    first

    question

    was, then,

    whether it

    would

    be

    possible o sing

    in

    ensemble

    n anechoic

    conditions at all. To start with, the quartette stood

    back

    to back

    in

    the large anechoic

    room

    at

    PTB. To

    everyone's

    surprise

    singing in ensembleunder

    such

    conditions

    was

    quite possible

    though observers

    noted some

    difficulties

    in intonation. Of

    the four

    subsequent

    occasions

    on

    which

    the

    quartette

    sang,

    in the hemi-anechoic

    oom, the first

    was

    used

    o test

    the consistency

    of

    judgements.

    Consistent

    udge-

    ments

    were

    found

    to be

    possible.

    The results re-

    ported

    here however,

    are derived from the

    last three

    sessions

    nly. The choir

    results

    were

    less clear. Not

    only

    were the singers

    relatively

    inexpert

    (i.e.

    un-

    practiced)

    at

    making the

    experimental

    judgements

    but some, notably the upper parts, proved quite

    unable

    to

    respond

    consistently

    at all. The

    raw

    choir

    results

    were thus

    rather smeared.

    A

    procedure

    to

    purge

    the unreliable

    udgements

    was

    adopted and

    is

    described

    n the Appendix.

    Finally, the results rom

    only

    7

    of the

    14choir

    singers

    were ncluded.

    6.

    Experimental

    variables

    In addition to

    reflection

    delay and level,

    reverbera-

    tion time was

    varied in the

    presentations

    through

    0,

    7. Results

    7.1.

    Correlation

    between ease

    of singing"

    and

    "ease

    of ensemble"

    For

    all

    groups

    here is a high

    correlation

    between

    the

    judgements

    of ease

    of singin

    and ease

    of en-

    semble.

    Correlation coefficients

    lie between

    0.82

    and 0.90

    for all trials. Fig.

    l0 shows

    he dependence

    of the correlation

    coefficients

    on

    the four reverbera-

    tion times presented for the quartette. The correla-

    tion

    was even higher for

    the choir

    (0.92).

    From this

    we conclude hat essentially

    nly

    one

    udgement

    was

    being

    made.

    The only exception

    was a combination

    of a

    long

    reverberation time and unfavoured

    reflection delays.

    In this situation,

    "ease

    of ensemble"

    seemed o be

    little

    worse han "easeof singing".

    7.2.

    Early

    reflections

    for

    the ensembles)

    Although it

    was

    quickly

    appatent

    that the

    princi-

    pal

    conclusion

    to

    be

    drawn from

    the experiments

    concerned the importance of reverberation to

    singers, approximately

    half the

    presntations to the

    subjects

    comprised only

    early

    reflections

    without

    reverberance.

    The results are

    very straight forward

    and

    will be

    presented

    lrrst.

    Fig. I I summarises

    hese

    results. t

    is

    a cumula-

    tive

    plot

    of

    normalised

    preference

    against

    he rela-

    tive

    energy

    of

    early

    reflections

    which as noted above

    0 1 2 s 3

    Reverberoi ion

    ime

    +

    Fig. 10. Correlation coefficient between "ease

    of ensemble"

    and

    "ease

    of singing"

    for

    different

    reverberation times

    presented.

    @

    O

    E

    U

    x

    O

    E

    Q

    O

    P

    E

    E

    X

    E

    E

    E

    E

    O

    O

    b

    O

    O

    v

    v

    O

    \

    .

  • 7/25/2019 Acustica1985 Marshall&Meyer

    8/11

    '

    ( -o )

    -12

    - t 0 -8

    -6

    -1 -?

    dB

    0

    Re lo t i ve nergYev l

    Fig. I

    l. Normalised

    preference (ensemble)

    or the

    quartette

    for reverberation-lree simulated refl ections.

    approximate to

    spherical divergence

    relative

    to the

    strongest reflections

    (situation

    code

    AA:

    0 dB). This

    reference

    evel is about 16dB lower

    than

    the sound

    power

    evel

    of the singer.

    It is

    clear that there

    is

    a simple dependenceof

    preference

    n the

    level

    ofthe early

    reflections.

    7.

    3. Reverberation

    or

    ensembles

    In the sound fields

    presented

    without early reflec-

    tions but with reverberation the reverberation time

    seems o have no systematic effect on

    preference

    n

    the

    range

    of the 3 reverberation imes

    presented 1,

    1.5

    and 3s respectively) o the

    quartette

    and the

    choir.

