acoustic transitions in khmer word-initial clustersjkirby/docs/kirby2014transitions-poster.pdf ·...

1
Acoustic transitions in Khmer word-initial clusters James Kirby Linguistics and English Language, University of Edinburgh, Scotland, UK [email protected] Introduction P HONOLOGICAL status of interconsonantal acoustic transitions () known to differ across languages [3, 7, 9, 10] Khmer (Cambodian) has an inventory of 70+ CC onset clusters C 1 C 2 p t tS k P b d m n ñ N l r s h p pt ptS pk pP pd pn pN pl pr ps ph t tp tk tP tb tm tn tN tl tr th tS tSp tSk tSP tSb tSd tSm tSn tSN tSl tSh k kp kt ktS kP kb kd km kn kñ kN kl kr ks kh s sp st sk sP sb sd sm sn sN sl sr m mt mtS mP md mn ml mr ms mh l lp lk lP lb lm lN lh Huffman [6]: clusters can be organised into three classes based on the type of acoustic transition that separates them Class 1 no transition /psaa/ > [psaa] Class 2 voiceless transition /pteah/ > [p h teah] Class 3 voiced transition /pd@j/ > [p @ d@j] What is the phonological status of in Khmer? One possibility: in Khmer result from gestural underlap [1] Primary evidence: duration of lexical C ˘ VC monosyllables > CC clusters; has more variable formant structure than ˘ V Alternatively: whatever is, it’s just different from ˘ V, but still has an associated phonological target One diagnostic of (phonetic) excrescence: disappearance of in fast speech [5] What happens to at different speech rates? Methods 0 8000 !"#ក ‘peacock’ k ə ŋ aok Time (s) 1.026 1.489 Frequency (Hz) 10 native speakers (3 female) recorded producing 79 items × 3 repetitions × 2 rates List included 61 C 1 C 2 V(C) items (35 cluster types) + 8 C@C monosyllables All items in carrier phrase /kñom tha: ti@t/ ‘I say again’ Results: Frequency of transition 0.00 0.25 0.50 0.75 1.00 r s h p t k n ŋ l c2 freq of V occurrence condition slow fast Class 1 /Cs Ch/: no intrusion; on /Cr/: [8] Class 2: transitions are more frequent at faster speech rates! 0.00 0.25 0.50 0.75 1.00 p t b d n ŋ l r s h c2 freq of V occurrence Class 3: transitions often less frequent in fast speech, but never disappear completely Results: Cluster duration & lexical C@C Class 1 Class 2 Class 3 0 50 100 150 200 slow fast slow fast slow fast condition mean cluster length (ms) hasV N Y Slow: CC CC (β = 13.5, SE = 2.8, p< 0.001) Fast: weak trend (β =5.98, SE = 3.13, p =0.06) Obs-Obs Obs-Nas 0 100 200 300 slow fast slow fast condition length of cluster/syllable (ms) id CC- CC Slow: CC > C@C; C@N CN Fast: CC C@C; C@N C@C Speech rate effects on vowel intrusion While increase in the relative overlap of extant articulatory gestures at fast speech rates may be a hallmark of intrusion [2, 5]... p @ d p d ... if relative timing is maintained but spatial displacement adjusted, two Cs may have an audible transition regardless of the speed of articulation [2, 4] C 1 offset C 2 onset p @ d p @ d Although disappearance of in fast speech may be a good in- dicator of excrescence, converse is not necessarily true [4, 5] Summary Both context and speech rate have variable effects on the ap- pearance of acoustic transitions in Khmer Single voiced C can license , but likelihood varies with class; for some cluster types, more common in faster speech Current acoustic data cannot rule out a gestural target associ- ated with acoustic transitions in this language References [1] BUTLER, B. A. Deconstructring the Southeast Asian sesquisyllable: A gestural account. PhD thesis, Cornell University, 2014. [2] B YRD, D., AND T AN, C. C. Saying consonant clusters quickly. Journal of Phonetics 24 (1996), 263–282. [3] COLEMAN, J. The phonetics and phonology of Tashlhiyt Berber syllabic consonants. Trans. Phil. Soc. 99 (2001), 29–64. [4] GAFOS, A. A grammar of gestural coordination. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 20 (2002), 269–337. [5] HALL , N. Cross-linguistic patterns of vowel intrusion. Phonology 23, 3 (2006), 387–429. [6] HUFFMAN, F. E. The boundary between the monosyllable and the disyllable in Cambodian. Lingua 29 (1972), 54–66. [7] JANNEDY , S., F UCHS, S., AND WEIRICH, M. Schwa out of control? In Proc. ISSP 8 (2008), R. Sock et al., Ed., pp. 129–132. [8] K IRBY , J. Incipient tonogenesis in Phnom Penh Khmer: Acoustic and perceptual studies. Journal of Phonetics 43 (2014), 69–85. [9] RIDOUANE , R., AND F OUGERON, C. Schwa elements in Tashlhiyt word-initial clusters. Laboratory Phonology 2 (2011), 275–300. [10] WARNER, N., J ONGMAN, A., C UTLER, A., AND MÜCKE , D. The phonological status of Dutch epenthetic schwa. Phonology 18 (2001), 387–420. 10th International Seminar on Speech Production, 5-8 May 2014, Cologne, Germany.