    7 . 3 . 1 .

    f f e c t o f s o u n d fi e l d s c o n s i s t i n g

    o f e a r l y

    r e f l e c t i o n s a n d

    r e v e r b e r a t i o n f o r t h e e n s e m b l e s

    The most

    striking

    result is the

    generalpreference

    for the sound fields

    with

    reverberation

    for both the

    ensembles. ig. 12 s a typical

    plot

    of results

    or the

    quartette and shows that with the exception of

    situation EE

    (delay

    about 40 ms) the fields with

    reverberationare strongly

    preferred.

    Similar results

    were obtained with the

    choir.

    Fig. 13 shows the

    sharpnessof the

    interaction between the early re-

    flections and the reverberant field by

    plotting

    the

    difference in

    preference

    between the

    reverberated

    and dry reflections. All the

    presentations

    to

    the

    choir and

    quartette

    involving reverberationsettings

    of 1.5s and

    85

    ms delay are included. Some

    of the

    fields

    presented

    were

    "symmetrical"

    while others

    were

    "asymmetrical"

    as shown n Fig. 8 b.

    A.H.MARSHALL and J.MEYER:DIREC

    Y AND AUDmRY IMPRESS10NS

    137

    Both

    for long

    delays

    (and

    corresponding low

    reflection

    levels),

    and

    for short

    delays

    with

    relative-

    ly

    more

    energetic

    early reflections

    the

    improvement

    in

    preference

    for

    the fields

    including reverberation

    is significant and about the same. This improve-

    ment

    disappears

    quite

    sharply

    at

    about

    40 ms delay

    relative

    to the direct.

    The

    effect is so

    strong

    and

    independent

    of

    group

    size

    (choir

    or

    quartette)

    or

    whether

    the

    presentations

    were

    symmetrical or

    EnsemblP

    A

    m _ ' R

    D A

    O

    @

    @

    @

    @

    E

    P

    P

    @

    @

    \

    B

    B

    B

    v B

    E

    B

    v

    v

    O

    O

    n

    d

    v

    O

    Q

    E [

    Fig.

    12. Normalised

    preference

    for the

    quartette

    for sound

    fields with

    (dark

    areas)

    and

    without

    (ight

    areas)

    everber-

    ation component.

    CodelX X GG

    36 CC

    E E D E M0 0 0 6 8 0 B A C C A A A

    ( -o )

    - ' 12 -10

    -8 -6

    -1 -?

    dB 0

    ne lo l i v

    nergyeve l

    Fig.

    13.

    Difference

    in normalised

    preference

    between

    sound

    fields

    with

    and without

    reverberation component as a

    function of relative reflection

    level.

    Reverberation

    ime

    1.5s. onsetdelav

    85

    ms.

    Code: A

    .

    quartette,

    sym.

    Iields,

    o

    quartette,

    asym. ields,

    r choir,

    sym. fields,

    o choir,

    asym. -relds.

    O

    C

    W

    O

    E

    U

    O

    C

    S

    O

    B

    e

    s

    h

    h

    P

    O

    O

    O

    v

    O

    O

    v

    @

    n

    n

    x

    O

    n

    O

    v

    O

    U

  • 7/25/2019 Acustica1985 Marshall&Meyer

    9/11

    A.

    H.

    MARSHALL

    nd J.

    MEYER:

    DIRECTMTY

    AND

    AUDITORY

    MPRESSIONS

    ffiHli:i

    2 d0 0

    O

    E

    W

    O

    C

    U

    O

    C

    S

    O

    E

    Q

    O

    E

    OQ

    P

    B

    B

    B

    v

    B

    E

    B

    v

    O

    B

    B

    Q

    b

    B

    B

    b

    n

    v

    O

    \

    B

    B

    E

    v

    B

    E

    E

    x

    v

    O

    d

    v

    B

    Q

    Fig. 14.Differencen normalised

    reference etween

    ound

    fields

    with

    and

    without reverberation omponent.Rever-

    beration

    ime

    1.0

    s,

    onset

    elay

    85

    ms.

    o

    lst

    series,

    o

    2ndseries.

    asymmetrical

    that

    we

    were convinced

    it

    is

    a real

    effect.

    However

    we addressed

    this

    question further

    by

    varying

    the

    reverberation

    time and

    by

    varying

    the

    onset

    delay

    for

    the

    reverberated

    signal.

    7 . 3 . 2 .

    e v e r b e r a t i o n

    t i m e

    v a r i a t i o n

    Two series were conducted with the quartette

    using

    I s

    reverberation

    time

    instead

    of

    the

    l'5

    s

    adopted

    as standard

    with

    onset

    delay

    85

    ms.

    Mask-

    ing

    of

    the

    early

    reflections

    by

    reverberation

    is

    reduced.