Upload: voxuyen

Post on 03-Mar-2019

219 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Acoustic transitions in Khmer word-initial clustersjkirby/docs/kirby2014transitions-poster.pdf · Acoustic transitions in Khmer word-initial clusters James Kirby Linguistics and English

Acoustic transitions in Khmer word-initial clustersJames Kirby Linguistics and English Language, University of Edinburgh, Scotland, UK

[email protected]

Introduction

PHONOLOGICAL status of interconsonantal acoustic transitions(∃) known to differ across languages [3, 7, 9, 10]

Khmer (Cambodian) has an inventory of 70+ CC onset clusters

C1 C2

↓ p t tS k P b d m n ñ N l r s hp pt ptS pk pP pd pn pñ pN pl pr ps pht tp tk tP tb tm tn tN tl tr thtS tSp tSk tSP tSb tSd tSm tSn tSN tSl tSh

k kp kt ktS kP kb kd km kn kñ kN kl kr ks khs sp st sk sP sb sd sm sn sñ sN sl srm mt mtS mP md mn mñ ml mr ms mhl lp lk lP lb lm lN lh

Huffman [6]: clusters can be organised into three classesbased on the type of acoustic transition that separates them

Class 1no transition

/psaa/ > [psaa]

Class 2voiceless transition/pteah/ > [phteah]

Class 3voiced transition/pd@j/ > [p@d@j]

What is the phonological status of ∃ in Khmer?

•One possibility: ∃ in Khmer result from gestural underlap [1]•Primary evidence: duration of lexical CV̆C monosyllables >

C∃C clusters; ∃ has more variable formant structure than V̆•Alternatively: whatever ∃ is, it’s just different from V̆, but still

has an associated phonological target•One diagnostic of (phonetic) excrescence: disappearance of ∃

in fast speech [5]

What happens to ∃ at different speech rates?

Methods

0

8000

!"#ក ‘peacock’

k ə ŋ aok

Time (s)1.026 1.489

Freq

uenc

y (H

z)• 10 native speakers (3 female)

recorded producing 79 items ×3 repetitions × 2 rates

• List included 61 C1C2V(C) items(35 cluster types) + 8 C@Cmonosyllables

•All items in carrier phrase /kñomtha: ti@t/ ‘I say again’

Results: Frequency of transition

0.00

0.25

0.50

0.75

1.00

r s h p t k n ŋ lc2

freq

of V

occ

urre

nce condition slow fast Class 1 /Cs Ch/: no

intrusion; on /Cr/: [8]

Class 2: transitionsare more frequent atfaster speech rates!

0.00

0.25

0.50

0.75

1.00

p t b d n ŋ l r s hc2

freq

of V

occ

urre

nce

Class 3: transitionsoften less frequentin fast speech, butnever disappearcompletely

Results: Cluster duration & lexical C@C

Class 1 Class 2 Class 3

0

50

100

150

200

slow fast slow fast slow fastcondition

mea

n cl

uste

r len

gth

(ms)

hasV N YSlow: C∃C ≫ CC(β = 13.5, SE =2.8, p < 0.001)

Fast: weak trend(β = 5.98, SE =3.13, p = 0.06)

Obs-Obs Obs-Nas

0

100

200

300

slow fast slow fastcondition

leng

th o

f clu

ster

/syl

labl

e (m

s)

id CC- CəәCSlow: C∃C > C@C;C@N ≫ C∃N

Fast: C∃C ∼ C@C;C@N ≫ C@C

Speech rate effects on vowel intrusion

•While increase in the relative overlap of extant articulatory gestures at fastspeech rates may be a hallmark of intrusion [2, 5]...

!!!!!!

!!!!!!!!!!!!! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! !! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! !! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! !! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! !! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! !!!! ! ! ! !

p @ d p d

• ... if relative timing is maintained but spatial displacement adjusted, two Csmay have an audible transition regardless of the speed of articulation [2, 4]

!!!!!!

!!!!!!!!!!!!! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! !! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! !! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! !! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! !! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! !!!! ! ! ! !

C1 offset

C2 onset

p @ d p @ d

Although disappearance of ∃ in fast speech may be a good in-dicator of excrescence, converse is not necessarily true [4, 5]

Summary

•Both context and speech rate have variable effects on the ap-pearance of acoustic transitions in Khmer

•Single voiced C can license ∃, but likelihood varies with class;for some cluster types, ∃ more common in faster speech

•Current acoustic data cannot rule out a gestural target associ-ated with acoustic transitions in this language

References[1] BUTLER, B. A. Deconstructring the Southeast Asian sesquisyllable: A gestural account. PhD thesis, Cornell University, 2014.

[2] BYRD, D., AND TAN, C. C. Saying consonant clusters quickly. Journal of Phonetics 24 (1996), 263–282.

[3] COLEMAN, J. The phonetics and phonology of Tashlhiyt Berber syllabic consonants. Trans. Phil. Soc. 99 (2001), 29–64.

[4] GAFOS, A. A grammar of gestural coordination. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 20 (2002), 269–337.

[5] HALL, N. Cross-linguistic patterns of vowel intrusion. Phonology 23, 3 (2006), 387–429.

[6] HUFFMAN, F. E. The boundary between the monosyllable and the disyllable in Cambodian. Lingua 29 (1972), 54–66.

[7] JANNEDY, S., FUCHS, S., AND WEIRICH, M. Schwa out of control? In Proc. ISSP 8 (2008), R. Sock et al., Ed., pp. 129–132.

[8] KIRBY, J. Incipient tonogenesis in Phnom Penh Khmer: Acoustic and perceptual studies. Journal of Phonetics 43 (2014), 69–85.

[9] RIDOUANE, R., AND FOUGERON, C. Schwa elements in Tashlhiyt word-initial clusters. Laboratory Phonology 2 (2011), 275–300.

[10] WARNER, N., JONGMAN, A., CUTLER, A., AND MÜCKE, D. The phonological status of Dutch epenthetic schwa. Phonology 18 (2001), 387–420.

10th International Seminar on Speech Production, 5-8 May 2014, Cologne, Germany.