    Early

    energetic

    reflections

    integrate

    with

    the

    reverberation

    so

    that

    preference for

    the

    rever-

    berated

    sound

    is

    negligible.

    Only

    for

    delay

    greater

    than

    about

    40 ms

    is

    the reverberated

    field

    strongly

    preferred

    again.

    See

    Fig.

    14.

    This

    result

    gives a clue

    as

    to

    what

    is happening

    n

    Fig.

    13.

    The

    pronounced

    dip

    occurs

    at

    the

    point where

    dependence

    solely

    on

    the

    reverberated

    signal

    takes

    place

    because

    he

    early

    reflections

    energy

    is as

    low

    that

    it

    is negligible.

    A

    further variation with the quartette used 3.0s re-

    verberation

    time.

    The

    reverberant

    signal at

    3

    s so

    obviously

    dominated

    the

    singing

    experience

    that

    only

    tests

    with

    the

    reverberation

    were run.

    Apart

    from

    a

    slight

    improvement

    in

    preference for

    the

    earliest

    and

    most

    energetic

    discrete

    reflections

    preference

    for

    all

    test

    fields

    was uniform

    within

    the

    experimental

    accuracy.

    From

    this

    series

    we

    conclude

    that the

    shorter

    the

    reverberation

    time,

    the

    more

    important

    the

    earliest

    reflections

    are

    for

    ensemble.

    However

    after

    about

    35

    ms

    of reflection

    delay the

    statistical

    reverberation

    Fig.

    15.

    Normalised

    preference

    s

    a function

    of

    relative

    reflectionevel.Reverberationime l-5 s.

    o

    onset elay

    60

    ms,

    o

    onset elay

    85 ms,

    r

    onset elay

    l0 ms.

    completely

    dominates

    the

    singer's

    perception

    of the

    performance environment,

    irrespective

    of the

    pres-

    ence

    of

    reflections.

    7 . 3 . 3 . n s e t

    d e l a y

    t i m e

    We

    investigated

    the

    possibility

    that

    the

    reduction

    in preference for

    the

    reverberant

    flreld

    at

    40 ms

    could

    have

    been

    an

    artifact.

    The

    reverberation

    signal

    started

    in

    the experimental

    set-up

    at

    a dis-

    crete

    time

    -

    virtually

    like

    a

    reflection

    at

    85

    ms

    delay.

    Accordingly

    wc ran

    a series

    in

    which

    the

    variable

    was the onset

    delay

    time

    for

    the

    reverbera-

    tion.

    The set-up

    did

    not

    permit us

    to include

    the

    unreverberated

    field

    for

    comparison

    as

    would

    have

    given

    us

    directly

    comparable

    results

    with

    those

    already

    shown.

    However

    Fig.

    15 shows

    that

    a

    signilicant

    dip

    in

    preference does

    occur

    with each

    of

    the onset

    delays

    at

    the

    40 ms

    region

    with a

    reduc-

    tion at smaller

    onset delays

    than

    about

    70 ms.

    That

    means, that the 40 ms effect is very important for

    the design

    of single

    reflectors,

    if the

    other

    reflecting

    areas

    have

    distances

    of more

    than

    about

    l2

    m.

    7.4. Soloists

    At the

    conclusion

    of

    the

    measuring

    programme

    described

    in the first

    section

    of this

    paper we asked

    the singers

    each

    to

    undertake

    at

    test

    series

    @ncern-

    ing

    "ease of singing"

    comparable

    to those

    described

    for

    the

    quartette

    and

    choir.

    Fig.

    16

    gives

    the

    initial

    plot of their

    responses

    while

    Fig.

    l7

    gives the

    preferences

    plotted

    against

    reflection

    energy

    in the

    -0,1

    ( - o

    I

    -10

    -0

    -6

    -L

    Relot ive

    nerqy

    evel

    _0) ]

    12 10 ]

    8 ]

    6 -4

  • 7/25/2019 Acustica1985 Marshall&Meyer

    10/11

    Fig. 16. Soloists: Normalised

    preference

    (ease

    of singing)

    for sound fietds

    with

    (dark

    areas) and

    without

    (light

    areas)

    reverberation

    component.

    Reverberation

    time

    1.5s, onset

    delay

    85 ms.

    o

    Baritone.

    ]

    0 ]

    ]

    ]

    Relotive Eneu

    y tevel \

    ACUSTICA

    V 58 (1985)

    (--) -12 -r0 -8 -6 -1 -? d8 0

    nelotivenergy-evel

    Fig. 17.

    Soloists: Differences

    n normalised

    preference

    between

    ound ields

    with

    and

    without reverberationom-

    ponent.

    Reverberation

    ime 1.5

    ,onset elay

    85ms.

    o average,

    r female

    ingers,

    o Baritone.

    presentation.

    Since

    there

    were only 3 subjects the

    Baritone

    is

    plotted

    separately

    from

    the two female

    singers, ogether

    with the average.

    The striking features are the both extreme

    variation in the

    plots

    and unanimity

    between

    voices.

    The

    strongly negative

    response

    o

    "AC" and

    "BG"

    was coupled

    to comment

    from the singers that

    having

    the reverberation

    arriving

    from a distinctly

    different

    direction

    from the early

    sound

    (overhead

    and back)

    was disturbing and

    one should

    not make

    too much of

    what is obviously

    an artifact. As before

    there is a clear

    preference

    for

    reverberation

    which is

    complicated by

    early energetic

    reflections, especial-

    ly if the side

    reflections arrive

    earlier

    than the

    overhead

    eflection.

    Fig. 18. String

    quartette:

    Differences n

    normalised

    pre-

    ferencebetween ound ieldswith and without reverber-

    ationcomponent.

    o Response

    o

    "ease

    f ensemble",

    o

    resporBe

    o

    "tone

    quality".

    7.5. nstrumentalists

    For

    comparison

    with the

    foregoing

    work on

    singers

    we invited a string

    quartette

    to

    play

    in the

    same experimental

    set-up.

    Within the

    obvious

    limitations concerning

    instrument

    directivity

    we

    found

    confirmation that

    the early

    reflections are

    critically

    important for ease

    of ensemble

    while

    re-

    verberation is not. On the other hand the aesthetic

    experience of

    the instrumentalists was

    clearly

    not

    highly correlated

    with ensemble conditions

    as

    was

    the case

    with singers. Responses o a

    question

    about

    ease

    of achieving tonal

    quality

    showed

    a consider-

    able

    dependence on

    the reverberant

    conditions.

    These

    esultsare

    plotted

    in

    Fig. 18.

    8. Conclusions

    In addition

    to measuring

    the directivity

    of the

    sung

    voice over

    3 vocal ranges,

    for 3 different

    vowels and in two vocal styles we have conducted

    experiments

    with vocal

    ensembles

    o establish

    pre-

    ferred acoustical

    conditions

    on stage.

    These experi-

    ments show

    that

    "ease

    of ensemble"

    for singers

    is

    inseparable

    from

    questions

    of singing

    comfort and

    that both

    are controlled

    by the

    reverberant condi

    tions. Energetic

    early reflections do

    contribute

    posi-

    tively

    if

    they

    are early enough but at

    about

    40 ms

    delay

    reduce

    preference

    well below

    that of a reflec-

    tionJess

    reverberant field.

    This result is

    in

    direct

    contrast

    with

    conditions

    preferred

    for

    instrumental

    ensemble n

    which the early reflections are

    essential.

    139

    P

    V

    P

    P

    P

    x

    O

    v

    v

    O

    n

    d

    v

    O

    \

    \

    \

    \

    \

    \

    ]

    n

    n

    v

    O

    O

    v

    v

    u

    n

    b

    v

    O

    Z

    n

    b

    n

    v

    O

    Z

    O

    C

    S

    B

    O

    E

    O

    E

    Q

    E

    O

    C

    S

    V

    PPP

    P

    @

    O

    O

    v

    O

    x

    O

    n

    v

    O

    O

    O

    v

    O

    Z

  • 7/25/2019 Acustica1985 Marshall&Meyer

    11/11

    A c k n o w l e d g e m e n t s

    The

    authors

    are most

    grateful

    to all the singers

    who

    participated

    in the

    work reported in this

    paper,

    particularly

    to

    Professor Claus

    Ocker and Dr.-Ing.

    Detlev Mencke.

    The

    project

    was

    supported

    by

    a

    grant

    from

    the PTB

    for Professor Marshall's

    visit

    and by

    the Research

    nd

    Study eave

    programme

    of

    the University

    of Auckland

    (NZ).

    Appendix

    Evaluation

    of chamber-choir

    esponses

    During

    the analysis

    of

    choir

    members esponses

    t

    became

    apparent

    that their

    individual reliability

    varied considerably.

    (Each

    singer indicated

    vocal

    part

    and

    approximate

    position

    on the

    form.)

    Since

    every

    presentation was repeated at least

    once

    it

    was

    possible o test he individuals reliability and purge

    inconsistent

    esults

    by comparing

    the two responses.

    Ideally

    of course

    each subject

    should have made

    an identical

    response o

    the repeated ield

    when it

    reappeared

    n the series.

    Subjective actors

    such as

    familiarity,

    fatigue,

    improving

    listening

    skills

    and

    confidence

    would

    prevent

    that occurring

    in

    practice

    but

    if there

    is

    no consistent

    change

    in the assess-

    ments reliable

    rank

    ordering

    of the

    results can be

    achieved.

    For

    every test

    subject

    we determined

    the differ-

    ence or eachsound ield. Usually the rangewas ess

    than three

    preference

    steps

    but occasionally

    there

    were as

    many as

    five steps

    difference

    in the assess-

    ment. Fig.

    19 shows

    3 examples.

    Singer

    (l)

    judges

    the

    repetition

    rather better.

    Singer

    (2)

    judges

    the

    first

    programme

    about

    the

    same

    yet

    again

    better on

    the repetition.

    (Perhaps

    t took

    the first three before

    he

    got

    used to

    it.) Singer

    (3)

    has such a broad

    distribution

    of results

    hat

    his responses

    hould be

    purged.

    To test

    reliability

    we counted

    the number of

    repeated

    ests n

    which nearly

    the same

    result or at

    least a

    consistent

    difference

    occurred,

    i.e. we

    summed up the number of tests n that group of

    three bars

    containing

    the

    highest

    number of tests.

    Thesebars

    are

    hatched n

    Fig. 19.

    With 14

    presenta-

    tions in each

    of

    2 series

    we had 28

    possible

    for a

    perfect

    score.

    Results

    ranged from

    14 to

    26. We

    accepted

    results

    of 21 or

    more. ln

    practical

    terms

    that

    meant

    that for

    at

    least

    75o/o f

    judgements

    he

    difference

    must be

    no more

    than

    *

    I step.Only

    7 of

    the

    choir

    of 14 achieved

    his

    result and

    it is their

    responses

    hat

    are

    ncluded

    n the

    paper.

    (Received

    ebruary

    1th, 985.)

    R e f e r e n c e s

    []

    Meyer, J., Akustik und musikalischeAuffiihrungs-

    praxis. VerlagE. Bochinsky,

    rankfurt a.M.,

    2. Auf-

    lage, 980.

    [2]

    Marshall,

    A. H., Gottlob,

    D., and Alrutz, H., Acous-

    tical conditions

    referred

    or ensemble.

    . Acoust.Soc.

    Amer.

    64

    978],

    1437.

    [3]

    Gade,

    A C., Subjective

    oom acousticexperiments

    with

    musicians.

    Acoust.

    Lab.

    Techn. Univ. of

    Den-

    mark,Report

    no.32,

    1982.

    [4]

    Ternstrilm,S.

    and Sundberg,

    .,

    How loudly should

    you

    hear

    your

    collegues

    nd

    yourself?

    Speech rans-

    mission

    Lab.

    Report

    STL-QPSR

    /1983,

    p.

    16,Stock-

    holm 1984.

    [5]

    Trendelenburg,

    ., Beitrag

    zur Frageder Stimmricht-

    wirkung.Z.

    techn.

    Physik

    0

    [

    929],558.

    [6] Dunn, H. K and Farnsworth,D. W., Explorationof

    pressureield around

    he human

    headduring speech.

    J.

    Acoust.

    oc. mer.

    0

    [939],

    184.

    [7]

    Niese,

    H., Untersuchung

    iber

    die

    Knallform bei

    raumakustischen

    mpulsmessungen.

    F u. ELA 65

    ue56l,

    8.

    [8]

    Slavic,

    J. S. and

    Tichy, J., unpublished

    lecture on the

    directivity

    ofspeech.

    Acoust.

    Conf. Budapest

    1964.

    [9]

    Winckel, F., How

    to measure

    the effectiveness of

    stage

    singers

    voices.Folia

    phoniat. 23

    ll97

    ll,

    228.

    01

    Sundberg,

    J., The acoustics

    of the singing

    voice.

    Scient.Amer.

    236

    l977l,

    82.

    Il]

    Schilz,

    W.,

    Richtcharakteristik

    der Schallabstrahlung

    einer durchstrcimten

    Offnung.

    Acustica

    17

    119661,364.

    Progromme

    J

    O

    v

    O

    Z

    O

    n

    O

    v

    n

    R

    Q

    Progromme

    | 1

    q

    1

    Proqromme

    ]

    3 ]2 0 1 2 3

    D i f i e r e n c e i n t h e i n d i v d u o l us u l l s

    Fig. 19. Reliability of

    repeated tests:

    number of repeated

    testshaving the same

    difference

    in the individual results.

    C

    O

    X

    d

    u

    u

    C

    O

    N

    V

    u

top related